Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    1/21

    Barbara Dancygier

    The University of British Colum bia

    Narrative Anchors and

    the Processes of Story Construction:

    The C ase of Margaret Atw oo d s The Blind Assassin

    Linguistic theory has often prompted new ways of addressing some of the central

    questions of stylistic analysis (to mention only the emergence of discourse analysis

    or pragm atics). The issues of correlations between form and function, which have

    been crucial to analyses of

    style,

    have recently been given a new form through the

    cogn itive linguists focus on the mechanisms ofmeaning construction.The term

    was coinedtorepresent the fact that describing the senses of simple linguistic forms

    cannot fully account for the resulting interpretation of complex expression w hich

    contains them. Instead, words and other linguistic forms can be treated as prompts

    for the construction of meaning, and the analys t s task is both to identify the

    meaning chunks the expressions prompt, and, perhaps more importantly, to

    describe the processes which explain the construction of the higher levels of

    meaning.

    Recent proposals have also extended stylistic analysis in ways which give

    more focus to cognitive considerations (cf. Semino and Culpeper, Stockwell),

    enriching our understanding of how vocabulary and sentence structure choices

    reflect conceptual constructs (consider the work on metaphor and blending,

    schem as, poetic forms, or speech and though t represen tation). It appears, then, that

    cognitive considerations are enhancing our understanding of both specific

    linguistic choices and general conceptual strategies involved in the construction of

    meaning.

    In retrospect, the definitive works on stylistic analysis, such as Leech and

    Short s Style in Fiction were the first to talk in clear theoretical terms about

    mean ing construction in a literary text. And indeed, literary discou rse is a perfect

    exam ple of

    n

    expression mode where the emerging interpretation relies on the use

    of specific expressions, but cannot be fully explained through those expressions

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    2/21

    134 Barbara Dancygier

    available can be naturally applied to narrative discourse. However, two

    interlocking frameworks which have emerged in the last twenty years, mental

    spaces and blending theories, show significant promise in this respect (see, for

    exam ple, Sanders and Redeker; Oakley; Turner Doub le-scope Stories; Semino

    A Cognitive Stylistic Ap proach, Blending and Charac ters, Text wo rlds,

    Dancygier Identity and Perspective, Visual View point, Blending and

    Narrative ).

    In this paper, I extend the basic concepts of mental spaces theory to identify

    some of the aspects of meaning construction in fragmented narratives. Using

    Margaret Atwood's The Blind ssassinas an example, I introduce the concept of

    narrative anchors to describe some of the mechanisms which underlie the

    construction of a cohe rent story out of several substories and explain som e of the

    sources of various understandings of the text individual readers may arrive at.

    1.

    Story Structure

    The authorial choices in sequencing the presentation of the story may rely on

    different organizational principles. While time seems to be invariably important in

    those choices, other factors often disturb the temporal sequence. In fact,

    contem porary fiction often experim ents with the presentation of the story in ways

    which deliberately make the reader's task harder. Disruptions affect all areas of

    story construction temporal organization, continued identity of characters,

    boundaries between fact and im agination, etc. And yet, in spite of llthe intricacy,

    most readers have no difficulty in understanding the underlying story, and often

    enjoy solving the puzzles presented by the text.

    The disrupted form of many contemporary narratives and the resulting

    challenges to narrative comprehension call for an elaboration of one oftheprimary

    categories of classical narratology the distinction between thestoryon the one

    hand and textanddiscourseon the other.'

    W hile the various aspects ofthe textual surface have been discussed in much

    detail, the story remains an under-defined concept, in spite of emerg ing research

    in cognitive narratology where the underlying nature of story-telling is in focus.

    Recen t work by Herm an ( Stories as a To ol ), for exam ple, discusses stori es as

    tools supporting important aspects of conceptualizing experience. The narrative

    form, in Herman's discussion, prompts for structuring experiences in specific

    ways,such as distinguishing parts, or chunks, seeking causal explanations and

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    3/21

    Narrative Anchors 135

    In classical na rratology, for com parison, a story is a largely intuitive concep t,

    defined primarily by a sequence of events (Chatman

    S tory and

    Discourse;

    Coming

    to Terms .

    The sequentiality of the story as a concept builds on the most basic

    unde rstanding of narrativity as temporal series of connected events, leading from

    some initial state to the final one. What is not discussed too often, though, is the

    very complexity of the mental leap from such a sequence to the story as it is told in

    the text, with its coheren ce and causal organization. Furtherm ore, the very concept

    of what constitutes a

    narrative event

    is an important issue, in view of significant

    differences among agentive acts, mental states, experiential events, etc. What

    emerges from the discussions is the ambiguity of the term story , between the

    characteristics of

    story as a conceptualization device in Herman's sense, and the

    specific story as it emerges from the text telling it. I will refer to the former generic

    concep t as story-G, and to the narrative construct em erging from a specific text as

    story-S,

    and attempt to clarify the processes which yield story-S from the

    interaction of the text and the principles of story-G.

    There are num erous questions regarding the cognitive aspects of story-S. T he

    primary one is what we might metaphorically refer to as the loca tion ofthestory.

    It can reside only in two place s : the text or the rea de r's mind . The text seems to

    be the assumed default choice, very much in the same way as folk semantics has

    meaning residing in the words. The words as containers for me aning idea is

    central to what is now known as the Conduit Metaphor (cf. Reddy, Lakoff and

    Johnson), whereby speakers package the intended meaning into language

    express ions for hearers to receive. If, alternately, we claim that the story resides in

    the reader's mind, then how did it get there? The text (or texts) should be

    responsible for its construction, though there is no one-to-one correspondence

    between textual fragments and story elemen ts. As an exam ple, consider

    The Great

    Gatsbys

    crucial event M yrtle W ilson's death. The story -S makes Daisy

    responsible for it, since she drove the car that hit Myrtle, but the text does not

    describe her doing it. The re is clearly enough in the text to let the reader figure it

    out, but it is not part of what is commu nicated. Th e story is thus richer than the

    text.

    The specific processes which guide the reader in creating the story-S are not

    very well investigated, with notable exceptions of the discussion of contextual

    frames in Em m ott's

    Narrative Comprehension,

    and He rm an's general framework

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    4/21

    136

    Barbara Dancygier

    mental spaces as cognitive domains activated or set up by the use of linguistic

    forms for the purposes of on-line m eaning construction; they are further described

    as not being a part of language

    itself

    or a hidden level of representation, but as

    having an indispensab le role in the construction of meaning. The role of linguistic

    expressions is thus seen not as con tainers for meaning, but as prom pts which

    guide its construction. Ifw assume, somewhat loosely, I admit, that the story -S

    is the meaning of the text, then we might learn more about the nature of the story

    by looking at the role specific textual choices play in its emergence.

    The concept of emergent meaning is most transparently described in terms of

    theblending of mental spaces, or conceptual integration (Fauconnier and Turner

    Blending as a Central Process, Conceptual Integration, Principles, The

    Way we Thin k, Coulson). For example, when the author Jonathan Raban recalls

    his childhood experience of Twain's

    Huckleberry Finn,

    he describes it as

    living

    inside the book and goes on to elaborate on the fantasy world thus set up. The

    expression cannot be understood literally (the narrator lived in an English village)

    and is not conventiona l, so we can assume that the reader has to undertake mental

    work to arrive at its meaning. A skeletal description of the blending processes is

    given in Figure 1.

    Childhood

    Book

    English v illage-4-

    boy

    stream -4

    no adventure

    Input 1

    ^Amer ica

    Huck Finn

    >-the Mississippi River

    adventure

    -America

    -Huck Finn

    -theMississippi^iver

    -adventure

    Input 2

    Blend

    ig 1

    There are two input spaces,that of Jonathan Raban's childhood (with its temporal

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    5/21

    Narrative Anchors 137

    spaces are linked viacross-mappings which reveal and/or construct counterpart

    elements in the inputs (e.g., the stream in the village and the Mississippi river).

    Blending these inputs selectively projects Raban's childhood persona into the

    temporal and spatial setting of

    th

    novel. Theemergent

    stru ture

    of

    th

    blend is not

    simply a combination of these elements, but an emergent mental space where a

    child living in 20th century England can experience adventure on the Mississippi.

    The blend thus creates its own topology and yields meanings not available in the

    inputs. At the same tim e, the blend may inform its inputs, viabackward projection

    so that we can understand the ways in which reading made the boy's village

    experience richer.

    The analysis of this example applies to a narrative fragment, but the

    mechanisms underlying its comprehension seem to work in similar ways when a

    broader narrative context is considered . The novel as a whole can be talked about

    in term sof narrative spaces which can be defined as cognitive dom ains activated

    or set up in the reader 's m ind by the use of linguistic form s, for the purposes of on-

    line story construction . A narrative space, once set

    up,

    is maintained through out the

    story and serves as input to blends which will be constructed . The final sto ry-S

    (with its event structure, sequential organization, cause-effect organization, and

    other generic story-like features, and also its text-induced specificity) results from

    subsequen t blend ing of lower level narrative spaces, and can be referred to as the

    emergent

    story.

    As a blended construct, it relies on selective projection from lower

    level spaces, but creates narrative structure of its own (roughly equivalent to an

    idealized read er's version of what happened ) . Like other blended constructs, it

    also projects new structure back to its input narrative spaces, thus enriching their

    content with new topology (especially, new cause-effect relations).

    The specific aspect of story-S construction which I elaborate here is the

    privileged status of selected linguistic forms in the setting up and m aintenance of

    narrative spaces, and in prom pting cross-space mappings further used in the blends

    leading to the emergent story. Such expressions will be referred to as narrative

    anchors.

    2.Narrative Spaces and Narrative Anchors

    An example of the scope of meaning cons truc t ion s imple express ions can

    accomp lish is the f irst sentence of M argare t A tw oo d 's nove l . The Blind Assassin

    given in (1) below.

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    6/21

    138 Barbara Dancygier

    The sentence seems to be a straightforward description of a tragic event, reported

    by La ura's sister. Iris. The highly specific description of

    the

    time of Laura's death,

    however, suggests that it is not simply an instance of temporal framing.

    Furtherm ore, the choice of the verbdriveimplies agenthood, and, consequently,

    intentional behavior, so that it is unlikely that Laura's death was a result of an

    accident. The policeman's report in the next paragraph also mentions two

    witnesses who testified that Laura had turned the car sharply and delibe rately. In

    the third paragrap h. Iris herself comments: It wasn't the brakes, I though t. She had

    her reasons.

    The novel is a fragmented narrative, with Iris's pseudo-autobiographical

    confession as its central element. Itisthe core of

    he

    Blind Assassinnarrative (I will

    refertoit sBA and is the fram ing story of all the remaining narrative fragments.

    She starts her writing as an old woman, describing her daily tasks and walks, and

    using them as prompts for reminiscing about her earlier life thus the actual

    autobiography proceeds chrono logically from her childhood to old age. The initial

    paragraphs, w here Lau ra's death is described in the terms quoted above, how ever,

    are taken out of the temporal sequence of the memoirs. The viewpoint of this

    section (called The bridge is unclear it is a part of the autobiog raphy, but Iris is

    also announcing herself

    s

    the primary narrator. The doub le role is reflected in the

    discourse. For exam ple. Iris introduces Laura s her sister while disregarding any

    description of other characters mentioned her husband Richard or her caregiver,

    Reenie. And yet, the fragment's main role is not to start Iris's narrative, but to set

    up the space of the focal event of Laura's death, while breaking the temporal

    sequence and further suggesting that it was suicide. The fact that The bridge

    violates temporal order is not remarkable in itself given that many texts do just

    that, but it does pose a question with regard to the role of temporal seq uence in the

    coherence of the story.

    The primary role of The bridgeis to set the theme of the novel as a whole

    what Laura really did and why, and the even t's role in the sequence is significantly

    less relevant. W e should probably note that among all the patterns of presenta tion

    mentioned in Leech and Short, the chronological may be the most obvious, but

    neither the most common nor the most interesting stylistically. Classical

    narratological accounts (e.g., Genette, Bal, Toolan) argue further that a

    chronological presentation is never really just chronological, given that there are

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    7/21

    Narrative Anchors 139

    what makes chronological disruptions interesting is not that they occur, but how

    they affect the understanding of the story. A flashback does not just break the

    sequence, but breaks it so that important aspects of story construction are

    highlighted such as the character's current psychological state or the way in

    which past actions affect future choices.

    Assuming thatB starts the way it does in order to establish the main them e of

    the novel allows us to also see how the m ain narrative spaces are initially setup.We

    have already looked at the way in which the text signals the suic ide space, along

    with its twin, the accident space. Later, towards the end of the section Iris refers

    to notebooks which Laura left for her to read:

    (2).

    . .the stack of cheap school exercise books that she must have hidden that very

    morning .. .. knowing would be the onetofind them.

    The reader can assume that the notebooks contain information explaining Laura's

    decision to killherself which only Iris, the narrator, can have access to. Another

    space is thus set-up, the no tebo oks space, subordinate to the suicide -spa ce. It

    is structured similarly to the boo k space in theH uck Finnexample above in

    both spaces the written text contains information (fictional story, letter, testimony ,

    etc.) which may be available to a person if the physical object containing the

    information can be accessed. The novel makes it clear that the content of the

    notebooks -space is now known to Iris and that it may explain Laura's reasons.

    But the space, similarly to the suicide -space, is merely signaled, and is expected

    to be elaborated throughout the narrative. In spite of their skeletal topology, the

    spaces will continue to structure the reader's understanding of the unfolding text,

    as new information is added.

    The acciden t -space, which is initially introduced in The bridge is further

    elaborated on the next page, in a clipping from The Toronto Star which describes

    Laura's death as an accident. Many of such clippings are spread throughout the

    text, creating a running com mentary on the events narrated by Iris, presented from

    the official or public point of view (which the reader will gradually learn to

    associate with Ir is's husband, Richard Griffen). Together, the clippings structure a

    shared v iewpoint space, where no new information is added, bu t the story told by

    Iris is given its public dimension. Just as an exam ple, the first fragment talks about

    accidental deathofMiss Laura C haseand adds thath er car swerved. The choice

    of words here avoids attributing any intention to Laura, in contrast to The bridge

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    8/21

    140 Barbara Dancygier

    The expressions such asdrove, the carswerved orexercise booksfunction as

    narrative place-ho lders for mental spaces which can only be signaled at this point

    in the narrative. They are expected to be expanded in the ensuing narrative and are

    presented scontributing to the topology or framing of other narrative spaces. Such

    place-ho lder or portal expressions can be calledn arrative anchors,since they

    open access to spaces which are only partially set up. As further exam ples will

    show , narrative ancho rs may not only set up spaces to be elaborated later, but they

    also build cross-mappings that link different narrative spaces, add structure to

    them, and prompt projections which set up subsequent levels of narrative blends.

    They are, then, expressions w hich con tribute in specific ways to the construction of

    constituen t spaces and the resulting network. As I will argue , any story relies on a

    number of narrative spaces and, while it may eventually yield a chronological

    understanding of the events, it develops its structure on the basis of developing

    links and cross-m appings. The setting up ofthe specific input narrative spaces and

    the overall organization of the network constructed through the text depend in a

    crucial way on narrative anchors.

    The concept of conceptual anchors emerges first

    in

    Hu tchins' discussion ofthe

    role m aterial objects may play in blending . In order to play a role, they have to be

    perceived, but, as Hutchins suggests, (selectively) perceiving objects is a step

    which does not need to be treated separately from (also selectively) projec ting them

    into blends. Hutchins comments further that material objects as anchors have the

    stability required to serve in further computations, since they are capable of

    h old ing conceptual elements in place. The discussion of material anchors such

    as clocks or calendars makes it clear that anchors do not have any intrinsic qua lity,

    but rather have to be used in a certain way, so that material elements are used as

    proxies for conceptual elements.

    Narrative anchors, as I discuss them here, share some features with material

    anch ors. They have a m aterial form in some sense, since they are written

    expressions in the text. They are used in connecting or representing conceptual

    elements (narrative spaces and aspects of their structure) and they are not

    inherently predestined to play the role of anchors, but narrative structure may select

    them to ho ld parts of narrative structure and cross-space links in place. In other

    words, they participate in blending processes in ways somewhat similar to their

    material counterparts.

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    9/21

    Narrative Anchors 141

    The novella (which will be termed fiA7-space, as a sub-pace of the overall BA

    story) is much more elaborate than other spaces in the network it is an

    independent text but its links to the rest of

    the

    text ofBAare not obvious. There

    are numerous aspects of

    the

    BAJwording which give

    it a

    special place in the overall

    narrative.

    First, there are no proper names in the text. There are two characters, referred

    to throughout via third person pronou ns heandshe.Personal pronouns may play

    very significant roles in texts

    see

    Em mott, Constructing Social Space ), and there

    are some narratives where pronouns are the only referring expressions. But in the

    case of BAl which is situated within the context of the rest of the novel,

    establishing the referents of the pronouns is a necessary part of understanding the

    whole

    story.

    The question of howcloselyBA

    needs to be blended into the n etwork

    starts with these pronouns.

    It

    is

    possible to assume that the text constructing theBA l narrative space could

    be just what it ostensibly is a post-modern romance, and any resemblance

    between the characters inBAl to other characters inBAcounts merely as playfully

    teasing the reader. On closer inspection, though, the links between BAl and other

    narrative spaces are most certainly meaningful. The logical chain which starts with

    discovering the identities of the lovers referred to as he and sh e leads to the

    discovery of crucial aspects ofthe story

    as

    a whole, such

    as

    the identity ofthe author

    of the novella constituting the BAl space, and, perhaps most importantly, the

    explanation of La ura 's reaso ns. In other words, the final story cannot be

    constructed without incorporating the structure of theB i/-spac e. At the end of her

    autobiograph ical narrative. Iris explicitly fills some (but not all) gaps , but she also

    adds that the reader could have discovered the untold facts without her help.

    Indeed, the reader

    can

    only do this by recognizing the anchors that linkBA to other

    spaces, and it is unlikely that all these links were purely coincidental.

    As I will show below, approaching the text as a network of interconnected

    narrative spaces reveals unexpected richness of meaning, while at the same time

    giving coherence to various narrative devices which could otherw ise be construed

    as impediments.

    The text ofBAl starts with aPrologue:

    (3) She has a single photograph of him. She tucked i t into a brown e nve lope . . . between

    the pages of Perennials for the Rock Garden where no one else would ever look the

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    10/21

    142 Barbara Dan cygier

    In reality, pho tographs, like books, are material anchors for representation spaces.

    The image in a photograph is like a timeless window to the specific time and space

    represented, whether identifiable or not. In the novel space, the photo plays a

    similar role for the characters, but its description also works as a narrative anchor

    for readers. The pho tograph is in fact the most am biguous of the anchors inBA,for

    a num ber of reasons. First, it is the only record of her and him together, but

    there is the han d of yet another, anonym ous, person. So the story involves three

    participan ts, not just two the han d is an anchor to the construction of another

    participant in the story. Also , the photograph is hidden in another book so that

    by discovering the owner of the book the reader can work out who sh e is.

    In fact, the Prologue to BAJ mimics the role The bridge plays in BA it

    prov ides the narrative anchors needed. But there is an important difference. W hile

    The bridge sets up the narrative spaces the novel as a whole will elaborate and

    explore, thePrologueopens up spaces needed to fill in the narrative structure ofits

    higher narrative, the BA, notBAJ space. Everything captured in the photo - the

    picnic, the man w ith a cigarette, or the hand cut off

    by

    the margin belongs in the

    structureof

    BA,

    but the woman inBAJholding the photograph is the link between

    the two sp aces. In spite of ostensibly writing theB story as an autobiography . Iris

    does not tell the reader all that she knows, so the rest is said through BAJ.

    A

    reader w ho exploresThebridgeand theProloguecarefully enough will then

    receive plenty of opportunities to understand the role BAJplays in the story as a

    wh ole. Just as an example, the identity of the man inB AJis easily established by

    cross-links such as the following description of the events at the Button Factory

    picnic:

    4)Thenhe took apicture forthe paper with hiscamera....AlexThomas raisedhis hand

    as iftofend him off. BA)

    TheBAJanchor of the photograph, representing the picnic and the man's gesture,

    now links the man to a character in BA. The story-structuring power of such

    anchors is quite substantial. Once the reader recognizes the man in the picture as

    Alex , all that is known about both Alex and him begins to characterize Alex and

    his participation in the

    events.

    The story being told

    in B is

    not coherent without the

    added inputs, though it remains very traditionally chronological, and in order to

    construct the complete story the reader has to incorporate information from BAJ

    intoBA.Since this is a pattern affecting most of the events that can explain La ura's

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    11/21

    Narrative Anchors 143

    causal links and deeper understanding of the characters and their motivations

    not a surprising observation at all, but often back-grounded in narratological

    discussions of the story .

    Another anchor of crucial importance is the identity of the woman in BA/.

    Here the narrative prompts are significantly more misleading, as they point

    alternately to Laura and to Iris. Indeed, they are intentionally misleading. The

    interesting question here is why the reader follows the promp ts and assumes one or

    the other oft he women to be the she character in BAl without explicitly being

    asked to do so. The text, in whichever sense, does not require such a conscious

    enquiry, and, as the comments by some critics suggest (cf. Roy),BAl could be

    treated as an independent story, simply elaborating some of the themes. Without

    the narrative structuring work that the reader does, however, the novel would be

    rather incoherent. Narrative anchors are story-level devices relying on the reade r's

    search for coherence the descriptions of identical gestures in identical situations

    have to be considered relevant when the identity of one of the (gesturing)

    characters is unclear, or the whole text becomes mess of conflicting story snippets

    and no coherence is possible. The cross-space links established and the plot

    elements read into narrative spaces establish narrative coherence across

    different spaces, which, as the goal of story-construction, leads to narrative

    comprehension.

    Though literary critics writing about The Blind Assassin have much to say

    about the significance of, let 's say, use of color or the recurring them e of violated

    women's bodies (Perrakis, Roy) in search for the more abstract levels of

    interpretation, narrative structure is not included in these consid erations. The

    fragmentation and deliberate deception are treated as com ponents of post-

    modern style, while they are, in this case, features of narration. They require that

    the participating narrative spaces be integrated, via narrative anchors and cross-

    space projection, into the final emergent story. The emergent blend is not

    dependent on any of the individual spaces alone, but on the story-construction

    mechanisms.

    The last ofth e major narrative spaces inThe Blind Assassinis another piece of

    fiction, embedded inBAl. It is a pulp science-fiction story, composed during the

    secret meetings ofthelovers. As the reader can conclude, the man, who is in hiding

    through most of the story, makes a living publishing such popular fiction. Not

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    12/21

    Barbara Dancygier

    to the genre , is murky. The only obvious indication of the link with the main story

    world is the character of a blind assassin I will refer to it, then, asBA2.

    The story told in theBA2-space,which is in fact never finished, takes place on

    the planet of Zycron, where blinded children weave carpets and young women,

    with their tongues cut out, are sacrificed on the ceremonial altar, after being raped

    by the lord of the underworld. With its style, themes, and genre the story

    constituting theBA2space seems ou t of place in the novel as a whole. W hile critics

    carefully pick out some of the themes (blindness, rape, practices equivalent to

    sacrificing young women by marrying them off to rich but cruel husbands, etc.),

    they do not mention any of the important narrative contributions the fiy42-space

    makes by filling in the gaps in the main story line.

    According to the mental spaces analysis proposed by Schnepf BA2 sets up a

    set ofroles,to be matched with valuesin other spaces. Role-value links are among

    the most salient mechanisms of mental space networks. In the most standard case,

    the frame of the US political system profiles the role of the President, (with its

    attendant duties and privileges), but also demands that a specific individual (the

    value) is determined by elections every four years. The role-value link, once

    established, allows speakers to access the value-individual through the role, and

    vice versa. If

    speaker saysG eorge Bush wasth governor of Texas,she might be

    referring to the role (the current president held a different political office before),

    or the value (the man called George Bush held a political office before). The roles

    are typically established within an appropriate frame, and the frame may be

    constructed in spontaneous discourse, supported by an understanding of what

    lexical items mean (the buyer and seller roles in the scenario evoked by the verb

    sell , or determined socially (the President). Proper names typically represent

    values, rather than roles, while specific phrases designate roles rather than values.

    Establishing the role-value links across spaces thus gives mental access from

    one space to the other. If the roles designated by the story inBA2(the king, the m ute

    girl to be sacrificed, the blind assassin, the lord ofthe underw orld) are matched with

    the names of characters in BA, the cross-space links thus set up are of the most

    com mo n and inconsp icuous kind, but there is a significant narrative gain, as access

    across narrative spaces fills more gaps in Iris s story. Let us consider jus t o ne

    example, as discussed bySchnepf.In BA2,the lord ofth e underworld plots against

    the king. He hires a professional (blind) assassin to kill the king, and also gives a

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    13/21

    Narrative Anchors 145

    mute girl, and that the lord of the underworld is Iris's husband, Richard, and,

    finally, that the blind assassin is Alex. The events leading to Iris's encounter with

    Alex and to her marriage to Richard now become known. It is clear that it was

    Richard w ho hired Alex to set Iris's father's factory on fire, and that Iris was forced

    to marry Richard in exchange for financial help for the family. Most importantly,

    we could not find out about Richard's role in the fire and the resulting damage to

    the Chase family from Iris alone. She does not know about all this. But w hen it is

    framed into the story that Alex tells her (under the pretence of 6/42), overall

    coherence is preserved, since he is the only person who knows what really

    happened then. Consequently, the gaps in the plot of

    BA

    could not be filled in any

    other way.

    At the same time we should note that coding the real story into what Alex

    writes is not as artificial a narrative device at it might seem Alex could not

    simply tell Iris about

    is

    role in the fire and thus adm it to have ruined her family and

    her own life in the process. Instead, Alex makes this convoluted confession as if he

    were inviting Iris to do what the reader does blend the narrative spaces into a

    coherent story.

    This aspect of story construction is also particularly interesting from the

    linguistic and stylistic point of

    view.

    The main charactersofB A Alex and Iris, are

    referred to in different narrative spaces via different referring expressions: proper

    names inB A pronouns inBAl and role descriptors in BA2.Since there are hardly

    any other ways to refer to people, the BAl and BA2 narratives exhaust all the

    available options of identifying a character, without coming up with other proper

    names. The linguistic construction is dauntingly simple, but the results spread

    through the entire story.

    Most surprisingly, the critical work on the novel that I have seen does not

    acknowledge the role

    ofBA

    as a story-constructing factor, instead focusing on the

    aspects of the character's construction (such as blindness or mutilation) which do

    not quite work w ithout the rest of what

    BA

    contributes (cf. Perrakis, Roy, W ilson).

    For exam ple, when these critics notice that Iris describes the marks of violence on

    her body and that the sacrificial virgin in BA2is also mutilated and is meant to be

    raped, they use these observations to talk about the treatmen t of w om en 's bod ies as

    one of the themes ofth e novel. The critics' focus on such them es effectively allows

    them to ignore narrative structure. But the links between the roles inBA2and the

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    14/21

    146 Barbara Dancyg ier

    not know (though perhaps guesses) that, even more ironically, her lover's leftist

    views have destroyed her father, a fair employer, only to supplant him with a

    ruthless business magnate. The inclusion of BA2 is also crucial with respect to

    viewpoint managem ent. Given thatB and B Al are told from Iris 's viewpoint,BA2

    is the only part of the narrative that can provide parts of the story not available to

    her, and so Atwood creates a masterly construction where Alex's viewpoint is

    blended into the rest of the text. Again, the major contribution a narrative space

    makes to the story is not related to its temporal sequen ce, but to its coherence and

    focus on character motivation.

    The links among the major spaces prompt an explanation of the title The

    Blind Assassin. The phrase, while ostensibly coined to refer to the group of

    professional killers in BA2 acquires a specific m etaphorical m eaning on the basis

    of the KNOWING IS SEEING mapping, whereby expressions such as unclear

    transparent vision being in the dark being illuminated etc. are used to describe

    epistemic states. It is also metaphorically conventional to call a personblindwhen

    they lack understanding. Building on this easily accessible mapping, many critics

    assume that the blind assassin in the title is Iris (cf. Wilson, Bouson) she

    unknowingly causes her sister's death, by casually informing her that the man

    Laura loved died, and thus rem oving her reason for living. But it is possible to use

    the same metaphorical argument in combination with the actual narrative, and then

    Alex emerges as an even better candidate for the role. Like the blind assassin in

    BA2

    he is paid to commit a crim e, and he does what he is paid for, but then falls in

    love and steals the precious bride from his employer. But he is also metaphorically

    blind, for he could not foresee that the knowledge of his affair with Iris will kill

    Laura in the end.

    Furthermore, in the story in BA2both lovers are mutilated he is blind, but

    she is mute. As Schnepf shows in detail, being a sacrificial bride and being made

    unable to talk app lies to Iris in a straightforward way. Iris matches the description

    of the girl in B A2as swollen with words and describes her own forced silence in

    similar term s. In BAshe is telling her story for the first time, and still hides much

    ofit even though she

    s

    the framing narrator. The role ofthe blind assassin

    s

    thus

    ambiguous, but the interpretation suggested in BA2 seems better supported.

    4.

    Conclusion

    As was noted in the introduction, cognitive approaches to the narrative are

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    15/21

    Narrative Anchors

    understanding of how stories create their own logic, and how that logic is then

    extended over other mental dom ains. What I have argued in this article, though, is

    that the search for cognitive underpinnings of the narrative can and indeed should

    include renewed focus on the text.

    As the discussion above suggests, linguistically relevant choices of referential

    expressions, verbs, or mental space builders can influence the understanding of the

    narrative in ways which affect both the very interpretation of events and cha racte rs'

    minds, and the narrative structure as such. These choices matter for specific

    elements of the story-S the text tells, the identity of the characters, and the very

    understanding of its main them es. They do not do so, however, by directly adding

    information in the way we expect all texts to do , but by prom pting meaning

    construction beyond the text as such through setting up n arrative spaces and links

    across those spaces. In other words, mental space structure mediates between the

    meaning contributed directly by the text and the meaning emerging from generic

    principles of narrative construction as such.

    The questions regarding the mental status of story-S which I posed at the

    beginn ing cannot yet be answered in full, but the concepts introduced here answ er

    some of

    them.

    The story em erging from any particular text is the intermediate level

    of m eaning, with its own charac teristics. On the one hand, the con struct builds on

    the more specific mean ing contributions of textual dev ices, while being framed on

    the other hand by the general narrative concepts such as chunking or search for

    causation. W hile the text prompts a myriad of mental spaces, it also uses narrative

    anchors to privilege those which participate in story construction I called them

    narrative spaces. The readers' search for overall story coherence helps in

    recognizing the signals given textually, hence some (often significant) differences

    in the readers' responses.

    The sequential nature of the story (traditionally treated as its defining

    characteristic) is a result, not the constitutive feature. Furthermore, events do not

    form a linear chronological sequence instead, they cluster together to form

    larger and larger hierarchically organized blended structures. The resulting story is

    therefore a multi-layered network of narrative spaces which yields a sequence,

    even if only a roughly conceived one, as a consequence of its organization.

    The final narrative structure of

    heBlind ssassin

    can be roughly represented

    in Figure 2, with the emergent story being the final product of the integrated

    network of spaces set up in the course of the text. Som e of its spaces (like the

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    16/21

    148

    Barbara Dancygier

    v-point: Iris

    notebooks

    v-point: Laura

    Iris remembers

    BA

    v-point: Iris

    BAI

    v-point: Iris

    BA2

    v-point: Alex

    accident

    v-point: Iris

    Emergent Story

    Fig 2.

    comparison, the spaces participating in the central blend BA, BAI and BA2) are

    effectively telling the same story and they do so by prompting for cross-space

    connectio ns, for identity blends (Iris is she and the mute girl) and for the overall

    integration of various aspects ofthestory presented in the input spaces in the guise

    of independent narratives. BA remains the main narrative space which projects

    most of its structure into the emergent blend (as indicated, among others, by its

    reliance on proper names), in contrast toB AJandBA2,which contribute plot and

    characterization pieces to fill in the gaps inB A.It might not be accurate, how ever,

    to callBAa framing narrative (for exam ple in the sense described by Herm an in

    Genette meets Vygotsky ). The relationship among the various spaces is much

    less that of embedding, and is more appropriately described as integration, which

    poses a further question of the cogn itive benefit of such stories (in contrast to

    framed stories, which Herman discusses in terms of distributed cognition).

    The B lind Assassinis a thought-provoking laboratory experiment in narrative

    spaces construction and blending , but the process is, I argue, the same in fictional

    narratives of

    ll

    kinds, even though they m ay be ostensibly less fragmented. In the

    analysis above, I have focused on selected aspects of story-construction, and the

    role of narrative anchors in the emergence of stories as blends of participant

    narrative spaces. Numerous questions, however, are still not formulated, let alone

    answered. How do narratives distinguish various kinds of narrative space

    interactions? How is viewpoint established and maintained? H ow are the different

    spatial and temporal features of the inputs reconciled in the blend? How is

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    17/21

    Narrative Anchors ' ^

    conceptualization device are numerous and intricate. It seems, however, that

    conceptual integration offers tools which might help us propose more and more

    accurate answ ers.

    Note

    ' Narratologists use a number of two-fold or three-fold distinctions here

    (fabula / sjuzhet, story / text, story / text / narration, etc). Because the focus of my

    discussion is on the tension between the story and everything else, I will stick to the

    'story' versus 'text,' while elaborating the concept of 'story-S.'

    Works Cited

    Atwood, Margaret.

    The Blind Assassin.

    Toronto: Seal Books, 2000.

    Bal, Mieke.Narratology: Introduction to the Th eory of Narrative.Trans. C hristine

    van Boheemen. Toronto: U of Toronto P, 1985.

    Bouso n, J. Brook s. A Comm emoration of W ounds Endured and Resented.

    Critique44.3 (2003): 25 1-7 0.

    Chatman, Seymour.

    S tory and Discourse: N arrative Structure in Fiction and Film.

    Ithaca: Cornell UP, 1978.

    .C oming to Terms:

    th

    Rhetoric of Narrative F iction and

    Film.

    Ithaca: Cornell

    UP, 1990.

    Coulson, Seana.

    Semantic Leaps: Frame-shifting and Conceptual Blending in

    Meaning Construction.Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 20 01.

    Dancygier, Barbara. Blending and narrative viewpoint: Jonathan Rab an's travels

    through Mental Spaces.

    Language and Literature

    14.2 (2005): 99-127.

    , Identity and Perspec tive: the Jekyll-and -Hyde Effect in Na rrative

    Discourse.

    Language Culture and

    Mind

    Ed. Michel Achard and Suzanne

    Kemmer. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 2004. 363-76.

    Visual Viewpoint, Narrative Viewpoint, and Mental Spaces in Narrative

    Discourse. Linguagem Cultura e Cognigao: Estudos de Linguistica

    Cognitiva.Ed. A. Soares da Silva, A. Torres, and M . Gonalves, 2004. Vol. 2.

    347-62.

    E m m ot t, Catherine. Constructing Social Space: Socio-Cognitive Factors in the

    Interpretation of Character Relations.

    Narrative Theory and the Cognitive

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    18/21

    Barbara Dancygier

    Fauconnier, Gilles. 1985.

    Mental Spaces.

    2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge UP

    1994.

    .

    Mappings in Thought and Language.

    Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 1997.

    Fauconn ier, Gilles, .and Eve Sweetser, eds.

    Spaces, Worlds, and Grammars.

    Chicago: U of Chicago P, 1996.

    Fauconnier, G illes, and Mark Turner. Blending as a Central Process of

    Grammar. Conceptual Structure, Discourse, and Language. Ed. A.

    Goldberg. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 1996. 113-30.

    . Conceptual Integration Netw orks. Cogni'fi've Sd enc e 22 2(1 99 8)- 13 3 -

    87.

    . Princip les of Conceptual Integra tion. Discourse and Cognition.Ed. J.P.

    Koenig et al. Stanford: C SLI Publications, 1998. 26 9- 83 .

    .

    The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending a nd the Mind s Hidden

    Complexities.

    New York: Basic Books, 2002.

    Genette, Gerard. Narrative Discourse: An Essay in Method Trans. J.E. Lewin.

    Ithaca: Cornell UP, 1980.

    Herm an, David. Genette meets Vygotsky: narrative embedding and distributed

    intelligence.

    Language and Literature

    15.4 (2006): 357-80.

    . Ed.N arrativeTheoryandthe ognitiveSciences.Stanford, California: C SLI

    Publications,

    2003.

    . Ed. Stories as a Tool for Th inkin g. Narrative Theory and the Cognitive

    Sciences.

    2003.

    163-93.

    .Story Logic: P roblems and Possibilities of Narrative.Lincoln and London:

    U of Nebraska P, 2002.

    Hutchins, Edw in. M aterial Anchors for Conceptual Blen ds.

    Journal of

    Pragmatics 37 (2005): 1555-77.

    akoff George, and Mark Johnson.

    Metaphors we Live by.

    Chicago: The U of

    Chicago P, 1980.

    Leech, Geoffrey N. and Michael H. Short, Style in Fiction: A Linguistic

    Introduction to English F ictional Prose.London: Longman, 1981.

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    19/21

    Narrative Anchors 51

    Oakley, Todd. Conceptual Blending, Narrative Discourse, and Rhetoric.

    Cogn itive Linguistics9.4 (1998): 321-36 0.

    Perrakis, Phyllis. Negotiating with the Looking G lass: Atwood, her Protagonists,

    and the Journey to the Dead . Ed. M oss and Kozakiewich 200 6. 349 -5 9.

    Raban, Jonathan.

    Old Glory.

    New York: Vintage Books, 1998.

    Reddy , M ichael. The conduit metaphor a case of frame conflict in our language

    about language. Metaphor and Thought. Ed. Andrew Ortony. Cambridge:

    Cambridge UP, 1979. 284-97.

    Roy, W endy. The body of/as evidence: M argaret Atwood, The Blind Assassin,

    and the Feminist Literary M ystery. Ed. M oss and Kozakiewich. 2006. 3 6 1 -

    71.

    Sanders, Jose and Gisela Redeker. Perspective and the Representation of Speech

    and Though t in Narrative Discourse. Ed. G. Fauconnier and E. Sweetser.

    2006 .290-317 .

    Sawyer, R. Keith.Social Emergence: Societies as Complex Systems. Cambridge:

    Cambridge U P, 2005.

    chnepf Jennifer. Role-Value M appings and Narrative Structure in Atw ood 's

    The Blind Assassin. Paper presented the Literature and Cognitive Science

    Conference. Storrs, CT, April 6-9, 2006.

    Sem ino, Elena. Blending and Characters: Mental Functioning in Virginia W oo lf s

    Lappin and Lapinova. Languag e and Literature

    15.1 (2006):

    55-73.

    . A Cognitive Stylistic Approach to Mind Style in Narra tive Fiction. Ed .

    Semino and Culpeper. 2002. 95 -12 2.

    . Text wo rlds.

    Cognitive Poetics.

    Ed. Geert Brone and Joeren Vandaele.

    Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter, forthcoming.

    Semino, Elena and Jonathan Culpeper, eds.

    Cognitive Stylistics: Language and

    Cognition in

    ext

    Ana lysis.Philadelphia: John Benjamins, 2002.

    Stockwell, Peter. Cogn itive Poetics.New York: Routledge, 2002.

    Tooian, Michael.Narrative: A Critical Linguistic Introduction. 2nd ed. London:

    Routledge, 2001 .

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    20/21

    ]52 Barbara Dancygier

    W ilson, Sharon. Blindness and Survival in M argaret Atw ood 's Major Nove ls.

    The Cambridge Companion to Margaret Atwood Ed. C. A. Howells.

    Cam bridge: Cam bridge UP, 2006.

    176-91.

  • 8/10/2019 Story Construction in the Blind Assassin

    21/21