40
Addressing Difficulties with Subtraction Aims of Research When I first took the role of mathematics subject leader I implemented an action plan to improve the quality of provision for calculation. Calculation refers to approaches used to find the answers to problems in each of the four operations of addition, subtraction, multiplication and division. As part of the action plan staff identified key learning objectives and written methods associated with calculation from The Primary Framework (2006) and ‘Guidance Paper: Calculation’ (DCSF 2006). We matched the objectives and written methods to different year groups and I produced ‘calculation pyramids’ that showed the progression for each operation from Foundation to Year 6 (see Appendix 1 for Subtraction Pyramid). The exercise helped staff to recognise the development in calculation and the resource was used in most classes. However, SATs analysis (see Appendix 2 for analysis of SATs results) indicated that a significant proportion of children continued to experience difficulties with calculation, suggesting that calculation was an area that needed to be improved. Alongside this a Year 6 test showed significant weaknesses within subtraction (see Appendix 3 for Year 6 test analysis). Amongst the children who found it most difficult were those who generally had lower levels of attainment, including those with special educational needs (SEN). This 1

Subtraction Assignment 18

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

 

Citation preview

Page 1: Subtraction Assignment 18

Addressing Difficulties with Subtraction

Aims of Research

When I first took the role of mathematics subject leader I implemented an action plan to

improve the quality of provision for calculation. Calculation refers to approaches used to

find the answers to problems in each of the four operations of addition, subtraction,

multiplication and division. As part of the action plan staff identified key learning objectives

and written methods associated with calculation from The Primary Framework (2006) and

‘Guidance Paper: Calculation’ (DCSF 2006). We matched the objectives and written methods

to different year groups and I produced ‘calculation pyramids’ that showed the progression

for each operation from Foundation to Year 6 (see Appendix 1 for Subtraction Pyramid). The

exercise helped staff to recognise the development in calculation and the resource was used in

most classes.

However, SATs analysis (see Appendix 2 for analysis of SATs results) indicated that a

significant proportion of children continued to experience difficulties with calculation,

suggesting that calculation was an area that needed to be improved. Alongside this a Year 6

test showed significant weaknesses within subtraction (see Appendix 3 for Year 6 test

analysis). Amongst the children who found it most difficult were those who generally had

lower levels of attainment, including those with special educational needs (SEN). This

reinforced the aim identified in the School’s Development Plan (SDP) which was to improve

the attainment of ‘school action’ SEN children (children who receive extra provision as stated

in a Provision Map). As part of the SDP staff needed to develop strategies to support these

children. As a result this assignment aims to focus on addressing difficulties with

subtraction; especially those experienced by children with lower levels of attainment.

Review of Literature

There has been much debate over the ‘correct’ (Brown, 2010) way to teach mathematics and

about what children need to learn or understand. In the Primary Framework (2006: p.66) it

states that mathematics ‘is a combination of concepts, facts, properties, rules, patterns and

processes.’ It also states that there should be ‘lessons where the emphasis is on technique and

[lessons where] children are steered to discover the rules, patterns or properties of numbers or

1

Page 2: Subtraction Assignment 18

shapes.’ Such an approach covers two teaching positions: procedural, the ‘accurate use of

calculating procedures’; and conceptual, the ‘possession of number sense which underlies the

ability to apply [calculation] procedures sensibly.’(Brown in Thompson, 2010: p.3). The

Primary Framework’s (2006) ‘emphasis on technique’ can be interpreted as a procedural

position and within the strategy there are calculating objectives that call for the use of

‘efficient written methods’ (The Primary Framework, 2006: p.80). Written methods vary

from informal methods such as an empty number line that can support children’s

understanding to formal or standard written algorithms. Many (Anghileri, 2006; Brown,

2010; Thompson, 2010) argue that the teaching of formal written algorithms is limiting and

that a conceptual position is required, developing children’s relational understanding where

learners’ knowledge ‘extends from how to do a calculation to why the procedure works’

(Skemp 1976, in Anghileri, 2006 p.8). The Primary Framework also holds conceptual

position though as learners are required to ‘understand the underlying ideas’ of calculation

and, by year 6, they should be have a variety of mental, written and calculator methods and

be able to ‘decide which method is appropriate’ (The Primary Framework, 2006, p.67). This

would appear to suggest that a combination of procedural and conceptual understanding is

needed in order to achieve success.

In the next section I will consider how young children begin to develop an understanding of

subtraction. The Ofsted Report ‘Good practice in primary mathematics: evidence from 20

successful schools’ (2011) identifies that to achieve a successful foundation in the four

operations there needs to be ‘a clear emphasis on practical, hands-on activities in the Early

Years Foundation Stage and Key Stage 1, with a high profile given to developing

mathematical language and mental mathematics’. Anghileri (2006) has shown that concrete

and practical experiences are at the heart of children’s early development in subtraction and

other operations. Children manipulate collections of objects and are taught how to refine the

language that they use, such as take away, subtract and difference. Counting experience is

also vital as it ‘help[s] children to associate the concrete models with some abstract number

patterns’ (Anghileri, 2006: p.50). A similar stance to early number work is taken in the

‘Development Matters in the Early Years Foundation Stage’ (EYFS) non-statutory guidance

material, and like the Primary Framework, the EYFS guidance is used in the school where I

work.

2

Page 3: Subtraction Assignment 18

Anghileri (2006) also argues that children need plenty of experience before moving onto the

use of symbols such as ‘–‘ as then they may see its use as a particular ‘ritual’ and can

misinterpret its meaning. For example, ‘-‘ can be interpreted as ‘taking away’, subtracting a

specified number (subtrahend) from an initial quantity (minuend) or ‘finding the difference’,

comparing how much greater or smaller two quantities are from one another.

Progression from these early practical experiences can be described in terms of subtraction

structures. Carpenter and Moser (1983, cited in Thompson, 2010) (see Appendix 4, Levels

for Mental Subtraction) identified levels of progression for mental subtraction beginning with

‘count out’ where the child counts out a number of objects, removes the amount to be

subtracted and counts the objects remaining. Next children ‘count back from’, and this

usually requires children to maintain two counts at the same time. For example, to calculate

9 – 3 start at nine and count back three ‘steps’: 9 – 8, 7, 6. Here one of the counts is

backwards (8, 7, 6) while the other is a mental count forwards (1, 2, 3) to keep track. With

the same calculation 9 – 3, a ‘count up’ strategy could be used where children count forwards

from three to nine: ‘four, five, six, seven, eight, nine’. Whilst doing so they keep a finger tally

of how much they have counted up; in this case six.

During children’s primary education they continue to develop mental and written strategies.

Reports such as the Cockcroft Report (1982, in Thompson, 2010) have argued and identified

reasons for the teaching of mental strategies. For example, it supports progression through to

written methods and develops children’s ‘number sense’ where they have an improved

knowledge of and facility with numbers and operations (MacIellan, 2001 in Thompson,

2010). However, the levels of progression for mental subtraction are not clear (Thompson,

2010), children’s understanding can be underestimated and ‘children themselves may not

realise the methods they are using’ (Anghileri, 2006: p.57). Thompson identified other

strategies used in mental methods:

Split method - partitioning numbers to carry out the subtraction, usually into tens and ones

Jump method – starting at one number and subtracting chunks of the second number

Split/jump method – a combination of the above

Compensation – subtracting more than is necessary and then adjusting, useful when

subtracting near multiples of ten or hundred

(Thompson, 2010 p.167)

3

Page 4: Subtraction Assignment 18

Beishuizen (2010) has identified how split or decomposition strategies can lead to

difficulties. For example, with the calculation 56 – 28, the calculation is split into 50 – 20

and 6 – 8 and the calculations are carried out separately. Subtracting the ones/units causes a

conflict. Many children try to solve this by subtracting the small number from the larger. So

6 – 8 is often misinterpreted as 6 from 8, giving 2. This can be an over generalisation of the

commutative law where children believe they can swap the numbers around and still get the

same answer (Lawton, 2005). Ryan and William’s (2007) suggest another reason for the

mistake is that children’s first experience of subtraction is to take the smaller from the larger

and they may have been taught this explicitly.

A jump or sequential strategy provides a ‘less vulnerable and more efficient computation

procedure’ (Beishuizen, 2010: p.176). The empty number line (ENL) is often used to support

these mental calculation strategies. The learner starts with the first number and shows jumps

on the number line as chunks are subtracted, for example, 56 – 28 becomes 56 – 10 – 10 – 8

(see Figure 1a below). Klein et al. (1998 cited in Beishuizen, 2010) identified how the ENL

supports informal strategies, leads to a higher level of mental activation, is a natural and

transparent model for operations and is flexible. For example, the ENL allows the learner to

split the ones/units in complements to (new) decadal tens and to use compensation methods

(see Figures 1b and 1c respectively).

Figure 1a

-8 -10 -10

28 36 46 56

Figure 1b

-2 -6 -20

28 30 36 56

4

Page 5: Subtraction Assignment 18

Figure 1c

-30+2

26 28 56

The ENL features in the ‘Guidance Paper: Calculation’ (DCSF 2006) that was used by the

school where I work. In the guidance paper the ENL is used in the first in three stages of

written approaches to calculation: the ENL; partitioning; and expanded layout leading to

column (or standard) methods. These were to be used for ‘building up to using an efficient

method for subtraction of two-digit and three-digit whole numbers by the end of Year 4’

(Guidance Paper: Calculation DCSF 2006: p.8). However, Thompson and Beishuizen

explain how there has been a misunderstanding in the purpose of the ENL. It was ‘never

envisaged as a link between mental and written strategies, but rather as a tool to support

mental calculation’ (Thompson, 2010: p.189).

Thompson explores other areas of difficulty outlined in the Guidance Paper (DCSF 2006).

One such area is the empty number line. The claim that it is used for ‘counting back’ is not

always true. Counting back is ‘reciting backwards as many number names as you are

subtracting (the subtrahend), and then giving the last number that you said as your answer to

the subtraction’ (Thompson, 2010: p.194). Thompson argues that no counting back happens

in the calculation in Figure 2. However, a similar example was used by Beishuizen (2010)

and was considered good practice.

Figure 2

Guidance Paper – Calculation: The Empty Number Line

5

Page 6: Subtraction Assignment 18

74 – 27 = 47 worked by counting back:

Another potentially problematic area is partitioning. Of the two examples in Figure 3 the first

is similar to the ENL but in a horizontal format, and the second example is used to partition

both minuend and the subtrahend. However, Thompson argues that there is no logical

progression from the ENL to partitioning. The ENL cannot be used to partition both numbers,

as they are in the second example, when the tens and ones/units numbers are treated

separately.

Figure 3

Guidance Paper – Calculation: Partitioning

74 – 27 = 74 – 20 – 7 = 54 – 7 = 4774 – 27 = 70 + 4 – 20 – 7 = 60 + 14 – 20 – 7 = 40 + 7

Other problems are suggested in the progression from expanded layout to compact methods.

Here a decomposition or column written method is used (see Figure 4) and the guidance

suggests that children will exchange between the split minuend numbers when necessary.

Initially children are to use the expanded layout where numbers are split into hundreds, tens

and ones. However, Thompson (2010) argues that the use of the addition symbol when

children are expecting to subtract is confusing and children are expected to begin with the

lowest value digit which goes against children’s previous experience. Thompson states that

the method is not a ‘natural’ form of progression for children, especially with the compact

method because this step ‘involves a major shift in the way the digits in the numbers are

interpreted: a shift from treating them as quantities to treating them as digits in columns’

(Thompson, 2010: p.218).

Figure 4

Guidance Paper – Calculation: Expanded layout, leading to compact written method

Example 563 – 271

6

Page 7: Subtraction Assignment 18

Nevertheless, it is assumed that children will need to use some form of standard written

procedure when dealing with larger numbers (Haylock, 2006). Haylock identifies how

decomposition as shown above is preferable to other standard written methods such as ‘equal

additions’ because ‘it is much easier to understand, in the manipulation of concrete materials,

the manipulations of the symbols and the corresponding language’ (Haylock, 2006: p.59).

However, others such as Hart (1989 cited in Thompson, 2010) have found that children found

it difficult to make connections between the manipulation of practical apparatus and their

written methods.

Thompson identifies advantages to the counting up or complementary addition method (see

Figure 5). Here children record on the ENL or in columns how they jump from the

subtrahend to the minuend. Because children can reduce the number of steps they take as

their mental strategies improve Thompson (2010) argues that it has built-in progression.

Furthermore, it leads to a more formal written notation.

Figure 5

Guidance Paper – Calculation: The Counting-up method

Example 74 – 27

Now that the problematic nature of progression has been identified I will turn to specific

difficulties that might be encountered by less able pupils.

It is a general consensus that ‘children should be encouraged to use any method that they are

confident fits the requirement’ (Anghileri, 2006: p.115). Unfortunately, research has shown

how less able children find it difficult to move away from concrete counting methods

7

Page 8: Subtraction Assignment 18

(Lawton, 2005). They can struggle to develop more flexible abstract approaches and fail to

understand written calculations because of poor place value knowledge.

Another difficulty faced by children is recognising the requirement and interpreting the

objective of ‘real life’ questions. Haylock has shown how different structures of subtraction:

partitioning, reduction, comparison and inverse-of-addition can meet ‘a daunting range of

situations’ (Haylock 2006, p.33). For example, if a child was asked how much taller a girl of

167cm is than a boy of 159cm, this requires the subtraction 167 – 159, and a comparison

structure could be used.

Nevertheless evidence has shown that mathematical understanding is ‘developed out of

problem-solving rather than learned separately and then applied’ (Hughes, 1986; Beishuizen,

1995: in Anghileri 2006: p.52). Furthermore, real activities and experience help to motivate

children and give a familiar situation making mathematics more accessible.

Ethics

The next sections consider the outcomes of the research events in different Year groups.

Permission to use children’s work has been granted from parents/guardians and to preserve

children’s anonymity any names have been removed or substituted by letters.

Current Position

Before I collected data on subtraction, myself and the teaching staff discussed the broader

question of how to improve children’s attainment in mathematics; especially the lower

attaining children. We analysed the results of Year 6 National Curriculum tests but they were

inconclusive as evidence suggested both division and subtraction as areas of weakness. For

this reason it was agreed that any intervention should begin by establishing the baseline more

clearly, so the first tests, for Year 3 and 4, contained questions on all four operations. The

questions were designed so that they were of a suitable level and would expose different

types of mistakes readily.

It was soon evident that we should focus on subtraction as tests indicated a more significant

weakness in this area (see Appendix 5a for Y3 Y4 Tests). For example, Figure 6 shows a

8

Page 9: Subtraction Assignment 18

typical response by children. In Year 3, where only seven out of twenty one children

correctly answered the question.

Figure 6

I then administered a test for Year 5 children, this time looking only at subtraction (see

Appendix 5b for Y5 Test). I also undertook a range of research activities with groups of

lower attaining children from Years 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5. In Years 1 and 2 groups of children

were given tasks, enabling myself to make observations and engage in discussion with the

children to assess their understanding (see Appendix 6 for Year 1 and Year 2 tasks and

assessment). Informal discussions were carried out with children in Years 3, 4 and 5

reflecting upon how to answer different test questions (see Appendix 7a, 7b and 7c for Year

3, Year 4 and Year 5 discussions). Such discussions were enlightening and support Ryan and

Williams (2010) argument for quality discussion as they helped to unpick the errors and

misconceptions children had.

9

Page 10: Subtraction Assignment 18

Lower attaining Year 1 children performed well at counting down in ones from numbers of

twenty or below. However, they did find this considerably more challenging than counting

up in ones. When working independently most children were able to subtract numbers up to

twenty, achieving the Assessing Pupil Progress (APP) Level 1 statement to ‘understand

subtraction as ‘taking away’ objects from a set and finding how many are left’ (Crown

Copyright 2009). To do this most used a ‘count out’ strategy (Carpenter and Moser, 1983

cited in Anghileri, 2006). Some average and higher attaining children had progressed onto the

‘count back from’ method, using their fingers. Children found it difficult to explain their

methods. For example, child A, who counted back, stated, “I just did it with my fingers.”

Only the higher attaining children could count back in numbers above twenty and cross the

tens boundary. Furthermore, a significant proportion of Year 1 children were still developing

their understanding of symbolizing relations (Anghileri, 2006) and were unsure of how to use

the ‘ – ‘ symbol. Figure 7 shows how child B initially wrote ‘ + ‘ symbol and they reverted

to addition when the question was subtraction.

Figure 7

In the first assessment activity most Year 2 children used a ‘count out’ strategy (see

Appendix 6) and chose to use counting resources such as unifix cubes to support them in

answering questions. When counting out, typically average attaining children were surprised

by the appearance of apparently small numbers such as 37. This indicated a lack of

opportunity for creating a visual image for two digit numbers greater than thirty. The class

teacher noted how some average ability children had previously used a ‘count back from’

strategy but they did not do so when using counting resources such as unifix cubes. This

reflects Carpenter and Moser’s (1982, cited in Maclellan, 1997) argument that some counting

aids encourage the count out strategy. This was confirmed in a subsequent activity (see

10

Page 11: Subtraction Assignment 18

Appendix 6) when the children used a different counting aid, fingers, and then used the

‘count back from’ strategy.

Lower attaining Year 2 children were restricted by their lack of place value knowledge as

they were unsure of numbers greater than twenty. When counting back both the average and

lower ability children found it difficult to cross the tens boundary with numbers greater than

twenty. For example, when counting back from 35 they counted 34, 33, 32, 31, 30, 39, 38

and so on. Whilst most average ability children quickly identified that the counting was

incorrect the less able children did not and needed extra support.

Throughout Key Stage 2 (KS2) tests and discussions (Appendices 3, 5a, 5b, 7a, 7b, 7c)

showed that lower attaining children experienced similar difficulties with mental strategies.

A number of SEN children had not progressed beyond using the ‘count out’ strategy. For

example, Figure 8 shows how a Year 6 child drew objects to support their counting. This

indicates that the child has not had sufficient experience at counting or using concrete

materials such as a bead string to support their visualisation of abstract number patterns

(Anghileri, 2006).

Figure 8

Nevertheless, most lower attaining KS2 children had progressed to using a ‘count back from’

strategy. They tended to apply a ‘jump’ method when subtracting a single digit number and a

‘split’ method (Thompson, 2010) when subtracting a two digit number. Poor place value

knowledge hampered some children’s counting back using the jump method because of

11

Page 12: Subtraction Assignment 18

difficulties crossing the tens or hundreds boundary (see Appendix 7a, Year 3 discussions,

question 1).

When subtracting two digit numbers using the ‘split’ method (Thompson, 2010) the lower

attaining children made a variety of mistakes. By far the most common error of children of

all abilities was to swap the ones around if the subtrahend was greater than the minuend or

the ‘taking the smaller from the larger bug’, see Figure 9.

Figure 9

This mistake was most prevalent in Year 3; for the calculation 82 – 29 nineteen out of twenty

five children used a split method incorrectly and only those children who used the less

vulnerable sequential or jump strategy answer correctly (Beishuizen, 2010). Children using

the split strategy tried to follow the procedure but did not understand the principles of

decomposition and over-generalised the commutative law which does not apply to

subtraction (Lawton, 2005). Sometimes use of the split strategy led to quite complicated

procedures that began to resemble what the DCSF (2006) describe as partitioning, but did not

provide a correct solution, see Figure 10.

Figure 10

12

Page 13: Subtraction Assignment 18

Some ‘split method’ errors were due to carelessness, such as adding the numbers instead of

subtracting them, see Figure 11a. Other children continued to subtract numbers when the

answers from subtracting the tens and the ones needed to be recombined, see Figure 11b.

Figure 11a

Figure 11b

13

Page 14: Subtraction Assignment 18

In upper KS2 many children, including lower attaining pupils, used a compact written

method or decomposition for more complex subtraction questions. Most lower and average

attaining children displayed poor procedural understanding. They did not understand how to

exchange and made the mistake of swapping numbers so that they were taking the smaller

from the larger, see Figure 12. In Year 5 only five children answered the question ‘782 – 367’

correctly.

Figure 12

Surprisingly, some older children were unfamiliar with the column layout. For example, at

first a Year 5 pupil could not answer a question presented in the vertical format, see Figure

14

Page 15: Subtraction Assignment 18

13, but after a short discussion with myself he exclaimed “Oh, so it’s eighty nine take away

fifty six!” and proceeded to answer the question correctly.

Figure 13

89 -56

With larger numbers many upper KS2 children also ‘lost sense of what they were doing and

the reasonableness of their result’ (Anghileri, 2006: p.114). For example, when discussing the

question 305 – 297, Child A, a Year 5 pupil, was surprised by the correct answer and

remarked, “so the answer’s just eight!” Using decomposition had resulted in that child seeing

the digits as individual quantities rather than whole numbers. Despite such difficulties the

decomposition method was popular because children perceived it as being the most efficient

method. For example, a Year 4 child who answered the question 309 – 198 incorrectly using

the compact decomposition method stated that it was, “the best method because I don’t have

to write as much.”

In the test a small number of lower attaining Year 4 children used a counting up or

complementary addition method. This method had been taught two weeks before the test but

when they were given similar questions a fortnight later they had reverted to using a ‘count

back from’ method with partitioning. This confirms Fischbein’s argument that because the

‘difference by building up’ interpretation is more demanding we tend, consequently to view

subtraction as ‘making fewer’ (1993 cited in Cockburn and Littler, 2008).

Lower attaining children’s ability to solve ‘real life’ problems or contextualised mathematics

varied considerably. Sometimes the context helped them to understand the process involved

but when it was more complex or unfamiliar they struggled. Ryan and Williams (2007)

explain how such questions are like a game with rules but many children misunderstand them

because they do not have a ‘feel’ for such games. Thus the ‘pedagogy needs to make the rules

of the game explicit’ (Ryan and Williams, 2007: p.19). Lower attaining Year 5 children

found questions which suggested a comparison structure especially difficult because they

could not ascertain what they were required to do. Interestingly, some children did not

necessarily interpret the comparison structure as a subtraction question. For example, Figure

11 shows how a Year 5 pupil has used an arrow instead of a + or – symbol for recording the

15

Page 16: Subtraction Assignment 18

calculation. This supports Thompson’s research (2010) that children in England normally

interpret subtraction as ‘taking away’.

Figure 14

As well as identifying pupils’ current position I carried out a planning scrutiny, focussing

upon subtraction (see Appendix 8 for example). Most teachers used the planning tool

provided in the ‘Abacus’ scheme, modifying it to meet the needs of the class. Teachers

usually planned for children to use the ‘Abacus’ work or text books. These modelled

different jottings to support children’s calculation but did not feature the ENL, helping to

explain why there was very little evidence of its use. In KS2 there was a lack of visual

representation of calculations, and an emphasis on using complementary addition for mental

calculations and decomposition method for written calculations.

The evidence provided a baseline for the ‘bookend’ evidence. It showed that children did

develop more sophisticated mental and written strategies as they progressed through the

school but many children, especially lower attaining children, had a poor procedural and

conceptual understanding of subtraction.

Action Taken

I considered the findings from the literature review and the evidence gathered at my school to

identify the change of teaching practice that was needed to achieve the intended outcome of

addressing difficulties with subtraction; especially for lower attaining children. The teaching

practice should: give children more regular practice with counting; provide concrete

examples to support visualisation; provide children with a clearer path of progression;

develop a more secure understanding of mental strategies and written methods; provide

16

Page 17: Subtraction Assignment 18

children with a better understanding of the properties of subtraction and develop a clearer

understanding of the structure of different subtraction word problems.

In order to develop teaching practice I coached staff using the GROW model (Brockbank and

McGill, 2006). With staff I shared the goal (G) of addressing difficulties with subtraction; the

reality (R) that was exposed by tests and other forms of assessment; considered options (O)

for individual teachers and the class they taught and identified how teachers were willing (W)

to carry out the actions. I provided ‘expert coaching’ where I had the expertise in the skill

being developed (Ofsted, 2010). Initially staff meetings were used to develop teachers’

subject knowledge and pedagogy. Extra support was given where it was most needed: Years

2, 3 and 4. For teachers of those classes I led demonstration lessons, supported colleagues

with their planning and helped to identify the ‘next steps’ for children. The degree and style

of support varied from teacher to teacher depending on the colleagues’ and the class’

requirements.

Analysis and impact of action taken

At the first staff meeting I presented an activity where staff analysed subtraction calculations,

methods and objectives. Staff sorted them into appropriate year groups and identified

difficulties that children experienced with them (see Appendix 9 for example). I used the

opportunity to engage in informal discussions with staff, analysing their perceptions of the

subject area and assessing their subject knowledge. Some members of staff felt insecure with

certain subtraction procedures and this was an area I would address in subsequent meetings.

Together we identified that a number of different methods were being taught for subtraction

and that some methods, most noticeably the ENL, were hardly used. Teachers evaluated the

Abacus scheme textbooks (2007) and found that the expanded written method was introduced

in Year 3 and, like the Guidance Paper, used confusing addition symbols when children are

expecting to subtract, see Figure 15a (Thompson, 2010). We agreed to remove the addition

symbol and leave a gap between the partitioned numbers instead. Unclear jottings were used

to support mental methods instead of the ENL, see Figure 15b. We agreed to use the ENL to

support mental methods and revisit subtraction as counting back/ taking away as KS2

children were not having sufficient experience at working with subtraction as ‘making fewer’

17

Page 18: Subtraction Assignment 18

(Fischbein, 1993 cited in Cockburn and Littler, 2008). At the next meeting we agreed to

bring evidence of children using the ENL.

Figure 15a

Figure 15b

When teachers shared evidence of children using the ENL we identified how most of the KS2

children were able to use a ‘count back from’ strategy successfully. The method allowed for

differentiation as the counting back ranged from younger lower attaining children using a

jump method, counting back in ones to older children partitioning the subtrahend into

thousands, hundreds, tens and ones, see Figure 16. We also noticed how some children had

split the ones so that they could jump to a multiple of ten.

Figure 16

18

Page 19: Subtraction Assignment 18

After referring to Carpenter and Moser’s ‘Levels for Mental Subtraction’ (1984 cited in

Thompson, 2010) we recognised that KS2 classes should also develop the ‘count up’ method

for subtraction using the ENL. We progressed onto an interesting discussion where we

identified how this method related to comparison and complementary addition structure

questions (see Appendix 10 for Staff meeting notes).

To promote teaching practice that would develop conceptual understanding (Cockburn and

Littler, 2008) I shared Haylock and Cockburn’s (1989) connective model of learning

mathematics, see Figure 17. It suggests that different mathematical elements need to be

experienced and connected to create full understanding. We agreed that our teaching should

include the different elements as it provided a useful model for mathematics teaching (see

Appendix 10, staff meeting notes). We also agreed to make contextualised questions more

accessible to lower attaining children by providing examples that were close to the child’s

experience and interests (Cockburn and Littler, 2008).

Figure 17

19

Page 20: Subtraction Assignment 18

After the two staff meetings, I met informally with the Year 2, 3 and 4 teachers to discuss

further options available in the development of their teaching practice. One teacher had

recently changed year group and another wanted to be shown how to use new methods and

resources so I led demonstration lessons. This helped to develop my own and the teachers’

awareness of the needs of the class. However, to make a greater impact upon teaching

practice we moved onto using a ‘self reflection-joint experimentation-feedback’ approach,

see Figure 18, based on a model by Showers, Joyce and Bennet (1988). We continued to look

at how the children progressed and identify next steps. Once this approach was embedded I

found that the teachers or ‘coachees’ acquired the desired skills, took ownership of the

lessons and were putting assimilation into practice, modifying their own practices

accordingly (Rhodes, Stokes, Hampton, 2004). They were enthusiastic about the lessons and

enjoyed discussing the outcomes which were often enlightening (see Appendix 11 for Lesson

Plans and Assessment).

Figure 18

20

Page 21: Subtraction Assignment 18

Teachers’ feedback and assessment (see Appendix 13) showed that children’s attainment was

improving. For example, activities helped lower attaining Year 2 children to understand the

‘logical structure of numbers’ (Anghileri, 2006: p11) with most independently counting back

from any two digit number, crossing the tens boundary. Year 3 children had a more secure

understanding of mental strategies and most lower attaining children used an ENL

successfully. Similarly, in Year 4 children developed a more secure understanding of mental

methods with all children being able to use the ENL to subtract a two digit number from

another two digit number. In Year 5 lower attaining children had progressed onto subtracting

three and four digit numbers. Use of the ENL helped children to become more efficient and

effective, and teachers were able to identify and work with children’s misunderstandings.

Furthermore, children were using it for other operations. For example, Figure 19 shows how

an ENL has been applied by a lower attaining Year 4 pupil for division.

Figure 19

21

Page 22: Subtraction Assignment 18

Such work helped children to compare, relate and understand the effect of different

operations. Other activities made explicit links between operations and Figure 20 shows how

children have demonstrated an understanding of the inverse relationship between addition

and subtraction by exploring a concrete example. This understanding helps children to

develop more flexible approaches (Anghileri, 2006) and have an awareness of different

methods (Askew in Thompson, 2010).

Figure 20

In Years 4 and 5 the decomposition method was taught in the expanded or compact form. To

address the ‘taking the smaller from the larger number bug’ teaching strategies made explicit

reference to the ‘bug’ as ‘awareness of the nature and impact of intuitions is a first step

towards resisting them’ (Fischbein 1987 cited Cockburn and Littler, 2008: p.67). Indeed

evidence showed that most children used the method more successfully and knew how and

22

Page 23: Subtraction Assignment 18

when to exchange. However, some lower attaining children still found decomposition

difficult especially if there was a zero in the middle of the minuend and they were required to

exchange, see Figure 21.

Figure 21

Haylock (2010) describes this as a ‘slight problem’ that is ‘easily understood if related

strongly to concrete materials and the appropriate language of exchange’. All children used

the recommended concrete base ten materials and interactive websites (see Figure 22) to

develop their understanding. However, they found it difficult to make connections between

the practical apparatus and their written methods which echoes research by Hart (1989; cited

in Thompson, 2010).

Figure 22 – Interactive programme using Dienes apparatus

I took such findings into consideration when producing a revised subtraction calculation

pyramid (see Appendix 12). In order to provide a clearer path of progression the use of the

23

Page 24: Subtraction Assignment 18

ENL was extended into Year 3 and decomposition delayed until Year 4. Previously children

had been introduced to the decomposition algorithm earlier without necessarily exchanging

and this led to overgeneralisations (Ryan &William, 2007). It is also intended that, like

successful schools identified by Ofsted (2011), the new pyramid with omitted and reduced

methods will be less confusing for lower attaining pupils and allow higher attaining pupils to

move more swiftly onto more efficient methods used in higher Year groups.

An area that continued to be a weakness was lower attaining children’s understanding of

comparison or complementary addition structured questions. This supports Anghileri’s (2006:

p.61) argument that ‘some expressions will be more difficult to verbalize than others’. More

work needs to be done in providing concrete examples and pictures/images for the children to

use. Figure 23 shows how one Teaching Assistant (TA) supported a lower attaining Year 5

pupil by drawing a picture of the problem. Support such as this needs to be used more

readily by teachers and TAs.

Figure 23

24

Page 25: Subtraction Assignment 18

Conclusion

From research carried out it was clear that lower attaining children were experiencing

difficulties with subtraction. There were weaknesses in both their procedural and conceptual

understanding, and this led to a range of difficulties with mental and written methods.

As a result of action taken most problems were resolved. For example, younger lower

attaining children improved their counting skills and afterwards many were able to count

down from a two digit number and cross the tens boundary. When subtracting smaller

numbers most children had progressed onto using a ‘count down from’ strategy.

Throughout the school the greatest success was the use of the empty number line (ENL) to

support mental calculation. Teachers found it easier to identify children’s misunderstandings

and children developed more secure mental methods. Children also displayed better ‘number

sense’ or feel for number as they built upon skills they had already acquired, linking new

information to their existing knowledge (Anghileri, 2006). The ENL also helped teachers to

progress children onto using more challenging numbers; to use different subtraction strategies

successfully such as ‘count back’ and complementary addition; and to develop children’s

conceptual understanding of subtraction and other operations.

Older lower attaining children had improved their ability at using a standard written method

but some children continued to find decomposition difficult; swapping numbers around to

‘take the smaller from the larger’, and not properly understanding when or how to exchange.

I believe the teaching of procedural skills has improved but it needs to continue to build on

pupils’ knowledge of place value, using practical equipment and concrete activities.

The study also provided a means for me to develop my own and other teachers’ subject

knowledge and together we improved the mathematics curriculum. All teachers have

participated enthusiastically, gained confidence at teaching subtraction and improved their

ability at teaching mental and written methods. Already teaching staff are looking forward to

building upon the progress made by lower attaining children so they continue to develop their

conceptual and procedural understanding of mathematics.

25

Page 26: Subtraction Assignment 18

Reference List

Anghileri, J. (2006) Teaching Number Sense London: Continuum International Publishing

Group

Askew, M. (2010) ‘It ain’t (just) what you do: Effective Teachers of Numeracy’ in

Thompson, I. (ed) Issues in Teaching Numeracy in Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open

University Press

Askew, M., Brown, M., Rhodes, V., Johnson, D. & Wiliam, D. (1997) Effective Teachers of

Numeracy London: King’s College London

Beishuizen, M. (2010) ‘The Empty Number Line’ in Thompson, I. (ed) Issues in Teaching

Numeracy Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open University Press

Brockbank, A. & McGill, I. (2006) Facilitating Reflective Learning Through Mentoring and

Coaching London: Kogan Page

Brown, M. (2010) ‘Swings and roundabouts’ in Thompson, I. (ed) Issues in Teaching

Numeracy in Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open University Press

Cockburn, A. & Littler, G (2008) Mathematical Misconceptions London: SAGE Publications

DCSF (Department for Children, Schools and Families), Crown Copyright (2006) Primary

Framework for literacy and mathematic, Primary National Strategy

DCSF (Department for Children, Schools and Families) (2006) Primary Framework for

Literacy and Mathematics – Guidance Paper – Calculation http://national-

strategies.standards.dcsf.gov.uk/ (accessed March 2010)

Early Education: The British Association for Early Childhood Education. Crown Copyright

(2012) Development Matters in the Early Years Foundation Stage: Non-statutory Guidance

26

Page 27: Subtraction Assignment 18

Haylock, D. (2006) Mathematics Explained for Primary Teachers (3rd Edition) London, Sage

Publications Ltd

Lawton, F. (2005) ‘Number’ in Hansen, A, (ed) Children’s Errors in Mathematics Exeter:

Learning Matters

Maclellan, E. (1997) ‘The importance of counting’ in Thompson, I. (ed) Teaching and

Learning Early Number Buckingham: Open University Press

Mertens, R. & Kirkby, D. (2007) Abacus Evolve Framework Edition, Year 3, Textbook 3

Oxford: Ginn, Harcourt Ltd.

Ofsted (The Office for Standards in Education, Children’s Services and Skills), (2011) Good

practice in primary mathematics: evidence from 20 successful schools Manchester: Crown

Copyright

Ofsted (The Office for Standards in Education, Children’s Services and Skills), (2010) Good

professional development in schools Manchester: Crown Copyright

Rhodes, C., Stokes, M. & Hampton, G. (2004) A practical guide to mentoring, coaching and

peer-networking: teacher professional development in schools and colleges London

Routledge Falmer

Ryan, J & Williams, J. (2007) Children’s Mathematics 4 – 15 Maidenhead: Open University

Press

Ryan, J. & Williams, J. (2010) ‘Children’s mathematical understanding as a work in

progress: learning from errors and misconceptions’ in Thompson, I. (ed) Issues in Teaching

Numeracy in Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open University Press

Showers, B., Joyce, B., and Bennet, B. (1987) Synthesis of Research on Staff Development: A

framework for Future Study and a State-of-the-Art Analysis Educational Leadership

November 1987 p77-87

27

Page 28: Subtraction Assignment 18

Thompson, I. (2010) ‘Getting Your Head Around Mental Calculation’ in Thompson, I. (ed)

Issues in Teaching Numeracy in Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open University Press

Thompson, I. (2010) ‘Written Calculation: Addition and Subtraction’ in Thompson, I. (ed)

Issues in Teaching Numeracy in Primary Schools Maidenhead: Open University Press,

Maidenhead

28