21
497 497 Thinking Styles and Emotions LI-FANG ZHANG University of Hong Kong ABSTRACT. This study aimed to explore the relationship between thinking styles and emotions among university students in Hong Kong. Participants were 99 2nd-year stu- dents (23 men and 76 women) who responded to the Thinking Styles Inventory–Revised (TSI-R), based on R. J. Sternberg’s (1988) theory of mental self-government, and to the Iowa Managing Emotions Inventory (IMEI), based on A. Chickering’s (1969) theory of psychosocial development. Results indicated not only that thinking styles were associated with emotions but also that thinking styles had predictive power for emotions beyond age. The author discusses implications of these findings for faculty members and student- development educators. Keywords: Chinese university students, emotions, thinking styles INTELLECTUAL STYLES IS AN ALL-ENCOMPASSING TERM for constructs such as cognitive styles, learning styles, thinking styles, and teaching styles and refers to people’s preferred ways of processing information and dealing with tasks (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). It has been established that intellectual styles matter in various domains of a student’s life, including his or her academic achievement (Bagley & Mallick, 1998; Mansfield, 1998), cognitive develop- ment (Globerson & Zelniker, 1989; Westreich, Ritzler, & Duncan, 1997), career development (Hilliard, 1995; Morgan, 1997), and personality traits (Deng, Li, & Zhang, 2000; Saleh, 1998). I considered the present study to be related to the final line of the aforemen- tioned investigations (i.e., research on the relation between intellectual styles and personality traits). In literature, some scholars consider constructs such as anxiety, assertiveness, depression, frustration, happiness, and optimism as personality traits (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964). Other scholars, however, refer to such constructs as emotional competence (e.g., Ciarrochi, Deane, The author is grateful to the Committee on Research and Conference Grants, adminis- tered by the University of Hong Kong, for funding this project. The author’s special thanks go to all the research participants. Address correspondence to Li-fang Zhang, Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong; [email protected] (e-mail). The Journal of Psychology, 2008, 142(5), 497–515 Copyright © 2008 Heldref Publications

Thinking Styles and Emotions

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Thinking Styles and Emotions

497497

Thinking Styles and Emotions

LI-FANG ZHANGUniversity of Hong Kong

ABSTRACT. This study aimed to explore the relationship between thinking styles and emotions among university students in Hong Kong. Participants were 99 2nd-year stu-dents (23 men and 76 women) who responded to the Thinking Styles Inventory–Revised (TSI-R), based on R. J. Sternberg’s (1988) theory of mental self-government, and to the Iowa Managing Emotions Inventory (IMEI), based on A. Chickering’s (1969) theory of psychosocial development. Results indicated not only that thinking styles were associated with emotions but also that thinking styles had predictive power for emotions beyond age. The author discusses implications of these findings for faculty members and student-development educators.

Keywords: Chinese university students, emotions, thinking styles

INTELLECTUAL STYLES IS AN ALL-ENCOMPASSING TERM for constructs such as cognitive styles, learning styles, thinking styles, and teaching styles and refers to people’s preferred ways of processing information and dealing with tasks (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). It has been established that intellectual styles matter in various domains of a student’s life, including his or her academic achievement (Bagley & Mallick, 1998; Mansfield, 1998), cognitive develop-ment (Globerson & Zelniker, 1989; Westreich, Ritzler, & Duncan, 1997), career development (Hilliard, 1995; Morgan, 1997), and personality traits (Deng, Li, & Zhang, 2000; Saleh, 1998).

I considered the present study to be related to the final line of the aforemen-tioned investigations (i.e., research on the relation between intellectual styles and personality traits). In literature, some scholars consider constructs such as anxiety, assertiveness, depression, frustration, happiness, and optimism as personality traits (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964). Other scholars, however, refer to such constructs as emotional competence (e.g., Ciarrochi, Deane,

The author is grateful to the Committee on Research and Conference Grants, adminis-tered by the University of Hong Kong, for funding this project. The author’s special thanks go to all the research participants.

Address correspondence to Li-fang Zhang, Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong; [email protected] (e-mail).

The Journal of Psychology, 2008, 142(5), 497–515Copyright © 2008 Heldref Publications

Page 2: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Wilson, & Rickwood, 2002), emotional intelligence (e.g., Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2001), mental health (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), subjective well-being (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999), emotions, (Mortenson, 2006; Spangler, Pekrun, Kramer, & Hofmann, 2002) or emotionality (e.g., Hayden, Klein, Durbin, & Olino, 2006).

This study adopts the term emotions to achieve consistency with those used in the theoretical framework on which one of the inventories employed in this study stands: the emotions dimension defined in Chickering’s (1969; Chicker-ing & Reisser, 1993) theory of psychosocial development. Furthermore, this construct will be examined against thinking styles, one of the specific style con-structs under intellectual styles.

Intellectual Styles and Emotions

Researchers have investigated the relation between intellectual styles and emotions for more than three decades. However, unlike research on the more general cognitive–affective personality system (e.g., Mischel & Ayduk, 2004; Mischel & Shoda, 1999), research on intellectual styles and emotions has been unfruitful. Campbell and Douglas (1972) conducted the earliest study in which researchers examined children’s responses to and level of optimism toward the threat of frustration in relation to their intellectual styles, on the basis of Witkin’s (1962) field-dependent/independent styles and Kagan’s (1965) reflective and impulsive styles. Compared with children who scored higher on field depen-dence and impulsivity, children who scored higher on field independence and reflectivity displayed higher levels of optimism toward frustration.

I conducted a systematic search on the PsycINFO database (2007), enter-ing two sets of terms in their maximum possible combinations. The first set included emotions, emotional competence, emotional intelligence, emotional-ity, mental health, and subjective well-being. The second set included cogni-tive styles, intellectual styles, learning styles, and thinking styles. This search resulted in five additional entries. These studies, however, were all published after the mid 1990s. The first study was conducted in the Netherlands. Jong, Merckelbach, and Nijman (1995) found that among 70 undergraduate students, those with a holistic mode of thinking (i.e., right-brained dominance) demon-strated higher levels of anxiety than did those with the analytic mode of think-ing (i.e., left-brained dominance).

Riding and Wigley (1997) examined Riding and Cheema’s (1991) two stylistic dimensions (verbal–imagery, analytic–wholistic) against psychoticism. The authors concluded that the wholistic style was highly associated with psychoticism.

Gadzella (1999) conducted a third study and administered the Human Information Processing Survey (Torrance, Taggart, & Taggart, 1984) and the 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (Cattell, Cattell, & Cattell, 1978) to 55 students enrolled in undergraduate psychology classes at a Midwestern university. Results

498 The Journal of Psychology

Page 3: Thinking Styles and Emotions

indicated that students with an analytic mode of thinking displayed higher levels of self-control than did students with a holistic mode of thinking, and that stu-dents comfortable with either the analytic or the holistic mode of thinking scored higher on the anxiety scale than did students with an integrative mode of thinking (i.e., whole-brained thinking).

Deng et al. (2000) investigated the relations among field-dependence/independence to assertiveness. Their results showed that the field indepen-dent participants scored significantly higher on assertiveness than did the field dependent ones.

Hayden et al. (2006) examined the links of positive and negative emotions at age 3 to depressive attributional styles at age 7 years. Although little evidence was obtained for a relationship between negative emotions and depressive attri-butional styles, lower positive emotions at age 3 predicted greater helplessness in the interpersonal domain.

There are at least two reasons to continue this investigation. First, the major-ity of the existing studies were based on style models that describe two bipolar styles (e.g., field-dependent versus field-independent; impulsive versus reflec-tive). The present research adopted a style model much broader in scope in that it describes multiple stylistic dimensions: Sternberg’s (1988, 1997) theory of mental self-government.

Second, the emotions examined against intellectual styles have been isolated and not those deemed important to a specific target group. The present study examines five types of emotions addressed in Chickering’s (1969; Chickering & Reisser, 1993) theory of psychosocial development targeted to university students: happiness, attraction, anger, depression, and frustration. These emo-tions were given special attention in Chickering’s work. According to him, these emotions are important to young university students because recognizing and effectively managing them would positively contribute to their formation of identity—a critical component of psychosocial development.

Theory of Mental Self-Government and Its Research

Sternberg (1988, 1997) has contended that just as there are different ways of governing a society, there are different ways in which people use their abilities, which are known as thinking styles. According to Sternberg, 13 thinking styles fall under five dimensions: function, form, level, scope, and leaning. Based on both empirical data and theoretical arguments (Kogan, 1980; Messick, 1996), Zhang (2003) reconceptualized the 13 thinking styles into three types.

Type I thinking styles include the legislative, judicial, hierarchical, global, and liberal styles, and tend to be more creativity-generating and denote higher levels of cognitive complexity. Type II thinking styles include the executive, local, monarchic, and conservative styles, and suggest a norm-favoring tendency and denote lower levels of cognitive complexity. Type III thinking styles include

Zhang 499

Page 4: Thinking Styles and Emotions

the anarchic, oligarchic, internal, and external styles and may manifest the char-acteristics of either Type I or Type II thinking styles, depending on the stylistic demands of a task. In Appendix A, the main characteristics of each of the 13 thinking styles are described.

The notion of three types of intellectual styles (Zhang & Sternberg, 2005) has been developed to accommodate additional style labels such as field- independent/dependent styles and reflective/impulsive styles. For example, along with Type I thinking styles, the field-independent and reflective styles were classified as Type I intellectual styles. Along with Type II thinking styles, the field-dependent and impulsive styles were classified as Type II intellectual styles. Moreover, along with Type III thinking styles, the feeling and integrative styles were classified as Type III intellectual styles. Individual styles from 10 theoreti-cal models were organized into the three types of intellectual styles. In Appendix B, the specific styles in each of the three types of intellectual styles are listed (see Zhang & Sternberg, 2006, for definitions of individual styles).

Much empirical evidence has supported the validity of Sternberg’s (1988) original theory and its reconceptualized notion of three types of thinking styles (Kaufman, 2001; Zhang, 2005). The most frequently used testing tool is the Thinking Styles Inventory (TSI; Sternberg & Wagner, 1992) and its revision, the Thinking Styles Inventory–Revised (TSI-R; Sternberg, Wagner, & Zhang, 2003). Research using these inventories suggests that thinking styles vary as a function of both personal characteristics (e.g., age, gender) and environmental characteristics (e.g., nature of academic discipline). This research also indi-cates that, in general, thinking styles make a difference in students’ academic achievement, cognitive development, and psychosocial development (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). Chickering (1969)—and later, Chickering and Reisser (1993)—delineated psychosocial development, which we examined in the aforementioned research.

Chickering’s Theory of Psychosocial Development and Its Research

Theories of psychosocial development originated from Erikson’s (1959) work. Such theories address developmental issues or tasks that occur throughout one’s life, as well as one’s pattern of responses. Chickering (1969; Chickering & Reisser, 1993) proposed seven vectors (i.e., dimensions) of developmental tasks for university students in the United States: (a) developing competence, (b) managing emotions, (c) developing autonomy, (d) establishing identity, (e) free-ing interpersonal relationships, (f) developing purpose, and (g) developing integ-rity. Since its publication, Chickering’s (1969) theory has been guiding much of the research on university students’ psychosocial development and has been operationalized through the Student Developmental Task and Lifestyle Inventory (SDTLI; Winston, Miller, & Prince, 1987) and the Iowa Student Development Inventory (ISDI; Hood, 1986, 1997).

500 The Journal of Psychology

Page 5: Thinking Styles and Emotions

The ISDI is composed of seven inventories, each measuring one devel-opmental task. In the managing emotions dimension, university students are portrayed as becoming more aware of their emotions and more able to integrate them as they advance through their education. Five types of both positive and negative emotions are especially pertinent to university students: happiness, attraction, anger, depression, and frustration. The Iowa Managing Emotions Inventory (IMEI; Hood & Jackson, 1997) assesses these emotions.

Apart from being tested among students from two universities in Iowa dur-ing the process of its development (White, 1986; White & Hood, 1989), the IMEI has not been examined in any other study. Both existing studies, however, have obtained satisfactory internal scale reliability data, with Cronbach’s alphas rang-ing from the mid .70s to the mid .80s. Furthermore, the correlations among the various scales ranged from the low .60s to the mid .70s. These high interscale correlations indicated that the levels of differentiation among the five scales could be improved.

These studies have also yielded a moderate amount of evidence for the external construct validity of the IMEI. For example, students who demonstrated higher levels of ability to manage their emotions (as this inventory measured) tended to rate themselves higher on dimensions such as personal development and social development; they also tended to rate themselves higher on critical thinking and on their ability to understand diverse philosophies and cultures. Students who evaluated themselves as being more capable of managing their emotions also tended to earn higher grade point averages.

I adopted the IMEI because it has good reliability and validity data and was designed to assess a wide range of emotions among university students. I also adopted this inventory because I expect significant relations between these emo-tions and the thinking styles defined in the theory of mental self-government, on the basis of past finding that intellectual styles and emotions were closely associated with each other.

The Present Study

There were two objectives of this study: to validate the IMEI among univer-sity students in Hong Kong and, more important, to explore the predictive power of thinking styles for students’ emotions.

Given that past research indicated that Type I intellectual styles (e.g., field-independent style and reflective style) were associated with positive emotions and that Type I thinking styles were related to attributes that are perceived as being more positive (e.g., higher levels of self-esteem and cognitive develop-ment; Zhang & Sternberg, 2006), it was predicted that in general, Type I thinking styles would be statistically predictive of a higher capacity for managing one’s emotions. Regarding Type II and Type III styles, I did not make specific hypoth-eses because there was no foundation for making them. Furthermore, because

Zhang 501

Page 6: Thinking Styles and Emotions

age and gender have been found to be significantly related to thinking styles (e.g., Zhang & Sachs, 1997) in examining the relation between thinking styles and emotions, this study took age and gender into account.

Method

Participants

Participants were 99 university students (23 men and 76 women) in their 2nd year at the Chinese University of Hong Kong. The participants ranged from 18–50 years old, with a mean age of 22 years (SD = 1.83 years) and a median age of 21 years. Studying toward their bachelor’s degree in either education or the arts, students from three sessions of an introductory educational psychology course participated in the research in exchange for extra credit.

Instruments

The participants responded to two self-report tests: the TSI-R (Sternberg, Wagner, & Zhang, 2003) and the IMEI (Hood & Jackson, 1997).

Consisting of 65 statements, the TSI-R assesses the 13 thinking styles described in Sternberg’s theory, with all five statements measuring 1 of the 13 thinking styles. For each statement, the participants rated themselves on a 7-point response scale, with 1 indicating that the statement does not at all represent the way in which they normally carry out their tasks, and 7 indicating that the state-ment characterizes extremely well the way in which they normally carry out their tasks. Two sample questions are (a) “When faced with a problem, I use my own ideas and strategies to solve it” (i.e., legislative style); and (b) “I like to figure out how to solve a problem following certain rules” (i.e., executive style).

The TSI-R has been used in more than a half dozen studies, including Zhang’s (2004a) study of university students in Beijing, Zhang’s (2004b) study of university students in Hong Kong, Fan’s (2006) study of university students in Shanghai, Zhang’s (2005) study of Chinese business personnel in mainland China, and Zhang and Higgins’s (2008) study of business personnel in Great Britain. These studies obtained good psychometric data for the inventory.

In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas are .73, .66, .72, .60, .61, .83, .81, .70, .64, .76, .64, .72, and .78, respectively, for the legislative, executive, judicial, global, local, liberal, conservative, hierarchical, monarchic, oligarchic, anarchic, internal, and external styles. These alpha coefficients are comparable in mag-nitude to those reported in the aforementioned studies. An exploratory factor analysis yielded a two-factor solution. The first factor was dominated by loadings of Type I thinking styles. The second factor was dominated by loadings of Type II thinking styles. Last, loadings of Type III thinking styles were split between the first two factors. These results reveal good validity of the TSI-R because the

502 The Journal of Psychology

Page 7: Thinking Styles and Emotions

first two factors are consistent with the characteristics of three types of think-ing styles. Furthermore, they support the validity data from previous studies of samples from not only the West (e.g., the United Kingdom, the United States) but also Asia (e.g., Hong Kong, mainland China).

The IMEI (Hood & Jackson, 1997) measures Chickering’s managing emo-tions dimension. Composed of 60 statements, this inventory pertains to five types of emotions: happiness, attraction, anger, depression, and frustration. For each statement, the respondents rated themselves on a 5-point response scale, with 1 indicating that the statement does not describe themselves at all regarding how they feel or act in various situations, and 5 indicating that the statement repre-sents extremely well how they feel or act in various situations.

Some of the statements are positively scored; others are reversely scored. For example, the statement “I try to understand my own anger” is a positively scored anger item (i.e., higher scores on this item indicate higher capacity for dealing with anger). The statement “I rarely look beyond my feelings of anger for causes,” however, is a reversely scored anger item (i.e., higher scores on this item indicate lower capacity for dealing with anger).

The present study employed a Chinese version of the inventory. Linguistic equivalency was obtained via the back-translation technique. As we discussed previously, the two existing studies obtained reasonably good internal scale reliability data and proved that the inventory possesses good external validity, although its interscale correlations had much room for improvement.

Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the present data are .68, .78, .70, .75, and .52, respectively, for anger, depression, frustration, happiness, and attraction. Although the first four alpha coefficients were similar in magnitude to those in the two existing studies, the alpha coefficient for the attraction scale was substantially lower than that reported in previous studies. The Pearson prod-uct–moment correlations ranged from .31 to .72, and the majority fell below .40. These interscale correlation coefficients suggest better differentiation among the five scales than those obtained in the United States.

Data Analysis

I conducted preliminary statistical analyses to examine possible significant group differences in the two main variables based on gender and age. I found no gender difference in the thinking style and emotion scales. However, I identified age differences in several scales across the two inventories. For example, older students tended to score higher on the legislative, judicial, and internal thinking styles and indicated better capability for dealing with frustration.

Two statistical analyses followed. First, I used a zero-order correlation procedure to explore the basic relations between thinking styles and emotions. Second, I used hierarchical multiple regressions in which thinking styles were the independent variables, emotions were the dependent variables, and age was

Zhang 503

Page 8: Thinking Styles and Emotions

the control variable because of its significant correlations with several of the thinking styles. These regressions aimed to test the predictive power of thinking styles for emotions when I took students’ ages into consideration.

Results

Zero-Order Correlation Coefficients Between Thinking Styles and Emotions

Zero-order correlation coefficients suggest significant relation between thinking styles and emotions. Of the 65 correlation coefficients (13 styles by 5 types of emotions), 22 (34%) were statistically significant. From the perspective of thinking styles, the hierarchical style was most frequently involved in the sta-tistically significant relations. The hierarchical style was significantly correlated with all of the emotion scales except the attraction scale. From the perspective of emotions, anger and frustration resulted in the largest number of statistically significant correlations. Anger related to 6 of the 13 styles, with 4 Type I styles (all but the global style) and 2 Type III styles (anarchic, external). Frustration was related to 7 thinking styles, with 4 Type I styles (again, all but the global style), and 3 Type II styles (conservative, executive, monarchic).

I identified three general patterns of correlations when I took all 22 statisti-cally significant correlations into account. First, Type I styles were positively associated with the ability to deal with emotions. Second, the anarchic and exter-nal styles (two Type III styles) also had a positive relation with the ability to deal with emotions. Last, significant correlations involving Type II styles were inconsistent; the executive and monarchic styles were positively associated with frustration, but the conservative style was negatively associated with the ability to handle both frustration and depression.

It is important to mention that although statistically significant, the majority of the correlations are weak and that the links of emotions to some styles are at best tenuous. Detailed statistics are presented in Table 1.

Predicting Emotions From Thinking Styles, Controlling for Age

Results from hierarchical multiple-regressions indicated that statistically significant predictive relations were obtained for all five types of emotions. Across the five emotion scales, 6 of the 13 thinking styles were involved in the statistically significant predictions. The anger scale was positively predicted by the hierarchical style, with 9% of the variance in the former being explained by the latter beyond age. The depression scale was positively predicted by the hierarchical style but negatively predicted by the oligarchic style. The 2 styles accounted for 17% of the variance in the depression scale beyond age. The frus-tration scale was positively predicted by the hierarchical and liberal styles but negatively predicted by the anarchic style. The 3 styles contributed to 20% of the

504 The Journal of Psychology

Page 9: Thinking Styles and Emotions

variance in the frustration scale beyond age. The happiness scale was positively predicted by the external and hierarchical styles but negatively predicted by the anarchic style. The unique contribution of the 3 styles to the variance in happi-ness over age was 18%. Last, the attraction scale was positively predicted by the judicial styles and its unique contribution was 6%. See Table 2 for other details from these regression procedures.

Discussion

This study aimed to achieve two objectives: to validate the Chinese ver-sion of the IMEI (Hood & Jackson, 1997) for a sample of university students in Hong Kong and, more important, to understand the relations between university students’ thinking styles and their capacity for being aware of and managing their emotions, in particular, how their thinking styles contributed to their capacity when researchers take age and gender into consideration. Both objectives were achieved.

The IMEI scales yielded reliabilities that are comparable to those obtained in the two existing studies of university students in the United States. It should be noted that the internal scale reliability for the attraction scale (α = .52) was substantially lower than that yielded by the U.S. data sets. This low internal scale consistency could have resulted from several possibilities; most notably, the notion of attraction might operate differently in the American culture and the

Zhang 505

TABLE 1. Zero-Order Correlation Coefficients for Thinking Styles and Emotions (N = 99)

Scale Anger Depression Frustration Happiness Attraction

Legislative .31** .16 .26* .11 .05Executive .06 .08 .23* .14 .17Judicial .22* .18 .27* .22* .26*

Global –.02 –.06 –.09 .02 –.09Local .20 .03 .11 –.03 .10Liberal .27* .17 .31** .18 .09Conservative –.16 –.22* –.22* –.10 .05Hierarchical .28* .33** .32** .25* .14Monarchic .07 –.04 .25* –.05 .08Oligarchic .12 –.13 .08 .02 .01Anarchic .27* .04 .07 .01 .06Internal .09 .02 .03 –.10 –.12External .26* .15 .22* .27* .08

*p < .05. **p < .01.

Page 10: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Chinese culture. Thus, future studies using the Chinese version of this inventory are recommended to further modify and test the items in the attraction scale. Out of the many possible revisions of the items in the attraction scale, understanding the meaning of attraction among American students and among Chinese students is fundamental.

I can discuss the validity of the inventory from three perspectives. The first pertains to the way in which the IMEI scales were correlated with one another. On the one hand, the moderate strength of the correlation coefficients among the scales suggests that the five scales assess an overarching construct: emotions. On the other hand, this moderate magnitude indicates that the correlations among the scales are low enough to be distinguished from one another.

Second, the validity of the inventory is evident through the fact that the older students scored in the more favorable direction than did the younger ones on three of

506 The Journal of Psychology

TABLE 2. Contributions of Thinking Styles to Emotions Beyond Age (N = 99)

Variable summary Model summary

Variable β weight R2 F df

Anger Age .11 .02a 2.109a 1, 77 Hierarchical .30** .11b 4.76b* 2, 76Depression Age .11 .04a 3.02a 1, 75 Hierarchical .41*** .15b 6.50b** 2, 74 Oligarchic –.26* .21c 6.49c** 1, 77Frustration Age .15 .05a 3.79a 1, 73 Hierarchical .41** .14b 5.93b** 2, 72 Liberal .34** .20c 5.82c** 3, 71 Anarchic –.30* .25d 5.90d*** 4, 70Happiness Age .14 .01a 1.10a 1, 76 External .33* .08b 3.66b* 2, 75 Anarchic –.36* .14c 3.97c* 3, 74 Hierarchical .27* .19d 4.13d** 4, 73Attraction Age .09 .02a 1.31a 1, 76 Judicial .24* .08b 4.01b* 2, 75

Note. List-wise cases exclusion was used. aPredictors were constant and age. bPredictors were constant, age, and the first style predictor. cPredictors were constant, age, the first style predictor, and the second style predictor. dPredictors were constant, age, the first style predictor, the second style predictor, and the third style predictor.*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Page 11: Thinking Styles and Emotions

the five scales: anger, depression, and frustration. Chickering’s theoretical underpin-nings expected this significant difference in emotions related to age. As they mature, students become more aware of and better at integrating their emotions.

In the third perspective to determine the validity of the inventory, researchers observe how the emotion scales were related to the thinking styles. The way in which the two constructs were related to each other in the present data is largely supportive of the research hypothesis of this study. As expected, Type I thinking styles demonstrated the strongest predictive power for students’ ability to recog-nize and manage their emotions. From this perspective, researchers determine external validity of the IMEI.

Apart from exploring the general relations between emotions and thinking styles by calculating zero-order correlation coefficients among the scales in the two inventories, I tested the predictive power of thinking styles for emotions, taking into consideration the effects of age on the two main variables. Putting together the two sets of the resultant statistics, I found that although most thinking styles (except the global, local, internal styles) are associated with emotions, only fewer than half (6 of 13) were significant predictors for emotions. Although the positive contributors to students’ abilities to cope with emotions are three of the five Type I styles (hierarchical, liberal, judicial) and a Type III style (the external style), the negative contributors are two Type III styles (anarchic, oligarchic).

Researchers could argue that given that only 6 of the 13 thinking styles sig-nificantly contributed to the variance in emotions beyond age, these significant results might have been obtained by statistical chance. I would maintain that such an argument would be ill grounded. For at least three reasons, researchers should argue that the statistically significant regression results were more likely to reflect true variations in emotions as a function of thinking styles than to have been obtained by statistical chance.

First, because there is no semantic resemblance between the statements in the TSI-R and in the IMEI, the statistically significant relations found between the two constructs cannot be considered coincidental. Second, the way in which thinking styles contributed to the variance in emotions (i.e., Type I styles posi-tively contributed to positive emotions or to better control of negative emotions) is supportive of the predictions made earlier. It is more important that these results make substantive sense. For example, both the liberal and hierarchical styles predicted students’ scores on the frustration scale in a favorable direction. An individual with a liberal thinking style prefers to engage in tasks that involve novelty and ambiguity. An individual with a hierarchical thinking style tends to distribute attention to several tasks that are prioritized according to his or her valuing of the tasks. In both of these thinking styles, the propensity for taking risks, working creatively, and taking on the challenges of the complexity of a task is inherent. To illustrate, performing tasks in new ways (i.e., using the liberal style) often involves the risk of failure because there is no guarantee for success in taking up something new.

Zhang 507

Page 12: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Likewise, prioritizing tasks may also encounter resistant forces because one’s learning and work environment may not appreciate priorities. Yet, if the environ-ment rewards such thinking styles (i.e., the use of such thinking styles are effec-tive in dealing with the tasks at hand), an individual would become more assured of his or her abilities. Such self-assurance would, in turn, help the individual to better deal with the emotions—in particular, frustration, anger, and depressotypic feelings—typically encountered when he or she challenges the well-established rules (i.e., using creativity-generating styles). This logical argument, however, accords with the fact that our regression analyses used the thinking style scales as the independent variables and the emotion scales as the dependent variables. Such a discussion does not imply a causal relation between the two constructs. It is possible that an individual’s high capacity for dealing with negative feelings such as frustration, anger, and depression enables him or her to use creativity- generating styles more often. A further caution is that people’s intellectual styles are malleable (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). In an institution in which Type II styles are highly appreciated, using these styles may allow students to become more aware of and better at dealing with their emotions.

Last, the statistically significant results were more likely to be reflective of the true differences in emotions partially because of thinking styles and because these findings are consistent with previous ones that suggested that intellectual styles are important in emotions. In particular, the main result of the present study supports the past general finding that Type I styles are more conducive to the development of people’s abilities to deal with negative emotions and to the enhancement of positive emotions (e.g., Campbell & Douglas, 1972; Deng et al., 2000; Hayden et al., 2006). Given these reasons, despite the fact that only 6 of the 13 thinking styles were statistically significant in predicting students’ emotions, researchers should conclude that thinking styles—along with factors such as culture, age, gender, and school environment—are significant in students’ emotions.

Readers would want to know how I would interpret some of the apparently contradictory findings. For example, theoretically, the same style (i.e., the anarchic style) would be related to negative emotions (i.e., anger, frustration) in the same direction, whether positive or negative. However, although the zero-order correla-tion procedure revealed a positive relation between the anarchic style and the anger scale, regression results indicated that the anarchic style was a negative contributor to the frustration scale. One possible explanation is that although anger and frustra-tion are related because both are negative emotions, they could differ significantly. As a result, they are related to the same thinking style (i.e., the anarchic style) in dif-ferent fashions. However, it should be noted that the correlation coefficient between the anarchic style and anger was .27 and significant only at the .05 statistical level. By the same token, the anarchic style was the third predictor for the frustration scale, after the hierarchical and liberal styles. These data indicate that the relations of the anarchic style to the two types of emotions are rather weak. Therefore, these find-ings should not be taken at face value. Such an explanation, of course, is only a post

508 The Journal of Psychology

Page 13: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Zhang 509

hoc conjecture. Further research needs to find out the complexity of the relations between particular thinking styles and each type of emotion.

Limitations, Conclusions, and Implications

This study has three limitations. First, the research sample could be biased because the participants were from merely two academic disciplines. Thus, the results may not apply to students in other disciplines. Second, the Chinese version of the IMEI was tested for the first time among university students in Hong Kong. Although the psychometric properties of the inventory are gener-ally good, further testing needs to determine the efficacy of the inventory in assessing the emotions of university students in Hong Kong. Third, inherent in this study might be a cultural bias because the IMEI is grounded in a theory intended for university students in the United States. Given the many differ-ences between the American and Chinese higher education systems, the factors that interact with thinking styles to contribute to the development of students’ positive emotions (or effective management of negative emotions) may be more complex than those I described here. With these limitations, the study can be viewed as only exploratory, and its results should be considered tentative rather than definitive.

Regardless of these limitations, the significance of this study can be derived from two major findings. First, the present study has obtained initial evidence for the IMEI among a sample of university students in Hong Kong. Further investiga-tion of the reliability and validity of the inventory for measuring the emotions of university students in Hong Kong, as defined in Chickering’s theory, is necessary because the present study was only the first of its kind, and because the internal reliability for one of the scales (i.e., attraction) was relatively low. Nevertheless, the initial reliability and validity data determined in this study denote that the inventory may become a potential assessment tool for university counselors in Hong Kong in helping students to understand and manage their emotions.

Second and more important, although the relational patterns of emotions to the Type II and the anarchic styles were not clear, it is obvious that Type I think-ing styles and the external thinking style were strongly and positively associated with (and some even significantly predicted) students’ emotions. Furthermore, it was established that the oligarchic style was strongly and negatively associ-ated with (and significantly predicted) students’ emotions. This general finding aligns with Zhang’s (2002a, 2008) finding that Type I thinking styles positively contributed to students’ psychosocial development, including their sense of pur-pose (Zhang, 2002a) and sense of identity (Zhang, 2008). The present findings have advanced our understanding of the significant function of thinking styles in psychosocial development.

As I reviewed earlier, the importance of thinking styles goes beyond its role in student learning. Previous studies have shown that thinking styles are related

Page 14: Thinking Styles and Emotions

510 The Journal of Psychology

to attributes such as self-esteem (Zhang & Postiglione, 2001), career personality types (Zhang, 2000), cognitive development (Zhang, 2002b), and personality traits (Zhang & Huang, 2001). Studies that investigated other style constructs found similar results. For example, I found that the field-dependence versus field-independence construct was significantly related to attributes such as moral maturity (e.g., Schleifer & Douglas, 1973) and sense of identity (e.g., Bhatnager & Rastogi, 1986), as well as emotions (Campbell & Douglas, 1972; Deng et al., 2000).

On the basis of repeated empirical evidence for the intricately entwined link between intellectual styles and other human attributes—including that between thinking styles and students’ emotions—researchers would argue that thinking styles and emotions should be considered as two integral parts of a holistic student development at a scientific level. Moreover, at a practical level, researchers would argue that the present findings, like many of the earlier ones, call for joint ventures between teaching faculty and student development edu-cators to achieve their goal of fostering students’ well-rounded development. Teaching faculty could support students in developing their capacity for deal-ing with emotions by promoting a wide range of thinking styles—in particular, Type I styles and the external style—and discouraging students from taking on too many tasks without any priority (i.e., avoiding using the oligarchic style). Meanwhile, student development educators could facilitate students’ effective use of thinking styles by helping them to understand and manage their emo-tions better.

AUTHOR NOTE

Li-fang Zhang is an associate professor and the associate dean (Research Higher Degrees) in the Faculty of Education at the University of Hong Kong. Her recent book with Robert J. Sternberg is titled The Nature of Intellectual Styles (Routledge, 2006). Her research interests include intellectual styles, giftedness, personality, and student develop-ment in higher education.

REFERENCES

Bagley, C., & Mallick, K. (1998). Field independence, cultural context and academic achievement: A commentary. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 581–587.

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-II (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Bhatnager, P., & Rastogi, M. (1986). Cognitive style and basic ideal disparity in males and females. Indian Journal of Current Psychological Research, 1, 36–40.

Biggs, J. B. (1978). Individual and group differences in study processes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 48, 266–279.

Campbell, S. B., & Douglas, V. I. (1972). Cognitive styles and responses to the threat of frustration. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 4(1), 30–42.

Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., & Cattell, H. E. (1978). 16 personality factor questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.

Page 15: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Zhang 511

Chickering, A. (1969). Education and identity. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Chickering, A., & Reisser, L. (1993). Education and identity (2nd ed.). San Francisco:

Jossey-Bass.Ciarrochi, J., Deane, F. P., Wilson, C. J., & Rickwood, D. (2002). Adolescents who need

help the most are the least likely to seek it: The relationship between low emotional competence and low intention to seek help. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling, 30, 173–188.

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The NEO-PI-R: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Deng, Z., Li, D., & Zhang, Q. (2000). Cognitive styles, scholastic attainments with the Cattell’s 16PF: A correlative approach. Psychological Science China, 23, 234–235.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302.

Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues Monograph (Vol. 1). New York: International Universities Press.

Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

Fan, W. Q. (2006). Thinking styles among university students in Shanghai: Comparing tra-ditional and hypermedia instructional environments. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Hong Kong.

Gadzella, B. M. (1999). Differences among cognitive-processing styles groups on person-ality traits. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 26, 161–166.

Globerson, T., & Zelniker, T., eds. (1989). Cognitive style and cognitive development. Human development (Vol. 3, edited). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Gregorc, A. F. (1979). Learning/teaching styles: Potent forces behind them. Educational Leadership, 36, 234–236.

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Hayden, E. P., Klein, D. N., Durbin, C. E., & Olino, T. M. (2006). Positive emotionality at

age 3 predicts cognitive styles in 7-year-old children. Development and Psychopathol-ogy, 18, 409–423.

Hilliard, R. I. (1995). How do medical students learn: Medical student learning styles and factors that affect these learning styles. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 7, 201–210.

Holland, J. L. (1973). Making vocational choices: A theory of careers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Hood, A. B. (Ed.). (1986). The Iowa Student Development Inventories (1st ed.). Iowa City, IA: HITECH Press.

Hood, A. B. (Ed.). (1997). The Iowa Student Development Inventories (2nd ed.). Iowa City, IA: HITECH Press.

Hood, A. B., & Jackson, L. M. (1997). The Iowa Managing Emotions Inventory. In A. B. Hood (Ed.), The Iowa student development inventories (2nd ed.; pp. 22–31). Iowa City, IA: HITECH Press.

Jong, P. J. D., Merckelbach, H., & Nijman, H. (1995). Hemisphere preference, anxiety, and covariation bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 363–371.

Jung, C. (1923). Psychological types. New York: Harcourt Brace.Kagan, J. (1965). Individual differences in the resolution of response uncertainty. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 154–160.Kaufman, J. C. (2001). Thinking styles in creative writers and journalists. Dissertation Abstracts

International (Section B): The Physical Sciences and Engineering, 62(3B), 1069. Kirton, M. J. (1976). Adaptors and innovators: A description and measure. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 61, 622–629.

Page 16: Thinking Styles and Emotions

512 The Journal of Psychology

Kogan, N. (1980). A style of life, a life of style (review of cognitive styles in personal and cultural adaptation). Contemporary Psychology, 25, 595–598.

Mansfield, E. A. (1998). Working memory development in adolescents: A neo-Piagetian investigation. Dissertation Abstracts International (Section A): Humanities and Social Sciences, 58(8-A), 3001.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion, 1, 232–242.

Messick, S. (1996). Bridging cognition and personality in education: The role of style in performance and development. European Journal of Personality, 10, 353–376.

Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 99–129). New York: Guilford Press.

Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within a unified theory of personality: The cognitive-affective personality system. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.; pp. 197–218). New York: Guilford Press.

Morgan, H. (1997). Cognitive styles and classroom learning. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Mortenson, S. T. (2006). Cultural differences and similarities in seeking social support as a response to academic failure: A comparison of American and Chinese college students. Communication Education, 55, 127–146.

Riding, R., & Cheema, I. (1991). Cognitive styles: An overview and integration. Educa-tional Psychology, 11, 193–215.

Riding, R. J., & Wigley, S. (1997). The relationship between cognitive style and personal-ity in further education students. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 379–389.

Saleh, A. I. (1998). The nexus of brain hemisphericity, personality types, temperaments, learning styles, learning strategies, gender, majors, and cultures. Dissertation Abstracts International (Section A): Humanities and Social Sciences, 58(8-A), 3004.

Schleifer, M., & Douglas, V. I. (1973). Moral judgments, behavior and cognitive style in young children. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 5, 133–144.

Spangler, G., Pekrun, R., Kramer, K., & Hofmann, H. (2002). Students’ emotions, physi-ological reactions, and coping in academic exams. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 15, 413–432.

Sternberg, R. J. (1988). Mental self-government: A theory of intellectual styles and their development. Human Development, 31, 197–224.

Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge University Press.Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). Thinking Styles Inventory. Unpublished manu-

script, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut.Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., & Zhang, L. F. (2003). Thinking Styles Inventory—

Revised. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut.Torrance, E. P. (1988). SOLAT (style of learning and thinking) manual. Bensenville, IL:

Scholastic Testing Service.Torrance, E. P., Taggart, B., & Taggart, W. (1984). Human information processing survey.

Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service.Westreich, A. H., Ritzler, B., Duncan, J. (1997). Relationship between cognitive style and

defensive style. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 1011–1023.White, D. B. (1986). An assessment and validation of Chickering’s seven vectors of stu-

dent development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, Iowa City.White, D. B., & Hood, A. B. (1989). An assessment of the validity of Chickering’s theory

of student development. Journal of College Student Development, 30, 354–361.

Page 17: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Zhang 513

Winston, R. B., Jr., Miller, T. K., & Prince, J. S. (1987). Student developmental task and lifestyle inventory. Athens, GA: Student Development Associates.

Witkin, H. A. (1962). Psychological differentiation: Studies of development. New York: Wiley.

Zhang, L. F. (2000). Are thinking styles and personality types related? Educational Psy-chology, 20, 271–283.

Zhang, L. F. (2002a). The role of thinking styles in psychosocial development. Journal of College Student Development, 43, 696–711.

Zhang, L. F. (2002b). Thinking styles and cognitive development. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163, 179–195.

Zhang, L. F. (2003). Contributions of thinking styles to critical thinking dispositions. The Journal of Psychology, 137, 517–544.

Zhang, L. F. (2004a). Do university students’ thinking styles matter in their preferred teaching approaches? Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1551–1564.

Zhang, L. F. (2004b). Thinking styles: University students’ preferred teaching styles and their conceptions of effective teachers. The Journal of Psychology, 138, 233–252.

Zhang, L. F. (2005). Validating the theory of mental self-government in a non-academic setting. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1915–1925.

Zhang, L. F. (2008). Thinking styles and identity development among Chinese university students. American Journal of Psychology, 121(2), 255–271.

Zhang, L. F., & Higgins, P. (2008). The predictive power of socialization variables for thinking styles among adults in the workplace. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 11–18.

Zhang, L. F., & Huang, J. F. (2001). Thinking styles and the five-factor model of personal-ity. European Journal of Personality, 15, 465–476.

Zhang, L. F., & Postiglione, G. A. (2001). Thinking styles, self-esteem, and socio-eco-nomic status. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 1333–1346.

Zhang, L. F., & Sachs, J. (1997). Assessing thinking styles in the theory of mental self-government: A Hong Kong validity study. Psychological Reports, 81, 915–928.

Zhang, L. F., & Sternberg, R. J. (2005). A threefold model of intellectual styles. Educa-tional Psychology Review, 17, 1–53.

Zhang, L. F., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). The nature of intellectual styles. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Original manuscript received July 24, 2007Final version accepted October 31, 2007

Page 18: Thinking Styles and Emotions

514 The Journal of Psychology

AP

PE

ND

IX A

Thi

nkin

g St

yles

in t

he T

heor

y of

Men

tal S

elf-

Gov

ernm

ent

Dim

ensi

on

Thi

nkin

g st

yle

Key

cha

ract

eris

tics

Func

tion

Leg

isla

tive

Wor

k on

task

s th

at r

equi

re c

reat

ive

stra

tegi

es; o

ne p

refe

rs to

cho

ose

one’

s ow

n ac

tiviti

es.

E

xecu

tive

Wor

k on

task

s w

ith c

lear

inst

ruct

ions

and

str

uctu

res;

one

pre

fers

to im

plem

ent t

asks

with

e

stab

lishe

d gu

idel

ines

.

Judi

cial

W

ork

on ta

sks

that

allo

w f

or o

ne’s

eva

luat

ion;

one

pre

fers

to e

valu

ate

and

judg

e th

e pe

rfor

man

ce

of

othe

r pe

ople

.Fo

rm

Hie

rarc

hica

l D

istr

ibut

e at

tent

ion

to s

ever

al ta

sks

that

are

pri

oriti

zed

acco

rdin

g to

one

’s v

alui

ng o

f th

e ta

sks.

M

onar

chic

W

ork

on ta

sks

that

allo

w c

ompl

ete

focu

s on

one

thin

g at

a ti

me.

O

ligar

chic

W

ork

on m

ultip

le ta

sks

in th

e se

rvic

e of

mul

tiple

obj

ectiv

es, w

ithou

t set

ting

prio

ritie

s.

Ana

rchi

c W

ork

on ta

sks

that

wou

ld a

llow

fle

xibi

lity

as to

wha

t, w

here

, whe

n, a

nd h

ow o

ne w

orks

.L

evel

G

loba

l Pa

y m

ore

atte

ntio

n to

the

over

all p

ictu

re o

f an

issu

e an

d to

abs

trac

t ide

as.

L

ocal

W

ork

on ta

sks

that

req

uire

wor

king

with

con

cret

e de

tails

.Sc

ope

Inte

rnal

W

ork

on ta

sks

that

allo

w o

ne to

wor

k as

an

inde

pend

ent u

nit.

E

xter

nal

Wor

k on

task

s th

at a

llow

for

col

labo

rativ

e ve

ntur

es w

ith o

ther

peo

ple.

Lea

ning

L

iber

al

Wor

k on

task

s th

at in

volv

e no

velty

and

am

bigu

ity.

C

onse

rvat

ive

Wor

k on

task

s th

at a

llow

one

to a

dher

e to

the

exis

ting

rule

s an

d pr

oced

ures

in p

erfo

rmin

g ta

sks.

Page 19: Thinking Styles and Emotions

Zhang 515

AP

PE

ND

IX B

Thr

ee T

ypes

of

Inte

llect

ual S

tyle

s

Styl

e ty

pe/c

onst

ruct

Ty

pe I

Ty

pe I

I Ty

pe I

II

a Lea

rnin

g ap

proa

ch

Dee

p Su

rfac

e A

chie

ving

b Car

eer

pers

onal

ity ty

pe

Art

istic

C

onve

ntio

nal

Rea

listic

, inv

estig

ativ

e,

soc

ial,

ente

rpri

sing

c Mod

e of

thin

king

H

olis

tic

Ana

lytic

In

tegr

ativ

ed P

erso

nalit

y ty

pe

Intu

itive

, per

ceiv

ing

Sens

ing,

judg

ing

Thi

nkin

g, f

eelin

g,

int

rove

rsio

n,

ext

rave

rsio

ne M

ind

styl

e C

oncr

ete

rand

om

Con

cret

e se

quen

tial

Abs

trac

t ran

dom

,

abs

trac

t seq

uent

ial

f Dec

isio

n-m

akin

g st

yle

Inno

vatio

n A

dapt

atio

n g C

once

ptua

l tem

po

Ref

lect

ivity

Im

puls

ivity

h S

truc

ture

of

inte

llect

D

iver

gent

thin

king

C

onve

rgen

t thi

nkin

g i P

erce

ptua

l sty

le

Fiel

d in

depe

nden

t Fi

eld

depe

nden

t j T

hink

ing

styl

e L

egis

lativ

e, ju

dici

al,

Exe

cutiv

e, lo

cal,

Olig

arch

ic, a

narc

hic,

glo

bal,

hier

arch

ical

, m

onar

chic

, con

serv

ativ

e i

nter

nal,

exte

rnal

j

udic

ial

Not

e. T

heor

etic

al f

ound

atio

ns: a B

iggs

’s th

eory

of

stud

ent l

earn

ing

(J. B

. Big

gs, 1

978)

; b Hol

land

’s th

eory

of

care

er p

erso

nalit

y ty

pes

(J. L

. Hol

land

, 19

73);

c Tor

ranc

e’s

cons

truc

t of

brai

n do

min

ance

(E

. P. T

orra

nce,

198

8); d J

ung’

s th

eory

of

pers

onal

ity ty

pes

(C. J

ung,

192

3); e G

rego

rc’s

mod

el o

f m

ind

styl

es, (

A. F

. Gre

gorc

, 197

9); f K

irto

n’s

mod

el o

f de

cisi

on-m

akin

g st

yles

(M

. J. K

irto

n, 1

976)

; g Kag

an’s

mod

el o

f re

flec

tivity

-im

puls

ivity

con

cept

ual

tem

po (

J. K

agan

, 196

5); h G

uilf

ord’

s m

odel

of

stru

ctur

e of

inte

llect

(J.

P. G

uilf

ord,

196

7); i W

itkin

’s c

onst

ruct

of

fiel

d-de

pend

ence

/inde

pend

ence

(H

. A.

Witk

in, 1

962)

; j Ste

rnbe

rg’s

theo

ry o

f m

enta

l sel

f-go

vern

men

t (R

. J. S

tern

berg

, 198

8).

Page 20: Thinking Styles and Emotions

CLASSIFIED ADS NOW AVAILABLE IN OUR PSYCHOLOGY TITLES!������������������������������ �������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������

������������� �����������������������������������������������������������������������������������

�������� ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������

����������������������������������������������������

Page 21: Thinking Styles and Emotions