26
 1 Abstract In this paper, based upon a longitudinal investigation of the domestic literacy development a trilingual child, my daughter, Pia, aged 3-9 years, I postulate, and critically review, a theory of mind which is both informed by recent findings in cultural psychology (e.g. Valsiner, 2008a,b) and located within a sociocultural approach to practice and mediated action (Wertsch, 1991). This interdisciplinary theoretical framework is complemented by diachronic and synchronic data analysis as a means of bringing to light potential variations between long-term and short-term development. It also stresses the importance of time as a complex variable in developmental issues in conjunction with a dialogical depiction of social agents and it underlines the proactive, as opposed to reactive, nature of social agents negotiating environmental cues at varying levels of consciousness. Whilst conceding that we have yet to fully grasp how the human mind works, I am nonetheless able to conclude that Pia’s writing development at home is both mindful and social. It is a knotty jungle of dynamic a3nd stable elements, whose complexity arises from the multitude of possible trajectories within and across each of the key areas of interaction: psyche, time, signs and environment. I argue for a reassessment of central notions regarding children’s learning and close the paper by proposing a number of implications such reassessment may have for educational practice. Key words: writing, child, development, cognition, home For a more detailed analysis of children’s literacy development and practice: Travers Simon, J. (2010) Mind the Gap: The Domestic Writings of a Child Practitioner and their Implications for Educational Practice. Saarbrücken: Lambert Academic Publishing.

Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 1/26

  1

Abstract

In this paper, based upon a longitudinal investigation of the domestic literacydevelopment a trilingual child, my daughter, Pia, aged 3-9 years, I postulate, and

critically review, a theory of mind which is both informed by recent findings in

cultural psychology (e.g. Valsiner, 2008a,b) and located within a socioculturalapproach to practice and mediated action (Wertsch, 1991).

This interdisciplinary theoretical framework is complemented by diachronic and

synchronic data analysis as a means of bringing to light potential variations between

long-term and short-term development. It also stresses the importance of time as a

complex variable in developmental issues in conjunction with a dialogical depiction

of social agents and it underlines the proactive, as opposed to reactive, nature of social agents negotiating environmental cues at varying levels of consciousness.

Whilst conceding that we have yet to fully grasp how the human mind works, I am

nonetheless able to conclude that Pia’s writing development at home is both mindfuland social. It is a knotty jungle of dynamic a3nd stable elements, whose complexity

arises from the multitude of possible trajectories within and across each of the key

areas of interaction: psyche, time, signs and environment. I argue for a reassessment

of central notions regarding children’s learning and close the paper by proposing a

number of implications such reassessment may have for educational practice.

Key words: writing, child, development, cognition, home

For a more detailed analysis of children’s literacy development and practice:

Travers Simon, J. (2010) Mind the Gap: The Domestic Writings of a ChildPractitioner and their Implications for Educational Practice. Saarbrücken: Lambert

Academic Publishing.

Page 2: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 2/26

  2

This breathing house, whose doors go squeak, go bang or make no

noise at all: towards a theory of mind

Joan Travers Simon

Introduction

In this paper, based on a trilingual child, my daughter, Pia, aged 3-9 years, I extend previous idiographic research into literacy development as situated cultural practice

(e.g. Bursch 2006; Simon & Bursch, 2009). Drawing on recent observations in

cultural psychology, my conceptual aim here is to postulate a theory of mind locatedwithin a sociocultural approach to practice and mediated action (Wertsch, 1991),

which foregrounds how flexible, equitable, implicit and holistic interactions

characteristic of informal settings structure this particular child’s learning.

To recapitulate, briefly, Pia is a trilingual (English, French, German) child, who lives

in Alsace, in the North East of France, with her Black-British mother-researcher, her 

German father (an engineer), and a sister who is seventeen months her senior. Initial

research into how the environments of home and school shape this child’s

understandings and uses of writing was both quantitative, qualitative and comparative

in nature. I exposed the funds of knowledge instanced in the wide scope of unsolicitedtexts Pia produced at home during the research period. I identified the developmental

issues which will be elaborated in the current paper, and analysed the linguisticcharacteristics of her authoring. Pia’s domestic writing skills are, I demonstrate, in

advance of curricular expectations and thus more indicative of her true level of 

competence. I argue, moreover, not only for the child’s sensitivity towards writing

and her skilful deployment it as situated cultural practice, but also that writing, and

therefore literacy practice, provide as a means for exploring the Self. I conclude that

the skills and strategies fostered at home prior to formal schooling are decisive in

shaping a child’s access - and success - with regard to reading and writing tasks

within institutional settings; an observation, which, though confirmed by other studies

(Czerniewska, 1992; Dyson, 2007), has unfortunately not made sufficient impact onclassroom practice.

In this paper, I draw upon current observations as upon the ‘syntax’ of cultural psychology, notably upon the work of Jaan Valsiner (e.g. 2008a), paying attention to

the extent to which biological analogies prevalent in their field, but equally echoed in

  psycholinguistic developmental research (e.g. Ferreiro, 2007), may successfully

transfer to my data.

The paper will be structured as follows. Having unpacked the title of the paper, I then

go on to outline the conceptual and theoretical framework within which the analysis isset. As a next step, I present the data collected, expressing thereby critical reflections

Page 3: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 3/26

  3

on my own methodology. I then present and conduct a detailed analysis of my modelfor the theory of mind. The analysis itself comprises three main sections. In the first

section, I explain the individual components of the model from a longitudinal

vantage, showing how cognitive and practical, how the mindful and social, bear upon

development. The second level of analysis comprises a more detailed investigation of 

a single interaction, by means of which I reply to the critique of a ‘snapshot’ look atdevelopment. The final level of analysis offers a critical appraisal, anchored in

reflections relating to the representation of and inherent tensions regarding human

development. The paper concludes with a recapitulation of the key findings, beforeoutlining the potential significance of these for educational practice.

The breathing house: a metaphor for the mind

Metaphors, like similes, serve to expand meaning in tacit directions (Cornejo, 2007).

The image of the toolbox is a popular one for depicting the strategies learners have

recourse to. For me, however and in the meantime, the toolbox evokes adisorganised, over-laden container, eyed with a groan every time we open the lid, for 

we know all too well that much of its contents should have been disposed of long ago.

One’s house, I concede, may not necessarily be much tidier. For a theory of mind,

however, the metaphor of the breathing house proves more compelling for a host of 

reasons:

• represents the construction aspects of learning and development - which only stand

if we build them

• draws attention to the transformation of the mind as something active, alive,

organic and even experiential

• comprises recognizable zones within which specific, though not prescriptive,

actions take place and which afford variegated opportunities for movement from onelocation to another (i.e. the various developmental spaces of the learner)

• possesses a number of basic, identifiable attributes (i.e. whilst respecting the

individual nature of learning, we may attempt to establish regularities between

individual learners and across diverse learning scenarios)

• ‘squeak’: certain activities appear gradually or seem defect (and will need ‘oiling’)

• ‘bang’: certain activities are immediately apparent

• ‘no noise at all’: implicit activities

• more than one door may be opened at the same time and at different rates (multiple

and simultaneous scopes of learning)• houses are built by a pool of expert craftsmen (i.e. interdisciplinary approach to

learning)

• houses are located within larger political macro social structures

The metaphor, naturally, also has its limitations. Physically, we may only be in one

room at a time, though we can, and do, leave traces of our passage throughout the

house. Mental processes, by contrast, reside in numerous ‘rooms’ simultaneously.

They are at one and the same time dialogical, dialectical, polysemous and, as I shalllater argue, even polychronic. Nor should we infer from the metaphor that the

complex structure of Western houses - in comparison, say, to the less elaborate mud

hut – be taken as indicative of a culturally contingent hierarchy with regard to the

Page 4: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 4/26

  4

development of mental skills. Although further reflection will no doubt alert one toadditional limitations of the metaphor, there is a strong case, given its interactive

  poignancy, for privileging it over and above the more passive connotations of a

toolbox, where we put items in and take them out, without any allusion to the

 presence or nature of activities within the toolbox itself.

Conceptual and theoretical framework 

An interdisciplinary approach, drawing notably on cultural psychology and

sociocultural theory, provides the heuristic, interpretive framework for my analysis.

When they ‘talk’ to each other, the criteria and methods specific to each discipline

help us to refine our understandings, making us conscious of how crucial the

 precision of interpretive coordinates are. Moreover, by identifying the concepts andmethods underpinning individual disciplines or paradigms, we may, via a compound

framework such as the one I propose, locate and compensate for the lacunae related toeach.

Cultural psychology

Whilst psychology in general, given its traditional experimental and nomothetic

semantic/functional – as opposed to semiotic – bias (Venuelo, 2008), fails to access

the very type of data required to provide satisfactory answers to distinctions between

learning and development as basic psychological functions (Salvatore & Valsiner,

2008; Molenaar & Valsiner, 2008), or indeed to capture the ‘radically contingent and

infinitely pluralistic universe’ of human action (Garrison, 2004, p106; Valsiner, 2005,

 p9), cultural  psychology, from an idiographic perspective, sets its focus onunderstanding and explaining the dynamism and regularity of individuals as active,

dialogical ‘speaking’, and ‘feeling’ - as opposed to merely ‘thinking’ subjects.Drawing frequently on analogies from the biological world, it studies:

The extraordinary nature of the most ordinary aspects of daily human living at any

 place on the planet. (Valsiner, 2007a, p18, cited in Madureira, 2008, p233)

Psychology is the science of the prediction of the unpredictable, of the control of the

uncontrollable, and of the detection of whatever ‘behaviour’ may be taken to mean.

(Valsiner, 2005, p9)

The latter quote bears such striking resemblance to one by Bronfenbrenner as to possibly have been inspired by it:

Much of developmental psychology, as it now exists, is the science of the

strange behavior of children in strange situations with strange adults for the

  briefest possible periods of time (Bronfenbrenner, 1979:19, cited in van Lier,

2004:124)

Page 5: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 5/26

  5

The syntax employed in the domain might at first appear foreign to researchers fromother disciplines, such as myself, who have predominantly remained, and thus read,

within the field of educational research from a sociocultural perspective1:

The inevitable task for the education agent is to find an opening in the self-

regulatory network of the developing system so as to create arenas for possibleimpact. (Valsiner, 2008b, p134)

Once the ear becomes acclimatised, however, it is then able to recognise well-known

terms such as pupil/learner/individual, teacher and subject/agent behind the

unaccustomed ones of ‘developing/cognitive system’, ‘education agent’ and ‘social

organism’ respectively. It learns to tolerate the description of writing as a ‘semiotic

system’ with similar, if not equal, ease as a more familiar, analogous view of writingas ‘urban furniture’. From this point on, many insights are to be gained.

Cultural psychology underlines the temporal dimension to learning and development,

thus to becoming  as opposed to merely being , given our nature as intransitive, i.e.open, and as ‘situated’ systems, that is, as cognitive systems whose development

relies upon a permanent exchange with the specificities, or catalytic features, of their 

social environment. Unlike Unsworth (2001), who dismisses ‘being’ as

‘anachronistic’, or Valsiner, for whom ‘being’ is a useless momentary snapshot

(Valsiner, 2008a, p2), I nonetheless regard attention to being as indispensable for an

understanding of becoming, as I hope to demonstrate later in the paper.

 Sociocultural theory

Central to a sociocultural perspective is the view of the individual not as a cognitiveisolate but firmly anchored and actively engaged in situated practice by means of 

social – and inherently political – tools (Bakhtin 1986; Bourdieu 1991; Street, 2004),

of both symbolic (e.g. language) and material nature (e.g. computers). As a precursor 

to evaluating the efficacy of situated practice, in which interactants either reproduce

or challenge dominant ideologies, attention is paid to close descriptions of social

contexts (Dyson, 2007; Kelly et al ., 2001; Rogoff et al ., 1998; Tharpe & Gallimore,

1998).

Acknowledging the situated nature of agency and practice yields a fluid,

heterogeneous, non-deterministic image of social agents interacting within andnegotiating the scope – the boundaries – between and beyond their various

  psychological, semiotic and ecological environments (Bomer, 2003; Fairclough,

1989). This view, moreover, underscores political and critical aspects to learning in

that it sets social action within relations of power which extend beyond the notion of 

‘resistance’ advocated within cultural psychology (e.g. Valsiner 2007). Such

‘zoniferous’ portrayal of heterogeneous individuals as members of multiple, and

1Action research, or ‘kidwatching’ (Goodman& Martens 2007), conducted by Vivian Paley, for 

example, never speaks of ‘systems’, ‘organisms’ ‘temporal-spatial dimensions’ or refers to points in

time as X+1, X+2, T1, T2, T3, yet beautifully, and convincingly, captures how, through situated and

distributed cognition, both the children’s and her own emergent understandings, their ‘wonderings and

wanderings’ (Merriam, 1991, cited in Goodman & Martens, 2007, p79), mutually transform theelements of the learner+sign+environment triad.

Page 6: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 6/26

  6

simultaneous, social communities places emphasis on agency; on a sense of ‘outthere’, as opposed to the more abstract, internally focused related concept of 

dialogism.

By blending the positions advocated by cultural psychology and sociocultural theory,

I effectuate a conceptual integration which highlights the various aspects I consider germane to an investigation of learning and development. Both disciplines draw

attention as much to the presence as to the differences in the reflexivity of individuals

engaging in social interaction, and thus away from the view of the agent as a passivelearner. The strands of cultural psychology complement the picture by privileging a

developmental dimension which is less addressed by the sociocultural theory.Sociocultural theory, for its part, promotes a critical dimension to meaning-making,

learning and social interaction that may often remain oblique from the psychological

vantage. The boundaries between the various perspectives is necessarily fluid, so that,

rather than being pidgeon-holed, a number of such perspectives may be

simultaneously ‘housed’ within diverse conceptual parameters (e.g. Ferreiro, 2007).

Methodology

An ethnographic approach to data collection resulted in the gathering, and creation, of 

data. In this section, I give an account of ethnographic methods then go on to specify

the primary and secondary sources of data collected at Pia’s home.

 Ethnographic methods

Ethnography is a generic term for a set of research tools originating from

anthropology and used as a means to understand and describe other cultures.

Ethnographic methods, later adopted and adapted by sociology, were employed to

describe and analyse others within Western society. The aim of ethnographic research

is to get alongside, or close to the participants, to be ‘taught’ by them and to thereby

illuminate, acknowledge and ultimately understand the interactive processes between

social agents and their symbolic worlds.

Ethnographic studies aim to produce a ‘cultural grammar’, yielding as comprehensive

a picture as possible of ‘living man’ and our ‘full-blooded facts’ (Malinowski, 1967).

I choose to cast my statements in more modest a mould, admitting merely to wantingto understand ‘something’, for although ethnographic methods are lengthy, data-rich

and well suited to unearthing the quality and complexities of social interaction which

may remain uncommented upon by research conducted in a positivist ethos, I stillconsider it over-ambitious to claim to tell the ‘whole’ truth, however wide the range

of methods employed, and however deep we delve.

Following the ethnographic tradition, I collected various types of data, on the strength

of which I propound my theory and model of mind for learning in informal, domestic

contexts.

 Primary data

Page 7: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 7/26

  7

The term primary data refers to the unsolicited texts Pia had written at home between

the ages of 3-9 years and which constitute my main research data. These texts form a

corpus of approximately 800 items. They range beyond work produced on paper to

encompass multimodal ‘texts’ e.g. a self-made computer, sms texts and ephemeral

texts written in sand or with beads or other tools, or at least such ‘texts’ I wasfortunate enough to have seen before they were washed, wiped or cleared away. For 

the large part, the items were brought to me by my daughter herself, with other items

retrieved from various sites around the house: the washroom, bedroom, hallway,kitchen walls, doors, study, sometimes long after they had been produced, but always

with Pia’s consent.

 Secondary data

Primary data is complemented by the range of secondary data comprising

conversational data, fieldnotes (FN) and a research diary (RD).

Conversational data

Observation of outcomes does not reveal the dynamic processes that enable and

maintain such seemingly smooth running of the social organism. (Valsiner,

2008b, p135)

An understanding of what Pia’s learning looks like and how it is ‘done’ can beenhanced by also paying attention to what Pia is saying. To this end, data was

gathered not only on the talk taking place during authoring events, but equally on talk 

‘around’ or ‘behind’ individual writing-related events themselves. Given thespontaneous nature of much domestic interaction, which largely defies recording, and

taking into account my inability to observe my daughter around the clock, it proved

impossible, but also unnecessary, to supply transcripts of the interactions generating

all the primary data. A selection of a/typical family interactional routines, mostly

  paraphrased and sometimes transcribed, would suffice to lend immediacy to the

semiotic texts.

 Fieldnotes & Research diary

More than just an unsystematic, motley means of data collection driven by merehunches and whims, and in fact frequently cited from in ethnographic studies,

fieldnotes (FN) allow discoveries in all areas identified as relevant to the overall

research aims to be shared with the reader in a less conventional form.

The Research diary (RD), like my fieldnotes (FN), may justly be regarded as piece of 

data in its own right. The central piece of data through which all other sources of 

information are networked, it provides direct access to the messy business of 

conducting research before this process is itself processed to become a coherent,

conventional text. Research is a creative process, thus we do not only collect, but also

generate data as much as understandings. Both fieldnotes and the research diary directattention to our responsibility as the author of our understanding, and thus remind us

Page 8: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 8/26

  8

that all  knowledge, and the presentation thereof, is constructed in one form or another. These generated understandings, offer an essential pathway back through my

reflective process, which I share in the following paragraphs.

 Assessing methodology

The critical reflections about my representation of mind which were generated by theanalysis of the data naturally extended to my methodology in general, to its discourse

and to how it positions both myself and my daughter.

The current analysis of someone else’s experiences which I interpret, get alongside, as

it were, and re-present, or argue, in the hope of furthering general understanding of 

the actions and processes researched, is a choice; an edifice, not a fact. At a certain

level, the truths are there, incontestably. Pia’s documents do exist. The conversations

and interactants are indeed authentic, yet at another level I have had to reconstruct

them, thereby imposing arbitrary boundaries to what is essentially an ongoing  process. Rather than claim a truth, I advocate a constructed understanding of 

‘disabused knowledge’ (Robertson, 2004), which necessarily shapes, but has also

limited, my understanding of the data.

On my role as a researcher 

The task, when striving to ‘disabuse knowledge’, is to foreground the limits of one’s

methodology and paradigms. There is a discrepancy, I believe, between the ‘full- blooded’ intentions of qualitative research and the somewhat bloodless convention of 

describing participants and their contributions as ‘data’. Likewise, I find the

neutralizing rhetoric prevalent in psychological research in general, and even in

studies in cultural psychology today, runs counter to the discipline’s aim to put full-

 blooded ‘Man’, in a Malinowskian sense, back into the discipline:

Psychology has re-written its history in ways that justify its lack of connection

with basic human cultural phenomena – the complex intentional forms of 

feeling, thinking and acting that characterize our everyday lives. Curiously, over 

  psychology’s formal history the basic reduction of human beings to be some

special cases of salivating dogs or industriously lever-pushing rodents – has

  passed as if that guaranteed the ‘scientific status’ of the discipline. The ‘hard’

data on rewarding or punishing humans with tokens of consumables – food,money, etc. – have led the way to our modern versions of explaining complexity

 by way of simple elementary ‘effects’ of some variable. Active persons – soul-

searchers filled with curiosity – who create, perform, and feel about theatre,

  poetry, music; who read novels, organize revolutions and political debates, and

worry about cholesterol levels, diets, prices, and marriages – are too ‘soft’ for an

‘objective’ study. (Valsiner, 2007, p254)

Increasingly conscious of the ideological nature of language, I also became

uncomfortable with my use of ‘I’. ‘I’, on the one hand, represents a conscious

decision to make myself transparent rather than hiding behind formulations in the passive voice as though some neutral hand were at play. On the other, ‘I’ provokes

Page 9: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 9/26

  9

my growing sensitivity to ‘voice’ and the lack of opportunity for Pia to say ‘I’, her  perceptions always being filtered by my own understandings.

On the child 

Children have been conceptualized as a person in the making, an inconclusive process

which continues in the adult, making it difficult for us to assure complete objectivity

or the epistemological rupture granting us the right of way from the naive to thescientific (Mollo, 1975). The French word for child –  enfant  – sets off a series of 

reflections which are not automatic when we consider the term in English.  Enfant  originally means ‘a person who cannot talk’. Whilst a similar English term exists,

‘infant’ refers to the earlier part of childhood, up to three years (Schaffer, 1996). The

French enfant , however, covers the whole developmental period up to adolescence. If 

we blend the notion of a person-in-process with that of one who cannot speak, we

easily slide into reflections on incompleteness and inability. These are etic, ‘outside’ 

  perspectives, which incite us to question the extent to which we exploit or counter invisibility/inaudibility vis-à-vis the child and her ‘voice’.

In reflecting upon questions of voice, we return to the emic-etic, inside-outside

debate. Attempts to redress potential imbalance, to accord Pia more voice, and indeed

to make myself transparent as researcher-mother-learner-editor, however, have been

undertaken in the paper, either via the use of direct citations of what Pia has to say

(e.g. Bursch & Simon, 2009) or else via excerpts from fieldnotes or the research

diary.

 Now it is time to illuminate what Pia appears to be doing as she learns to write at

home.

Pathway through learning: a longitudinal perspective

Pia’s basic pathway through learning to write at home may be charted as follows:

Page 10: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 10/26

  10

Diagram 1: pathway through learning in informal contexts

The model consists of three interdependent components;

-  environmental (social and material)-  action/behaviour 

-   personal/intramental

Environmental cues or stimuli, which may be social and/or material in nature, trigger 

thought processes or insights in the learner, which may translate into action. Such

thought processes take place in a cognitive ‘corridor’ between adjacent actions. It is

this thought process, often leading to what we call an A-ha effect, which opens the

door, as it were, to the next developmental level, and thus ‘drives’ Pia’s development

forward. Thought processes, as we shall see, need not translate into positive volition,

  but may equally result in the decision not to perform a certain act, as when Pia

decides that she does not need any help anymore, a decision that will manifest itself inher attempts to resist certain types of assistance. In keeping with a sociocultural

 perspective on learning, I have positioned the intermental level above the intramental

one, despite my awareness of the fact that such a depiction, moving from the ‘outside’

to the ‘inside’, may be contested. My intention is to underline the social nature of 

thought, which is stimulated by actions taking place in one’s social environment.

Later in the paper, I will elaborate upon the extent to which development may be

conceived of as an outside inside process, or indeed vice versa.

The model aims to schematise not only the key stages of Pia’s developmental  pathway with regard to learning to write at home, but it equally seeks to draw

attention to the varying forms of autonomy and competence involved. I will gothrough and exemplify each stage in turn.

 Awareness

Mother: Where can you see writing in your house?

Pia: In my Diddle book... one time Beate gave me some writing I stuck on the

 bedroom door with blu-tack... in my writing book... (FN#1, Pia: 6yrs 1m)

Mother: Where can you see writing outside your house?

Pia: On the street signs. I see numbers on the houses and once at school I sawwriting with a pencil and there was a hole in the middle. (FN#2. Pia: 6yrs 1m)

The impetus for learning must come from the learner, who must want to learn,

either because of a natural human propensity to do so, or because of an interest

in the material. (van Lier, 1996, p13)

Interest in a phenomenon, in this case the A-ha in relation to writing, is aroused by the

environment: Pia is ‘sensitized’. Now aware, sensitized, or what Piaget, van Lier 

recalls, would term ‘vigilant’ (van Lier, 1996, p51), Pia’s genuine interest in her new

discovery – her  A-ha! - leads to action. Pia is born into a ‘signs-infested’ world(Valsiner   2008a) and she is not insensitive to this. Interest in a phenomenon is

Page 11: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 11/26

  11

  preceded by a heightened consciousness of that phenomenon: you cannot beinterested in something you remain unaware of. Intuitive interest, or ‘apprehension’2,

reacting to environmental cues, transforms to an analytical stance of ‘comprehension’

which structures further, I propose, conscious, development (van Lier, 1996).

Awareness coupled to volition translates into the thought: ‘I want to know how to do

that’. This thought translates into the next level of action, which manifests itself as procedural play.

 Procedural Play

In order to learn, a person must be active, and the activity must be partly familiar 

and partly new, so that attention can be focused on useful changes and

knowledge can be increased (...) Learning takes place when the new is embedded

in the familiar, so that risks and security are in balance. (Marry & Trevarthen,

1985, cited in van Lier, 1996, p171)

  New skills being based on previous competence, Pia’s initial writing leans on her 

ability to draw, so that her writing often seems more like sketching. Still unaware of 

the rules of writing, Pia first ‘pretends’ to write, or plays with the ‘procedure’ of 

writing words, hence the term ‘procedural play’. Much writing related activity takes

 place before Pia is able to independently produce comprehensible texts. Such activity

may involve the merger of script and drawing. Alternatively, they may entail stringing

letters together or writing squiggly lines as a precursor to joined-up handwriting:

illustr.1: Letter play, 4yrs 11m

2The terms ‘apprehension’ and ‘comprehension’ may be seen to map onto what in cultural psychology

is respectively termed the axis of identification (AI) and the axis of development (AD) (e.g. Salvatore

& Valsiner, 2008, p14). Van Lier reminds us that, although the distinction has been discussed for centuries in philosophy and psychology, it remains marginal to language, and therefore literacy,

learning theory (van Lier, 1996, p54)

Page 12: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 12/26

  12

illustr.2: Writing, 5yrs 4m

Procedural play, together with vigilance leads Pia to the discovery that there are rules.

This, in turn, triggers the next reflection: ‘I need to learn the rules’. It is time for Pia

to seek help.

 Assisted Acquisition

Many people can help; my family and friends. Not only teachers, you know!

(Bursch & Simon, 2009, p39)

During this phase of Pia’s writing development, autocatalytic processes become auto-

allocatalytic ones: guided by oneself and by others, important others, acting as

‘guiding lights’ (Padmore, 1994). The result is a proliferation in the number of 

collaborative writing activities, in which the whole family is sometimes involved.

Initially, Pia might dictate texts for her sister or mother to write down. Later, Pia

seeks help with her own handwriting, as in a bi-lingual letter to a classmate, Lo ïc,

  begun in German, continued in French and written with the aid of her older sister,

who dictates the spelling, although it is Pia herself who decides to use dotted lines to

scaffold her own handwriting:

Page 13: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 13/26

  13

Dear Loic, I saw Keli on TV and it was super. You can win Batman with it.

illustr.3: Liebe Loïc, 5yrs 4m

One might, with reason, ask why Pia scaffolds herself via dotted lines, when she has

evidently progressed beyond the need of such scaffolding, as we see in illustr.2. Here,

different activities conjoin. On the one hand, Pia is genuinely writing a letter to her friend, and seeks assistance in spelling, having understood the correlation between

sound and symbol. On the other hand, she uses this authentic activity to repositionherself playfully as someone learning to write, using the dotted lines she has

encountered in her writing exercise books.

This collaborative phase is characterised by much talking as Pia negotiates the degree

of help sought. Assistance is given, but conscious teaching is not the uppermost priority at home, where Pia’s interests and needs must jostle to find space alongside

the many other activities that make up daily family life:

I am busy at the computer. Papa brings the kids to bed. Pia rushes in and puts

this on my table. I push it aside and continue working.

 Bringst du mich jetzt ins bett? Oui/non

Or have I got 5 minnits? OUI/NON 

 FOM Pia 

(will you tuck me in? Yes/No, or have I got 5 minutes? Yes/No. From Pia)

  NB answers are in French. The questions are in German or English. This text

doesn’t have the desired effect in that I do not read it straight away. By the time I

read it and go upstairs, she is already asleep... (FN#3, Pia: 8yrs 5m)

The above text is a testimony of Pia’s skills as a writer and social agent, who has

  progressed beyond the need for assistance and therefore seeks none: ‘I don’t need

help anymore’. From this point on, we can speak of her entering the phase of mastery.

 Mastery

Page 14: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 14/26

  14

Mastery is when learning becomes automized; progressively ‘energy-efficient’ bymeans of heterogeneous practice (van Lier, 1996, p41). Once the rules – or principles

 – of writing gradually become internalised, collaborative activities, in particular those

motivated by Pia’s need of assistance as opposed to situations in which she co-

constructs as a competent peer, begin to peter out. Concomitantly, the volume of 

unassisted texts increases.

Pia is as an active member of the literate community from the moment she uses

writing and is able to produce texts which make sense to her. She masters writing to adegree which legitimizes and reinforces her status as a member of the literate

community once she is able to interact with the conventional literate community viatexts which also make sense to others. Significant is also the fact that her membership

is acknowledged. Both her father (illustr.4) as well as her German teacher, Beate,

(illustr.5) take the time to reply:

illustr.4: Letter to papa, 6yrs 9m

Page 15: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 15/26

  15

Dear Beate, today I am ill. I have done hard maths for you… Dear Pia, thanks for your letter. You have

done maths from Year 2! Well done! Come back soon! Beate.

illustr.5: Letter to Beate, 6yrs 5m

In a sense, from this point on, Pia is already operating beyond her Zone of Proximal

Development. In the letter to her father, self-editing gives evidence of Pia’s awarenessof the difference between what she knows and what she does in the first instance. In

her letter to her German teacher, we sense a confident author, not someone positionedas a novice in Year One, although literacy as ‘life experience writing’ (Kitagawa &

Kitagawa, 1987, cited in Moro, 1999, p172) is sometimes also fostered at schools3.

Such confidence, willingness and readiness evokes a new image of the child author;

not as a learner-apprentice, dependent upon others, but rather as a practitioner, using

whichever level of skills is at her disposal in order to interact with the world and the

 people around her.

Pia’s first unassisted text is written in French:

3compare Pia’s letter to Beate to an exchange in a First Grade Japanese class, in which the pupil wrote

the following: ‘I saw an earthworm the earthworm Suzuki grabbed the earthworm’. The teacher 

replied: ‘I understood this composition because I happened to be there and to see her face at the time of 

the event. But even if I did not, still I would want to read into it and to appreciate the mind of the child

who wants to express herself through writing’. The teacher’s reflective comments take on the quality of fieldnotes rather than speak directly to the child, yet demonstrate Seiskatsu Tsuzurikata: writing as

social practice (Moro, 1999, p172)

Page 16: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 16/26

  16

illustr.6a : Ho ho! 5yrs 11m

illustr.6b: Ho ho! 5yrs 11m

This series of principled, that is to say, rule-governed texts, each consisting of 2-3

lines and each written in a different colour, is composed in a single writing session.Conventional spacing and spelling would be as follows:

Ho ho, qui est est là? Ah, tu es là, Marie. Merci pour le joli cadeau d’Alissia pour 

l’anniversaire d’Alissia (text a)

Hi hi qui peut vivil hahahahaha oh! Tu me visiteras (text b)

Toc toc toc, venez ici, c’est toi ! Où est le cadeau ? (text c)

Ho ho ho, qui est là ? Ah! Tu es là, Marie! Merci pour le joli cadeau (text d)

Toc toc toc! Ho Ho! Qui est là? Ha ha! Tu es là, Vivianna. Merci (text e)

Translated, these texts mean:

Ho ho, who’s there? Ah, you are there, Marie. Thanks for the lovely present for Alissia’s birthday (text a)

Page 17: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 17/26

  17

Hi Hi who can (vivil is an invented word) hahahahaha, oh! You’ll visit me (text b)

Ding dong, come here, it’s you! Where’s the present? (text c)

Ho ho ho, who’s there? Ah, you are there, Marie. Thanks for the lovely present (text d)

Ding dong! Ho ho, who’s there? Ha ha! It’s you, Vivianna. Thanks (text e)

Pia’s understanding of the sound-sign correlation is made evident as she ‘writesspeech’ (Vygotsky, 1978):

Standard French Pia’s French at this developmental stage

Qui Ci

Est E

Merci Messi, mersi

Cadeau Kado

Venez vene

Ici Issi

Pour Puor 

Anniversaire

Ah

Aniverser 

ATable 1: Appropriating French spelling

Authorship is ‘reflective generation of text’ (Jackson, 1993, p67), hence we need to

expose and understand such reflexivity for its capacity to indicate cognitivedevelopment in preference to focusing on polished end results (Kress, 1994; Valsiner,

2008b, p135). It is in this sense, too, that attention to ‘being’ is an important reference

for charting ‘becoming’, for reflexivity is best understood by analysing instances of itin the ‘here-and-now’. In the first text (illustr.6a), Pia begins to write on the right

hand side of the page then continues on the left hand side. The directionality of words

is correct and her use of colour guides the reader through the message. In the

subsequent texts (illustr. 6b), she exhibits control over letter size and directionality,

not only of the word, but of the page as a whole. Pia has appropriated the correct use

of exclamation marks. We note, however, that she does not use question marks at the

end of the questions:

HO HO CI ELA A! TU E LA MARI

Ho, ho who’s there? Oh! You are there, Marie

These Ho Ho texts differ to earlier texts exhibiting word as a concept, such as  Liebe

 Loï c, because the latter instantiates her sister’s knowledge of word and not her own.

Here, we see the first traces of Pia’s understanding of word, as shown by her spacing

on the first page of her text (illustr. 6a) but less so for the rest (illustr. 6b). In these

unassisted   Ho Ho texts, Pia is not re  producing, but producing, thus she has

 progressed, in her opinion, beyond the need to always have a model. Her texts aremeaningful, purpose-driven, rule-governed and embedded; Pia’s birthday in a

month’s time explaining her heightened interest in presents and birthdays. Her texts

are legible and, given the right contextual information and provided we are able to

refocus to her cognitive level, comprehensible. She possesses all the information she

Page 18: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 18/26

  18

needs in order to use writing in a transformative manner: ‘I can apply the rules for new results’. The next step in her development entails, therefore, diversification.

 Diversification

Principled competence leads to further diversification4. Once the rules or abstract

concepts (C) have been internalised and reduced (R), they may be applied (A) to new

situations:

C R  A1 A2 A3 etc

The child’s thoughts to the above formula might read as follows: once I’ve

understood it (C), that is, the basic idea (R), then I can use it in lots of different ways

(A1, A2, A3), try it out (Bursch, 2006). This is precisely what children do when they

approach writing (Kress, 1997; Vygotsky, 1978).

 Moving backwards and forwards and in spurts through development 

Children will continue to use techniques from the past in order to understand and

work out ways to live securely in the present (Paley, 1990, p142)

Progression through the depicted developmental stages is, however, not always linear.

The data has revealed that Pia’s writing development may entail recursive loops asPia re-engages with a developmental stage she has long since mastered. This is

evidenced when Pia, now a confident author, teaches Benni, a fictive pupil, how to

write and spell:

4Development is said to entail over/hyper-production (Valsiner, 2008b). The idea is echoed in the view

of children’s overuse of particular linguistic/semiotic formulations as ‘surplus’ of meaning. I, however,

do not regard diversification as overproduction, but see the child as producing as much as is necessary

in order to consolidate his/her knowledge. See also Bruner (1999), who observes that children select pieces of information to attend to that fit their capacity limits and then work within those limits, hence,

like all humans, they dislike banality and overload from the start.

Page 19: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 19/26

  19

illustr.7: Benni learns to write, 6yrs 11m

Almost seven years old, Pia returns to a developmental stage achieved at least a year 

and a half earlier. Scaffolded letter formation is followed by the progression to sounds

incorporating the phoneme o :  ponpon. New vowels are introduced: pa pion. One of 

the new vowels is practised in conjunction with the starting consonant: Pa pa.

‘Ponpon’ and ‘papion’ are not real words but sound clusters, transformative exercises

aimed at learning the correlation between graphemes and phonemes, as used in the

early writing exercise books. Benni learns to  spell  in the same manner as Pia herself did, that is to say, structurally, as at school, using what is already known as a starting

 point for diversification. In her role as teacher, Pia also evaluates, crossing out badly

formed letters and giving Benni the overall grade of 1,5. We should note, however,that Pia learns how to write (as opposed to spell) and is an active writer at home long

 before formal schooling. More to the point, in her non-institutional context, Pia learns

to write holistically, not as a vehicle for knowledge display but as a means of 

authentic social action.

A return to earlier stages of development, recast as play, as we see in   Liebe Loic or 

  Benni learns to write, may serve to consolidate and confirm knowledge. Principled

and procedural learning blend in such transformative activities as learning becomes playful teaching and nothing is at stake. Such practice invalidates a view of learning

as always, or automatically, synchronised with the original catalytic input, for itreveals that learning may take place at alternative temporal and cognitive levels. We

can learn, therefore, not only by moving forwards, but equally by moving backwards.

A review of the production dates of Pia’s texts, further, demonstrate that writing at

home, guided not only by her developmental level, but also by her social and

emotional needs, is not produced ‘seamlessly’ and regularly, but may involve ‘spurts’,

contesting the smooth line view at the heart of much language and literacy pedagogy.

As Pia says in her own words:

Page 20: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 20/26

  20

Sometimes I don’t write anything at all… and sometimes I write loads and loads!

(Bursch & Simon, 2009, p38)

This last developmental phase is an open one in the sense that future adaptations or 

transformations of writing (writing new words, genres, languages, etc) take place inthis part of the model, with the previous sections feeding this development, if needs

be. Pia might not seek assistance to write new words in French, for example, althoughshe will be sensitised to French spelling, become aware of its rule-bound nature and

of her need to acquire these rules. Nor need she go through procedural play,

‘pretending’ to write French. It seems, therefore, that certain phases of the model, I

 propose, may be skipped if skills learnt in one area are easily transferable to another,

as is the case with writing.

Diversification is the motor which moves development forward, enabling the learner 

or social organism to maintain the ability to adapt to new contexts. There is a

metacognitive dimension here, for Pia knows that she knows how to diversify. It isthis knowing  about doing that enables her to transform knowledge and that is at theheart of her autocatalytic processes, with their attendant changes to her environment.

Diversification, thus, is a cognitive tool, a cognitive semiotic, which she learns to use.

Diversification, moreover, is characteristic of each developmental level in the sense

that there are numerous ways to ‘perform’ at that level. Hence, awareness, for 

example, may be expressed in several forms. Pia might first become aware of writing

in a specific context, but will then have heightened sensitivity to it in other contexts.

Procedural play, similarly, may take several forms, yielding many forms of text.Regarding assisted acquisition, the type of assistance and the person assisting may be

diversified, and so on.

It has been said that ‘being’ is anachronistic for we are never ‘being’, always

‘becoming’ (Unsworth, 2001). At school, we note, children are often positioned as

‘becoming’ literate via an atomistic skills approach to instruction, as reflected in

 Benni learns to write (illustr.7). Such means are not dynamic, as one would expect of 

development, i.e. something in motion. The learner is ‘becoming’, yet there is a stasis

related to a contractual, structural model of language/literacy acquisition and practice

that exposes a tension between intentions. Is being, then, anachronistic? Let’s see. Inthe next section, I pursue the analysis from a synchronic vantage, instantiating Pia’s

‘being’ at a given moment in relation her informal writing development in order toascertain whether such a ‘snapshot’ may in fact help us to generate a deeper 

understanding of a theory of mind.

A single text is not an isolated text: a ‘snapshot’ perspective

In the street you learn what human beings really are; otherwise, or afterwards,

you invent them. (Miller, 1938, p11)

The data I have chosen to exemplify my theory of mind as evidenced by a single

event is entitled Natascha. At the time, Pia was 5yrs 6m. The text was produced on asheet of A4 paper, which Pia then folded in two to form a book. The text, begun at

Page 21: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 21/26

  21

school with a classmate, Natascha, was nonetheless, and more to the point, completed  by Pia at home. The text is a declaration of friendship: ‘  Natascha, tu étais ma

meilleure copine aujourd’hui’ (Natascha, you were my best friend today). A picture

complements the text by depicting children in the playground, in the centre, Pia and

her best friend for the day arm-in-arm:

illustr.8: Natascha, 5yrs 6m

Whereas the writing is achieved with some deliberation and in silence, the drawing is

rattled off in stages and with ease, to the accompaniment of Pia’s runningcommentary:

Stage 1: Pia and Natascha arm-in arm

Stage 2: boy and girl below

Stage 3: heaven clouds and sun, later adds children and some school buildings.

The children in this section are cold, so she draws ‘freezing’ lines down the sides

of their bodies; it’s December. (FN#5)

The interaction lasts approximately thirty minutes. It is documented in detailed

fieldnotes, exposing the learning of both interactants5.

Pia, emphasizing the fact it is not a letter, but a book, told me what she wanted to

write. I then wrote the phrase on a piece of paper for her. She got lost quickly in the jungle of letters, e.g. writing ‘p’ from ‘copine’ after ‘tu’. The result of this error, for 

her, is frustration. The result for me: an a-ha! Pia’s slip revealed that the directionalityof her gaze did not move conventionally from left to right, but from up to down, since

5I am learner and teacher in one, attempting to pitch my help at Pia’s level and learning from every

clue she reveals about her development. Pia, likewise, although less consciously, is both learner (howsigns look, how they are produced, and what they stand for) and teacher, assisting me to new levels of 

competence regarding her writing development.

Page 22: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 22/26

  22

 p was the letter on the line immediately below tu in the sample I had provided(illustr.9). Learning from this cue, I suggested she crossed out each letter after having

copied it. She expanded this new task into a secondary activity of colouring the

letters. This way she was able to direct her gaze to the appropriate letter and integrate

an element of personal creativity rather than simply carry out an order 6.

Illustr.9: Scaffolding

Similarly, when I explain that she can break up the word meilleure with a hyphen (for 

she has run out of space), she applies this most recently acquired principle in a new,

in her eyes, appropriate situation when she once again runs out of space and cannot

 put the words copine and aujourd’hui on the same line as on my draft, thus separates

them with a hyphen. Another a-ha! for me, as her action reveals that she associatesthe hyphen with the concept of  space, not the concept of  word . If we return to the

formula presented above:C R  A1 A2 A3 etc

We see that Pia’s use of hyphen relates to the phases A1 A2 A3, so that her thoughts may be: ‘if I can use a hyphen when I run out of space for  meilleure (A1), I

can use it when I run out of space elsewhere… (A2). She has understood the concept(C), yet her understanding is limited by the fact that she has not yet internalised the

relevant concept of word. This is made evident by the fact that, in spite of my clear spacing, it is not easy to identify the start and the end of words in Pia’s text (illustr.8).

Pia does not simply take my meaning/recommendation, she re-interprets it. The

 process of her letter formation is not always conventional, as can be seen by the

6

If anyone can be said to simply carry out an order, then it is I in as much as Pia tells me what to writeand I, dutifully, write it down, though even here, the original task is extended, transformed, for I break 

it down into ‘manageable’ sub-activities (crossing out letters, using hyphens)

Page 23: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 23/26

  23

arrows in the illustrating below, denoting where her penstrokes go up and down, eventhough the final product remains the same:

Illustr.10: Negotiating the directionality of letters

I observe, further, how she adds strokes to certain letters, much in the manner of 

drawing/painting, e.g. letters m,a,e, thereby moving in and out of writing and drawing

as discrete visual representations.

Pia was uneasy about the straight line at the beginning of the letter ‘m’ because shesaid she had not ‘learnt’ this letter yet. As I knew that she was familiar with it in the

cultural context of the sign MacDonald’s, her hesitancy drew my attention to a

discrepancy between what she knows and what she thinks she knows. Similarly, shedid not want to put an accent on the ‘e’ because, again, she said she has not ‘learnt’ it

yet, so insisted that I did it for her. Learning, thus, for her, seems to be something

which must be done explicitly. At home, however, and as she says, it is as if you learn

secretly (Bursch & Simon, 2009). In retrospect, I realise this activity is pitched above

her ZPD. I should have used capitals, as this is what she was writing at the time (cf 

Ho Ho txts). I wrote in lower case because it was natural for me, but I see now that it

was demanding too much of Pia, who nonetheless rose to the challenge.

Pia, insisting that she has written a book not a letter, obviously has very concreteideas on the form of her text, thus she is the authority on her own authoring. On the

other hand, her unwillingness to attempt anything she has not yet ‘learnt’ at schoolreveals her doubts about any such claim to authority. She oscillates between

know/don’t know; between positive volition and outright resistance. By the end of the

interaction, and despite her resistance, she has nonetheless attained a higher level of 

writing competence thanks to the finely-tuned help of her mother. The

accomplishment exemplifies progress through the Zone of Proximal Development,

 but is not based on an understanding of learning as an abstract, solitary activity aimedat some future benefit. Rather, it is based on an understanding of learning as being

deeply anchored in a specific, here-and-now, authentic, socio-affective context,instigated by a proactive child not primarily interested in the structural aspects of 

learning in the classical, formal sense, but in using knowledge, in this case literacy, as

a tool to get something – a socially purposeful act - done.

Page 24: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 24/26

  24

To relate these findings to my model, we see how the social and material environmentimpacts on Pia’s awareness, readiness and willingness. An allocatalytic incident with

a classmate and some writing material triggers off Pia’s willingness to create the text,

although she doubts her cognitive readiness to produce it on her own, thus she later 

solicits the help of a more competent other. We note, nonetheless, the absence of 

 procedural play, yet the introduction of a new creative element as Pia colours in thecrossed out letters, thereby controlling, or learning to master, what initially appeared

to be a ‘jungle of letters’. This new creative element is set up by the allocatalytic

intervention of Pia’s mother. Environmental cues, thus, frame, but do not prescribe,the direction of development, leaving Pia the room to appropriate the activity and give

it her own meaning as opposed to blindly copying7.

The text is pre pared, (first with Natascha, then with Pia’s mother). It is co-constructed

(with Natascha and Pia’s mother). And it is redesigned (e.g. colouring and drawing

activities are added, unconventional directionality of certain penstrokes, picture

added). To need the help of others does not mean the child is wholly dependent upon

others, however, and at several points, Pia can be seen to exhibit independent action.When analysing emergent activities or skills, therefore, attention must be paid not

only to how children are helped, but equally to how they help themselves.

A particular developmental level may give evidence of residual, emergent and

mastered or dominant skills/knowledge at one and the same time (Williams, 1977,

  p132). Pia’s letter formation still has traces of drawing skills, for example, and

although she encounters difficulty with a number of letters, she has mastered how to

write, in this case, twice as many letters (a,m,l,r,c,o,p,j,d,h: N=10) as those we could

describe as being emergent (s,e,i,u,t: N=5). Pia not only transforms mastery of thesequence of letters on her draft into a colouring activity, but she also transfers the

colouring activity to the writing of the text itself, each letter in the book for Natascha

  being written in the same colour as the one used to control her gaze across her 

mother’s model. Via the colouring/writing activity, therefore, we are given further 

indications of simultaneous, yet divergent, levels of skill: emergent control over 

reading a string letters in addition to writing others, and diversification as a

consequence of mastery, reflected the application of internalised rules for new results

(e.g. the use of hyphen in new situations). It is interesting to note that no comment is

made about Pia’s ‘emergent’ letters. She is not told that her way of writing them is

wrong in any way. Pia herself, more to the point, appears only to exhibit insecuritywith regard to the letter é, which she refuses to write, and the letter m. Hence, from

the child’s perspective, these letters alone are deemed new or emergent, so thatconclusions about a child’s level of competence must be deferred until we have a

solid understanding of what the child him/herself thinks or reveals about his/her own

abilities.

7The notion of copying appears frequently in discussions of literacy learning and practice. I embrace

the view posited by scholars such as Kress (1994, 1997, 2003a,b) or Rowe (2003), who recognise the

‘design’ and ‘recasting’ aspects to children’s reproductive writing. Anyone observing children closely

will discover how much they hate being copied (Travers, 2009). Paley (1990) however suggests that a

child’s eagerness to copy another child might be interpreted as an invitation to friendship and a meansto establish a bridge between their two worlds. As such, ‘copying’ may be an invitation to dialogue

(and dialogism) beyond the merely verbal plane.

Page 25: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 25/26

  25

The fact remains: a purely chronological depiction of development - from novice toexpert – is not unproblematic. Learning, it would seem, is not always a forward-

feeding, conscious process. In addition to the simultaneous presence of varying levels

of skill, the developmental pathway may also entail periods of un/awareness, by

which I do not mean a progression from unawareness to awareness, but equally a shift

from awareness to unawareness:

(5yrs 6m)

M: Has anyone in your family ever helped you to learn something about writing?

P: No…

(after much thinking....)

You helped me to read with the Mico book, but not to write. (FN#4)

(5yrs 9m)

M: Has anyone ever helped you with writing at home?

P: Yes. You! (FN#5)

(8yrs 2m)

M: Where did you learn to write?

P: At school. (FN#6)

Despite the confirmation given by the  Natascha interaction that Pia’s writing skills,

learnt at home, are in advance of those transmitted in the classroom, she remains

largely unaware of how instrumental her home environment, and her mother in

 particular, is in her writing development. It is as she says elsewhere: ‘at home, it’s as

if you learn secretly’ (Bursch & Simon, 2009). The shifts in her degree of awareness

mean that each developmental block represented in the diagram, therefore, should be

seen as containing within itself backward and forward moving sub/conscious

developmental activity in a general progression towards mastery.

If we embrace the dialogical – as opposed to merely dialectical - self, a central

  problem becomes where to allocate learning, which is typically still spoken of 

according to an underlying notion of a unified (ontological) self, even within the

 paradigms of both sociocultural theory and cultural psychology. The ‘zoniferousness’

inherent in a dialogical perspective of the individual is further underscored by the

various semiotic ‘zones’ within the text (drawing, writing, words of different colours),

along with movement ‘in’ and ‘out’ at the psychological level (meaning making-

taking), linguistic level (the written text is in French, I speak English, hence theoverall interaction is bilingual), and ecological level (home, school). The combinationof a dialogical and  plurilingual self alone magnifies the complexity of attempts, and

claims, to depict the nature of learning. Movement ‘in’ and ‘out’ can also be

established at the temporal level, being at times ‘quick’, as when Pia embellishes her 

written text with the drawing, or indeed ‘slow’, when we think that it took Pia almost

half an hour to produce her one-sentence story, though here, once again, to judge her 

efforts, and success, as ‘slow’ is an adult perspective, thus must be treated with a

certain caution.

Taking a ‘snapshot’ of Pia’s writing development, we see, enables us to move from

the general character of longitudinal development to the complex dynamics of individual developmental stages when looked at more closely within a microgenetic

Page 26: Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

8/9/2019 Travers Simon, J. B. (2010) This Breathing House: Towards a Theory of Mind

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/travers-simon-j-b-2010-this-breathing-house-towards-a-theory-of-mind 26/26

instance, thereby allowing us to become alert to nuances which otherwise run the risk of being glossed over. In sum, we experience text and text production as a much

larger concept than placing words on a page, as a much more dynamic, ontopotential  communication ensemble (Kress, 2003a,b) in its own right. More than a ‘mere’

snapshot or empty statement in developmental terms, the text, as an instance of 

‘being’ in which development is mirrored, has the potential to be an open system anda ‘living agent’8 (Smorti, 2008).

The second half of the paper is currently under revision...

8Cf however Wertsch’s comments on Bakhtinian authority in texts and their potential univocal

qualities (footnote 15)