44
TRUSTED NETWORK-BRIDGING TIES: A DYADIC APPROACH TO THE BROKERAGE-CLOSURE DILEMMA Daniel Z. Levin Management and Global Business Department Rutgers Business School – Newark and New Brunswick Rutgers University 1 Washington Park Newark, NJ 07102 Phone (973) 353-5983, Fax (973) 353-1664 [email protected] Jorge Walter Department of Strategic Management and Public Policy School of Business The George Washington University 2201 G Street, NW, Funger Hall 615 Washington, DC 20052 Phone: (202) 994-7908, Fax: (202) 994-8113 [email protected] Melissa M. Appleyard Ames Professor in the Management of Innovation and Technology School of Business Administration Portland State University P.O. Box 751 Portland, OR 97207 Phone (503) 725-9581, Fax (503) 725-5850 [email protected] September 2011 Working paper The authors wish to thank the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation and the Research Resources Commit- tee at Rutgers Business School for funding; the management and employees of the firms in- volved for their willingness to participate in the survey; and Virginie Lopez-Kidwell, Bill McE- vily, and Matt Smith for suggestions, advice, and assistance.

Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

TRUSTED NETWORK-BRIDGING TIES:

A DYADIC APPROACH TO THE BROKERAGE-CLOSURE DILEMMA

Daniel Z. Levin Management and Global Business Department

Rutgers Business School – Newark and New Brunswick Rutgers University 1 Washington Park Newark, NJ 07102

Phone (973) 353-5983, Fax (973) 353-1664 [email protected]

Jorge Walter Department of Strategic Management and Public Policy

School of Business The George Washington University 2201 G Street, NW, Funger Hall 615

Washington, DC 20052 Phone: (202) 994-7908, Fax: (202) 994-8113

[email protected]

Melissa M. Appleyard Ames Professor in the Management of Innovation and Technology

School of Business Administration Portland State University

P.O. Box 751 Portland, OR 97207

Phone (503) 725-9581, Fax (503) 725-5850 [email protected]

September 2011

Working paper

The authors wish to thank the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation and the Research Resources Commit-tee at Rutgers Business School for funding; the management and employees of the firms in-volved for their willingness to participate in the survey; and Virginie Lopez-Kidwell, Bill McE-vily, and Matt Smith for suggestions, advice, and assistance.

Page 2: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

TRUSTED NETWORK-BRIDGING TIES:

A DYADIC APPROACH TO THE BROKERAGE-CLOSURE DILEMMA

ABSTRACT

We propose and test a novel solution to the brokerage-closure dilemma that combines the

network and dyadic (relationship) levels of analysis. Specifically, we argue that tie strength can

act as a substitute for the benefits of network closure, and so a network-bridging tie will yield

more useful knowledge when it is also a strong tie. We further disentangle the mechanism under-

lying the beneficial effect of tie strength by formulating three hypotheses on interaction fre-

quency, emotional closeness, and relational trust. Using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) on a

sample of 408 knowledge-seeking relationships in a large U.S. engineering firm, we find that

relational trust has the largest positive impact, whereas emotional closeness has a much more

limited impact and interaction frequency actually seems to inhibit the value of a bridging tie. Fi-

nally, we find that many bridging ties are trusted, thereby allowing knowledge seekers to “un-

lock” the value embedded in these ties.

Keywords:

Social networks, structural holes, tie strength, trust, knowledge transfer

1

Page 3: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Generating and making use of new knowledge can be a source of competitive advantage

(Argote, 1999; Grant, 1996; Zander & Kogut, 1995), and hence the cultivation of effective

knowledge-transfer relationships—to develop and obtain this knowledge—is essential. Such re-

lationships, in the aggregate, have been studied extensively in the literatures on social networks

and social capital. For example, these literatures have established the benefits of brokering be-

tween disconnected parties for generating novel insights (e.g., Burt, 1992, 2004). Yet knowledge

seekers can sometimes face a dilemma in that this network structure affords novelty but often

impedes knowledge transfer. Specifically, when relationships among actors are not surrounded

by mutual third parties, i.e., when they lack network closure (Coleman, 1988, 1990; Granovetter,

1985), then the willingness to cooperate may be diminished—and this may in turn hinder the

transfer and absorption of new knowledge (Burt, 2005). In a sense, the very network structure

most likely to provide access to novel knowledge can sometimes be ill-suited for the cooperation

needed to successfully transfer that knowledge.

Recent studies have examined several possible approaches to this “brokerage-closure di-

lemma” (see Burt, 2005; Reagans & McEvily, 2008 for recent reviews). All of these, however,

involve extensive adjustments at the network level—either to the overall network structure, i.e.,

the pattern of relationships among a set of actors, or to the network’s composition, i.e., the types

of actors in a network characterized in terms of their stable traits, features, or resource endow-

ments (Phelps, 2010; Wasserman & Faust, 1994). Such network-level adjustments are likely to

be complicated, time-consuming, and difficult, if not impossible, for individuals to achieve on

their own. The role that the characteristics of a given relationship can play in resolving this net-

work dilemma has largely been overlooked, however. As Moran (2005: 1132) observed, “Unfor-

tunately, dyad-specific qualities of social capital have been given much less empirical attention

[and] have not been empirically disentangled from social capital’s structural attributes.”

2

Page 4: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Addressing this gap in the literature, we argue that—although the surrounding network

structure can and does affect how people feel about each other (e.g., Paxton & Moody, 2003)—

not all significant features and benefits of a relationship (i.e., dyad) derive from the surrounding

network structure. At the dyad level, the strength of a relationship can provide similar benefits

and thus substitute for network-level closure, which tends to engender a sense of cooperation and

sharing (Coleman, 1988, 1990; Granovetter, 1985). Applying this insight to the brokerage-

closure dilemma, we propose that a network-bridging tie—i.e., a tie that links two parties who

share few or no third-party contacts in common—that is also a strong tie can provide both access

to novel knowledge (by virtue of its bridging qualities) and the willingness to cooperate (by vir-

tue of its strength). More specifically, given the theoretical (Krackhardt, 1992) and empirical

ambiguities (Marsden & Campbell, 1984) surrounding the concept of tie strength, we further

strive to disentangle and pinpoint the dyadic mechanism—i.e., interaction frequency, emotional

closeness, or relational trust—that is responsible for enhancing the knowledge value of network-

bridging ties.

By examining the interplay between network structure and the characteristics of a given

tie, we are able to provide a simple but novel, dyadic approach to the brokerage-closure di-

lemma. Moreover, dyadic characteristics like interaction frequency, emotional closeness, and

relational trust can be developed and cultivated independent of the network structure, e.g., when

two people spend time together, have things in common, act in a trustworthy manner, etc.

(Dwyer, 2000; Levin, 2008), thus making this dyadic approach particularly appealing to network

actors.

THEORY AND HYPOTHESES

Individuals benefit from being embedded in networks of relationships, which can provide

them with preferred access to knowledge flows (Burt, 1992; Granovetter, 1973). For our pur-

3

Page 5: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

poses, we define useful knowledge as information and/or advice that is actionable by recipients,

i.e., that can be used to improve their performance on some task or project (Cross, Borgatti, &

Parker, 2001; Cross & Sproull, 2004).

The early literature on social capital recognized two competing perspectives as to which

type of network structure is more beneficial to knowledge acquisition. The brokerage view em-

phasized the benefits of obtaining diverse, novel knowledge by brokering across a structural

hole, i.e., across otherwise disconnected clusters in a social network (Burt, 1992, 2001).1 The

closure view, in contrast, stressed the benefits of interacting with others in a dense network,

where close-knit connections are governed by social norms and reputation effects that encourage

support, cooperation, and the exchange of information (Coleman, 1988, 1990).

Although the premise that brokerage and closure are in opposition and create a trade-off

among their associated benefits was widely subscribed to in the early literature, recent research

suggests several possible resolutions for this dilemma (see Burt, 2005; Reagans & McEvily,

2008 for reviews). The first option is to take a contingency approach, choosing brokerage for

certain situations, closure for others. For example, Moran (2005) found that network closure en-

hanced managers’ performance at more routine, execution-oriented tasks but did not have a sig-

nificant effect on new, innovation-oriented tasks.

This first option faces the limitation, however, that oftentimes the benefits of both bro-

kerage and closure would be helpful. This leads to a second option: “hybrid” network positions

(Baum, van Liere, & Rowley, 2007), such as when a “group consists of people strongly con-

nected to one another, with extensive bridge relations beyond the group” (Burt, 2005: 165). Such

1 Brokerage can include both an individual network actor being a broker at the nodal level (e.g., Reagans & McE-vily, 2008) as well as bridging ties between disconnected others at the dyadic level (e.g., Baum, McEvily, & Row-ley, forthcoming).

4

Page 6: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

a group could reap the benefits of closure inside the group and the benefits of brokerage beyond

the group (Reagans & McEvily, 2008; Walter, Lechner, & Kellermanns, 2007). Alternatively,

actors may create hybrid network positions by emphasizing closure among their direct contacts

and substituting their partners’ bridging ties for their own (Baum et al., 2007).These hybrid op-

tions, however, focus exclusively on the network level of analysis, which makes unilateral ad-

justments complicated and time-consuming for an individual actor to make. For example, even if

a network actor could assemble a group of people from different social circles, organizations, or

backgrounds, they may not have the time or even want to forge a tight-knit group.

In this paper, we propose an alternative hybrid option. Specifically, we examine the net-

work and dyadic levels simultaneously and argue that elements associated with tie strength can

act as a substitute for the benefits of network closure. This is because some substantial variation

in these dyadic elements is determined by factors other than the surrounding network structure.

Strong Network-Bridging Ties

A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

defined by the network structure that surrounds the tie, and it can be either a strong or weak tie

(Burt, 2005) (see Fig. 1). While some prior studies have conceptualized “bridging” ties as ties

across a formal organizational boundary (e.g., Tortoriello & Krackhardt, 2010), across areas of

expertise (Phelps, 2010; Reagans & McEvily, 2003; Reagans & Zuckerman, 2001; Tiwana,

2008), or across demographic groups (Reagans & Zuckerman, 2001), we focus here on bridging

ties in the traditional sense, i.e., as links across the informal network structure to contacts that are

not connected to each other (Burt, 1992, 1997a).

[ Insert Figure 1 about here ]

At first glance, a strong bridge seems like an oxymoron. After all, building on balance-

theoretic arguments (Heider, 1958; Newcomb, 1961), early work on tie strength concluded that

5

Page 7: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

“except under unlikely conditions, no strong tie is a bridge.” (Granovetter, 1973: 1364, emphasis

in original). Only recently has the social network literature acknowledged the possibility of

strong network-bridging ties. Burt (2005: 24), for example, defines a bridging tie as “a (strong or

weak) relationship for which there is no effective indirect connection through third parties.”

Sometimes people whose relationship spans a structural hole simply “hit it off” by either finding

things in common other than mutual contacts, such as similar attitudes, values, working style,

outside interests, professional background, and so forth, or by recognizing that they are comple-

mentary to each other (Dwyer, 2000). These things can forge a strong bond even without the

benefit of knowing people in common, i.e., without network closure around the relationship.

Early research on tie strength has established that stronger ties in general lead to greater

knowledge exchange. In particular, tie strength increases people’s motivation to be more easily

available, treat each other well, and assist each other (Granovetter, 1982; Krackhardt, 1992). Ex-

tending those arguments, we propose that tie strength will be especially useful for receiving

knowledge from a network-bridging tie because tie strength can act as a substitute for the bene-

fits of network closure. Unlike weak network-bridging ties, which have only the potential to pro-

vide access to novel knowledge (Burt, 1992; Granovetter, 1973), strong network-bridging ties

make both sides less suspicious of each other and more willing and able to engage in a knowl-

edge transfer (Granovetter 1982, Krackhardt 1992), thereby allowing the knowledge seeker to

make fuller use of the opportunity provided by the network-bridging tie. Thus, we would expect

that tie strength will enhance the value of a tie that bridges a structural hole, “unlocking” that

tie’s potential by making deep, meaningful exchanges between the two actors more likely. Con-

versely, since network closure already encourages the kind of cooperation and sharing necessary

for knowledge exchanges to be useful (Coleman, 1988, 1990), we would expect tie strength to

have little or no effect on a tie that is densely embedded in the surrounding network structure.

6

Page 8: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

We believe, however, that this baseline expectation, i.e., that tie strength moderates the

value of structural holes, is insufficient without further examining the conceptual underpinnings

of what one means by the “strength” of a tie. In particular, the construct of tie strength is actually

in many ways an umbrella construct (Hirsch & Levin, 1999) in that it contains multiple elements

that may not always act in concert. Indeed, Granovetter (1973: 1361) originally defined tie

strength not as a unitary concept but rather as “a (probably linear) combination of the amount of

time, the emotional intensity, the intimacy (mutual confiding), and the reciprocal services which

characterize the tie.” Since then, subsequent scholars have tended to conceptualize it as either

emotional closeness (Moran, 2005; Seibert, Kraimer, & Liden, 2001; Yli-Renko, Autio, & Sapi-

enza, 2001), as interaction frequency (Aral & Van Alstyne, forthcoming; McFadyen & Cannella,

2004; McFadyen, Semadeni, & Cannella, 2009; Reagans & McEvily, 2008; Reagans & Zucker-

man, 2001; Tortoriello & Krackhardt, 2010), or as combinations of the two (Cross & Sproull,

2004; Hansen, Mors, & Lovas, 2005; Hansen, Podolny, & Pfeffer, 2001; Lechner, Frankenber-

ger, & Floyd, 2010; Levin & Cross, 2004; Molina-Morales & Martinez-Fernandez, 2009; Morri-

son, 2002; Perry-Smith, 2006; Reagans, 2005; Reagans & McEvily, 2003). Still other scholars

have conceptualized tie strength as a proxy for trust (Gulati, 1995) and argued that relational

trust is the actual dyadic mechanism responsible for these knowledge-transfer benefits (Levin &

Cross, 2004). In some instances, these various elements work in parallel—e.g., when two people

communicate frequently and feel a close connection (e.g., Reagans & McEvily, 2003)—but this

is not necessarily the case for a variety of relationships. In fact, several studies provide empirical

evidence suggesting that these different dyadic elements are not unitary (Aral & Van Alstyne,

forthcoming; Hansen et al., 2001; Lechner et al., 2010; Wegener, 1991) and may actually operate

independently (Ibarra, 1995; Marsden & Campbell, 1984).

Burt’s (1997b) study provides some conceptual insights into differences among these dy-

7

Page 9: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

adic elements. He found that managers sorted relationships on two dimensions of strength—

intimacy (especially close versus distant) versus activity (frequent versus rare contact), which led

him to conclude that “[m]anagers, like people in the general population, do not distinguish rela-

tions on a single dimension of strong versus weak. They distinguish on orthogonal dimensions of

intimacy and activity” (Burt, 1997b: 363). This finding also resonates with more recent work,

such as Levin et al. (2011), who found that “dormant” ties that had once been close but where

interaction frequency has shrunk to zero in recent years nevertheless retain the benefits of close

ties—thereby suggesting that closeness and interaction frequency are not only conceptually dis-

tinct but can diverge markedly. Similarly, Levin and Cross (2004) demonstrate that a combina-

tion of closeness and interaction frequency is conceptually and empirically distinct from rela-

tional trust, i.e., sometimes people trust that someone will care about them and look after their

interests even if they do not know the other person very well—or even at all (Meyerson, Weick,

& Kramer, 1996).

Moreover, the antecedents and consequences of each of these dyadic elements are differ-

ent. For example, interaction frequency is typically the result of working together, physical prox-

imity, and so forth (Newcomb, 1961). In contrast, relational trust can be the result of interactions,

too, but can also result from similar demographics (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2006) or su-

perordinate identity (Kane, 2010), an observed pattern of trustworthy behavior (Whitener, Brodt,

Korsgaard, & Werner, 1998), a corporate culture of cooperation (Abrams, Cross, Lesser, & Le-

vin, 2003), or any number of other factors unrelated to dyadic frequency or closeness.

Thus, to better understand the precise dyadic mechanism that might duplicate the benefits

of network closure and that could therefore enhance the knowledge value of a network-bridging

tie, it is helpful to examine which basic concept—i.e., frequency, closeness, and/or trust—is the

critical element. As Krackhardt (1992: 216) has argued, “[t]his is not simply a question for the

8

Page 10: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

methodologically curious. It is an important part of the theory itself”. Such a more fine-grained

approach therefore has the advantage that it allows us to tease apart what is the underlying dy-

adic mechanism that moderates the knowledge benefits of structural holes.

High-Frequency Network-Bridging Ties

Conceptually, there are two main reasons to expect frequent interactions to have a benefi-

cial effect on knowledge transfer. The first is a direct effect of higher interaction frequency, in

that it provides more opportunities for communication and more total information flow (Aral &

Van Alstyne, forthcoming; Granovetter, 1973). That is, the more conversations that take place

along the network-bridging tie, the more likely it might be that each party will share and learn

from each other. The second reason is an indirect effect, in that more frequent interactions

among individuals may lead to more effective communication through, for example, the devel-

opment of a common language and relationship-specific heuristics that facilitate cooperation,

joint problem solving (Uzzi, 1997), and innovations (Tortoriello & Krackhardt, 2010). These

heuristics are particularly important for assimilating idiosyncratic, context-specific, and complex

forms of knowledge, which often need multiple opportunities to assimilate (Hansen, 1999).

These benefits may be particularly critical for network-bridging ties, in that such ties often suffer

from a lack of shared understanding or framework and thus may require additional interactions

before they can become a source of useful knowledge. This reasoning suggests that frequent in-

teractions will allow knowledge seekers to unlock the value of the opportunity provided by the

network-bridging tie. In formal terms, we hypothesize a structural holes * interaction frequency

moderator effect:

Hypothesis 1 (H1): All else equal, interaction frequency will enhance the value of a tie

that bridges a structural hole, such that the more frequent the interactions for the net-

work-bridging tie, the more useful the knowledge transferred via that tie will be.

9

Page 11: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Conceptually, however, the beneficial effects of frequent interactions on knowledge

transfer across a bridging tie may be indirect, i.e., it may not be interaction frequency per se that

enhances the willingness of source and recipient to engage in a productive exchange, but through

another variable. Building on Homans’s (1950) insight that the more frequently people interact

with one another the stronger their sentiments of friendship for each other will be, several au-

thors have argued that this increased affection for one another should make contacts more willing

to treat one another well and do whatever it takes to make a knowledge exchange succeed (Cas-

ciaro & Lobo, 2005, 2008; Krackhardt, 1992; McFadyen & Cannella, 2004). In other words, in-

teraction frequency might sometimes enhance emotional closeness, which may in fact have a

more potent impact in enhancing the value of a knowledge exchange via a bridging tie. In light

of this possibility, we turn next to the role of emotional closeness.

Close Network-Bridging Ties

Emotional closeness measures the affect (Casciaro & Lobo, 2008; Reagans, 2005) or per-

sonal familiarity (Moran, 2005) in a relationship. Krackhardt (1992), in particular, has empha-

sized the importance of this element by arguing that Granovetter’s (1973) original theory on tie

strength draws on balance theory (Heider, 1958; Newcomb, 1961) and that the underlying ra-

tionale for balance is psychological. For this reason, “[w]ithout positive affect, there is less moti-

vation to maintain Heiderian balance, to share confidential information or refrain from malfea-

sance” (Krackhardt, 1992: 219). Several more recent studies have since concurred with this as-

sessment and have suggested emotional intensity (McEvily & Zaheer, 1999) or closeness (Lech-

ner et al., 2010) as the main mechanism behind the benefits of strong ties. In particular, the level

of emotional attachment or commitment to the relationship affects the motivation to provide as-

sistance or support (Granovetter, 1982; Reagans & McEvily, 2003), i.e., close contacts are gen-

erally more inclined to make the effort to carefully explain, detail, or listen to novel or complex

10

Page 12: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

ideas (Hansen, 1999; Moran, 2005).

As noted earlier, what brokerage often lacks is the willingness between knowledge source

and knowledge seeker to help each other. This willingness, however, is often what emotional

closeness contributes to a relationship; i.e., the motivation for knowledge sources to share their

insights, and for knowledge seekers to listen and assimilate the exchanged information. Thus,

while this feeling of dyadic closeness may be redundant in an already-dense network, it should

be particularly helpful in amplifying the degree of useful knowledge transfer across a network-

bridging tie. In formal terms:

Hypothesis 2 (H2): All else equal, emotional closeness will enhance the value of a tie that

bridges a structural hole, such that the closer the network-bridging tie, the more useful

the knowledge transferred via that tie will be.

Trusted Network-Bridging Ties

Lastly, and apart from the dyadic elements of frequency or closeness, it may actually be a

feeling of trust that encourages people to share and learn from each other across a network-

bridging tie. A trusted tie is a relationship where there is a willingness to be vulnerable (Mayer,

Davis, & Schoorman, 1995; Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998; Whitener et al., 1998),

and in the case of relational trust—which is our focus—this willingness is due to a belief that the

other party is benevolently concerned for one’s welfare (Mayer et al., 1995; Whitener et al.,

1998). Frequency and closeness can each create such trust (Levin & Cross, 2004; Tsai & Gho-

shal, 1998), but so can non-relationship factors like an observed pattern of trustworthy behavior

(Whitener et al., 1998), a third-party referral, a common background, similar demographics (Le-

vin et al., 2006), a superordinate identity (Kane, 2010), a corporate culture of cooperation (Ab-

rams et al., 2003), subliminal cues (Huang & Murnighan, 2010), or any number of other factors.

In fact, the trust literature has identified dozens of “trust builders” (for an overview, see Levin,

11

Page 13: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

2008), and trust has been shown to occur swiftly even among strangers in newly formed groups

(Meyerson et al., 1996). Thus, trusted ties can be high- or low-frequency ties and can be close or

distant (Levin & Cross, 2004).

Prior research has found that trust acts as a governance mechanism that facilitates knowl-

edge exchange by decreasing competitive and motivational barriers (Krackhardt, 1990, 1992;

McEvily, Perrone, & Zaheer, 2003), thereby enhancing the value of such exchanges (Levin &

Cross, 2004; Levin et al., 2011; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998), especially when these exchanges en-

compass high-quality information or tacit knowledge (Uzzi, 1996). For example, trust allows a

knowledge recipient to act upon and use new knowledge, rather than having to expend time and

effort verifying its validity (Currall & Judge, 1995; McEvily et al., 2003; Zaheer, McEvily, &

Perrone, 1998). Moreover, given the fact that trust is usually reciprocated (Ferrin, Bligh, & Koh-

les, 2008; Ferrin, Dirks, & Shah, 2006), a trusted tie can also help reduce a knowledge source’s

concerns about knowledge appropriation and misuse; thus, actors should be more willing to share

sensitive and proprietary details when trust is in place (McEvily et al., 2003; Nahapiet & Gho-

shal, 1998; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998). Trust may therefore make “the difference between a short

and possibly guarded hallway conversation about a new idea and active and open brainstorming

and tweaking of a new initiative” (Moran, 2005: 1136). Following this line of reasoning, we sug-

gest that, while trust may not provide as much additional benefit in the context of network clo-

sure, trust will disproportionately enhance the value of a network-bridging tie. Formally:

Hypothesis 3 (H3): All else equal, relational trust will enhance the value of a tie that

bridges a structural hole, such that the more trusted the network-bridging tie, the more

useful the knowledge transferred via that tie will be.

12

Page 14: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

METHODS

Sample and Procedures

To test our hypotheses, we approached a large U.S. engineering firm which had a reputa-

tion for world-class engineering capabilities and which was one of the top three global producers

in its primary product category. The firm’s senior engineering manager agreed to have a web

survey distributed to all 303 members of his staff who were believed to have worked on a par-

ticular project. This project required numerous areas of engineering expertise, ran for more than

five years, and spanned research and development, prototype design and testing, and manufactur-

ing. Individual participation in the survey was voluntary and strictly confidential (though not

anonymous). We pre-tested and revised our survey based on feedback from 15 engineers work-

ing at a firm providing engineering services on a contract basis to the target firm. Some of these

engineers were former employees of the target firm. We then emailed a password and link to the

web-based survey to the 303 people at the target firm, with non-respondents receiving at least

three email reminders over a two-month period. Of the 303, a total of 34 indicated they were in-

eligible to participate, e.g., they had not worked on the focal project.2 Of the remaining 269, we

received data from 62 (response rate = 23%). While this may seem low, it is actually fairly typi-

cal for social network-related surveys of this length (e.g., Seibert et al., 2001, reported a response

rate of 28%). More importantly, whereas response rates are of substantial concern for bounded

network studies, where the accuracy of network-level measures depends on having data from

most, if not all, network members, our focus is on respondents’ egocentric networks. These, by

definition, do not require information on the network structure beyond a respondent’s direct ties

2 We suspect that more employees than just these 34 who wrote back were ineligible to participate—a fact which would have increased our stated response rate (by lowering the denominator of eligible employees)—but we were unable to confirm this one way or the other with the company. It is doubtful that any ineligible employee would have completed the survey, given the survey instructions’ repeated emphasis on the focal project.

13

Page 15: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

(Burt, 1992; Wasserman & Faust, 1994), and therefore only need to be representative of—and

not cover in its entirety—the overall network structure.

We use hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) for our statistical analyses, allowing us to

tease apart the impact of respondents from the effects of their dyadic relationships. HLM auto-

matically deletes any observations with missing data for even a single survey item at the respon-

dent level, a restriction which further reduced the usable sample to a total of 408 dyadic knowl-

edge-seeking relationships, assessed by 41 engineers. Given our focus on dyadic ties as the unit

of analysis, however, we were less concerned about the number of respondents than the number

of ties. This is consistent with other social network studies that typically study a small number of

network actors in connection with knowledge transfer (e.g., Hansen, 1999; Tsai, 2002; Tsai &

Ghoshal, 1998).

To see if there was a systematic difference between respondents and non-respondents, we

compared their gender, which was the only demographic variable made available to us for both

groups, and found no significant difference (t = 0.17, p = .862). In addition, within the group of

62 initial respondents, we detected no significant differences between those who provided partial

versus full data, e.g., in terms of gender (t = 0.23, p = .820), how long they had been working on

the focal project (t = 0.71, p = .484), how much of their time the project took (t = 1.03, p = .310),

and how many people they had consulted (t = 0.19, p = .847).

Demographically, respondents were 87.8% male, an average of 44.8 years old, and had

an average tenure of 5.1 years in their current job and 17.2 years at the company. All respondents

had attended at least some college, and nearly two thirds (64%) had a graduate degree. On aver-

age, respondents had worked on 11.4 previous projects in the same technical area as their in-

volvement in the focal project, which consumed about half (52.5%) of their time. Average pro-

ject involvement was 29.7 months.

14

Page 16: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

At the start of the survey, we asked respondents, “Within the [focal project] effort, think

of a major task or project that holds significance for your career.” We then told respondents to

“answer the rest of the questions as they pertain specifically to your involvement with the se-

lected project or task, a part of the larger [focal project] effort.” By focusing on a specific task or

project, we aimed to ground the responses in a more concrete set of experiences (Levin & Cross,

2004; Levin et al., 2011), thereby reducing any recall or other biases (Marsden, 1990, 1993).

Using standard egocentric network survey techniques (Burt, 1992; Wasserman & Faust,

1994), we created respondents’ egocentric advice networks by asking them to list by name—or

by a more anonymous shorthand, such as initials—up to 20 people either inside or outside the

firm whom they had consulted as part of their work on their task or project; i.e., people “who

may have tried to provide: information, advice on a technical issue, or advice on an organiza-

tional issue like budgeting, deliverables, etc.” To reduce selection bias, respondents were asked

to include all sources they consulted, “whether or not they were useful in this particular in-

stance.” We then asked a series of questions about all of these knowledge sources, including how

well they knew one another. On average, our respondents consulted with 10.0 people, 85.3% of

whom were in the surveyed company and 74.4% of whom were working on the same project.

Measures

Outcome (dependent) variables. While there are a number of typologies of knowledge,

such as knowledge tacitness or complexity (Zander & Kogut, 1995), our focus here is on the use-

ful, substantive, micro-level content that gets exchanged between knowledge source and recipi-

ent in an organizational setting. To that end, we draw on the literature that examines the micro

foundations of organizational learning and knowledge transfer, which conceptualizes this phe-

nomenon as a process of solving problems, or more precisely, as the matching of problems with

solutions (Cyert & March, 1963; Greve, 2003). Taking this view, we suggest that there are at

15

Page 17: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

least two types of useful, substantive content that knowledge recipients seeking help on a prob-

lem can receive: either solutions to their problem or a new way of looking at their problem

(Cross & Sproull, 2004).

Thus, for solutions and problem reformulation, we obtained the survey instrument used

by Cross and Sproull (2004) and used nearly identical wording (see Appendix for all of the

items). One minor difference is that, whereas they asked about the impact on “completing” one’s

work, we felt that the usefulness of knowledge can extend beyond task completion, and so we

asked how positively or negatively each type of useful knowledge from each knowledge source

contributed to the respondent’s project performance. While other types of actionable knowledge,

such as referrals, can be useful as well (Cross & Sproull, 2004), these lack the substantive con-

tent provided by solutions and problem reformulation. To test our hypotheses, we undertook two

separate analyses to examine the role of network-bridging ties on the transfer of these two types

of useful knowledge, with each analysis designed to validate the other.

Predictor (independent) variables. Using Burt’s (1992) approach and language, we

measured closeness on a 3-point scale (0 = distant, 1 = in-between, 2 = especially close, reverse

coded as 2, 1, 0). To compute the degree to which a given tie spanned structural holes—i.e., it

was a network-bridging tie—we generated an adjacency matrix for each respondent’s egocentric

advice network, using the valued data on closeness. Following Burt (1992), we analyzed the

egocentric network data using UCINET 6.135 (Borgatti, Everett, & Freeman, 2002) to calculate

structural holes, defined as one minus the value of constraint (cij) posed by an individual alter j:

,,,2

jiqforpppcq

qjiqijij ≠⎟⎟⎠

⎞⎜⎜⎝

⎛+= ∑

where pij is the proportion of i’s relations invested in j, piq is the proportion of i’s relations in-

vested in q, and pqj is the proportion of q’s relations invested in j. One minus constraint (1 – cij) is

16

Page 18: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

thus the lack of constraint—or, the extent of structural holes—present around i’s tie to contact j.

For relational trust, also known as perceived benevolence (Mayer & Davis, 1999; Mayer

et al., 1995), we used a measure from Mayer and Davis (1999) of the respondent’s perception

that the knowledge source cared about the respondent’s welfare. We focused on this type of trust,

because it “is typically a function of a specific relationship” (Levin et al., 2006: 1164)—which is

our interest for H3—whereas other forms of trust, such as perceptions of an individual’s ability,

“are mainly characteristics of the individual” (Levin et al., 2006: 1164). For interaction fre-

quency, we used the same scale as Levin and Cross (2004), with a focus here on communication

since the start of the focal project. Before creating the interaction terms to test for the moderation

effects, we mean-centered all independent variables.

Control variables. To rule out alternative explanations, we controlled for a number of

factors that might confound our results. Because demographic similarity (i.e., homophily) is of-

ten correlated with perceptions of trustworthiness, helpfulness, and performance (Ferrin et al.,

2006; Ibarra, 1993; Shore, Cleveland, & Goldberg, 2003; Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989), we included

same age (+/- 5 years), same gender, and same race/ethnicity as control variables in our analysis.

To account for respondents selecting relationships based on happenstance or convenience, we

also controlled for physical proximity between the respondent and knowledge source, using an

item from Levin and Cross (2004), as well as for communication in person, i.e., as mostly face-

to-face versus mostly via other communication modes like phone or email. Additionally, we in-

cluded the perceived competence of the knowledge source (as perceived by the respondent) using

an item adapted from one of Butler’s (1991) competence measures. To control for the cognitive

dimension of the relationship, we used a measure—adapted from items used by Levin et al.

(2006) and Tsai and Ghoshal (1998)—for the extent to which respondents felt they had a shared

perspective with each source. Finally, we controlled for relationship length, as this may affect

17

Page 19: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

both the quality of the relationship and of the knowledge being transferred. We measured rela-

tionship length as the logarithm of the number of months since the respondent had first met each

knowledge source (Currall & Judge, 1995; Levin et al., 2006).

Reliability Issues

To reduce survey length and accommodate our respondents’ time constraints, we relied

on single-item measures for our variables. Although single-item measures are not ideal, they are

typical for network research (e.g., Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Seibert et al., 2001). A review by

Marsden (1990) suggests they are largely reliable when the appropriate procedures are followed

to help respondents accurately report on their network contacts. Accordingly, each of our items

was as specific as possible—including a number of examples—to enhance recall. We also asked

respondents to assess typical interactions, an approach which previous research has found to be

highly accurate (Freeman, Romney, & Freeman, 1987).

Concerning the accuracy of recall for respondents’ indirect ties, McEvily and Zaheer

(1999) found that, in their study, almost three-quarters of the time, a respondent’s assessment of

the tie between a pair of his or her contacts corresponded with both contacts’ report of the tie be-

tween themselves; and 86% of the time, a respondent’s report of the tie between a pair of con-

tacts was in agreement with at least one of those contacts.

A related concern is that all of our data come from the knowledge recipients; i.e., there is

a potential for common methods bias. In designing our study, we considered using other data

sources for measuring useful knowledge, such as project outcomes, supervisor ratings, or the

perceptions of knowledge sources, but we concluded that these data sources would be either too

far removed, uninformed, or biased to be of much use. We assumed that third parties, such as

supervisors, are rarely in a position to know the details of a knowledge transfer, let alone its use-

fulness. Thus, given our focus on knowledge recipients’ definitions of value, we followed previ-

18

Page 20: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

ous prescriptions “that—at the dyadic level of analysis—a knowledge seeker is the best, perhaps

the only, judge of the usefulness of knowledge received from a particular source” (Levin &

Cross, 2004: 1482, emphasis in original).

In testing for any “common methods variance” that might result from our approach, we

were reassured, first, by Harman’s one-factor test. Specifically, when we conducted a principal

components factor analysis of all the interval-scale variables used in each of our regression mod-

els, there were always two or three factors—never just one—with eigenvalues higher than 1.0.

Moreover, the largest eigenvalue never accounted for more than 33.8% of the total variance, well

below the 50% rule-of-thumb cut-off (Podsakoff & Organ, 1986). Second, the web-based nature

of our survey instrument made it more difficult for respondents to use previous answers to fill in

retrieval gaps (because each question had its own web page), a feature which helps reduce com-

mon methods bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). Third, our structural holes

measure is fairly concrete (and is computed in ways that would not be obvious to respondents)

and hence should be considerably less vulnerable to the possibility that respondents unthinkingly

rated each person as either always high or low across the board (Doty & Glick, 1998). Fourth,

moderator effects, as in our study, are less vulnerable to common methods bias, as noted by

Brockner, Siegel, Daly, Tyler, and Martin (1997) and others, “because it shows that respondents

did not unthinkingly rate all items as either high or low” (Levin & Cross, 2004: 1482). Indeed,

based on a series of Monte Carlo simulations, Evans (1985) concluded that the likelihood of ob-

taining significant moderator effects is reduced, not enhanced, to the extent that a method effect

is present. Thus, we conclude that any common methods variance resulting from our approach

appears to be minor and unlikely to affect our results.

Analysis Techniques

We used hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to test our hypotheses (Bryk & Rauden-

19

Page 21: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

bush, 1992). HLM is ideally suited for nested data—in our case, knowledge-seeking, dyadic rela-

tionships (“Level 1”) nested within respondents (“Level 2”) (Cross & Sproull, 2004; van Duijn,

van Busschbach, & Snijders, 1999)—as it does not rest on the assumption of independent obser-

vations, as traditional ordinary least squares (OLS) regression does (Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin,

2000). In contrast to OLS, HLM allows the researcher to tease apart the variance explained by

characteristics of the individual respondents from the variance explained by each dyadic tie by

formally representing each level of analysis with its own sub-model (cf., Hofmann, 1997).

RESULTS

Supporting our argument that interaction frequency, closeness, and relational trust are

conceptually distinct, these three dyadic elements were only moderately correlated (Table 1,

r’s = .29 to .37), suggesting that respondents were able to distinguish among these three ele-

ments. Interestingly, 46% of the ties in our sample were above average for structural holes and

also for interaction frequency, i.e., 46% were higher-frequency network-bridging ties; 42% were

more-trusted network-bridging ties; and 6% were “especially close” network-bridging ties and

another 57% “in-between” (in terms of closeness) network-bridging ties. This suggests that many

bridging ties are not necessarily very low in interaction frequency, relational trust, or closeness,

and thus the kinds of bridges that we are interested in investigating are not so rare after all.

[ Insert Tables 1-3 and Figures 2a and 2b about here ]

Not explicitly hypothesized, but in line with prior research that has found ambiguous re-

sults with respect to tie strength (see our literature review above), the main effects of interaction

frequency, emotional closeness, and relational trust are ambiguous across our two dependent va-

riables, with no consistently significant main effect for any of the three (Table 2, Model 1 and

Table 3, Model 6). Turning next to our hypotheses, when our outcome variable is the receipt of

useful knowledge in the form of solutions (Table 1), we find no support for H1: the structural

20

Page 22: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

holes * frequency interaction term is not statistically significant when it is the only moderator

effect included (Model 2, p = .520), and it is marginally significant but negative, i.e., the oppo-

site of what we hypothesized, when all three interaction terms are included (Model 5, p = .063).

As shown in Table 3, H1 is also not supported for the receipt of useful knowledge in the form of

problem reformulation: the relevant interaction term is not statistically significant when it is the

only moderator effect included (Model 7, p = .135), and it is statistically significant but negative

when all three interaction terms are included (Model 10, p = .011). Thus, all else equal, not only

is H1 not supported, but the opposite seems to be the case, i.e., the more frequent a tie’s commu-

nication is, the less impact structural holes have on the receipt of useful problem-reformulation

knowledge for that tie.

H2 is partially supported in that the structural holes * closeness interaction term in Ta-

ble 2 is statistically significant when it is the only moderator effect included for the outcome va-

riable of receipt of useful solutions (Model 3, p = .034). However, once we take into account the

other two moderator effects, the structural holes * closeness interaction term becomes non-

significant (Model 5, p = .635). Further, in Table 3, when the outcome variable is the receipt of

useful problem-reformulation knowledge, H2 is also not supported, i.e., the structural holes *

closeness interaction term is non-significant in both Model 8 (p = .269) and Model 10 (p = .917).

On balance, then, H2 receives at most only partial support.

H3 is fully supported. In Table 2 the structural holes * trust interaction term is statisti-

cally significant both by itself (Model 4, p = .004) and when the other moderator effects are in-

cluded (Model 5, p = .035). Similarly in Table 3, the relevant interaction term is statistically sig-

nificant both by itself (Model 9, p = .014) and when the other moderator effects are included

(Model 10, p = .011). In other words, the more trusted a tie is, the greater the impact of structural

holes on the receipt of useful knowledge, both for solutions and problem reformulation.

21

Page 23: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

To illustrate this moderator effect visually, we draw a figure of two simple slopes (see

Figs. 2a and 2b): the effect of structural holes on receipt of useful knowledge when relational

trust is a standard deviation above the mean and when it is a standard deviation below the mean.

As these figures suggest, when trust is high, structural holes lead to the receipt of more useful

knowledge, but when trust is low, we find the opposite effect; namely, it is network closure (i.e.,

fewer structural holes) that is more valuable when trust is low.

Robustness Checks

Recent literature on closure and brokerage has focused on ties that serve as a bridge

across formal organizational boundaries (Burt, 2005; Reagans & McEvily, 2003; Tortoriello &

Krackhardt, 2010). We therefore analyzed whether there were any differences in our sample be-

tween network-bridging ties to colleagues on the same work task/project and those to colleagues

outside the task/project. In particular, we wanted to know if the role of relational trust in enhanc-

ing the value of network brokerage applies to network-bridging ties whether the other person is

working on the same task/project or not. When we controlled for ties bridging project groups,

however, all results remained the same with the exception of structural holes * frequency, which

went from marginally negative (Model 5, p = .063) to non-significant (p = .151) for solutions.

Based on these results, we conclude that relational trust enhances the value of network-bridging

ties, regardless of whether these ties connect colleagues within or outside a work task/project.

Thus, it would appear that our findings—which combine dyadic and egocentric network struc-

ture—occur over and above the impact of more formal social structures.

We also checked for multicollinearity in all our models, to rule this out as an alternative

explanation. Although there is no direct diagnostic test for multicollinearity in HLM, we tested

for this potential problem using OLS regression in the SPSS statistical package. The result was

that all variance inflation factors (VIFs) were below 2.8, well below the standard cut-off of 10.

22

Page 24: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Finally, to assess the robustness of our findings further, we also tested eight additional

control variables at the respondent (i.e., “Level 2”) level of analysis: (1) the number of previous

projects in the same technical area that the respondent was involved in (a proxy for the respon-

dent’s level of experience); (2) percentage of a respondent’s work day spent on the task/project;

(3) education (with dummy variables corresponding to some college, bachelor’s, master’s, and

doctorate); (4) tenure at the company; (5) tenure in the current job; (6) gender; (7) age;

and (8) network density (i.e., the number of ties among the respondent’s contacts, divided by the

maximum possible number of such ties). The latter variable was included to take into account

any effects of information volume (i.e., the quantity of information available to the knowledge

seeker) in our results (Koka & Prescott, 2002). When we added these eight controls (not shown

due to space considerations), our hypothesized results remained the same with respect to both

directionality and significance, with the exception of the structural holes * closeness interaction,

which went from fully (in Model 3, p = .034) to marginally significant (p = .052), and the struc-

tural holes * frequency interaction, which increased in significance from p = .063 (Model 5) and

p = .011 (Model 10) to p = .028 and p = .004, respectively. Thus, our results were robust to sev-

eral alternative explanations, with statistically significant results over and above both dyadic-

and respondent-level controls.

DISCUSSION

Analyzing 408 advice-seeking relationships in a large U.S. engineering firm provides

strong empirical support for our argument that tie strength enhances the value of network-

bridging relationships. Our results further shed light on the mechanism behind the benefits of

strong network-bridging ties. That is, we found that it is relational trust, as predicted by H3, that

enhances the value of network-bridging ties for the receipt of useful knowledge, even controlling

for other possible dyadic mechanisms, such as interaction frequency (H1) or emotional closeness

23

Page 25: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

(H2), both of which did not consistently or significantly enhance the value of network-bridging

ties. Interestingly, for both dependent variables tested, we found that when a particular tie’s rela-

tional trust was low, then our respondents benefitted from network closure, i.e., from having

densely interconnected contacts. Our results thus provide support for our argument that relational

trust at the dyadic level and structural trust (i.e., closure) at the network level may act as substi-

tutes; both allow network actors to reap the benefits of being embedded in social relationships.

One implication of our results is an indication of when having a tie that spans a structural

hole is more versus less beneficial for obtaining useful knowledge. Specifically, such a network-

bridging tie is especially valuable when it is also a trusted tie. However, when there is little trust,

one appears to be better off having fewer structural holes around the tie, i.e., having more net-

work closure. A non-bridging tie might transfer knowledge even without high relational trust be-

tween the two parties, e.g., because of the feeling of obligation (or fear of negative conse-

quences) due to the surrounding mutual ties. A network-bridging tie, however, might do so only

when the two parties care about each other personally. Thus, another way of looking at our re-

sults is that relational trust has a bigger impact for network-bridging ties than for non-bridging

ties, because trust can be partially substituted by obligation (or fear) in the case of non-bridging

ties but there is no substitute for trust when it comes to network-bridging ties.

One potential drawback to using trust as a substitute for network closure is that closure—

unlike relational trust alone—is more likely to create cooperative norms that are enforced by

third parties (Granovetter, 1992). For example, if a potential knowledge source refuses to help a

knowledge seeker, news of this would likely spread within a tight-knit group and could lead to

the source’s ostracism, thereby limiting the source’s own ability to receive future support from

fellow group members (Reagans & McEvily, 2003). Such third-party monitoring and reputa-

tional effects might also reduce trust violations within the group, such as stealing someone else’s

24

Page 26: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

ideas. With a trusted network-bridging tie, however, knowledge seekers do not have such re-

course, since the two sides have few or no third parties in common. Nevertheless, even without

this network-based constraint, relational trust (i.e., a confidence that the other person cares about

you) is likely to allay most fears of the potential for uncooperative or untrustworthy behavior.

Other factors would likely help as well, such as appeals to a superordinate (e.g., companywide)

identity (Kane, 2010) or, if it exists, to a collaborative corporate culture (Abrams et al., 2003) or

someone with hierarchical or oversight authority. Thus, we would argue that relational trust at

the dyadic level as well as other organization-level factors make the threat of third-party inter-

ventions by members of a tight-knit group, which network closure can provide, less necessary.

Our findings for trusted network-bridging ties are, in some sense, an extension of earlier

work on the benefits of trusted weak ties (Levin & Cross, 2004), i.e., of knowledge-exchange

partners who do not know each other well but who nonetheless trust each other. Levin and Cross

(2004), however, do not account for network structure, relying solely on tie strength as a proxy

for structure. They also follow a somewhat different logic. Specifically, they suggest that tie

strength and trust can be seen independently, where high trust gives a relational benefit and weak

ties, a structural benefit, but they did not find a moderator effect between these two factors. In

our study, we do find a moderator effect such that high trust seems to enable actors to unleash

and reap the benefits of bridging a structural hole. Thus, our results address “the trust needed to

realize the value of bridging a structural hole” (Burt, 2005: 97).

More broadly, by examining dyadic ties, our study follows the calls of Rowley, Behrens,

and Krackhardt (2000), Moran (2005), and others for more attention in the social capital litera-

ture to the actual tie between two actors, i.e., to the “line” that connects two nodes in a social

network diagram. In contrast to prior work such as Moran’s (2005), however, we present empiri-

cal evidence not just for the independent effects of structural and relational social capital, but for

25

Page 27: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

how these two effects interrelate. In particular, our study extends recent studies of closure and

brokerage, which have focused on ties that serve as a bridge across formal organizational boun-

daries (Burt, 2005; Reagans & McEvily, 2003; Tortoriello & Krackhardt, 2010). A robustness

test described above showed that our findings—which combine dyadic and egocentric network

structure—occur over and above the impact of more formal social structures. Based on these re-

sults, we conclude that relational trust enhances the value of network-bridging ties, regardless of

whether these ties connect colleagues within or outside a work task/project.

Our study also extends recent work by McFadyen, Semadeni, and Cannella (2009), who

find that the average interaction frequency of an individual’s portfolio of ties can enhance the

value of a network with low density. Extrapolating from our findings at the dyadic level to an

individual’s portfolio of ties, frequent interaction, by definition, constitutes substantially more

maintenance than less-frequented ties (Granovetter, 1973; Hansen, 1999; McFadyen & Cannella,

2004). Actors with high-frequency ties are thus likely to be constrained in their ability to main-

tain a large number of strong network-bridging ties. This is consistent with our finding that in-

creased interaction frequency may actually diminish the value of a network-bridging tie. On the

other hand, we find that relational trust enhances the value of network bridges. Moreover, unlike

interaction frequency, many trust-building behaviors—such as being consistent, discreet, open,

fair, receptive, warm and engaging, and so forth (Abrams et al., 2003; Levin, 2008; Levin et al.,

2006; Whitener et al., 1998)—are not necessarily time-consuming, so building trust directly of-

fers another option for practitioners interested in enhancing the value of network brokerage.

Somewhat surprisingly, interaction frequency not only did not enhance, but in some cases

significantly diminished the value of network-bridging ties. One likely reason for this effect is

that frequent interactions can sometimes lead to increasingly redundant knowledge, i.e., to hear-

ing the same things over and over again. Thus, repeated interactions can lead to a congruence of

26

Page 28: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

knowledge stocks between source and recipient (Coleman, 1988), eventually diminishing the

marginal returns to any additional interaction. In line with this argument, McFadyen and Canella

(2004) found a curvilinear relationship between the number of interactions between scientists

and the amount of knowledge created, where too much interaction was counter-productive. This

point is echoed in Aral and van Alstyne (forthcoming), who found that information advantages to

brokerage depend on whether or not the knowledge stock of alters refreshes enough over time to

justify updating; otherwise, more frequent interaction simply leads to receiving the same infor-

mation again and again. Moreover, this negative effect of interaction frequency should be par-

ticularly salient for advice that is dependent on generating novel ideas and insight, such as prob-

lem reformulation, which, indeed, is reflected in our results (Model 10 versus Model 5).

Emotional closeness did not have much, if any, impact in enhancing the value of net-

work-bridging ties. While feeling close to someone may sometimes be helpful in unlocking a

network-bridging tie’s potential, it does not appear to be the critical element. Rather, there are

many different scenarios and other types of relationships besides closeness that can encourage a

willingness to engage in a productive knowledge exchange. We would argue that what these al-

ternative scenarios and relationships have in common is relational trust; and it is trust—even

without much emotional closeness—that can help ensure that a bridging tie provides value. Thus,

our results point to relational trust as the key dyadic mechanism for enhancing brokerage.

Compared to network-level approaches to the closure-brokerage tension offered by Burt

(2005) and others, our approach—i.e., trusted network-bridging ties—may be the easiest for ac-

tors to implement, as it does not require much investment in ongoing maintenance (e.g., as with

closeness and frequency) or any additional actions by third parties (e.g., it does not require clo-

sure to form around a bridging tie). From a strategic standpoint, we also suspect that trusted net-

work-bridging ties can be cultivated, just as trust in general can be cultivated (Abrams et al.,

27

Page 29: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

2003), making such ties less rare but just as valuable.

Our findings can also shed some light on ambiguous prior results for tie strength, e.g.,

some studies have reported an overall advantage for stronger relationships (Aral & Van Alstyne,

forthcoming; Ghoshal, Korine, & Szulanski, 1994; Hansen et al., 2001; Krackhardt, 1992; Moli-

na-Morales & Martinez-Fernandez, 2009; Reagans & McEvily, 2003, 2008; Seibert et al., 2001;

Tortoriello & Krackhardt, 2010); others, for weaker relationships (Constant, Sproull, & Kiesler,

1996; Granovetter, 1973, 1982; Perry-Smith, 2006; Yli-Renko et al., 2001); and still others, con-

tingent effects (Cross & Sproull, 2004; Hansen, 1999; Lechner et al., 2010; Levin & Cross, 2004;

McFadyen & Cannella, 2004; McFadyen et al., 2009; Moran, 2005; Morrison, 2002). Our dis-

tinctive results for interaction frequency, emotional closeness, and relational trust, however, sug-

gests that the plethora of conceptual and measurement approaches employed by these prior stud-

ies may have contributed to these ambiguous empirical results. Authors might therefore carefully

select a measurement approach that is congruent with the theoretical logic of their hypotheses, or

ideally include and compare several measurement approaches to disentangle the unique impact

of these dyadic characteristics on knowledge and performance outcomes.

Lastly, all studies have limitations, and ours is no exception. One limitation of our study

is that we measured structural holes using egocentric, not bounded, network data. As a result, it

is possible that what looks like a network-bridging tie in our sample is in fact linked indirectly to

the respondent’s other contacts via people not in the respondent’s egocentric network. Though

much of the prior research on structural holes relies on egocentric network data (e.g., Burt,

1992)—supported by evidence that brokerage benefits are concentrated mainly in the immediate

network around a person (Burt, 2007)—future researchers may want to examine the issue of

trusted network-bridging ties in a bounded network as well.

Our study also focused on bridging in a traditional, network structural sense (Burt, 1992),

28

Page 30: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

thereby neglecting bridges across other types of social domains. Future research could thus apply

our idea of trusted bridging ties to, for example, obtaining novel knowledge from different or-

ganization divisions, functions, hierarchical levels, or domains of expertise (Phelps, 2010; Rea-

gans & McEvily, 2003; Reagans & Zuckerman, 2001; Tiwana, 2008; Tortoriello & Krackhardt,

2010). Moreover, our results may apply to firms as well, since trust has been shown to operate

not just between individuals but also between organizations (Zaheer et al., 1998). For instance,

firms are more likely to cite the patent of a geographically or technologically distant firm, i.e.,

obtain useful knowledge from that firm, if they have an alliance with or have recently hired an

inventor from that other firm (Rosenkopf & Almeida, 2003). Though not measured directly, as in

our study, we suspect that relational trust may be one of the mechanisms underlying these useful

“bridges to distant contexts” (Rosenkopf & Almeida, 2003: 751). Future research may uncover

other examples of trusted bridging ties among firms as well.

CONCLUSION

In sum, our study provides empirical support for the value of strong network-bridging

ties: whereas a network-bridging tie provides access to novel and non-redundant knowledge, a

strong tie—or, more precisely, for our purposes, a tie with high levels of relational trust—

enables network actors to realize the potential of this knowledge. By focusing on the key social

capital dimensions of structural holes and tie strength—conceptualized at the intersection of the

network and dyadic levels of analysis—we not only suggest an additional approach to the net-

work brokerage-closure dilemma, but we also offer a systematic way to understand how and why

interpersonal relationships can enhance the transfer of useful knowledge.

29

Page 31: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

REFERENCES

Abrams, L. C., Cross, R., Lesser, E., & Levin, D. Z. 2003. Nurturing interpersonal trust in know-ledge-sharing networks. Academy of Management Executive, 17(4): 64-77.

Aral, S. & Van Alstyne, M. forthcoming. Networks, information and brokerage: The diversity-bandwidth tradeoff. American Journal of Sociology.

Argote, L. 1999. Organizational learning: Creating, retaining, and transferring knowledge. Bos-ton, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Baum, J. A. C., van Liere, D. W., & Rowley, T. J. 2007. Between closure and holes: Hybrid network positions and firm performance, Paper presented at the Academy of Manage-ment Annual Meeting. Philadelphia, PA.

Baum, J. A. C., McEvily, B., & Rowley, T. J. forthcoming. Better with age? The longevity and the performance implications of bridging and closure. Organization Science.

Borgatti, S. P., Everett, M. G., & Freeman, L. C. 2002. Ucinet for Windows: Software for social network analysis. Boston, MA: Analytic Technologies.

Borgatti, S. P. & Cross, R. 2003. A relational view on information seeking and learning in social networks. Management Science, 49(4): 432-445.

Brockner, J., Siegel, P. A., Daly, J. P., Tyler, T., & Martin, C. 1997. When trust matters: The moderating effect of outcome favorability. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42(3): 558-583.

Bryk, A. S. & Raudenbush, S. W. 1992. Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Burt, R. S. 1992. Structural holes: The social structure of competition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Burt, R. S. 1997a. The contingent value of social capital. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42(2): 339-364.

Burt, R. S. 1997b. A note on social capital and network content. Social Networks, 19: 355-373.

Burt, R. S. 2001. Structural holes versus network closure as social capital. In N. Lin & K. S. Cook & R. S. Burt (Eds.), Social capital: Theory and research: 31-56. New York: de Gruyter.

Burt, R. S. 2004. Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology: 349-399.

Burt, R. S. 2005. Brokerage and closure. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Burt, R. S. 2007. Secondhand brokerage: Evidence on the importance of local structure for man-agers, bankers, and analysts. Academy of Management Journal, 50(1): 119-148.

30

Page 32: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Butler, J. K. 1991. Toward understanding and measuring conditions of trust: Evolution of a con-ditions of trust inventory. Journal of Management, 17(3): 643-663.

Casciaro, T. & Lobo, M. S. 2005. Competent jerks, lovable fools and the formation of social networks. Harvard Business Review, June: 92-99.

Casciaro, T. & Lobo, M. S. 2008. When competence is irrelevant: The role of interpersonal af-fect in task-related ties. Administrative Science Quarterly, 53: 655-684.

Coleman, J. S. 1988. Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociol-ogy, 94(Supplement Issue): 95-120.

Coleman, J. S. 1990. Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

Constant, D., Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. 1996. The kindness of strangers: The usefulness of elec-tronic weak ties for technical advice. Organization Science, 7(2): 119-135.

Cross, R., Borgatti, S. P., & Parker, A. 2001. Beyond answers: Dimensions of the advice net-work. Social Networks, 23(3): 215-235.

Cross, R. & Sproull, L. 2004. More than an answer: Information relationships for actionable knowledge. Organization Science, 15(4): 446-462.

Currall, S. C. & Judge, T. 1995. Measuring trust between organizational boundary role persons. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 64(2): 151-170.

Cyert, R. M. & March, J. G. 1963. A behavioral theory of the firm. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-tice-Hall.

Doty, D. H. & Glick, W. H. 1998. Common methods bias: Does common methods variance real-ly bias results? Organizational Research Methods, 1(4): 374-406.

Dwyer, D. (Ed.). 2000. Interpersonal relationships: Routledge.

Evans, M. G. 1985. A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in moder-ated multiple regression analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Proc-esses, 36(3): 305-323.

Ferrin, D. L., Dirks, K. T., & Shah, P. P. 2006. Direct and indirect effects of third-party relation-ships on interpersonal trust. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(4): 870-883.

Ferrin, D. L., Bligh, M. C., & Kohles, J. C. 2008. It takes two to tango: An interdependence analysis of the spiraling of perceived trustworthiness and cooperation in interpersonal and intergroup relationships. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107(2): 161-178.

Freeman, L. C., Romney, A. K., & Freeman, S. C. 1987. Cognitive structure and informant accu-racy. American Anthropologist, 89(2): 310-325.

31

Page 33: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Ghoshal, S., Korine, H., & Szulanski, G. 1994. Interunit communication in multinational corpo-rations. Management Science, 40(1): 96-110.

Granovetter, M. 1973. The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology, 78(6): 1360-1380.

Granovetter, M. 1982. The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. In P. V. Marsden & N. Lin (Eds.), Social structure and network analysis: 105-129. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Granovetter, M. 1985. Economic action and social structure: The problem of embeddedness. American Journal of Sociology, 91(3): 481-510.

Granovetter, M. 1992. Problems of explanation in economic sociology. In N. Nohria & R. G. Ec-cles (Eds.), Networks and organizations: Structure, form, and action: 25-56. Boston: Har-vard Business School Press.

Grant, R. M. 1996. Toward a knowledge-based theory of the firm. Strategic Management Jour-nal, 17: 109-122.

Greve, H. R. 2003. Organizational learning from performance feedback: A behavioral perspec-tive on innovation and change. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gulati, R. 1995. Does familiarity breed trust? The implications of repeated ties for contractual choices in alliances. Academy of Management Journal, 38(1): 85-112.

Hansen, M. T. 1999. The search-transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing knowledge across organization subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(1): 82-111.

Hansen, M. T., Podolny, J. M., & Pfeffer, J. 2001. So many ties, so little time: A task contin-gency perspective on corporate social capital in organizations. In S. M. Gabbay & R. T. A. J. Leenders (Eds.), Social capital in organizations: 21-57. Oxford, UK: Jai Press.

Hansen, M. T., Mors, M. L., & Lovas, B. 2005. Knowledge sharing in organizations: Multiple networks, multiple phases. Academy of Management Journal, 48(5): 776-793.

Heider, F. 1958. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

Hirsch, P. M. & Levin, D. Z. 1999. Umbrella advocates versus validity police: A life-cycle mod-el. Organization Science, 10(2): 199-212.

Hofmann, D. A. 1997. An overview of the logic and rationale of hierarchical linear models. Journal of Management, 23(6): 723-744.

Hofmann, D. A., Griffin, M., & Gavin, M. B. 2000. The application of hierarchical linear model-ing to organizational research. In K. Klein & S. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, re-search, and methods in organizations: 467-511. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.

Homans, G. 1950. The human group. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

32

Page 34: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Huang, L. & Murnighan, J. K. 2010. What's in a name? Subliminally activating trusting behav-ior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 111(1): 62-70.

Ibarra, H. 1993. Personal networks of women and minorities in management: A conceptual framework. Academy of Management Review, 18(1): 56-87.

Ibarra, H. 1995. Race, opportunity, and diversity of social circles in managerial networks. A-cademy of Management Journal, 38(3): 673-703.

Kane, A. A. 2010. Unlocking knowledge transfer potential: Knowledge demonstrability and su-perordinate social identity. Organization Science, 21(3): 643-660.

Koka, B. R. & Prescott, J. E. 2002. Strategic alliances as social capital: A multidimensional view. Strategic Management Journal, 23(9): 795-816.

Krackhardt, D. 1990. Assessing the political landscape: Structure, cognition and power in or-ganizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35: 342-369.

Krackhardt, D. 1992. The strength of strong ties: The importance of Philos in organizations. In N. Nohria & R. G. Eccles (Eds.), Networks and organizations: Structure, form, and ac-tion: 216-239. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Lechner, C., Frankenberger, K., & Floyd, S. W. 2010. Task contingencies in the curvilinear rela-tionships between intergroup networks and initiative performance. Academy of Manage-ment Journal, 53(4): 865-889.

Levin, D. Z. & Cross, R. 2004. The strength of weak ties you can trust: The mediating role of trust in effective knowledge transfer. Management Science, 50(11): 1477-1490.

Levin, D. Z., Whitener, E. M., & Cross, R. 2006. Perceived trustworthiness of knowledge sources: The moderating impact of relationship length. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(5): 1163-1171.

Levin, D. Z. 2008. Trust. In S. R. Clegg & J. R. Bailey (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of Or-ganization Studies: 1573-1579. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Levin, D. Z., Walter, J., & Murnighan, J. K. 2011. Dormant ties: The value of reconnecting. Or-ganization Science, 22(4).

Marsden, P. V. & Campbell, K. E. 1984. Measuring tie strength. Social Forces, 63(2): 482-501.

Marsden, P. V. 1990. Network data and measurement. In W. R. Scott & J. Blake (Eds.), Annual Review of Sociology, Vol. 16: 435-463. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

Marsden, P. V. 1993. The reliability of network density and composition measures. Social Net-works, 15(4): 399-421.

33

Page 35: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. 1995. An integration model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20(3): 709-734.

Mayer, R. C. & Davis, J. H. 1999. The effect of the performance appraisal system on trust for management: A field quasi-experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(1): 123-136.

McEvily, B. & Zaheer, A. 1999. Bridging ties: A source of firm heterogeneity in competitive ca-pabilities. Strategic Management Journal, 20(12): 1133-1156.

McEvily, B., Perrone, V., & Zaheer, A. 2003. Trust as an organizing principle. Organization Sci-ence, 14(1): 91-103.

McFadyen, M. A. & Cannella, A. A., Jr. 2004. Social capital and knowledge creation: Diminish-ing returns of the number and strength of exchange relationships. Academy of Manage-ment Journal, 47(5): 735-746.

McFadyen, M. A., Semadeni, M., & Cannella, A. A., Jr. 2009. Value of strong ties to discon-nected others: Examining knowledge creation in biomedicine. Organization Science, 20(3): 552-564.

Meyerson, D., Weick, K. E., & Kramer, R. M. 1996. Swift trust and temporary groups. In R. M. Kramer & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research: 166-195. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Molina-Morales, F. X. & Martinez-Fernandez, M. T. 2009. Too much love in the neighborhood can hurt: How an excess of intensity and trust in relationships may produce negative ef-fects on firms. Strategic Management Journal, 30: 1013-1023.

Moran, P. 2005. Structural vs. relational embeddedness: Social capital and managerial perform-ance. Strategic Management Journal, 26(12): 1129-1151.

Morrison, E. W. 2002. Newcomers' relationships: The role of social network ties during sociali-zation. Academy of Management Journal, 45(6): 1149-1160.

Nahapiet, J. & Ghoshal, S. 1998. Social capital, intellectual capital, and the organizational ad-vantage. Academy of Management Review, 23(2): 242-266.

Newcomb, T. M. 1961. The acquaintance process. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Paxton, P. & Moody, J. 2003. Structure and sentiment: Explaining emotional attachment to group. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66(1): 34-47.

Perry-Smith, J. E. 2006. Social yet creative: The role of social relationships in facilitating indi-vidual creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 49(1): 85-101.

Phelps, C. C. 2010. A longitudinal study of the influence of alliance network structure and com-position on firm exploratory innovation. Academy of Management Journal, 53(4): 890-913.

34

Page 36: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Podsakoff, P. M. & Organ, D. W. 1986. Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12(4): 531-544.

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. 2003. Common method bi-ases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended reme-dies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5): 879-903.

Reagans, R. & Zuckerman, E. W. 2001. Networks, diversity, and productivity: The social capital of corporate R&D teams. Organization Science, 12(4): 502-517.

Reagans, R. & McEvily, B. 2003. Network structure and knowledge transfer: The effects of co-hesion and range. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48(2): 240-267.

Reagans, R. 2005. Preferences, identity, and competition: Predicting tie strength from demo-graphic data. Management Science, 51(9): 1374-1383.

Reagans, R. & McEvily, B. 2008. Contradictory or compatible? Reconsidering the "trade-off" between brokerage and closure on knowledge sharing. In J. A. C. Baum & T. J. Rowley (Eds.), Network strategy: Advance in Strategic Management, Vol. 25: 275-313. Bingley, UK: JAI Press.

Rosenkopf, L. & Almeida, P. 2003. Overcoming local search through alliances and mobility. Management Science, 49(6): 751-766.

Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. 1998. Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3): 393-404.

Rowley, T. J., Behrens, D., & Krackhardt, D. 2000. Redundant governance structures: An analy-sis of structural and relational embeddedness in the steel and semiconductor industries. Strategic Management Journal, 21(3): 369-386.

Seibert, S. E., Kraimer, M. L., & Liden, R. C. 2001. A social capital theory of career success. Academy of Management Journal, 44(2): 219-237.

Shore, L. M., Cleveland, J. N., & Goldberg, C. B. 2003. Work attitudes and decisions as a func-tion of manager age and employee age. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3): 529-537.

Tiwana, A. 2008. Do bridging ties complement strong ties? An empirical examination of alliance ambidexterity. Strategic Management Journal, 29(3): 251-272.

Tortoriello, M. & Krackhardt, D. 2010. Activating cross-boundary knowledge: The role of Simmelian ties in the generations of innovations. Academy of Management Journal, 53(1): 167-181.

Tsai, W. & Ghoshal, S. 1998. Social capital and value creation: The role of intrafirm networks. Academy of Management Journal, 41(4): 464-476.

35

Page 37: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Tsai, W. 2002. Social structure of "coopetition" within a multiunit organization: Coordination, competition, and intraorganizational knowledge sharing. Organization Science, 13(2): 179-190.

Tsui, A. S. & O’Reilly, C. A. 1989. Beyond simple demographic effects: The importance of rela-tional demography in superior-subordinate dyads. Academy of Management Journal, 32(2): 402-423.

Uzzi, B. 1996. The sources and consequences of embeddedness for the economic performance of organizations: The network effect. American Sociological Review, 61: 674-698.

Uzzi, B. 1997. Social structure and competition in interfirm networks: The paradox of em-beddedness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42: 35-67.

van Duijn, M. A. J., van Busschbach, J. T., & Snijders, T. A. B. 1999. Multilevel analysis of per-sonal networks as dependent variables. Social Networks, 21(2): 187-210.

Walter, J., Lechner, C., & Kellermanns, F. W. 2007. Knowledge transfer between and within al-liance partners: Private versus collective benefits of social capital. Journal of Business Research, 60(7): 698-710.

Wasserman, S. & Faust, K. 1994. Social network analysis: Methods and applications. Cam-bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Wegener, B. 1991. Job mobility and social ties: Social resources, prior job, and status attainment. American Sociological Review, 56(1): 60-71.

Whitener, E. M., Brodt, S. E., Korsgaard, M. A., & Werner, J. M. 1998. Managers as initiators of trust: An exchange relationship framework for understanding managerial trustworthy be-havior. Academy of Management Review, 23: 513-530.

Yli-Renko, H., Autio, E., & Sapienza, H. J. 2001. Social capital, knowledge acquisition, and knowledge exploitation in young technology-based firms. Strategic Management Journal, 22(6/7): 587-613.

Zaheer, A., McEvily, B., & Perrone, V. 1998. Does trust matter? Exploring the effects of interor-ganizational and interpersonal trust on performance. Organization Science, 9(2): 141-159.

Zander, U. & Kogut, B. 1995. Knowledge and the speed of the transfer and imitation of organ-izational capabilities: An empirical test. Organization Science, 6(1): 76-92.

36

Page 38: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

FIGURE 1

Hypothetical Egocentric Networks

Figure 1a

Figure 1b

37

Page 39: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

FIGURE 2a Moderator Effect for Receipt of Useful Knowledge: Solutions

Use

ful S

olut

ions

Less MoreStructural Holes

High Trust

Low TrustU

sefu

l Sol

utio

ns

Less MoreStructural Holes

High Trust

Low Trust

FIGURE 2b

Moderator Effect for Receipt of Useful Knowledge: Problem Reformulation

Less More

Use

ful P

robl

em R

efor

mul

atio

n

Structural Holes

High Trust

Low Trust

Less More

Use

ful P

robl

em R

efor

mul

atio

n

Structural Holes

High Trust

Low Trust

Note: Based on output from Table 2’s Model 4 (for Solutions) and Table 3’s Model 9 (for Prob-lem Reformulation).

38

Page 40: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

39

TABLE 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations

Variable Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 1. Solutions 5.82 1.25 2. Problem reformulation 5.37 1.29 .67** 3. Same age 0.39 0.49 .10 .08 4. Same gender 0.85 0.36 -.07 -.04 .01 5. Same race/ethnicity 0.86 0.35 .13* .11* -.01 -.02 6. Physical proximity 4.15 1.90 .25** .25** .10 .01 -.13*

7. Perceived competence 6.10 1.37 .53** .46** .07 .05 .12*

8. Communication in person 0.72 0.45 .30** .23** .10 .01 .02 9. Relationship length 1.61 0.47 -.06 .08 .00 -.03 .01 10. Shared perspective 5.23 1.27 .60** .58** .08 -.09 .27**

11. Structural holes 0.95 0.06 -.12* -.12* .03 .24** .03 12. Interaction frequency 4.80 1.87 .29** .24** .03 .05 .01 13. Closeness 1.98 0.49 .34** .29** .12* -.12* -.02 14. Relational trust 4.71 1.29 .42** .48** -.01 .01 .05 15. Structural holes * frequency -0.01 0.09 .02 -.08 -.01 -.07 -.05 16. Structural holes * closeness -0.01 0.04 .01 -.01 -.02 .12* .07 17. Structural holes * trust -0.01 0.08 .10 .06 .01 .01 .08

Variable 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 6. Physical proximity 7. Perceived competence .19** 8. Communication in person .59** .20** 9. Relationship length .10 .02 .00 10. Shared perspective .11* .33** .17** .06 11. Structural holes -.14** .05 -.01 -.11* -.22** 12. Interaction frequency .26** .14** .26** .01 .28** -.07 13. Closeness .22** .20** .24** .25** .40** -.25** .29**

14. Relational trust .21** .30** .25** .04 .46** -.11* .30**

15. Structural holes * frequency .06 .01 -.02 .00 .08 .03 .16**

16. Structural holes * closeness -.04 .05 -.03 -.01 .02 .59** .02 17. Structural holes * trust -.09 -.14** -.12* -.05 .06 .26** .00

Variable 13 14 15 16 13. Closeness 14. Relational trust .37** 15. Structural holes * frequency .04 .00 16. Structural holes * closeness -.15** -.07 .27** 17. Structural holes * trust -.10* -.06 .29** .60**

Note: For variable 1, N = 359; variable 2, N = 347; variables 3-17, N = 376. Two-tailed tests. * p < .05; ** p < .01

Page 41: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

TABLE 2 HLM Regression Results for Receipt of Useful Knowledge: Solutions

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5

Same age 0.06 (0.08) 0.06 (0.08) 0.06 (0.08) 0.06 (0.08) 0.07 (0.08)

Same gender 0.20 (0.18) 0.20 (0.18) 0.21 (0.18) 0.20 (0.19) 0.19 (0.19)

Same race/ethnicity -0.07 (0.19) -0.07 (0.19) -0.08 (0.19) -0.07 (0.18) -0.09 (0.17)

Physical proximity 0.02 (0.04) 0.03 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) 0.03 (0.04)

Perceived competence 0.43***(0.11) 0.43***(0.11) 0.43***(0.11) 0.43***(0.11) 0.43***(0.11)

Communication in person 0.23† (0.14) 0.23 (0.14) 0.24† (0.14) 0.25† (0.14) 0.24† (0.13)

Relationship length -0.28* (0.14) -0.28* (0.13) -0.30* (0.14) -0.30* (0.14) -0.32* (0.14)

Shared perspective 0.30** (0.09) 0.31***(0.09) 0.29** (0.09) 0.30***(0.09) 0.30***(0.09)

Structural holes -0.08 (1.16) 0.00 (1.13) -1.91 (1.57) -1.18 (1.19) -1.70 (1.54)

Interaction frequency 0.06† (0.04) 0.07† (0.03) 0.07† (0.04) 0.06† (0.04) 0.07* (0.03)

Closeness 0.37* (0.16) 0.37* (0.16) 0.33* (0.15) 0.38* (0.16) 0.36* (0.14)

Relational trust 0.06 (0.07) 0.06 (0.07) 0.07 (0.07) 0.04 (0.07) 0.04 (0.06)

H1: Structural holes * frequency -0.33 (0.51) -0.98† (0.52)

H2: Structural holes * closeness 2.42* (1.14) 0.85 (1.80)

H3: Structural holes * trust 1.66** (0.56) 1.89* (0.90)

R 2 = .540 .539 .542 .546 .547

Note: Unstandardized coefficients shown, with robust standard errors in parentheses, based on a random coefficient regression model using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM). N = 346 observations. All variables grand-mean centered. † p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

40

Page 42: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

41

TABLE 3 HLM Regression Results for Receipt of Useful Knowledge: Problem Reformulation

Model 6 Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10

Same age 0.01 (0.07) 0.02 (0.07) 0.01 (0.07) 0.01 (0.07) 0.02 (0.07)

Same gender 0.27** (0.10) 0.26* (0.11) 0.28** (0.11) 0.27* (0.11) 0.26* (0.12)

Same race/ethnicity 0.22 (0.24) 0.21 (0.23) 0.22 (0.24) 0.22 (0.24) 0.20 (0.22)

Physical proximity 0.03 (0.03) 0.03 (0.03) 0.03 (0.02) 0.03 (0.03) 0.04 (0.02)

Perceived competence 0.27***(0.06) 0.27***(0.06) 0.27***(0.06) 0.27***(0.06) 0.26***(0.06)

Communication in person 0.23† (0.12) 0.22† (0.12) 0.24* (0.12) 0.25* (0.12) 0.24* (0.12)

Relationship length 0.09 (0.12) 0.08 (0.11) 0.08 (0.12) 0.07 (0.12) 0.04 (0.12)

Shared perspective 0.35***(0.07) 0.35***(0.07) 0.34***(0.07) 0.35***(0.07) 0.36***(0.07)

Structural holes -0.05 (2.28) 0.13 (2.17) -1.35 (2.17) -1.22 (2.35) -1.15 (1.92)

Interaction frequency 0.02 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) 0.02 (0.04) 0.01 (0.04) 0.03 (0.03)

Closeness -0.25 (0.20) -0.25 (0.20) -0.27 (0.19) -0.25 (0.20) -0.25 (0.19)

Relational trust 0.36***(0.09) 0.36***(0.09) 0.36***(0.09) 0.33***(0.09) 0.33***(0.08)

H1: Structural holes * frequency -0.77 (0.51) -1.45* (0.56)

H2: Structural holes * closeness 1.75 (1.58) -0.26 (2.45)

H3: Structural holes * trust 1.78* (0.72) 2.56** (0.95)

R 2 = .465 .465 .467 .475 .480

Note: Unstandardized coefficients shown, with robust standard errors in parentheses, based on a random coefficient regression model using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM). N = 334 observations. All variables grand-mean centered. † p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Page 43: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

APPENDIX: Survey Items

Outcome Variables

For both types of useful knowledge, instructions said: “Please respond for each person using a scale from a Very Negative to a Very Positive contribution, with NA meaning you did not re-ceive anything like this from this person on this task/project.”

Solutions. Sometimes when we consult with people, we benefit from their ability to provide spe-cific answers to our question or solutions to our problems. To what extent did this person’s s-pecific answers or input contribute to your performance on [name of task/project]? (1 = very negative, 2 = negative, 3 = somewhat negative, 4 = neither positive/negative, 5 = somewhat positive, 6 = positive, 7 = very positive, NA [recoded as missing value])

Problem reformulation. Sometimes when we consult with people, we benefit from their helping us think through a problem (even when they may not have specific information that solves our original problem). These interactions may help us consider important dimensions of a problem and/or anticipate issues likely to appear in the future. To what extent did this kind of problem-solving assistance from this person contribute to your performance on [name of task/project]? (1 = very negative, 2 = negative, 3 = somewhat negative, 4 = neither positive/negative, 5 = somewhat positive, 6 = positive, 7 = very positive, NA [recoded as missing value])

Predictor Variables

Same age. Relative to yours, what is this person’s age? (1 = younger than me by more than 5 years; 2 = my age plus or minus 5 years; 3 = older than me by more than 5 years; 4 = don’t know) [recoded as 1 = same age +/- 5 years; 0 = different age].

Same gender. What is this person’s gender? (1 = male; 2 = female) [recoded as 1 = same gender as respondent; 0 = different gender].

Same race/ethnicity. Do you consider this person to be the same race/ethnicity as you? (1 = yes; 2 = no) [recoded as 0 = no].

Physical proximity. Please indicate each person’s physical proximity to you during your work on [name of task/project]. (1 = worked immediately next to me, 2 = same floor and same hallway, 3 = same floor but different hallway, 4 = different floor, 5 = different building, 6 = different city, 7 = different country) [item reverse-coded].

Perceived competence. This person is very capable at the work he or she performs. (1 = strongly disagree; 2 = disagree; 3 = somewhat disagree; 4 = neutral; 5 = somewhat agree; 6 = agree; 7 = strongly agree).

Communication in person. During [name of task/project], what is the main way you have com-municated with this person? (1 = in person; 2 = on-line (such as via the Internet or an intranet); 3 = by telephone; 4 = other) [recoded as 1 = in person; 0 = all others].

Relationship length. How long have you known each person? [logarithm of the number of months].

42

Page 44: Trusted Network-Bridging Ties - Rutgers University · Strong Network-Bridging Ties A network-bridging tie is a relationship that spans a structural hole in a network, i.e., it is

Shared perspective. This person and I share the same perspective, in that we think in a similar way, have similar goals and objectives, and understand each other’s language/jargon when we communicate. (1 = strongly disagree; 2 = disagree; 3 = somewhat disagree; 4 = neutral; 5 = somewhat agree; 6 = agree; 7 = strongly agree).

Structural holes. For the calculation of structural holes around each tie, we asked respondents about the closeness of their direct ties (see below for closeness) and indirect ties: “Please con-tinue to use this scale to rate how well this person knows the others. 1 = Especially close—in the sense that they are one of each other’s closest personal contacts; 2 = Relationships that are somewhere between “especially close” and “distant.”; 3 = Distant—in the sense that they are to-tal strangers or do not enjoy one another’s company.” [reverse-coded scale as 2, 1, 0]

Interaction frequency. How often have you communicated with each person since starting on [name of task/project]? (1 = daily; 2 = twice a week; 3 = once a week; 4 = twice a month; 5 = once a month; 6 = once every 2nd month; 7 = once every 3 months or less) [item reverse-coded].

Closeness. This next section deals with the relationship between you and the sources of informa-tion you listed in the first part of the questionnaire. It will also ask you to consider their relation-ships with each other. To start with you, how well do you know each of your sources? Please rate each relationship on the following scale. 1 = Especially close—in the sense that this person and I are one of each other’s closest personal contacts; 2 = Relationships that are somewhere between “especially close” and “distant.”; 3 = Distant—in the sense that this person and I are total strang-ers or do not enjoy each other’s company. [reverse-coded scale as 2, 1, 0]

Relational trust. This person is very concerned about my welfare. (1 = strongly disagree; 2 = disagree; 3 = somewhat disagree; 4 = neutral; 5 = somewhat agree; 6 = agree; 7 = strongly agree).

43