105
The Pennsylvania State University The Graduate School College of Health and Human Development UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS ACROSS EARLY CHILDHOOD: UNITING NEUROBIOLOGICAL, PARENTING, AND DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVES A Dissertation in Human Development and Family Studies by Christine K. Fortunato © 2011 Christine K. Fortunato Submitted in Partial Fulfillment for the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2011

UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    7

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

The Pennsylvania State University

The Graduate School

College of Health and Human Development

UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS ACROSS EARLY CHILDHOOD:

UNITING NEUROBIOLOGICAL, PARENTING, AND DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVES

A Dissertation in

Human Development and Family Studies

by

Christine K. Fortunato

© 2011 Christine K. Fortunato

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

for the Requirements

for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

December 2011

Page 2: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

ii

The dissertation of Christine K. Fortunato was reviewed and approved* by the following: Lisa M. Gatzke-Kopp Professor of Human Development & Family Studies Dissertation Co-Advisor Co-Chair of Committee Douglas A. Granger Professor of Biobehavioral Health Dissertation Co-Advisor Co-Chair of Committee Mark T. Greenberg Professor of Human Development & Family Studies Nilam Ram Professor of Human Development & Family Studies Karen Bierman Distinguished Professor of Psychology Douglas Coatsworth Professor-In-Charge of Graduate Studies in Human Development & Family Studies *Signatures are on file in the Graduate School.

Page 3: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

iii

ABSTRACT

Heterogeneity in externalizing behaviors such as conduct problems impedes the elucidation of

underlying developmental mechanisms. Recent studies suggest that individuals exhibiting conduct

problems may show entirely discrepant patterns of biological vulnerabilities depending on whether they

have comorbid internalizing problems or not. However, no research to date has examined this from a

developmental perspective. The current study examines theoretically derived hypotheses regarding the

developmental risk pathways that differentiate children with conduct problems who do and do not

manifest comorbid anxiety. Biological vulnerability was examined with respect to blunted or

exacerbated stress responsivity. Salivary cortisol was collected in response to a stressful task at 7, 15,

and 24 months of age. Additionally, home visitors reported children’s behaviors at each time point, and

parenting styles were observed in a structured interaction with their child.

Results showed that children with conduct problems only at 5 years of ages were characterized

by the exacerbated stress responsivity at 7 months of age followed by blunted stress responsivity at 24

months old. In contrast, children with comorbid internalizing problems were typified by the

interrelationship of heightened stress responsivity and high behavioral approach tendencies at 15

months of age. While both children with and without comorbid anxiety displayed exacerbated stress

responsivity with greater exposure to intrusive parenting, the developmental time points that this

biological sensitivity to context became apparent was different.

The present study provides a better understand of the developmental mechanisms in that

undergird distinct externalizing problems phenotypes with and without comorbid internalizing problems

in early childhood. Overall, these findings suggest that it may be more efficacious for intervention

programs to target biological, behavioral, and parenting domains of risk at key development time points

to prevent the later development of distinct conduct problem phenotypes.

Page 4: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................................... v

LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................................... vi

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................................ vii

Chapter 1 Introduction............................................................................................................ 1

Chapter 2 Methods ................................................................................................................. 23

Chapter 3 Results .................................................................................................................... 35

Chapter 4 Discussion ............................................................................................................... 62

REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................. 78

Page 5: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

v

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1: Changes in Children’s Irritability across Early Childhood within each Conduct Problem Subgroups…………………………………………………………………………………………………………..............................42

Figure 2: Changes in Children’s Exposure to Intrusive Parenting Across Early Childhood within each

Conduct Problem Subgroups………………………………………………………………………....................................42 Figure 3: Interrelationships between HPA Activity and Intrusive Parenting at 7 Months of Age Predicting

CP Subgroup Membership………………………………………………………………………........................................46 Figure 4: Interrelationships between Children’s HPA Activity and Approach Tendencies at 15 Months of

Age Predicting COMORBID Subgroup Membership……………………......................................................53 Figure 5: Interrelationships between Children’s HPA Activity and Intrusive Parenting at 15 Months of Age

Predicting COMORBID Subgroup Membership…………………….............................................................53 Figure 6: Interrelationships between Approach Tendencies and Sensitive Parenting at 15 Months of Age

Predicting CP Subgroup Membership…………......................................................................................54 Figure 7: Interrelationships among Children’s HPA Activity, Approach Tendencies, and Sensitive

Parenting at 24 Months of Age Predicting CP Subgroup Membership…………………………………………...60 Figure 8: Interrelationships among Children’s HPA Activity, Irritability, and Intrusive Parenting at 24

Months of Age Predicting COMORBID Subgroup Membership………………………………………….............60

Page 6: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

vi

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1: Percentages and Means (Standard Deviations) of Demographic Variables for Conduct Problem Subgroups.………………………………..............................................................................26

Table 2: Means (SD) Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles across Early Childhood by Conduct Problems Subgroups…………................38

Table 3: Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 7 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.…………..................................................................................................................44

Table 4: Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 7 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups………………..…………………………………………….45

Table 5: Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 15 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.…………………………….…...........................................................................................48

Table 6: Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 15 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups…………………………………………………………….50

Table 7: Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 24 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….....56

Table 8: Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 24 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.……………………………………………………………57

Table 9: Summary of Risk Factors across Early Childhood that Characterize the Development of Conduct Problem Subgroups at 5 Years of Age.…………………………………................................61

Page 7: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

vii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Many people were an essential part of the successful completion of my dissertation, and I will

never be able to truly thank them for all they have done for me. These individuals have walked with me

through my graduate career, supporting and inspiring me every step of the way.

Lisa Gatzke-Kopp – I want to thank you for being a supportive and inspiring advisor and mentor.

Your intellectual and professional guidance had meant the world to me. I aspire to reach emulate your

commitment to research and generosity to your students in my career. I waited three years to work with

someone like you, and it was well worth the wait! (Remember, it is not DopaNICE, it is DopaMEAN.)

Doug Granger – I am grateful for your mentorship and support over the past 5 years. You have

not only providing me with the necessary skills to be a good researcher, but you have also taught me the

importance of work-life balance. Most importantly, you modeled the significance of being excited about

your research. (I hope to always shout the word “Eureka” loud enough so that you can still hear it!)

Mark Greenberg – Like most students who have collaborated with you, I am in awe of your

intellect, professionalism, and the great work you have accomplished in the prevention research. I want

to thank you for allowing me to work on research project that integrate neurobiological risk factors in

developmental and prevention research before a clear pathway existed in HDFS.

Nilam Ram and Karen Bierman – Thank you for providing me with excellent input on my

dissertation. The different perspectives and expertise has greatly added enhanced my work. I am certain

that this guidance has greatly improved my chances for the successful publication of these findings.

Labmates (Dave, Cindy, Heather, and Liza) – I love my lab family! The insight all of you gave me

on my research findings, your stellar editing skills, and your ability to make me laugh helped with the

successful completion of my dissertation and my overall mental well-being. (P.S. Cindy – You are one of

the best editors a girl could have! THANKS!!!)

Page 8: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

viii

Michael Coccia – Thank you for always providing me with excellent analytical advice. Your

willingness to talk with me about conceptual and methodological issues was a life saver!

Manuela Rosengard–You are my sister, my best friend, and my editor all in one. Thank you for

laughing with me through my tears, frustration, and stress. You always knew exactly what I needed to

make it through, whether it was a pep talk, a funny David story, or a kick in the butt. I think everyone in

life should have someone like you to be by their side through life. I love you.

Marlene and Salvatore Fortunato – You believed in me even when I didn’t believe in myself, and

for that I am thankful. Your hard work and sacrifice over the years has given me the inspiration to keep

going when time got really tough. I love you.

Amy Syvertsen – Without you, I would not have completed my graduate career. You became a

part of my family in State College. Together we laughed, ran, or ate our graduate school stress into

submission. I am thankful for the loving friendship that you have given me over the past 6 years.

Sunam Ellis – Thank you for getting me through my toughest year of graduate schools. Thank

you for the times I slept on your couch when I was too stressed to be alone. You and your family were a

fantastic distraction from the stressors of graduate life and a touchstone to reality.

Michael Stinger – Your support through this whole process has meant the world to me. Thank

you for letting me work on your computer when my laptop died. But most of all, thank you for providing

me with encouragement at the times I thought I would not complete the dissertation. You are the best!

All other friends (especially Erin, Anita, Nate, Amber, Anna, Eliza, Nissa, Ben, Harshini, Mary,

Joche, Buttercup, Kari, Lauren) – Thank you for all your support, love, and laughter.

Page 9: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

1

Chapter 1

Introduction The inherent heterogeneity of externalizing problems in early childhood makes it difficult to

determine the antecedents, correlates, and developmental pathways underlying these problems (e.g.,

Beauchaine et al., 2008; Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, 2006; Raine, 2002). To date, most attempts to identify

the heterogeneity of externalizing problems have focused on parsing the various forms of problem

behavior, such as whether or not externalizing problems are reactive responses to stimuli or proactively

enacted toward instrumental goals (e.g., Berkowitz, 1993; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Gendreau & Archer,

2005; Vitaro, Gendreau, Tremblay, & Oligny, 1998). Unfortunately, reliable identification of externalizing

subgroups based on these behavioral factors alone has been difficult to achieve (Tremblay & Nagin,

2005; Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2002), with many studies revealing high correlations between the

reactive and proactive classifications (e.g., Dodge & Coie, 1987; Poulin & Boivin, 2000; Price & Dodge,

1989). Due to the lack of success of parsing classifications solely based on externalizing behaviors,

researchers have combined clinical and biological perspectives to propose that it may be beneficial to

determine subgroups based on patterns of comorbidity involving externalizing and internalizing

symptoms (review, Granic & Lamey, 2002; Hinshaw & Anderson, 1996; Hodgins, de Brito, Siminoff,

Vloet, & Viding, 2009; Lahey & Loeber, 1994; Raine, 2002; Steiben et al., 2007). While this is a potentially

promising theory, the study of externalizing and internalizing comorbidity as an effective way to

elucidate the inherent heterogeneity of externalizing problems is still in its nascent phase, highlighting a

need for further empirical investigation.

Externalizing and internalizing problems during childhood may appear to be opposing

phenomena, but the co-occurrence of these problems has been demonstrated in many epidemiological

and longitudinal studies (Caron & Rutter, 1991; Keiley, Lofthouse, Bates, Dodge, & Kenneth, 2003;

Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). What has yet to be well-established is whether children with comorbid

Page 10: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

2

externalizing and internalizing problems have different antecedents and developmental pathways than

children who exhibit externalizing behaviors without internalizing problems. Different domains of risk

are thought to provide unique contributions to the development of internalizing and externalizing

problems including both biological and behavioral aspects of the child as well as exposure to various

parenting styles (e.g., Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1998; Hodgins et al., 2009). For example,

a longitudinal investigation of children from 5 to 14 years old revealed that child temperamental

characteristics such as resistance to control and harsh parental discipline were related to externalizing

symptoms only, while maternal reports of difficult temperament and overall family stress were related

to comorbid externalizing and internalizing symptoms (Keiley et al., 2003).

Yet, there is still a large research gap in determining the interrelationships among individual and

contextual domains of risk during infancy and toddlerhood that may provide the most explanatory

power for the early emergence of externalizing problems and comorbid internalizing symptoms.

Understanding the association between specific risk factors and psychological outcomes becomes

increasingly complicated when examined over developmental time (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998;

Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994; Rutter, 1990; Sameroff & Chandler, 1975; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). Some

researchers have argued that risk factors that are transient do not function in the same way as those

that are chronic, and that single time-point assessments capture noise by failing to distinguish between

qualities that are unique to this assessment from those that are truly characteristic of the individual.

This hypothesis suggests that repeated measurements should be employed and only individuals who

repeatedly demonstrate the presence of a particular risk factor should be examined. However, because

of the normative developmental changes that occur from infancy to school entry, the predictive validity

of one risk factor may be more apparent at certain developmental time points. This is likely a function

of the fact that psychological constructs such as threat identification and regulatory ability have

different meaning across developmental time.

Page 11: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

3

For instance, it may be highly adaptive for an infant to demonstrate high behavioral reactivity

and low biological reactivity to stress (e.g., Gunnar & Davis, 2003; Gunnar & Donzella, 2002).

Presumably the high behavioral reactivity serves to signal to the caregiver the need for attention, and

the biological system need only be taxed if the caregiver’s response to the behavioral signal does not

sufficiently reduce the stress. However, as infants transition into independent movement, it may be

more appropriate for them to engage their own biological and behavioral resources to resolve

challenges independently, without necessarily signaling distress to an adult unless they find they are

unable to resolve the challenge alone (e.g., Gottlieb, 1992; Sameroff, 1990; Shaw, Keenan, & Vondra,

1994). It is possible that no absolute association between biological and contextual risk characterizes

children across development, but rather profiles that are developmentally inappropriate for the

particular measurement time have greatest explanatory value. Thus, it is proposed here that the

predictive validity of interactions between risk domains changes across developmental time, and that

these interactions over time may be more significant than the change or stability of any one risk factor

alone.

Thus, the overarching objective of the current study is to integrate neurobiological, behavioral,

parenting, and developmental research perspectives to explore the risk pathways in infancy and

toddlerhood that may result in distinct subgroups of externalizing problems, specifically those with and

without comorbid internalizing symptoms, at 5 years of age. In the current investigation, the

externalizing behaviors consist of conduct problems ranging from temper tantrums to physical

aggression, while the internalizing problems primarily entail symptoms of anxiety. Overall, this study

proposes that the complex interplay among biological stress responsivity, children's behavioral reactivity

and regulation, and parenting styles at 7, 15, and 24 months of age will contribute to children's

membership in behavioral subgroups characterized by high levels of conduct problems without anxiety

Page 12: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

4

(CP), comorbid conduct problems and anxiety (COMORBID), or normal development (COMPARISON)

(see Figure 1). In general, knowledge regarding these early risk factors may be necessary to designing

more effective early prevention programs, especially when considering that externalizing problems have

been shown to become more resistant to treatment over time (Kazdin, 1985, 1987; Loeber, Green,

Lahey, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1991).

Chapter Overview. In this first chapter, the background and rationale for the current analyses

will be presented and framed around Figure 1. The sections will proceed from the individual to the

contextual levels of risk (the bottom to top left of Figure 1). Each of the three sections includes how

interrelationships among children’s biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation,

and parenting styles at key developmental time points (7, 15, and 24 months of age) may be different

when predicting development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years of age.

Figure 1 Interrelationships across domains of risk influence the development of early onset

externalizing symptoms.

Note: HPA = Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal system. Subgroups of externalizing outcomes at 58 months of age: CP = Conduct

Problems without Anxiety; COMORBID = Conduct Problems with ANXIETY; COMPARISON = No or Few Symptoms on Either Problem.

Page 13: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

5

Initial discussions in first section further elucidate the differences in biological stress responsivity

that may undergird the three theoretically-distinct subgroups associated with conduct problems.

Consistent with the literature on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) as an indicator of biological

stress responsivity, this study focuses on cortisol response to challenge—the end product of the

neuroendocrine cascade involved in HPA functioning—as an indicator of susceptibility. The second and

third sections discuss how children's behavioral reactivity and regulation as well as children’s exposure

to different parenting styles may moderate (and be moderated by) HPA activity in infancy and

toddlerhood to result in different developmental trajectories underlying distinct behavioral outcomes.

Lastly, the overall aims and hypotheses of the current study are identified.

Biological Stress Responsivity

As seen in Figure 1, the most basic domain of risk (depicted as the lowest risk level) to

potentially differentiate the development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups is biological

stress responsivity. Scientists propose that two distinct biological pathways to externalizing behavior

exist: elevated activation of biological responses to perceived threat that makes appropriate behavioral

control challenging (heightened response), or a deficit in reactivity (blunted response) that may result in

lowered sensitivity to punishment and ultimately poor socialization (Hodgins, 2007; Hodgins et al., 2009;

Raine, 2002; Steiben et al., 2007). Therefore, it is hypothesized that children who develop comorbid

symptoms are likely to be characterized by elevated stress responsivity (a reduced threshold for threat

identification and a rapid and heightened biological response to stress), whereas those with CP are likely

to be characterized by a reduction in or absence of stress responsivity (a high threshold for threat

identification and blunted biological stress responsivity)(review, Granic & Lamey, 2002; e.g., Hinshaw &

Anderson, 1996; Lahey & Loeber, 1994).

Heightened HPA Activity. In general, children who have low thresholds for threat detection, and

consequently heightened biological reactivity to contextual challenges, are believed to have a higher

Page 14: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

6

propensity for displaying comorbid anxiety (e.g., Hubbard et al., 2002; McBurnett et al., 1991). Biological

reactivity to threat consists of a sequence of responses across multiple physiological systems. The

autonomic nervous system is engaged in immediate response to threat, preparing the body for fight-or-

flight reactivity. Activation in this system is a shared correlate of both anxiety (flight) and reactive

aggression (fight) in children (Hubbard et al., 2002; Tyson, 1998; van Goozen et al., 1998). If the threat is

perceived as severe, or engagement of the autonomic system does not result in rapid diffusion of threat,

HPA responsivity is engaged resulting in a systemic release of cortisol to minimize catabolic activity as

well as reproductive and immune system functioning in order to maximize resources to sustain efforts

to escape or resolve the threat (e.g., Chrousos, 1998; Chrousos & Gold, 1992; Porges, 2001; Ulrich-Lai &

Herman, 2009). Because of the sequential nature of this process, measurement of cortisol is conducted

both before the task and approximately 20 minutes after the onset of the experimental challenge, and

cortisol reactivity is proposed to be evidence of subjective assessment of the challenge as being

extremely stressful and difficult to cope with. In other words, failure to exhibit a cortisol response is

considered to indicate that the individual did not perceive the experience as stressful, or felt sufficiently

capable of coping with the stressor. Consistent with this model, evidence suggests that children with

externalizing problems comorbid with anxiety exhibit higher salivary cortisol responsivity following an

interpersonal frustration task than children with conduct problems only (van Goozen et al., 1998).

Additionally, children with conduct disorder exhibit higher cortisol baseline levels if they have comorbid

anxiety symptoms (McBurnett et al., 1991; 2000).

HPA axis activation results in a systemic release of cortisol which not only induces changes in

peripheral physiology, but modulates activity in a wide range of brain regions. In particular, a

heightened cortisol concentration results in down-regulation of higher-order prefrontal systems (e.g.,

Teicher et al., 2003). This effect of cortisol is considered to be adaptive, as the higher-order cognitive

processing of the prefrontal system is computationally slower than limbic systems and may be

Page 15: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

7

detrimental under acute and immediate threat. However, activation of this system that is

disproportionate to the actual contextual challenge will disrupt children’s ability to enact self-regulation

skills and executive processing (e.g., McBurnett et al., 1991), leading to greater disruption of behavior in

both internalizing and externalizing domains. This context-specific profile of cognitive disruption

explains why children who are capable of identifying appropriate and prosocial responses to challenging

experiences presented hypothetically often fail to enact those responses in ecologically-valid contexts

when physiological arousal is more likely to be engaged (Dearing et al., 2002).

Blunted HPA Activity. Evidence from research on autonomic arousal as well as HPA responsivity

suggests that children characterized by blunted baseline cortisol levels and reactivity to challenge have

greater amounts of externalizing problems in later childhood and adulthood (reviews, Fowles, 2000;

Raine, 2002), as well as lower than average levels of anxiety (Hodgins et al., 2009; Loney et al., 2006).

However, blunted HPA activity appears in only a small percentage of the population (Del Giudice, Ellis, &

Shirtcliff, 2010; Hawes, Brennan, & Dadds, 2009; Raine, 2002; Raine, Venables, & Mednick, 1997). It has

been proposed that blunted HPA reactivity represents a biological adaptation to protect the individual

from the physiological consequences of prolonged cortisol exposure that may occur in environments of

chronic threat (Del Giudice et al., 2010; Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006; Gustafsson, Janlert, Theorell, &

Hammarstrom, 2010; Susman, 2006; Tarullo & Gunnar, 2006). To date, however, very little research has

tested the developmental progression of the predicted relationship between blunted HPA activity and

psychopathology.

Biologically blunted stress responsivity may be associated with a decreased sensitivity to

punishment (Beauchaine, 2001; Corr, 2004; Gray, 1987; review, van Goozen, Fairchild, Snoek, & Harold,

2007). Decreased sensitivity to punishment makes it difficult to internalize consequences and may lead

to greater avoidance conditioning and deficits in moral socialization that enhance risk for developing

conduct problem symptoms (Frick & White, 2008; Hodgins et al., 2009). At the extreme end of the

Page 16: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

8

spectrum, blunted stress reactivity across the full spectrum of challenge severity may be associated with

a child's likelihood to display callous and unemotional traits.

Stability and Change over Time. Although there is substantial plasticity in children's state HPA

responsivity in infancy (Davidson, Jackson, & Kalin, 2000), the theoretical trait of biological sensitivity

that HPA responsivity is believed to reflect is likely to become canalized and more resistant to change

over time (Turkheimer & Gottesman, 1991). One possibility is that children characterized by consistently

blunted responsivity are at much greater risk for developing conduct problems without comorbidity

(e.g., Hawes et al., 2009; Raine et al., 1997), and that those with consistently heightened responsivity

are at greater risk for developing comorbid conduct problems (e.g., McBurnett et al., 1991; van Goozen

et al., 1998). Alternatively, changes in HPA reactivity patterns over time may be more predictive.

Specifically, the down-regulation of biological stress responsivity over time as an adaptation to early

heightened activity (the attenuation hypothesis; Susman, 2006) may be more indicative of long term

changes in physiological and psychological systems that reflect greater canalization of risk (Gunnar &

Vazquez, 2006; Gustafsson et al., 2010; Tarullo & Gunnar, 2006). This is especially likely to predict the

presence of conduct problems without comorbid anxiety.

Interrelationships of Risk Domains at 7 Months of Age. In infancy, two main interrelationships

are expected to significantly predict differences in the development of CP, COMORBID, and

COMPARISON subgroups: 1) the reciprocal interplay between behavioral reactivity and adverse

parenting styles and 2) adverse parenting exacerbating the influences of vulnerabilities in biological

stress responsivity. These relationships are depicted in the dark blue box on the far left side of Figure 1.

Before children have acquired and mastered various self-regulation skills (e.g., Gunnar &

Donzella, 2002), how behavioral aspects of overall negative reactivity and irritability reciprocally interact

with adverse parenting styles may have significant explanatory power when determining whether

children will become members of CP, COMORBID, versus COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years of age.

Page 17: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

9

Many empirical investigations have demonstrated that behavioral-level risk factors and adverse

parenting styles reciprocally interact in early childhood to influence the later development of conduct

problems (Bates & Bayle, 1988; Bates, Pettit, Dodge, & Ridge, 1998; review, Putnam et al., 2002; Rubin,

Burgess, Dwyer, & Hastings, 2003). As such, adverse parenting style may be a critical factor affecting

children’s developing behavioral reactivity and regulation during early childhood, and vice versa (see

Figure 1).

That being said, biological stress responsivity is the most plastic in early infancy as a means of

“appropriately” adapting to the amount and severity of stressors a child is (and will likely be) exposed to

in a particular environment (e.g., Joseph, 1999; Nelson & Carver, 1998). At very early ages (before 12

months of age), children have a very limited behavioral repertoire and have not yet developed the

motivation or ability to regulate emotions such as frustration (e.g., Gunnar & Donzella, 2002; Rothbart,

Sheese, & Posner, 2007). Therefore at this stage, it is hypothesized that biological measures of stress

reactivity may be a better predictor of how children respond to adverse parenting. This is especially

likely given recent theories of stress reactivity as moderating the extent of sensitivity to contextual

factors and explaining individual differences in risk and resilience to environmental stress (Boyce & Ellis,

2005; Del Giudice et al., 2010). Therefore, it may be that the interactions between adverse parenting

styles and biological stress responsivity at 7 months of age to distinguish the development of CP,

COMORBID, versus COMPARISON subgroups.

Behaviors of Reactivity and Regulation

Behavioral reactivity and regulation, especially after the first year of life, is the key intermediate

domain of risk for early development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups (see Figure 1). In

general, behavioral reactivity and regulation risk is hypothesized as having moderating effects on the

relationship between biological stress responsivity as well as parenting styles and conduct-related

symptoms at 5 years of age. Specific behavioral risks empirically associated with early conduct problems

Page 18: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

10

include behavioral reactivity qualities such as irritability, as well as indices of low behavioral regulation

such as high approach tendencies and difficulties with sustained attention (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2009;

Farmer, Bierman, & CPPRG, 2002; Keiley et al., 2003; Nigg, 2006).

Irritability. Behavioral reactivity such as irritability has been associated with conduct problems

both with and without anxiety (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 2001, 2009; Farmer et al., 2002; Milich & Landau,

1984). However, negative reactivity and irritability in the early school years appears to be more

characteristic of children comorbid for externalizing and internalizing problems versus externalizing

alone (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Keiley et al., 2003; Khan et al., 2005). Therefore, it is unclear how the

sensitivity and specificity of this trait changes over developmental time. It is possible that in infancy,

negative reactivity is a primary behavior of a very limited repertoire, and thus, is broadly predictive of

risk for psychopathology. As children age, those who persist in high levels of irritability at

developmentally inappropriate time points may be more likely to develop high levels of pathology

including comorbidity. Alternatively, for a large majority of normally developing children with early

negativity will successfully develop coping skills that diminish the presence of irritability at later time

points.

Attention Regulation. Deficits in emotion regulation during stressful situations may also result

from impaired control over lower-order brain regions involved in emotion processing by higher-order

brain regions in the frontal lobes (MacDonald, 2008). Children with conduct problems, on average, have

been shown to have difficulties attending to environmental feedback when trying to extinguish

ineffective behaviors (Giancola & Tartar, 1999; Sagvolden, Johansen, Aase, & Russell, 2005). For

instance, an empirical investigation of a subset of the low income, nonmetropolitan population utilized

in the current analyses demonstrated that attention problems (lack of persistent attention, low levels of

behavioral constancy on tasks of difficult levels, and limitations in sustained interests in objects/test

materials) in the toddler years, among children exposed to high interparental conflict, was associated

Page 19: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

11

with conduct problems at 3 years of age (Towe-Goodman, Stifter, Coccia, Cox, & The FLP Key

Investigators, 2011).

In general, the ability to focus attention is linked to the ability to plan behaviors that can be used

to mitigate stressful contexts (e.g., NICHD, 2005). Sustained attention and the ability to shift attention

have been associated with both a reduction in internalizing symptoms of anxiety and overall anger and

defiance. This makes it difficult to determine whether the degree of attention regulation problems

significantly varies for children who develop externalizing problems with and without internalizing

symptoms. Some studies have revealed that children within comorbid externalizing-internalizing

problem subgroups had greater parent- and teacher-reported problems with attention, including

difficulties with concentrating and sustaining interest in tasks and high levels of impulsivity in

kindergarten (Farmer et al., 2002) and grade school (Boivin et al., 1994; Kellam et al., 1991) as compared

to children with externalizing problems alone. In contrast, other investigations have demonstrated that

attention problems, such as difficulties maintaining focus on tasks or appropriately shifting focus to

manage task-related demands, are similar for both externalizing problem subtypes (Eisenberg et al.,

2009; Steinberg et al., 2009). Therefore, the only difference that is clearly expected in the current study

is that both CP and COMORBID subgroups will be characterized by greater precursors of attention

regulation difficulties than children in the COMPARISON subgroup.

High Approach Tendencies. Unbridled approach tendencies are hypothesized as being

characteristic of individuals with externalizing behaviors such as CP. Extreme or context inappropriate

approach behavior has been associated with blunted arousal across multiple physiological systems, and

it is possible that failure to recognize risk negates normal processes that are designed to increase

cautiousness when faced with novelty or potential threat (Beauchaine, Katkin, Strassberg, & Snarr, 2001;

Quay, 1993). Some researchers have postulated that blunted arousal is perceived as subjectively

aversive, ultimately motivating sensation seeking behavior in attempt to increase physiological arousal

Page 20: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

12

(Beauchaine, 2001; Eysenck, 1977; Raine, 2002). Importantly, high levels of approach behavior in

infancy have also been associated with the development of higher IQ in childhood (Guerin, Gottfried,

Oliver, & Thomas, 2003), so it is important to consider how additional risk factors moderate the

relationship between early behavioral disposition and later symptoms of externalizing behavior. In

addition, the presumption that approach behaviors are consistent with externalizing symptoms lends to

the hypothesis that this trait should differentially predict conduct problems without anxiety from the

comorbid presentation. Many studies have reported this pattern (Johnson, Turner, & Iwata, 2003;

Oldenhinkel, Hartman, Dewinter, Veenstra, & Ormel, 2004), although others have reported a positive

association with early approach behavior and comorbid externalizing and internalizing symptoms (Keiley

et al., 2003).

It is important to note that HPA reactivity and behavioral reactivity and regulation represent

distinct components of individual-level risk and are not necessarily coordinated (Gunnar & Davis, 2003).

Some theories posit that the ability of biological and behavioral systems to respond to context at least

partially independently fosters adaptability across development (Glassman, 1973; Quas, Hong, Alkon, &

Boyce, 2000), but it is important to note that adaption in one domain may confer consequences in other

domains (Crespi & Denver, 2005). For instance, blunted HPA activity may transpire as a means of

adapting to a chronically adverse context to protect the body against physiological damage from

repeated acute stress reactivity (Fries, Hesse, Hellhammer, & Hellhammer, 2005; Susman, 2006). To

date, no empirical research has been performed on how the dissociation between biological and

behavioral reactivity and regulation in infancy and toddlerhood may differentially predict later patterns

of conduct problems with and without anxiety symptoms.

Stability and Change over Time. The stability and change of affective behaviors over time may

also be central to determining which children will develop externalizing problems with and without

internalizing problems. While most empirical findings have indicated that children with externalizing

Page 21: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

13

behaviors have more attention regulation difficulties than their same age counterparts (Boivin et al.,

1994; Eisenberg et al., 2009; Farmer et al., 2002), there are inconsistencies in the data as to whether

children who show co-occurring internalizing and externalizing symptoms have greater deficits in

attention than do those who have externalizing symptoms only.

It is possible that the developmental period during which these issues arise, and/or the stability

of these problems across early childhood, may be at the root of these inconsistencies. For instance, high

approach tendencies that are stable across early childhood in a community sample were significantly

associated with the development of comorbid externalizing and internalizing symptoms versus no

symptoms or only conduct problems at 4.5 years of age (Stifter, Putnam, & Jahromi, 2008). The authors

speculated that this relationship may have occurred because children with high approach tendencies are

also likely to become easily frustrated or depressed over time if their goals are blocked or do not

produce adequate progress, increasing their risk for comorbidity. While a promising study, more

investigations are necessary to determine if this relationship between stable high approach tendencies

and comorbid outcomes still holds in high risk samples.

Interrelationships of Risk Domains at 15 Months of Age. On average, as children enter later

infancy (beginning of their second year of life), they have more control over their behavioral reactivity

and utilize necessary self-regulatory behaviors as an initial way to mitigate responses to stress (Gottlieb,

1992; Rothbart et al., 2007). For instance, children with warm and sensitive parents may have formed an

internal working model that caregivers will come to their aid when faced with stressors (e.g., Ainsworth,

Blehar, & Wall, 1979; Bowlby, 1969). This allows children to express emotional distress and elicit help

without unnecessarily triggering a “fight or flight” response from the sympathetic branch of the ANS or

the negative feedback response of HPA system—biological and behavioral reactivity in this instance are

uncoupled. Therefore, it is only when behavioral reactivity and regulation are unsuccessful in mitigating

stress that biological systems are activated (Gottlieb, 2002). Given this theoretical premise, the most

Page 22: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

14

significant and prevalent mechanisms undergirding the development of conduct problem subgroups

may involve the interrelationships between behavioral reactivity and regulation and parenting style as

well as children’s biological stress responsivity and behavioral reactivity and regulation (see Figure 1).

Increased mobility in late infancy allows for the inverse relationship between social approach

tendencies and biological stress responsivity to potentially become more evident at 15 months of age.

This relationship between blunted HPA activity and high approach tendencies in later infancy may be

related to the development of children's membership into CP versus COMORBID subgroups. What is

uncertain is whether high approach tendencies may moderate the relationship between heightened

biological stress responsivity and COMORBID subgroup membership at 5 years of age. Empirical

research has demonstrated that individuals with only internalizing problems in childhood and

adolescence tend to have heightened HPA activity and low approach tendencies (e.g., Granger, Weisz, &

Kaunekis, 1994; Granger, Weisz, McCracken, Ikeda, & Douglas, 1996), but no known empirical

investigations have determined, on average, how HPA activity relates to approach tendencies in children

with comorbid externalizing and internalizing symptoms.

Since children are able to exert increased effortful control, such as sustained attention, by the

second year of life (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997; Kochanska, Murray, & Coy, 1997), reciprocal

interactions of attention difficulties with biological stress responsivity and with parenting styles in early

childhood are believed to contribute to the unique and disparate development of externalizing

problems with and without anxiety (MacDonald, 2008). Yet, there is a significant knowledge gap in the

literature regarding the explanatory power and exact interrelationships involving these attention

regulation difficulties as the predictor of these conduct problems subgroups.

Parenting Styles

The review of the interrelationships among risk domains thus far has already begun to highlight

how parenting style (depicted at the top Figure 1) is central in determining the pathways to the

Page 23: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

15

development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years of age. While parental

insensitivity and intrusiveness are believed to play key roles in the development of conduct problems

(e.g., Frick et al., 1990; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992), there is a research gap as to whether

parenting styles differentially relate to the likelihood of developing comorbid anxiety. Maternal

sensitivity—responding to children's needs in a timely, warm, and responsive fashion—appears to be

associated with reduced risk for heightened HPA reactivity (e.g., reviews, Adam, Klimes-Dougan, &

Gunnar, 2006; Gunnar & Donzella, 2002), and potentially later mental health problems (e.g., Blair et al.,

2008). Yet, the relationship among these variables is unclear. Some researchers hypothesize that

maternal sensitivity allows children to face stressors with a greater sense of efficacy and safety, thus not

requiring the engagement of the HPA system. Conversely, maternal insensitivity may impede the child’s

development of effective coping skills, leading to a greater likelihood of heightened biological reactivity

to stress. This is likely to create a transactional process whereby increased and prolonged stress

responsivity may result in more parenting challenges such as difficulties in calming a child once s/he

becomes aroused, which may exacerbate a parent’s tendency to withdraw.

Stress reactivity is also proposed to enhance engagement with, and attention to, environmental

factors, leading to greater susceptibility to both positive and negative contextual influences (e.g., Belsky,

1997, 2005; Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Del Giudice et al., 2010). Therefore, it is possible that only when

biological and parenting risks are both present is the child’s likelihood of developing symptoms of

psychopathology increased. This model suggests that reduced stress responsivity may actually serve a

protective effect by “tuning out” the presence of adverse or insensitive parenting. However, it is

alternatively possible that the interaction between proximal child behaviors and parenting styles are

stronger predictors of psychopathological outcomes because these behaviors define the dyadic

interactions that shape child development. Clearly, a greater understanding of how stress reactivity

interacts with parenting style is needed.

Page 24: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

16

Maternal intrusiveness—imposing one’s own agenda, ignoring the child’s needs, and expressing

harsh feelings towards the child (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999)—provides an

additional source of unpredictability and stress for children (Adam et al., 2007; Gunnar & Donzella,

2002; Teicher et al., 2003), and may be particularly detrimental for those with heightened HPA reactivity

who are hypothesized to be especially sensitive to their context (Del Giudice et al., 2010; Hudson &

Rapee, 2001; Rogosch & Cicchetti, 1994). As suggested above, children with heightened reactivity to

stress may evoke increases in maternal intrusiveness during parent-child interactions as a result of

parents’ efforts to control and alter the child’s emotionally labile responsivity. In turn, children may,

over time, become hypervigilant to hostile cues, further exacerbating their tendency to overidentify

threat, and ultimately increasing risk for overreacting in mild or ambiguous social interactions (Dodge,

1991). These reciprocal relationships will theoretically result in later externalizing problems with

comorbid internalizing symptoms.

As suggested above, blunted HPA responsivity may buffer the effects of intrusive parenting, as

children with reduced reactivity may be more likely to engage in avoidance conditioning and to

circumvent interactions with parents once they initiate intrusive behaviors (Blair, 1995; Blair, Colledge,

Murray, & Mitchell, 2001; McBurnett, Swanson, Pfiffner, & Tamm, 1997). This may result in protection

against the development of anxiety symptoms. However, children with blunted HPA responsivity may

be adversely affected by inconsistent parenting because of their purported deficiencies in socialization

through punishment. For these children, high levels of positive reinforcement for appropriate behavior,

rather than a reliance on punishment for inappropriate behavior, may be especially effective in

decreasing risk for the development of later externalizing behaviors (Blair, 2004; Blair, Peschardt,

Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, 2006; Kochanska, 1997). Therefore, although adverse parenting is likely to

interact with biological stress responsivity to enhance risk for psychopathology, it is proposed that the

specific profile of biological vulnerability (heightened or blunted HPA activity) differentially interacts

Page 25: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

17

with different types of adverse parenting, and that these combinations predict diverse patterns of

comorbidity in the development of conduct problems.

Stability and Change over Time. Although parental warmth and sensitivity tend to be stable

across early childhood, adverse parenting styles such as intrusiveness, detachment, and hostility are

more likely to be inconsistent (Dallaire & Weinrab, 2005). Moreover, inconsistent parenting styles may

be prevalent in children characterized by conduct problems without anxiety (Blair, 2004; Blair et al.,

2006; Kochanska, 1997). While this has been empirically demonstrated in later childhood and

adolescence with regard to parental coerciveness and hostility (e.g., review, Bugental, Johnson, New, &

Sylvester, 1998; Patterson et al., 1992), there is a paucity of investigations on how change or stability in

parenting styles in early childhood may predispose children to the development of conduct problems

alone or in combination with internalizing symptoms. Determining whether changes or stability in

adverse parenting styles across development put children more at risk for developing conduct problems

with and without internalizing problems could be helpful in designing family-based interventions that

target particular techniques to increase the likelihood of an immediate response or improvement from

the child at an early age.

Interrelationships of Risk Domains at 24 Months of Age. Between 24 and 36 months of age, it

becomes imperative to view the development of externalizing behaviors via an entirely transactional

approach, whereby multiple and reciprocal exchanges between risk factors predict deleterious

outcomes (Sameroff, 1990; Shaw, Keenan, & Vondra, 1994). The acquisition of increased mobility allows

for greater opportunity to encounter potentially novel environments and stressors. Additionally, the

child’s increasing ability to proactively influence his or her environment allows for more complex

interactions with parenting styles. Thus, it follows that behavioral reactivity and regulation will

moderate the reciprocal interactions between biological stress responsivity and parenting style to

distinguish children’s development into CP, COMORBID, versus COMPARISON subgroups (see Figure 1).

Page 26: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

18

Although it is clear theoretically that the interrelationship between these three domains of risk will have

the most explanatory power for predicting the development of CP, COMORBID, versus COMPARISON

subgroups at 5 years of age, the lack of empirical investigations make it difficult to determine a priori the

exact nature of these interrelationships.

Aims and Hypotheses

The overarching objective of the current study is to determine how children’s biological stress

responsivity, children’s behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parents’ parenting styles in early

childhood differentially influence the development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5

years of age. To accomplish this objective, the following research aims will be addressed:

Aim 1. To perform an exploratory investigation regarding the stability or instability of each

potential risk indicator —children’s biological stress responsivity, children's behavioral reactivity and

regulation, and parenting styles—across infancy and toddlerhood separately by behaviorally-identified

subgroups at 5 years of age.

In theoretical alignment with the reviewed literature, early childhood precursors of the

COMORBID subgroups versus the CP and COMPARISON subgroups may include the following:

consistent heightened biological stress responsivity (hyperactivity of HPA axis);

stable and high levels of irritability;

deficits in attention;

high initial displays of social approach followed by decreased approach tendencies

during toddlerhood as a result of increased negative parent-child interactions;

stable exposure to high levels of intrusive parenting styles.

In contrast, early risk factors for the CP subgroup versus the COMORBID and COMPARISON

subgroups are proposed to include the following:

Page 27: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

19

blunted stress responsivity (consistently blunted HPA activity or decreases in HPA

activity over time based on the attenuation hypothesis; Susman, 2006);

deficits in sustained attention;

stable and consistently high social approach tendencies;

inconsistent exposure to intrusive parenting and lack of maternal warmth/sensitivity.

In general, however, it was hypothesized interactions between multiple risk domains at key time

points in infancy and toddlerhood, as opposed to stability or change in any one domain of risk, will have

the most explanatory power in the development of these distinct subgroups.

Aim2. To determine how the interplay among children’s biological stress responsivity, children’s

behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles at key time points (7, 15, and 24 months of

age) differentially predicts the development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years

of age.

In accordance with the reviewed theoretical constructs regarding biological-behavioral-

contextual interrelationships (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; Del Giudice et al.,

2010; Gottlieb, 1992), it was hypothesized that the reciprocal interactions among children’s biological

stress responsivity, children’s behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles will become

more intertwined at 15 and 24 months of age relative to earlier time points. In fact, it is proposed that

by toddlerhood (24 months of age) only these complex interactions among all three domains will

significantly and differentially predict CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years of age.

In summary, the major differences expected among children’s biological stress responsivity,

children’s behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles at 7, 15, and 24 months of age for

the three subgroups are as follows:

Aim 2a. 7 Months of Age. Based on numerous empirical findings (review, Hodgins et al., 2009),

children’s exposure to adverse parenting styles may foster and/or exacerbate a pattern of heightened

Page 28: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

20

HPA activity in early infancy (e.g., Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Gunnar & Donzella, 2002), and may ultimately be

a precursor for membership in the later COMORBID (Hudson & Rapee, 2001) and CP (Del Giudice et al.,

2010; Susman, 2006) versus the COMPARISON subgroup. Moreover, in accordance with prior empirical

work (Boivin et al., 1994; Eisenberg et al., 2001, 2009; Farmer et al., 2002; Milich & Dodge, 1984), the

reciprocal interaction between behavioral negative reactivity, such as irritability, and adverse parenting

style may also contribute to the development of later conduct problem subgroups at 5 years of age.

Examination of all other interrelationships of children’s biological stress responsivity, children’s

behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting style indices at 7 months of age and their relation to

development of the three subgroups will be exploratory in nature due to the lack of extant empirical

research. Due to limited mobility and the incomplete maturation of attention skills in early infancy,

behavioral indices of approach tendencies and sustained attention would be unlikely to be significantly

correlated with the differential development of the three groups; however, exploratory analyses of

approach tendencies as well as sustained attention behaviors and their relation to CP, COMORBID, and

COMPARISON subgroups were conducted.

Aim 2b. 15 Months of Age. In line with theoretical premises and empirical research indicating

that children learn adaptive behavioral reactivity and regulation skills through interaction with their

environment (Gottlieb, 1992; Rothbart et al., 2007), by 15 months of age affective behavioral indices will

be involved in almost all of the significant interrelationships that predict membership in CP, COMORBID,

and COMPARISON subgroups at 5 years of age.

In general, it is expected that demonstrations of high irritability and/or overall negative

reactivity from children may continue to evoke a lack of maternal warmth and sensitivity as well as

maternal intrusiveness (e.g., Rutter et al., 1997), which may in turn reciprocally solidify the patterns of

behavioral irritability and overall negative reactivity in children. It is clear that such interactions would

be significantly more likely to aid in the development of CP and COMORBID subgroups. Additionally,

Page 29: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

21

since children with COMORBID symptoms have a higher propensity for negative behavioral interactions

with family and peers (Boivin, Poulin, & Vitaro, 1994; Farmer et al., 2002; Milich & Landau, 1984), it is

postulated that children's high levels of irritability paired with adverse parenting styles at 15 months of

age would be more prevalent with COMORBID versus CP subgroups.

Regarding attention regulation, maternal warmth and responsivity have been shown to be

associated with emotional and social regulation competencies such as attention (Campbell & von

Stauffenberg, 2008; Greenberg, Kusche, & Speltz, 1991), whereas a lack of maternal warmth as well as

maternal intrusiveness may result in difficulties in these self-regulation skills. Therefore, while high

levels of maternal sensitivity along with high levels of sustained attention would be likely precursors to

COMPARISON subgroup membership, children exposed to adverse parenting styles would exhibit

greater deficits in attention and be more likely to develop membership in the CP and COMORBID

subgroups. Exploratory analyses will be performed to determine if the sustained attention and parenting

associations at 15 months of age differentially predict CP versus COMORBID subgroups.

Furthermore, based on theoretical premises (Eysenck, 1977; Raine, 2002) and empirical research

(Stifter et al., 2008) discussed earlier, children at risk for CP and COMORBID subgroup membership tend

to have high approach tendencies. It may be that the “loose coupling” of high approach tendencies with

biological stress responsivity in infancy differentially predicts the development of these distinct

subgroups. Specifically, it is proposed that blunted HPA activity in conjunction with high approach

tendencies at 15 months of age may confer risk for becoming part of the CP subgroup in the early school

years, while heightened HPA activity along with high approach tendencies may be more likely to be

significant precursors of the COMORBID subgroup.

Lastly, it should also be noted that the relationship between high maternal intrusiveness and

heightened HPA activity may be pervasive across infancy and toddlerhood (e.g., Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Del

Page 30: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

22

Giudice et al., 2010). This relationship at 15 months of age is also expected to be a precursor for later

COMORBID as opposed to CP and COMPARISON subgroups.

Aim 3c. 24 Months of Age. In toddlerhood, only interrelationships among all three factors—

children’s biological stress responsivity, children's behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting

styles — are expected to differentially predict the development of CP, COMORBID, versus COMPARISON

subgroups at 5 years of age. Due to the lack of investigations into how both biological and behavioral

indices moderate parenting styles, most interrelationships that are tested are exploratory. Overall, it is

proposed that blunted HPA activity along with high approach tendencies or deficits in attention in

toddlerhood would predict CP versus the other subgroups in contexts with less exposure to maternal

warmth and sensitivity. Alternatively, heightened HPA activity along with high irritability, deficits in

attention, or high approach tendencies in toddlerhood may predict later COMORBID subgroup

membership versus membership in the other subgroups when exposed to greater levels of maternal

intrusiveness.

Page 31: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

23

Chapter 2

Methods

Study Design and Participants

The Family Life Project. Participants for the current study were drawn from the Family Life

Project, an ongoing longitudinal investigation of child development and family functioning in

predominately low-income, nonmetropolitan communities in Pennsylvania and North Carolina.

Families were recruited from local hospitals shortly after the birth of a child, over-sampling for

low-income and African American families. For detailed information regarding the sampling plan and

recruitment procedures in the Family Life Project (see Vernon-Feagans et al., 2008).

Of the 1,292 families who participated in the first wave of data collection when the child was 2-

months of age, 1,204 (93%) families participated when the child was approximately 7 months of age,

1,169 (90%) at 15 months of age, and 1144 (89%) at 24 months of age, and 1081 (83%) at 5 years of age.

Approximately half of the children were female (47%), and 42% were identified as black (58% identified

as white). The majority of families were less than 200% of the poverty line at the 7-month visit.

Due to cancellations and difficulties in scheduling family assessments during the particular age

range of interest, the analyses were restricted to the following ages at each assessment: 1) between 5

and 9 months of age at the early infancy assessment (M age=7.31 and SD=1.00 at 7-month visit); 2)

between 13.5 and 19 months of age at the late infancy assessment (M age=15.47 and SD=.90 at 15-

month visit); and 3) between 23 and 31 months of age at the toddler assessment (M age=24.70 and

SD=1.47 at 24-month visit). This resulted in the exclusion of 137 participants at the 7-month visit, 49

participants at the 15-month visit, and 63 participants from the 24-month visit. At the 5 years visit age

ranged from 57 to 75 months old (M age=60.50 and SD=3.21).

From this larger sample of FLP participants, a subset of children were identified and selected for

the present study who met criteria for the behaviorally-based subtypes of interest. Specifically, children

Page 32: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

24

the current analysis included subgroups of children who exhibited high levels of symptoms in conduct

problems (n=72, 7.0% of the restricted sample), both conduct problems and anxiety (n=64, 6.1% of the

restricted sample), or low levels of difficulties on both conduct problems and anxiety (n=370, 35.5% of

the restricted sample) at 5 years of age. Unlike the larger epidemiological population of diverse,

impoverished, and non-metropolitan families, this subset of children (total N=506) only represents the

clinically-significant subset of children with conduct problems and comorbid anxiety as well as the

extremely low-risk subset of children concerning these problems.

Identification of Conduct Problem Subgroups. Within the current sample, subgroups with

behaviorally-based differences in conduct problems were identified at 5 years of age. These subgroups

were formed based on findings from epidemiological investigation of mental health outcomes in over

ten thousand children (4 to 17 year olds) in the U.S., which utilized the parent-reported data from the

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Bourdon, Goodman, Rae, Simpson, & Koretz, 2005).

In particular, the Conduct Problem and Emotional Symptoms (assessment of internalizing

problems) subscales from parent-reported SDQ were utilized to form the behaviorally-based conduct

problem subgroups. Previously, the SDQ with multiple informants—parents, teachers, and older

children—successfully identified 5 to 15 year olds with a psychiatric diagnosis based on criteria from the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM IV) in 70% of the individuals with conduct

problems as well as internalizing disorders including generalized anxiety and depression (Goodman,

Ford, Simmons, Gatward, & Meltzer, 2000). Out of over 8000 participants, the SDQ identified childhood

psychopathology with both a high specificity (94.6%; range 94.1-95.1%) and moderate to high sensitivity

(63.3%, range 59.7-66.9%). Generally, both parent- and teacher-reported problems on the SDQ were

equally successful at diagnosing psychopathology, although the use of both informants predicted the

most specificity and sensitivity. (Details of Conduct Problem and Emotional Symptoms subscales are

described in the Measures section.)

Page 33: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

25

A slightly more conservative subscale range than reported by Bourdon and colleagues (2005)

were used to identify the subgroups at the 5 year assessment. The range of each of the subscales is from

0 (no symptoms) to 10 (maximum number of symptoms). In the epidemiological population, 10% of the

children had scores from 4 to 10 on the conduct problems subscale, while 8% of the children had scores

of 5 to 10 on the internalizing subscale. In the current study, the subgroups were comprised accordingly:

a) COMPARISON subgroup was defined as children who ranged from 0 to 1 on the conduct

problem subscale and 0 to 2 on the emotional symptom subscale;

b) CP subgroup was comprised of children who ranged from 4 to 10 conduct problem subscale

and 0 to 2 on the emotional symptom subscale; and

c) COMORBID subgroup was defined as children who ranged from 4 to 10 on the conduct

problem subscale and 5 to 10 on the emotional problems subscale.

Table 1 contains the demographic information of the sample by subgroup. In general, the CP

subgroup members were more likely to be males. Alternatively, the COMORBID subgroup members

were more likely to be black and to be low income. Both CP and COMORBID subgroups had children

whose mothers were single-status and had significantly lower educational achievement than the

COMPARISON subgroup.

Page 34: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

26

Table 1 Percentages and Means (Standard Deviations) of Demographic Variables for Conduct Problem

Subgroups.

Notes: a White=0 and Black=1; bNC=1 and PA=0; cIncome-to-Needs of 200% below government-defined poverty level; dYears of school completed. 1p-value based on chi-square tests or (for maternal education achievement) ANOVA to determine independence between each demographic variable and subgroups. Percentages and means (SD) in the same row that share “f” subscripts do not differ at p < .05 according to chi-square test of independence pair-wise comparisons or (for maternal education achievement) post-hoc Tukey analyses. † p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001.

Procedures

Families were visited in their home by trained research assistants when the target child was

approximately 7, 15, and 24 months of age in order to conduct in-home interviews, and to collect

observational and biological assessments of the target child. Mothers completed questionnaires via

laptop computer and reported demographic information on all household members. At 5 years old,

parent-reported questionnaires on child mental health were collected.

At 7-, 15-, and 24-month assessments, children’s salivary cortisol samples were collected around

a series of emotionally-eliciting challenge tasks that were developmentally salient. Additionally, home

visitors independently completed post-visit ratings of the child’s behavior over the course of the entire

visit in early childhood. Finally, parent-child interactions were video-taped during a set of free play

activities, and later coded for parenting styles of maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness.

Challenge Tasks. At each visit, children participated in a series of developmentally appropriate

challenging tasks designed to elicit emotional reactivity and self-regulatory behavior, which were

COMPARISON (n=370)

CP (n=72)

COMORBID (n=64)

p-value1

Demographics Male 47.84%f 62.50% 54.69%f † Race (Black)a 34.59%f 41.67%f 57.81% ** Living in NCb 55.68% 54.17% 65.63% Low-Incomec 52.26% 66.67% 78.13% *** Single-Parent Status 41.74% 62.50% f 64.06%f *** Maternal Education Achievementd

15.43 (2.49) 13.99 (2.73)f 13.23 (2.95)f ***

Page 35: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

27

videotaped for later coding (e.g., Buss & Goldsmith, 1998; Kochanska, Tjebkes, & Forman, 1998; Stifter &

Braungart, 1995). At the 7-month visit, infants were presented with series of three tasks while seated in

a walker: a mask presentation, followed by a barrier challenge, and finally an arm restraint procedure.

For the mask presentation, mothers were seated beside their child while the child was presented with a

succession of four unusual masks for 10-second intervals. While wearing each of the masks, the research

assistant leaned towards the seated child, turned their head from side to side, and repeated the child’s

name. Mothers were asked not to interfere or distract their child, but to respond as they normally

would if their child looked to them. For the barrier challenge, mothers were asked to step outside of the

infant’s line of sight, but to remain within hearing distance of their child. Infants were given an attractive

toy to play with for 30 seconds, which was then removed just beyond the child’s reach by the research

assistant and placed behind a clear, plastic barrier for 30 seconds. The toy was returned to the child, and

the procedure was repeated twice for a total of three cycles. For the arm restraint procedure, the

mother remained outside of the child’s line of sight, and a research assistant crouched behind the infant

and gently restrained the child’s arms for 2 minutes.

At the 15-month and 24-month visit, children were presented with two tasks while seated in a

booster seat: a toy-removal procedure (replacing the barrier task), followed by the mask presentation.

For the 15-month toy removal task, the mother was given an attractive toy, and asked to play with the

child and the toy for one minute. The mother was then instructed to remove the toy and place it outside

of the child’s reach, at which point the home visitor and the mother engaged in conversation while the

child remained in the booster seat. After two minutes, the mother returned the toy to the child, but did

not play or engage the child with the toy in any way. For the 24-month toy-removal a similar procedure

was followed. After playing together with the toy for one minute, the mother was asked to remove the

toy and place it inside a clear container with the lid screwed on tightly. The container was then given to

child while the mother and research assistant conversed for two minutes. The mother then returned the

Page 36: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

28

toy to the child and soothed or engaged the child in play for one minute. The mask presentation

followed the same protocol as in the infancy assessment. For all of the tasks at each time point, mothers

were informed that they could stop the procedure at any time, and the research assistant terminated

the tasks if the child engaged in 20-seconds of hard crying.

Salivary Cortisol Collection. To assess overall levels of HPA activity as well as cortisol reactivity in

response to the emotionally arousing challenge tasks, three saliva samples were collected from children:

1) a baseline sample collected prior to the challenge tasks, 2) a sample collected 20-minutes after the

child’s peak arousal to the tasks, and 3) a sample collected 40-minutes after the child’s peak arousal.

Peak arousal was clearly outlined for home visitors in the protocol, with a substantial portion of the

children reaching peak arousal during the final task in the series at each time point. Saliva samples were

collected using cotton or hydrocellulose absorbent material and expressed into cryogenic storage vials

(cotton) or by centrifugation upon arrival at the laboratory (hydrocellulose) (Granger et al., 2007). Prior

studies have found no differences in cortisol levels depending on the use of cotton or hydrocellulose

collection techniques (Granger, et al., 2007; Harmon, Granger, Hibel, & Rumyantseva, 2007). After

collection, samples were placed on ice, temporarily stored in a -20°C freezer, shipped overnight in

batches to the Behavioral Endocrinology Laboratory at the Pennsylvania State University, and then

stored in -80°C freezers until they were assayed.

All samples were assayed in duplicate for cortisol using a highly sensitive enzyme immunoassay

(510k) designed to measure adrenal function from saliva samples (Salimetrics). The tests utilized 25 µL

of saliva, had average intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation less than 10 and 15%, and had a

lower limit of sensitivity of .007 µg/dL (range of sensitivity from .007 to 3.0 µg/dL).

Measures

Conduct Problem Subgroups. Children’s behavioral outcomes of conduct problems and anxiety

were assessed at the 5 year assessment through parent ratings on the Strengths and Difficulties

Page 37: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

29

Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997). The current proposal utilizes items from the conduct problems

and emotional problems subscales. Items are rated on a 3-pt Likert scale from ‘not true’=0 to ‘certainly

true’=2, with 5-items per subscale. The items on the Conduct Problems subscale included “often has

temper tantrums or hot tempers,” “generally obedient, usually does what adults request,” “often fights

with other children or bullies them,” “often lies or cheats,” and “steals from home, school, or

elsewhere.” The items on the Emotional Symptoms subscale included “often complains of headaches,

stomachache, or sickness,” “many worries, often seems worried,” “often unhappy, down-hearted, or

tearful,” “nervous or clingy in new situations, easily loses confidence,” and “many fears, easily scared.”

The internal consistencies are adequate for the Conduct Problem subscale (𝛼 ranges from .46 to .76) and

Emotional Symptoms subscale (𝛼 ranges from .60 to .76; review, Stone, Otten, Engels, Vermulst, &

Janssens, 2010). Mean test-retest reliability correlations across studies are .66 for both subscales. On

average, the concurrent validity with the Child Behavior Checklists (CBCL) revealed weighted

correlations of .71 Conduct Problem subscale and Emotional Symptoms subscale .64.

These two subscales were used to determine the CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups

(see previous Identification of Conduct Problem Subgroups subsection for details).

Biological Stress Responsivity. Pre-task baselines are believed to represent “trait”-like or acute

anticipatory stress patterns of HPA activity; therefore, pre-task cortisol levels (ug/dL) were assessed at

7-, 15-, and 24-month visits as separate indicators of stress than cortisol reactivity. To assess children’s

cortisol reactivity to the challenge tasks, 20-minute post-task cortisol values were subtracted from the

pre-task cortisol values. The level of pre-task cortisol involves a negative association between the initial

pre-task level and the magnitude of change (e.g., higher initial value of cortisol restricts the range of

reactivity to be a smaller change), this phenomenon is called the Law of Initial Value (LIV; Lewis &

Ramsay, 1999; Lewis & Thomas, 1990). Adding pre-task cortisol levels to each of the analyses accounted

Page 38: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

30

for the variance due to this initial value when determining whether cortisol reactivity is a significant

predictor of the developmental outcomes of interest.

Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation. At each of the assessments in infancy and toddlerhood,

behavioral reactivity and regulation that may be associated with later conduct problem outcomes were

assessed via observation by home visitors. Home visitors independently rated the child’s behavior using

an adaptation of the Infant Behavior Record (IBR; Bayley, 1969). In the present study, the IBR was

applied globally to behavior observed across the entire home visit (see Stifter & Corey, 2001). Four IBR

items/scales of interest in the present study were overall reactivity, irritability, sustained attention, and

approach tendencies. Overall reactivity is comprised of one item assessing sensitivity and excitability to

stimuli in general, which is scored from 1='Unreactive; responds only to strong or repeated stimulation'

to 9='Very reactive; startles, reacts quickly, seems keenly sensitive to things going on around him/her.'

Irritability is also comprised of one 9-point item (1=‘No irritability; infant passively responds to all

stimulation' to 9='Irritable to all degrees encountered throughout the home visit'). Sustained attention is

a superordinate scale comprised of the mean across three 9-point items assessing tendency towards

persistent attention, behavioral constancy on tasks of difficult levels, and level of sustained interests in

objects/test materials. Approach tendencies forms a superordinate scale comprised of a mean across 3

items. The first 9-point item asked about the responsiveness to persons (1='Behavior towards persons is

not different from behavior towards objects' to 9='Behaviors seems to be continuously affected by

awareness of persons present'). Next were two items included 5-point Likert scales on the

responsiveness to the examiner and to their mother/caregiver (1='Avoiding or withdrawn ' to 5 =

'Inviting (initiating, demanding)'). The mean of the home visitors’ ratings were used; cross-rater

correlations ranged from .56 to .59.

Page 39: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

31

Parenting Styles. Mothers were given four standard, age-appropriate toys, and instructed to

play with their infants as they normally would for 10 minutes while seated on a baby blanket on the

floor. Interactions were later coded to assess levels of intrusive behavior or warm engagement.

Coders were trained to reliably assess maternal interaction behaviors during this parent-child

free-play episode (Cox, Paley, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999), and on a random selection of approximately

30% of the total sample to prevent coder drift. Maternal sensitivity, detachment, intrusiveness, positive

regard, negative regard, developmental stimulation, and animation were globally coded on a 1 to 5

scale, ranging from not at all characteristic to highly characteristic. Based on the results of factor

analyses conducted with an oblique rotation (i.e., Promax), maternal sensitivity (ICC=.87) was defined as

the mean of mother’s scores on four characteristics: detachment (reversed; degree of emotional

disengagement), positive regard (expressed level of positive feelings towards the child), animation

(energy level), and developmental stimulation (scaffolding activities with the child appropriately).

Maternal intrusiveness/negativity (ICC= .80) was defined as the mean of mothers’ scores for three

characteristics: sensitivity (reversed; level of responsiveness to the needs, gestures, and expressions of

the child), intrusiveness (amount the mother imposed her own agenda on the interaction, and ignored

the baby’s needs), and negative regard (degree of harsh, negative feelings expressed toward child). The

composite scores for maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness were negatively correlated (r=-.35; p < .01).

Reliability was assessed on a random subset of the sample (~30% of the recordings), each videotape was

double-coded ( range=.62-.70).

Covariates. There are a wide range of sociocultural risk factors that have been linked to

externalizing behaviors throughout childhood, including poverty (e.g., McLoyd, 1990) and adverse family

characteristics (e.g., single-mother home; Achenbach, Howell, Quay, & Conners, 1991), regardless of

phenotype. Poverty and family structure are among the most pervasive sociocultural risk factors

regarding the onset and development of conduct problems (e.g., Capaldi, DeGarmo, Patterson, &

Page 40: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

32

Forgatch, 2002; Mcloyd, 1990). Therefore, factors associated with these sociocultural risk factors were

included in the study as covariates.

Mothers reported on demographic information on all household members, including age,

gender, ethnicity/race, marital status, and income from all sources. In order to calculate the income-to-

needs ratio of the family, the total household income from all sources was divided by the federal

poverty threshold for that year, adjusted for the number and types of individuals in the household. An

income-to-need ratio of 1.00 indicates the family income is at the poverty line.

Due to the variable time in which saliva samples were collected and the distinct diurnal rhythm

that occurs throughout the day (Blair et al., 2008), time of day for sample collection was entered as a

covariate in the all of the analyses. In addition, since the use of medications including acetaminophen

by infants has also been shown to account for variance in cortisol levels and reactivity (Hibel, Granger,

Kivlighan, Blair, & the FLP Investigators, 2006), medication use was also tested.

Overview of the Analytical Plan

The analytical plan for the present study is as follows: First, the conduct problem subgroups (CP,

COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups) were identified. Then descriptive statics were explored for all

demographic, biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting style

variables of interest by the three conduct problem subgroups. Chi-square tests were performed to

assess subgroup differences in all categorical (non-continuous) demographic variables; while, analysis of

variances (ANOVAs) were used to assess subgroup difference in continuous variable. Followed up post-

hoc chi-square test of pairwise comparisons and Tukey tests were performed when significant omnibus

subgroup differences were found. ANOVAs—controlling for demographic risk factors—assessed

subgroup differences in biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting

style variables at each developmental time point (7-, 15-, and 24-months). Post-hoc Tukey analyses were

used to identify mean differences in these indices of interest across subgroups.

Page 41: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

33

Second, linear mixed model (LMM) were run for each factor of interest—HPA activity, affective

behaviors of reactivity and regulation, as well as parenting style—using PROC MIXED (SAS Institute 1990)

to identify mean changes (or stability) in these variables across early childhood (7, 15, and 24 months of

age) within each subgroup. This statistical technique was conducted instead of repeated measures

ANOVA because it uses maximum likelihood instead of listwise deletion when handling missing data

(Collins, Schafer, & Kam, 2001). Additionally, LMMs biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity

and regulation, and parenting style accommodate static and dynamic covariates of change as well as

more accurately model random effects of variability within individual than repeated measure ANOVAs.

Lastly, three separate multinomial logistic regressions (MLRs) utilizing imputed datasets

(discussed below) assessed how biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and

parenting styles at each developmental time point (7, 15, or 24 months of age) significantly influences

differences in the development of conduct problem subgroups. MLRs were performed because this

regression technique can estimate the relation between a nominal dependent variable with multiple

groups (the three subgroups) and independent variables that are categorical as well as continuous

(Demaris, 1992; Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989). In MLRs, the Wald chi-square statistic tests the unique

contribution of each independent variable over and above other predictors in the model. Additionally,

the odds ratio represents a probability ratio with respect to the reference group, whereby the closer the

odds ratio is to 1.00, the lower the likelihood that the covariate, independent variable, and/or

interaction can predict subgroup membership.

Significant demographic variables were entered into the logistic regression model as covariates

in all of the MLRs. Independent variables included pretask cortisol levels, cortisol reactivity to emotional

tasks, approach tendencies, sustained attention behaviors, overall behavioral reactivity, behaviors of

irritability, sensitive parenting, as well as intrusive parenting. Interactions hypothesized a priori as well

as exploratory interactions among independent variables were entered separately into the multinomial

Page 42: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

34

logistic regression; these were only retained in the model if they explained a significant amount of the

variance in the dependent conduct problem subgroups.

Multiple Imputations of Missing Data. To account for missing data and avoid estimation bias

using listwise deletion when running the multiple logistic regressions, data on the subset of FLP

participants included in the present study was multiply imputed using PROC MI command in SAS version

9.1. In accordance with the multiple imputation approach (Rubin, 1987; Schafer, 1997; Schafer &

Graham, 2002), 30 imputed datasets were estimated with the assumption that data was primarily

missing at random. The variables utilized for the imputation included all demographic information and

variables contained in each of the models at all time points (7-month, 15-month, 24-month, and 5 year

assessments). Data from measures that were not included in the models but would theoretically aid in

the accurate estimating these 30 datasets (auxiliary variables) were also used in the imputation process.

Measures of these auxiliary variables included: Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ) at the 7-month

assessment, micro-coded behaviors during the stress task at all assessments, as well as the ADHD

Questionnaire (Report of DSM-IV ADHD Symptoms) and Child Behavior Questionnaire at the 5 year

assessment.

The development of appropriate Wald χ2 statistic that summarize multinomial logistic regression

estimates across multiple imputations has yet to be created; thus, ranges across the 30 imputed data

sets were reported. The statistical significance given is based on the mean Wald χ2. PROC MIANALYZE

(SAS Institute, 2002) was used to summarize the parameter and covariance estimates as well as

calculate the appropriate standard error estimates across the 30 imputed datasets for each of the

multinomial logistic regression models. The specific contrasts between subgroups do not list a Wald χ2

statistic but do provide the betas, standard errors, and significance values to assess post-hoc differences

between subgroups. Parameters and covariance estimates were used to calculate the odds ratios and

confidence intervals for each of the contrasts.

Page 43: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

35

Chapter 3

Results

Descriptive Statistics

The percentages or means (standard deviations) for gender, race, income-to-needs ratio, marital

status, and maternal educations are shown in Table 1 by behaviorally-derived conduct problem

subgroups at 5 years of age. Chi-square tests revealed significant differences in race (χ2(2 ,

N=506)=12.77, p=.002), low-income (χ2(4 , N=506)=24.47, p<.001), marital status(χ2( 2, N=506)=19.14 ,

p<.001), and maternal education achievement (F (2, 506)=25.54, p<.001) among the subgroups. Post-

hoc chi-square independence tests and Tukey analyses showed that both CP and COMORBID subgroups

were comprised of a significantly higher proportion of children with demographic risk than the

COMPARISON subgroups. Specifically, COMORBID and CP subgroups had higher proportion of low

income-to-needs (65.63% and 54.17%, respectively) and single-parent households (64.06% and 62.50%,

respectively), as well as mothers with lower educational achievement versus the COMPARISON

subgroup (52.26% were low-income and 41.74% were from single-parent families). In addition, the

COMORBID subgroup included a greater proportion of Black children than CP or COMPARISON

subgroups; while, the CP subgroup revealed a trend toward a significantly higher proportion of males

compared to the COMPARISON subgroup.

All demographic variables that demonstrated significant differences between the subgroups

were included in the subsequent analyses: gender, race, income-to-needs ratio, marital status, and

maternal education level. Other potential covariates were tested in each of the models—time of saliva

collection, teen mother status, state of residence, and acetaminophen use in early childhood. These

potential covariates were tested in each of the models in the present analyses. However, because they

did not explain a significant amount of the variance predicting the subgroups of interest, they were

removed from all analyses.

Page 44: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

36

Table 2 depicts the means and standard deviations for biological stress responsivity, behavioral

reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles by developmental time point (7, 15, and 24 months of

age) for each conduct problem subgroup at 5 years of age. One-way ANOVAs—controlling for race,

gender, maternal education achievement, marital status and income-to-needs ratio—revealed

significant mean differences in pretask cortisol levels (F (2, 506)=3.60, p=.03), cortisol reactivity (F (2,

506)=3.45, p=.03), and sensitive parenting style (F (2, 506)=5.72, p=.01) among the three groups.

Post-hoc Tukey analyses revealed that CP subgroup had higher levels of pretask cortisol (M=.28,

SD=.44) at 7 months of age compared to both the COMPARISON (M=.21, SD=.27) and the COMORBID

(M=.19, SD=.21) subgroups. Additionally, the CP subgroups during the 7-month assessment showed

significant decreases in cortisol (blunted HPA activity) during the stress exposure (M=-.05, SD=.44); in

contrast, the COMPARISON subgroup showed significantly greater increases in cortisol (heightened HPA

activity) during the emotion-eliciting task (M=.06, SD=.35).

In terms of behavioral reactivity and regulation, contrary to the current studies prediction, the

COMPARISON subgroup demonstrated a trend toward higher observed overall reactivity (M=6.12,

SD=.85) than the COMORBID subgroup (M=5.82, SD=1.23) at toddlerhood (24-month assessment).

Finally, across all of early childhood, the COMPARISON subgroup was exposed to significantly

greater amounts of observed maternal sensitivity (M=3.05, SD=.76 at 7 months old; M=3.00, SD=.76 at

15 months old; M=3.11, SD=.83 at 24 months old) than children with CP (M=2.68, SD=.91 at 7 months

old; M=2.61, SD=.70 at 15 months old; M=2.62, SD=.87 at 24 months old) and COMORBID (M=2.57,

SD=.83 at 7 months old; M=2.37, SD=.95 at 15 months old; M=2.53, SD=.85 at 24 months old).

In sum, a greater proportion of kindergarteners in both CP and COMORBID subtypes were

exposed to more demographic risk factors (poverty, divorced and single-parent households, and low

maternal education) than the COMPARISON subgroup during infancy and toddlerhood. Additionally, the

CP subgroup had the greatest proportion of males; whereas, the COMORBID subgroup had the highest

Page 45: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

37

proportions of Black children. Significant biological differences in stress level and stress responsivity

between the subgroups were apparent in early infancy, wherein the CP subgroup (on average) has the

significantly higher pretask cortisol levels and blunted cortisol reactivity in early infancy in comparison to

COMORBID subgroup and the COMPARISON subgroups. Significant mean differences in behaviors did

not occur until toddlerhood, revealing that the COMPARISON subgroup demonstrated greater overall

behavioral reactivity. Alternatively, (on average) both CP and COMORBID subtypes were exposed to

lower levels of maternal sensitivity than the COMPARISON subgroup.

Page 46: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

38

Table 2 Means (SD) Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles across Early Childhood by Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Notes: aCortisol Reactivity= ((20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels); bHome visitor reported on children's observed behaviors across the entire visit; cMicro-behavioral coding of

parent-child interactions during play tasks. 1p-values are based on analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and indicate significant omnibus mean differences among subgroups for each variable of interest

within each developmental time point. Means (SD) in the same row and developmental time point (e.g., “7 mos old”) column that share subscripts do NOT differ at p < .05 according to post-hoc Tukey

analyses.

†p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

COMPARISON Subgroup (n=370) CP Subgroup (n=72) COMORBID Subgroup (n=64)

7 mos old 15 mos old 24 mos old 7 mos old 15 mos old 24 mos old 7 mos old 15 mos old 24 mos old Biological Stress Responsivity

Pretask Cortisol Levels ( ug/dL)

.21*d

(.27) .24 (.46)

.19 (.28)

.28* (.44)

.20 (.26)

.25 (.48)

.19*d

(.21) .23 (.38)

.21 (.21)

Cortisol Reactivity across Emotion Task

a

.06* (.35)

.03 (.37)

.02 (.21)

-.05*e (.44)

.03 (.29)

- .03 (.25)

.01*e (.27)

.09 (.39)

-.02 (.15)

Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation

b

Approach Tendencies 13.43 (1.56)

13.94 (1.77)

13.95 (1.99)

13.32 (1.63)

14.00 (1.97)

13.77 (2.00)

13.10 (1.81)

13.65 (1.74)

13.31

(2.46) Sustained Attention 17.67

(2.55) 17.86 (2.55)

18.01 (2.90)

17.58 (2.68)

17.65 (2.38)

17.17 (3.27)

17.46 (2.81)

17.04

(3.41) 16.72 (3.84)

Overall Reactivity 5.56 (.85)

6.01 (.95)

6.12†

f (.85)

5.51 (.91)

6.09 (.94)

6.04†

f (.95)

5.54 (1.12)

5.78 (.98)

5.82†

(1.23) Irritability 3.08

(1.01) 3.28 (.91)

3.00 (1.24)

3.06 (1.15)

3.16 (1.01)

3.13 (1.24)

2.93 (1.01)

3.49 (1.08)

3.36 (1.42)

Parenting Stylesc

Sensitive/Responsive 3.05** (.76)

3.00*** (.76)

3.11*** (.83)

2.68**g (.91)

2.61***h (.70)

2.62*** i (.87)

2.57**g (.83)

2.37***h (.95)

2.53***i (.85)

Intrusive/Negative 2.33 (.72)

2.15* (.63)

2.25 (.82)

2.44 (.75)

2.38 (.59)

2.63 (.94)

2.54 (.83)

2.40 (.71)

2.66 (1.04)

Page 47: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

39

Aim 1. Descriptions of Stability and Change of Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and

Regulation, and Parenting Styles across Early Childhood with Conduct Problem Subgroups.

A 3 (developmental time point) X 3 (conduct problem subgroups) linear mixed models (LMM)

was run for each risk factor of interest— biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and

regulation, and parenting styles—to determine mean changes within the behaviorally-derived conduct

problem subgroup across early childhood (7, 15 , and 24 months of age). These LMMs controlled for

race, gender, maternal education level, income-to-needs ratio, and marital status. Each model was

tested with an unstructured, autoregressive, and toeplitz covariance structure. Based on the AIC and BIC

goodness of fit statistics, all models were best fit using an unstructured covariance matrix.

Only indices of irritability (AIC=4312.06, BIC=4400.23) and intrusive parenting (AIC=2753.69,

BIC=2841.85) demonstrated mean changes within the conduct problem subgroup across early childhood

(7, 15, and 24 months of age). The LLM for with behaviors of irritability as the dependent factor revealed

a trend towards a significant fixed effect for the interactions between subgroup and developmental time

point (F (2, 584)=2.21, p=.06). Simple effect comparisons were performed to determine which contrasts

were significant within each subgroup, and least-squared means (LS-Means)—adjusted for covariance

model effects—were plotted (see Figure 1). In the COMPARISON subgroup, children’s displays of

irritability behaviors were significantly greater at 15 months of age versus 7 months of age (β=.21,

SE=.07, t(490)=3.07, p=.002) and 24 months of age (β=-.29, SE=.08, t(488)=3.98, p<.0001). In the

COMORBID subgroup, children’s irritability was significantly lower at 7 months of age versus 15 months

of age (β=-.55, SE=.17, t(494)=3.31, p=.001) and 24 months of age (β=.44, SE=.19, t(484)=2.33, p=.02).

There was no significant difference in children’s irritability across early childhood for the CP subgroup.

The LLM for with exposure to intrusive parenting as the dependent factor revealed a trend

towards a significant fixed effect for the interactions between subgroup and developmental time point

(F (2, 554)=2.11, p=.07). As seen in Figure 2, for the COMPARISON subgroup, children’s exposure to

Page 48: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

40

intrusive parenting was significantly less at 15 months of age versus 7 months of age (β=-.18, SE=.04,

t(460)=4.60, p<.0001) and 24 months of age (β=.11, SE=.05, t(464)=2.35, p=.02). In the COMORBID

subgroup, children at 15-month assessment revealed significantly less exposure to intrusive parenting

compared to the 24-month assessment (β=-.34, SE=.11, t(478)=3.02, p=.002), but no significant within

subgroup differences were revealed regarding intrusive parenting levels at 7 months of age. Similarly, in

the CP subgroup, children at 15-month assessment revealed significantly less exposure to intrusive

parenting compared to the 24-month assessment (β=-.30, SE=.11, t(464)=2.79, p=.005), but no

significant within subgroup differences were revealed for intrusive parenting levels at 7 months of age.

In contrast, two behavioral reactivity and regulation variables showed differences in main

effects between developmental time points with no significant interactions effects: social approach

tendencies (AIC=5825.01, BIC=5913.18) and overall reactivity (AIC=3878.14, BIC=3966.31). Children’s

displays of social approach demonstrated significant differences across developmental time points (F (2,

484)=9.83, p<.0001). On average and regardless of subgroup, children demonstrated significantly lower

approach tendencies at 7 months of age (M=13.35) than at 15 months (M=13.93) and 24 months of age

(M =13.71), (ts(484)=4.48 and 2.00, ps=.0001 and .05, respectively). Additionally, children’s overall

behavioral reactivity demonstrated significant differences across developmental time points (F (2,

482)=9.83, p<.0001). In particular, children demonstrated significantly lower overall reactivity behaviors

at 7 months old (M=5.70) than at 15 (M=5.99) and 24 months of age (M=6.03), (ts (494)=2.19 and 2.22,

ps=.03, respectively).

Alternatively, maternal sensitivity was only significantly associated with main effect differences

in subgroup status at 5 years of age (AIC=2511.11, BIC=2639.28; F (2, 486)=4.10, p=.02), with no

significant interactions effects. Children exposed to higher levels of maternal sensitivity were

significantly associated with the COMPARISON subgroup (M=2.89) versus children within CP (M=2.72)

and COMORBID (M=2.74) subgroups, ts(486)=2.52 and 2.53, ps =.01, respectively). Finally, LMMs (AIC

Page 49: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

41

ranges=620.26-7058.16, BIC ranges=708.43-7146.35) with pre-task cortisol level, cortisol reactivity, and

sustained attention behaviors as the dependent variables showed no significant differences across

subgroups (Fs (2, 492) ranged from .68 to 2.50, ns), developmental time points (Fs (2, 492) ranged from

.43 to .97, ns), and subgroup by developmental time point interaction (Fs (4, 492) ranged from 1.43 to

1.70, ns).

Page 50: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

42

Figure 2 Changes in Children’s Exposure to Intrusive Parenting Across

Early Childhood within each Conduct Problem Subgroups. Children in

the COMPARISON subgroup had significant decreased exposure to

intrusive parenting from 7 to 15 months of age that stayed low from

15 to 24 months of age. Alternatively, both CP and COMORBID

subgroups had lower level of intrusive exposure at 15 versus 7

months of age with an increase in levels by 24 months of age.

Figure 1 Changes in Children’s Irritability across Early Childhood within each

Conduct Problem Subgroups. Children in the COMPARISON subgroup had

significant increased irritability from 7 to 15 months of age followed by a

significant decrease in irritability demonstration by 24 months. In contrast,

the COMORBID subgroup had increased irritability from 7 to 15 months of

age that stayed higher at 24 months of age. No significant differences were

found for the CP subgroup.

Page 51: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

43

Aim 2. Early Childhood Indices of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON Subgroup Development

Aim 2a. 7 months of age. MLR for Model 1 utilized 7-month assessment indices of biological

stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles to predict children’s

membership in three behavioral-derived conduct problem subgroups at 5 years of age. The overall

model was significant across the 30 imputed data sets, Wald χ2(28, 506)=73.46-83.91, p<.001. The

likelihood ratio tests revealed that maternal education achievement (Wald χ2(28, 506)=12.42-18.21,

Wald χ2 was p<.001), pretask cortisol levels (Wald χ2(28, 506)=2.35-9.38, p<.05), cortisol reactivity (Wald

χ2(28, 506)=1.40-10.42, p<.05), as well as interactions between cortisol reactivity and intrusive parenting

(Wald χ2(28, 506)=2.83-8.75, p<.05) significantly predicted children’s membership into the three conduct

problem subgroups (see Table 3). As shown in Table 4, contrasts of parameter estimates demonstrated

that infants who mothers had the lowest levels of maternal achievement were more likely be in the CP

(β=-.15, SE=.06, p=.04, OR=.86, CI=.76-97) and COMORBID (β=-.23, SE=.06, p<.001, OR=.80, CI=.70-90)

subgroups versus the COMPARISON subgroup. Moreover, the infants with higher pretask cortisol levels

were 1.49 times more likely to be in the CP versus COMORBID subgroup (β=.41, SE=.21, p=.02, OR=1.49,

CI=1.05-2.18).

Page 52: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

44

Table 3 Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity

and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 7 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Note: Multiple imputations were performed, resulting in a range of Wald χ2 across 30 imputed datasets.

† p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. a White=1 and Black=2; bIncome-to-Needs of 200% below government-defined poverty level; cYears of

school completed; dCortisol Reactivity= ((20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels); eHome visitor reported cortisol levels across

the entire visit; fMicro-behavioral coding of parent-child interactions during play tasks.

AIC Wald χ2 df Overall Model Fit 781.09 73.46-83.91*** 28 Covariates Male 3.44- 4.73 2 Race (White)a 2.39-4.21 2 Low-Incomeb 1.88-2.92 2 Married .57-2.65 2 Maternal Education Achievementc 12.42-18.21*** 2 Biological Stress Responsivity

Pretask Cortisol Levels ( ug/dL) 2.35-9.38* 2

Cortisol Reactivity across Emotion Tasksd 1.40-10.42* 2

Behavioral Reactivity and Regulatione Approach Tendencies .46-1.73 2 Sustained Attention 1.39-4.37 2 Overall Reactivity 3.29-5.22 2 Irritability .74-2.17 2 Parenting Stylesf Sensitive/Responsive .97-6.84 2 Intrusive/Negative .01-1.35 2 Interactions Cortisol Reactivity X Intrusive Parenting 2.83-8.75* 2

Page 53: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

45

Table 4 Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological

Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 7 Months of Age to

Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Contrasts β SE OR CI

COMPARISON vs CP Maternal Education Achievementa -.15* .06 .86 .76 -.97 Cortisol Reactivityb X Intrusive Parenting .31* .14 1.36 1.06-1.75 COMPARISON vs COMORBID

Maternal Education Achievementa -.23*** .06 .80 .70 -.90

COMORBID vs CP

Pretask Cortisol Levels (ug/dL) .41* .21 1.49 1.05-2.18 † p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. aYears of school completed; bCortisol Reactivity= (20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels)

Note: The COMPARISON subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMPARISON vs CP’ and ‘COMPARISON vs COMORBID’ contrasts. The

COMORBID subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMORBID vs CP’ contrast.

In addition, only one significant interaction distinguished the COMPARISON from the CP

subgroup, the interrelationship between cortisol reactivity and intrusive parenting (β=.31, SE=.14, p=.03,

OR=1.36, CI=1.06-1.75; see Table 4). The plot of infant cortisol reactivity moderating the relationship

between exposure to intrusive parenting at 7 months of age and the probability of developing CP is

shown in Figure 3. This plot revealed that infants with high cortisol reactivity (top 25%) had an almost

25% increase in probability of being in the CP subgroup (compared to COMPARISON subgroup) as the

level of exposure to intrusive parenting increased. There was no significant difference in the infants that

had the lowest cortisol reactivity (bottom 25%)—no change or decreases in cortisol levels during an

emotional eliciting task—for predicting the probability of being in the CP subgroup.

Page 54: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

46

Figure 3 Interrelationships between HPA Activity and Intrusive Parenting at 7 Months of Age Predicting

CP Subgroup Membership. Children with high cortisol reactivity to stressors (n=111) had greater

probabilities of becoming part of the CP versus COMPARISON subgroup as exposure to intrusive

parenting increased.

Aim 2b. 15 months of age. MLR for Model 2 utilized 15-month assessment indices of biological

stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles to predict children’s

membership in the conduct problem subgroups. The overall model was significant across the 30

imputed data sets, Wald χ2(36, 506)=74.48-87.26, p<.001. As in early infancy, the likelihood ratio test

revealed that maternal education achievement in early infancy significantly predicted children’s

membership into the three subgroups, Wald χ2(28, 506)=6.92-10.80, p=.008 (see Table 5). Again,

contrasts of parameter estimates demonstrated that infants whose mothers had the lowest levels of

maternal achievement were much more likely to be categorized into CP (β=-.14, SE=.07, p=.04, OR=.87,

CI=.77-1.00) and COMORBID (β=-.15, SE=.07, p=.04, OR=.85, CI=.75-.98) subgroups versus the

Page 55: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

47

COMPARISON subgroup (see Table 6). Moreover, there was a trend (Wald χ2(28, 506)=2.34-7.41, p=.06),

towards children in families with the lowest income-to-needs ratios being more likely to obtain

membership in the COMORBID versus COMPARISON subgroups (β=-.44, SE=.23, p=.07, OR=.63, CI=.40-

1.00). All other main effects cannot be interpreted because they are involved in interactions that were

significantly associated in the three subgroups at 5 years of age.

Page 56: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

48

Table 5 Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity

and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 15 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Note: Multiple imputations were performed, resulting in a range of Wald χ2 across 30 imputed datasets.

† p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. a White=1 and Black=2; bIncome-to-Needs of 200% below government-defined poverty level; cYears of

school completed; dCortisol Reactivity= ((20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels); eHome visitor reported on children's observed

behaviors across the entire visit; fMicro-behavioral coding of parent-child interactions during play tasks.

AIC Wald χ2 df Overall Model Fit 781.09 74.48-87.26*** 36 Covariates Male 3.60- 5.49 2 Race (White)a 2.72-4.70 2 Low-Incomeb 2.34-7.41† 2 Married .74-2.65 2

Maternal Education Achievementc 6.92-10.80** 2 Biological Stress Responsivity

Pretask Cortisol Levels ( ug/dL) .02-4.44 2

Cortisol Reactivity across Emotion Tasksd 1.47-9.29† 2

Behavioral Reactivity and Regulatione

Approach Tendencies .01-1.37 2

Sustained Attention .53-3.16 2

Overall Reactivity 2.84-6.18 2

Irritability 4.87-10.01** 2

Parenting Stylesf Sensitive/Responsive 5.91-13.16* 2 Intrusive/Negative .48-3.69 2 Interactions Pretask Cortisol X Intrusive Parenting 3.25-10.20* 2

Cortisol Reactivity X Behavioral Approach 3.02-13.48* 2

Behavioral Attention X Sensitive Parenting 4.41-11.45* 2

Behavioral Approach X Sensitive Parenting 3.01-10.01* 2

Behavioral Irritability X Sensitive Parenting

5.54-10.76* 2

Page 57: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

49

A majority of the interactions at 15 months of age significantly predicting subgroup differences

in mental health involved behavioral demonstrations of reactivity and regulation (Table 4). Specifically,

contrast analyses shown in Table 5 revealed that all but one interaction predicted differences in group

membership between COMORBID and COMPARISON subgroups.

The interactions between children’s sustained attention and sensitive parenting (Wald χ2 (36,

506)=4.41-11.45, p=.03) as well as children’s irritability and sensitive parenting (Wald χ2(36, 506)=5.54-

10.76, p=.03) in later infancy significantly predicted group differences among conduct problems

subgroups. Contrasts revealed that the significant differences in interactions of sustained attention and

sensitive parenting (β=-.36, SE=.15, p=.02, OR=.70, CI=.53-.92) as well as irritability and sensitive

parenting (β=-.36, SE=.14, p=.02, OR=.69, CI=.50-.93) were between the COMPARISON and COMORBID

subgroups (Table 6).

Probing of sustained attention and sensitive parenting as well as irritability and sensitive

parenting revealed similar results (Aiken & West, 1991). All individuals began at approximately a 50%

probability for co-occurring conduct problems and anxiety versus no-to-low symptom subgroup

membership when maternal sensitivity was at its minimum. Yet, children with low levels of observed

irritability (bottom 25th percentile, n=80) and high levels of sustained attention (top 25th percentile,

N=129) had a much steeper and abrupt decrease in the probability of being part of the COMORBID

versus COMPARISON subgroup. That is, at moderate level of exposure to sensitive parenting (2.5 out of

5 behavioral coding), these individuals had a less than 10% chance of being in the COMORBID subgroup

and a 0% chance at the maximum level of sensitive parenting. Alternatively, children with high levels of

observed irritability (top 25th percentile, n=100) and low levels of sustained attention (top 25th

percentile, n=114) had gradual and steep decrease that never reached below a 9% probability of being

part of the COMORBID versus the COMPARISON subgroup.

Page 58: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

50

Table 6 Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological

Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 15 Months of Age to

Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Contrasts β SE OR CI COMPARISON vs CP Maternal Education Achievementa -.14* .07 .87 .77-1.00 COMPARISON vs COMORBID

Low-Incomeb -.44† .23 .63 .40-1.00 Maternal Education Achievementa -.15* .07 .85 .75-.98 Pretask Cortisol X Intrusive Parenting .33* .17 1.40 1.05-1.90 Cortisol Reactivityc X Behavioral Approach .51* .27 1.72 1.12-2.63 Behavioral Attention X Sensitive Parenting -.36* .15 .70 .53-.92 Behavioral Irritability X Sensitive Parenting -.36* .14 .69 .50-.93 COMOBID vs CP

Behavioral Approach X Sensitive Parenting .40* .19 1.53 1.09-2.12 † p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. a Years of school completed; bIncome-to-Needs of 200% below government-defined poverty level;

cCortisol Reactivity= (20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels)

Note: The COMPARISON subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMPARISON vs CP’ and ‘COMPARISON vs COMORBID’ contrasts. The

COMORBID subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMORBID vs CP’ contrast.

The interactions between children’s cortisol reactivity and approach tendencies (Wald χ2(36,

506)=3.02-13.48, p=.04) in later infancy significantly predicted group differences among the conduct

problems subgroups. Contrasts revealed that the significant differences in interactions of cortisol

reactivity and approach tendencies (β=.51, SE=.27, p=.01, OR=1.72, CI=1.12-2.63) were between the

COMPARISON and COMORBID subgroups (Table 6).

Figure 4 shows the plot of children’s cortisol reactivity moderating the relationship between

social approach tendencies and the probability of developing membership into the COMORBID versus

COMPARISON subgroup. For children with high cortisol reactivity (top 25th percentile; heightened HPA

Page 59: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

51

activity) at 15 months of age, there was a significant increase in the probability (~30%) of developing

COMORBID status at 5 years old as their tendencies toward social approach increased. Alternatively,

children with low cortisol reactivity (bottom 25th percentile; blunted HPA activity), there was a

significant decrease in the probability (~20%) of displaying later COMORBID status as their tendencies

toward social approach increased.

The interactions between children’s pretask cortisol levels and intrusive parenting (Wald χ2(36,

506)=3.25-10.20, p=.04) in later infancy significantly predicted group differences among the conduct

problems subgroups. Contrasts revealed that the significant differences in interactions of pretask

cortisol levels and intrusive parenting (β=.33, SE=.17, p=.02, OR=1.40, CI=1.05-1.90) were between the

COMPARISON and COMORBID subgroups (Table 6).

Figure 5 depicts the plot of pretask cortisol levels the relationship between observed intrusive

parenting at 15 months of age and the probability of developing CP versus COMORBID subgroup status.

Interaction probing revealed that children with high pretask cortisol levels (top 25%) had over a 10%

increase in probability of being in the CP subgroup as the levels of exposure to intrusive parenting

increased (versus the COMORBID subgroup). There was no significant difference in the infants that had

the lowest cortisol reactivity (bottom 25%)—no change or decreases in cortisol levels during an

emotional eliciting task—for predicting the probability of being in the CP subgroup.

The interactions between children’s social approach tendencies and sensitive parenting (Wald χ2

(36, 506)=3.01-10.01, p=.04) in later infancy significantly predicted group differences among the

behaviorally-derived conduct problems subgroups. Contrasts revealed that the significant differences in

interactions of approach tendencies and sensitive parenting (β=.40, SE=.19, p=.03, OR=1.53, CI=1.09-

2.12) were between the COMORBID and CP subgroups (Table 6).

Figure 6 shows the plot of children’s social approach moderating the relationship between

maternal sensitive parenting exposure and the probability of developing CP versus COMORBID subgroup

Page 60: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

52

status. For children with high social approach tendencies (top 25th percentile) at 15 months of age, there

was a significant increase in the probability (from 65% to 95% likelihood) of developing CP subgroup

status as exposure to sensitive parenting increased. Alternatively, children with low social approach

tendencies (bottom 25th percentile), there was a significant increase in the probability (~10%) of

displaying later COMORBID subgroup status as exposure to sensitive parenting increased.

Page 61: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

53

Figure 4 Interrelationships between Children’s HPA Activity and Approach

Tendencies at 15 Months of Age Predicting COMORBID Subgroup

Membership. Children with high cortisol reactivity to stressors had a

significantly higher probability of developing COMORBID versus

COMPARISON subgroup status as the tendencies for exhibiting social

approach increased. In contrast, children with low cortisol reactivity to

stressors were significantly less likely to developing COMORBID status as

the tendencies for exhibiting social approach increased.

Figure 5 Interrelationships between Children’s HPA Activity and Intrusive

Parenting at 15 Months of Age Predicting COMORBID Subgroup

Membership. Children with high pretask cortisol levels had a significantly

higher probability of developing COMORBID versus COMPARISON

subgroup status as their exposure to intrusive parenting increased. No

significant differences in infants with low cortisol levels in relation to

intrusive parenting exposure were revealed.

Page 62: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

54

Figure 6 Interrelationships between Approach Tendencies and Sensitive Parenting at 15 Months of Age Predicting

CP Subgroup Membership. Children with high demonstrations of approach tendencies (n=49) had a significantly

higher probability of developing CP subgroup status the greater their exposure to maternal sensitivity versus the

COMORBID subgroup. In contrast, children with low approach tendencies (n=68) were significantly more likely to

developing COMORBID versus CP subgroup status with increased maternal sensitivity exposure.

Aim 2c. 24 months of age. MLR for Model 3 utilized 24-month assessment indices of biological

stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles to predict children’s

membership in the conduct problems subgroups. The overall model was significant across the 30

imputed data sets, Wald χ2(42, 506)=75.19-93.89, p<.001. Similar to 7 and 15 months of age, the

likelihood ratio test revealed that maternal education achievement (Wald χ2(42, 506)=.91-15.24, p<.001)

significantly predicted children’s membership into the conduct problems subgroups (see Table 7). In

particular, mothers had the lowest levels of maternal achievement were much more likely to be

categorized as CP (β=-.14, SE=.06, p=.04, OR=.87, CI=.78-.98) and COMORBID (β=-.22, SE=.07, p=.007,

OR=.80, CI=.74-.92) subgroups versus the COMPARISON subgroup (see Table 7). Moreover, cortisol

Page 63: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

55

reactivity to emotion eliciting tasks significantly predicted differences among the conduct problem

subgroups (Wald χ2(42, 506)=1.83-10.82, p=.04). To explicate, children with decreased cortisol reactivity

were more likely to be members of the CP subgroup (see Table 9); whereas, increased cortisol reactivity

was more likely to be associated with the COMPARISON subgroup (β=-.27, SE=.13, p=.03, OR=.83,

CI=.76-1.12).

Page 64: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

56

Table 7 Likelihood Ratio Tests for Models Relating Biological Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity

and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 24 Months of Age to Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Note: Multiple imputations were performed, resulting in a range of Wald χ2 across 30 imputed datasets.

† p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. a White=1 and Black=2; bIncome-to-Needs of 200% below government-defined poverty level; cYears of

school completed; dCortisol Reactivity= ((20 min posttask cortisol levels – pretask cortisol levels); eHome visitor reported on children's observed

behaviors across the entire visit; fMicro-behavioral coding of parent-child interactions during play tasks.

AIC Wald χ2 df Overall Model Fit 781.09 75.19-93.89*** 42 Covariates Male 2.51- 5.61 2 Race (White)a 1.93- 4.29 2 Low-Incomeb 3.30-5.46 2 Married .32-2.14 2 Maternal Education Achievementc 8.91-15.24*** 2 Biological Stress Responsivity Pretask Cortisol Levels ( ug/dL) .52-9.46 2 Cortisol Reactivity across Emotion Tasksd 1.83-10.82* 2 Behavioral Reactivity and Regulatione Approach Tendencies .40-2.41 2 Sustained Attention .10-1.82 2 Overall Reactivity 1.33-4.58 2 Irritability 1.77-5.32 2 Parenting Stylesf Sensitive/Responsive .01-2.86 2 Intrusive/Negative .56-3.82 2 Interactions Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Approach .01-6.21 2 Pretask Cortisol X Sensitive Parenting .09-2.39 2 Behavioral Approach X Sensitive Parenting .33-2.60 2 Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Approach X Sensitive Parenting 7.91-19.28*** 2 Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Irritability .02-4.21 2 Behavioral Irritability X Negative Parenting 1.15-9.28 2 Pretask Cortisol X Negative Parenting .08-6.41 2 Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Irritability X Negative Parenting 4.77-13.25** 2

Page 65: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

57

Table 8 Parameter Estimates for the Multinomial Logistic Regression Associating Children’s Biological

Stress Responsivity, Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation, and Parenting Styles at 24 Months of Age to

Conduct Problem Subgroups.

Contrasts β SE OR CI COMPARISON vs CP

Maternal Education Achievementa -.14* .06 .87 .78-.98

Cortisol Reactivity across Emotion Tasksb -.27 .13 .83 .76-1.12 Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Approach X Sensitive Parenting

.48** .16 1.64 1.21-2.19

COMPARISON vs COMORBID

Maternal Education Achievementa -.22** .07 .80 .74-.92 Pretask Cortisol X Behavioral Irritability X Intrusive Parenting

-.49** .19 .68 .52-.94

COMORBID vs COMPARISON

No significant differences † p<.08, *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. a Years of school completed.

Note: The COMPARISON subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMPARISON vs CP’ and ‘COMPARISON vs COMORBID’ contrasts. The

COMORBID subgroup is the referent group in the ‘COMORBID vs CP’ contrast.

At 24 months of age, primarily three-way interactions among pretask cortisol levels, affective

behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles were associated with significant differences in

developing CP versus COMPARISON subgroup status (see Table 7). Specifically, interrelationships among

pretask cortisol levels, social approach behaviors, and sensitive parenting significantly predicted conduct

problem subgroup differences (Wald χ2 (42, 506)=7.91-19.28, p<.001). The moderating effects of pretask

cortisol levels and approach tendencies on sensitive parenting at 24 months of age were associated with

differences in developing CP and COMPARISON subgroup status (β=.48, SE=.16, p=.005, OR=1.64,

CI=1.21-2.19; see Table 7).

Plots of interactions revealed that toddlers who exhibit high pre-task cortisol levels as well as

high approach behaviors (top 25th percentile for both variable; n=29) had approximately a 7% increase in

Page 66: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

58

their probability of becoming a member of the CP versus COMPARISON (see Figure 7). Children who

were low on both pretask cortisol levels and social approach behaviors (bottom 25th percentile for both

variables; n=24) or had uncoordinated pretask cortisol levels and approach tendencies (high or low on

either) showed a decreased probability of developing later CP with increased maternal sensitivity

exposure. For instance, children with low pretask cortisol levels and high approach tendencies (bottom

25th percentile for both variable; n=38) had nearly a 30% decrease in risk for developing membership

into the CP subgroup as the maternal sensitivity increased from its lowest to highest levels.

Additionally, at 24-months, the interrelationships among pretask cortisol levels, irritability, and

intrusive parenting significantly predicted conduct problem subgroup differences (Wald χ2 (42,

506)=4.77-13.25, p=.009). The moderating effects of children’s pretask cortisol levels and irritability on

intrusive parenting at 24 months of age was associated with differences in developing COMORBID

versus COMPARISON subgroup status (β=-.49, SE=.16, p=.007, OR=.68, CI=.52-.94; see Table 8).

Figure 8 demonstrates that children (n=20) with low pretask cortisol levels (bottom 25th

percentile) and high behavioral displays of irritability (top 25th percentile) had a significant increase in

the probability (from 80% to nearly 100%) of having COMORBID versus COMPARISON subgroup status

when maternal intrusive behaviors increased from minimum to maximum exposure levels. In contrast,

children low on both pretask cortisol levels and behavioral display of irritability (both are the bottom

25th percentile, n=35) had significant decreases in probability (from 48% to nearly 20%) of developing

COMORBIS subgroup status as maternal intrusive behaviors increased. There was no to little change in

children who showed high levels of pretask cortisol with high or low levels of irritability in the probability

of developing COMORBID subgroup status in relation to changes in maternal sensitivity exposure.

Brief Summary of Findings for Aim 2

Table 10 depicts a summary of all main and interaction effects by children’s age (7, 15, and 24

months of age) and by subgroup membership (CP, COMORBID, versus COMPARISON subgroups). Within

Page 67: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

59

the table “↑” indicates that high levels of the given risk variable are characteristic of this subgroup;

whereas, “↓” indicates that low levels of the given risk factor are characteristic of this risk factor. When

the term “with” is seen after a given risk factor this means that there was an interaction with the

subsequent risk factor. For example, this table shows that in late infancy children with low levels of

observed irritability are more likely to be members of the COMPARISON Subgroup as exposure to

sensitive parenting increased.

Page 68: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

60

Figure 7 Interrelationships among Children’s HPA Activity, Approach

Tendencies, and Sensitive Parenting at 24 Months of Age Predicting CP

Subgroup Membership. Children with high pre-task cortisol levels and

high approach tendencies (n=29) had a greater probability of developing

CP versus COMPARISON subgroup status as exposure levels of maternal

sensitivity increased. In contrast, children with low-low or mixed levels of

pretask cortisol levels were significantly less likely to develop CP

subgroup status as maternal sensitivity level increased.

Figure 8 Interrelationships among Children’s HPA Activity, Irritability,

and Intrusive Parenting at 24 Months of Age Predicting COMORBID

Subgroup Membership. Children with low pretask cortisol levels and

high irritability (n=20) had a greater probability of developing

COMORBID versus COMPARISON subgroup status as their exposure to

intrusive parenting increased. Alternatively, children with low pretask

cortisol levels and low irritability (n=35) had a significantly lower

probability of developing COMORBID subgroup status as the levels of

intrusive parenting increased.

*Note: High and low cutoffs of the moderating variables (pre-task cortisol levels, social approach, and irritability) were formed at +/- 1SD above the mean.

Page 69: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

61

Table 10 Summary of Risk Factors across Early Childhood that Characterize the Development of Conduct Problem Subgroups at 5 Years of Age.

COMPARISON Subgroup (n=370) CP Subgroup (n=72) COMORBID Subgroup (n=64)

7 months of age

------

↑ Pretask Cortisol Levels

↑ Cortisol Reactivity with ↑ Intrusive Parenting

Moderate Pretask Cortisol Levels

15 months of age

↓ Irritability with ↑ Sensitive Parenting

↑ Attention with ↑ Sensitive Parenting

↑ Approach Tendencies with ↑ Sensitive Parenting

↑ Cortisol Reactivity with ↑ Approach Tendencies

↑ Pretask Cortisol Levels with ↑ Intrusive Parenting

24 months of age

↑ Cortisol Reactivity during Emotional Tasks

↓ Cortisol Reactivity during Emotional Tasks

↑ Pretask Cortisol Levels with ↑ Approach Tendencies with ↑ Sensitive Parenting

↓ Pretask Cortisol Levels with ↑ Irritability with ↑ Intrusive Parenting

Note: ↑ indicates high levels of the given risk variable are characteristic of this subgroup; whereas, ↓ indicated low levels of the given risk factor. When the term “with” is in the

bullet point this means that there was an interaction with the subsequent risk factor.

Page 70: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

62

Chapter 4

Discussion

In an attempt to unlock the heterogeneity of externalizing problems, the present study explored

how children’s biological stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and exposure to

different parenting styles in early childhood contribute to the development of CP, COMORBID, and

COMPARISON subgroups. In general, findings demonstrated that the interrelationships among these

three domains of risk at key developmental time points in early childhood had greater explanatory

power than the change or stability of any one risk factor alone. In fact, out of the eight major risk factors

assessed, only changes of maternal intrusiveness and behavioral irritability explained children’s

membership into conduct problem subgroups at 5 years of age.

Among the many complex interrelationships among risk domains that significantly predicted the

conduct problem subgroups (see Table 10 for summary), some lent evidentiary support to, while others

refuted, the initial conceptual model proposed in Figure 1. To explicate, as broadly theorized (see left

side of the blue box in Figure 1), the interplay between heightened biological stress responsivity and

adverse parenting styles at 7 months of age did significantly discern whether children would develop CP,

COMORBID, or COMPARISON subgroup membership. However, counter to expectations and findings

from other empirical investigations (e.g., Bates & Bayle, 1988; Bates et al., 1998; review, Putnam,

Sanson, & Rothbart,2002), behavioral reactivity did not reciprocally interact with parenting styles to

significantly predict conduct problems subgroups. By 15 months of age, as speculated (see center of the

blue box in Figure 1), the behavioral reactivity and regulation domain of risk was involved in the majority

of interrelationships associated with the development of conduct problem subgroups. Finally, findings at

24 months of age (see right side of the blue box in Figure 1) were in-line with theoretical expectations

that multiple and reciprocal exchanges among risk factors by the toddlerhood and pre-school years

Page 71: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

63

becomes the primary predictors of deleterious outcomes (Sameroff, 1990; Shaw et al., 1994). In

particular, the current study revealed that behavioral reactivity and regulation moderated the reciprocal

interactions between HPA activity (heightened and blunted) and parenting styles at 24 months of age to

distinguish the development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups.

Stability and Change in Developmental Risk Predicting Conduct Problem Subgroups

Patterns of stability and change in maternal sensitivity and behavioral displays of irritability

across 7, 15, and 24 months distinguished the development of conduct problem subgroups in the

present study. While all subgroups began with similar levels of observed intrusive parenting at 7 months

of age, higher instability in intrusive parenting was observed from 15 to 24 months of age among

children with conduct problems (with and without anxiety). In fact, children who were members of the

CP and COMORBID subgroups demonstrated significant increases in their levels of intrusive parenting

exposure, on average, versus the COMPARISON subgroup from infancy to toddlerhood. As previously

reviewed, negative parenting styles such as detachment and hostility tend to show instability over the

first 6 years of life (e.g., Dallaire & Weinrab, 2005). The inconsistency in this type of adverse parenting

style has been linked to aggressive and antisocial behaviors in middle childhood and adolescence (e.g.,

Dodge & McCourt, 2010). Yet, this study is unique because it is the first to demonstrate that increased

exposure to intrusive parenting styles even in early childhood is characteristic of children who have

conduct problems with or without comorbid anxiety.

Additionally, significant between-group differences revealed themselves in the level of

irritability displayed across early childhood. Children who were members of the CP subgroup at age 5

revealed consistently lower levels of observed irritability across infancy and toddlerhood, while children

in the COMORBID subgroup showed higher levels of irritability in late infancy and those levels remained

high at 24 months of age. This finding lends credence to the hypothesis that, in early infancy, observed

irritability is similar for children who later develop conduct problems with and without anxiety, but

Page 72: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

64

children with comorbid anxiety may encounter increased negative social interactions with families and

peers as well as higher deficits in self-regulation skills, resulting in increases in observed irritability over

time (e.g., Boivin et al., 1994; Farmer et al., 2002; Milich & Landau, 1984).

Interrelationships of Risk Domains Underlying the Development of Conduct Problem Subgroups

As an initial note, findings from this unique sample further support the extant research by

demonstrating that low socioeconomic status and parent educational achievement may increase the risk

of early-onset conduct problems both with and without co-occurring anxiety (McLoyd, 1990; Offord,

Boyle, & Racine, 1991; West & Farrington, 1973). Moreover, males were more likely to exhibit conduct

problems without comorbid anxiety symptoms (see Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff, & Marceau, 2008). The

current study also provides empirical substantiation that individual to contextual domains of risk

influence the development of externalizing problems above and beyond sociocultural risk (e.g.,

Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; McCloyd, 1990; Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002).

More notably, this investigation indicated that the interrelationships of risk domains—biological

stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles—with the most predictive

validity varies at different developmental time points in infancy and toddlerhood when distinguishing

membership into conduct problems subgroups. This suggests that repeated displays of risk factors may

not be the only or most important way to examine risk factors when predicting later behavioral

problems, and examining risk domains that have different meanings and importance at key

developmental times in infancy and toddlerhood may help to discern the mechanisms underlying

externalizing problems with and without comorbid anxiety.

Biological Stress Responsivity and Parenting Styles. Ultimately, the findings from this empirical

investigation does not lend empirical support to theories that elicit a straightforward biological stress

responsivity distinction between children with conduct problems only having blunted biological stress

activity and those with comorbid anxiety having heightened biological stress reactivity (e.g., Hodgins et

Page 73: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

65

al., 2009; Steiben et al., 2007). Instead, children’s HPA activity and its interrelationship between

behavioral and parenting domains of risk at different developmental time points were central to

differentiating whether children would develop into CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups.

Counter to expectations, heightened biological stress responsivity interacted with intrusive

parenting to characterize both the CP and COMORBID subgroups versus the COMPARISON subgroup.

The subset of children with heightened HPA reactivity to the emotion-eliciting tasks at 7 months

increased their likelihood of becoming a member of the CP subgroup as their exposure to intrusive

parenting levels increased. On the other hand, children with heightened pretask HPA levels (which could

represent "trait"-like or anticipatory stress, as discussed in the limitation section below) at 15 months of

age had a greater likelihood of developing COMORBID subgroup membership as their exposure to

intrusive parenting increased. Jointly, these results imply that children with conduct problems with and

without comorbid anxiety are likely to be biologically sensitive to contextual stressors during infancy,

particularly adverse parenting styles. Since heightened HPA responsivity are speculated as being

especially sensitive to their contexts (Del Giudice et al., 2010; Hudson & Rapee, 2001; Rogosch &

Cicchetti, 1994), exposure to the addition source of stress such as intrusive parenting may further

exacerbate their overidentification of hostile cues and overreactions to mild or ambiguous stressors

(Dodge, 1991). Over time, such reciprocal interactions between children's biological stress responsivity

and maternal intrusiveness may result in more externalizing behaviors when trying to cope with

environments that are perceived as challenging and/or threatening.

Further, biological stress responsivity alone was able to distinguish the development of CP,

COMORBID, versus COMPARISON subgroups. At 7 months of age, on average, greater HPA pretask levels

significantly predicted CP versus COMORBID subgroup membership at 5 years of age. Conversely, at

children’s 24 month assessment, on average, blunted HPA reactivity to emotion-eliciting tasks was

associated with CP membership, while heightened HPA reactivity predicted COMPARISON subgroup

Page 74: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

66

membership. This was counter to what was expected—the COMORBID subgroup did not have

heightened stress responsivity versus the CP and COMPARSIONS subgroups, further reinforcing that

mechanisms underlying these subgroups are comprised of various domains of risk that may manifest

themselves in particular ways depending on the developmental time frame.

As previously discussed, two developmental pathways are speculated as leading to the

development of conduct problems without anxiety (review, Del Giudice et al., 2010). The more recently

theorized pathway consists of an initial propensity for heightened biological stress responsivity in

response to threat and challenge with developmental shifts towards blunted biological stress

responsivity when environmental contexts are plagued by chronic stressors (Del Giudice et al., 2010;

Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006; Gustafsson et al., 2010; Susman, 2006; Tarullo & Gunnar, 2006). This

attenuation from heightened to blunted arousal is thought to occur in middle childhood or adolescence,

but the exact timing remains unknown. Although not specifically tested within each individual in the

current study, the findings highlight that the downregulation of HPA reactivity for the subgroup of

children who develop early conduct problems may occur as early as toddlerhood. Specifically,

heightened HPA reactivity to challenges with increased exposure to intrusive parenting for children at 7

months had the most explanatory power for differentiating whether children will develop membership

into the CP versus COMPARISON subgroup, but blunted HPA reactivity alone at 24 months predicted CP

versus COMPARISON subgroup membership. However, this trend may be specific to this high-risk

population, since intrusive parenting also demonstrated significant increases from infancy to

toddlerhood in the CP and COMORBID subgroups. This increased exposure to adverse parenting styles

may be indicative of severe and chronic stressors in the familial context, resulting in a faster attenuation

in stress system functioning from primarily heightened to blunted biological stress responsivity.

The alternative pathway that researchers theorize for the CP subgroup is thought to occur in all

contexts regardless of the level and severity of chronic stressors and involves a robust genetic

Page 75: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

67

predisposition to blunted biological stress responsivity even in early infancy and continuing into

adulthood (Del Giudice et al., 2010; Hawes et al., 2009; Raine, 2002; Raine et al., 1997). Although the

children in the CP subgroup did not demonstrate this biological stress responsivity profile on average, a

post-hoc investigation revealed that there is a very small subset of children within this subgroup who

exhibited consistently blunted cortisol levels across 7, 15, and 24 months of age. It should also be noted

that these consistently blunted HPA levels did not exist in the COMORBID or COMPARISON subgroups.

Taken together, this analysis and the post-hoc investigation calls for a within person differences

investigation of the stability of blunted or downregulation in biological stress responsivity to help further

explain the existence of within CP subgroup heterogeneity. Moreover, future analysis should examine

whether subgroups of children with conduct problems defined according to these distinct biological

stress responsivity patterns—attenuated HPA responsivity versus consistently blunted HPA

responsivity—may display disparate interrelationships between “loosely” coupled behaviors and

parenting styles in predicting later mental health outcomes. This is especially important because

researchers believe these two divergent stress responsivity pathways may have very different

implications for later prevention strategies (Del Giudice et al., 2010; O’Neal et al., 2010). A detailed

discussion of these prevention implications is presented later.

Behavioral Reactivity and Regulation as a Key Risk Domain. The findings from the current

investigations highlight that behavioral reactivity and regulation at 15 and 24 months of age becomes

the key intermediate domain of risk for distinguishing membership of children into the early subgroups

of CP, COMORBID, or COMPARISON subgroups. These results are consistent with the view that the

“loose” coupling of children’s biological and behavioral manifestations of vulnerabilities, as well as the

reciprocal interactions of risky children’s behaviors and parenting styles, in early childhood are essential

in determining whether children will develop later conduct problems (Bates & Bayle, 1988; Bates et al.,

1998; Putnam et al., 2002; Rubin et al., 2003). Overall, these findings underscore that after the first year

Page 76: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

68

of life, risky behaviors are a key factor in the developmental mechanisms underlying conduct problem

outcomes.

For example, high levels of approach tendencies—a potential precursor of elevated sensation-

seeking tendencies (Beauchaine et al., 2001; Quay, 1993)—were associated with CP and COMORBID

subgroups. It is not surprising that this relation emerged after the first year of life when children have

increased mobility and therefore greater opportunity to display approach behaviors. Unexpectedly,

however, this relation was most evident in children exposed to responsive and sensitive parenting for

the CP subgroup. Previous research has shown that the propensity to approach novel and potentially

stressful situations is more evident in securely attached children whose mothers sensitively respond to

their children’s needs (e.g., Ainsworth et al., 1979; Bowlby, 1969). It may be that sensitive parenting

style fosters children’s approach tendencies, allowing them to have greater opportunities to engage in

aggressive and other externalizing behaviors associated with later conduct problems. Alternatively,

children who display high levels of approach to potentially stressful and novel situations may evoke

increases in maternal engagement to help ensure their safety.

In contrast, for the children who became members of the COMORBID subgroup the

interrelationships of biological stress responsivity and social approach tendencies had the most

predictive validity at 15 months of age. In particular, children with heightened HPA reactivity to stressors

showed an increased probability for developing COMORBID subgroup membership as their observed

social approach tendencies increased. This finding may suggest that high approach tendencies in these

children increase their likelihood of encountering social interactions wherein they become too

physiologically aroused to enact effortful inhibition or executive processing, resulting in behaviors

associated with reactive aggression and other externalizing behaviors (Hubbard et al., 2002; Tyson,

1998; van Goozen et al., 1998). With increased negative experiences when socially interacting, these

children may become more anxious about engaging with other family members and/or peers (e.g.,

Page 77: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

69

Eisenburg et al., 2009; Farmer et al., 2002; Keiley et al., 2003; Nigg, 2006). In general, the moderating

effect of biological stress responsivity may explain why there are inconsistencies in the literature on the

relationship between approach tendencies and comorbid internalizing and externalizing problems in

children (Johnson et al., 2003; Keiley et al., 2003; Oldenhinkel et al., 2004).

Moreover, at the 15 month assessment, certain behavioral reactivity and regulation factors

significantly enhanced the relationship between positive parenting styles and membership into the

COMPARISON subgroup at 5 years old. Specifically, children with low irritability and high levels of

sustained attention had greater than 90% probabilities of being in the COMPARISON subgroup when

exposed to only moderate levels of sensitive parenting. These findings highlight that mastery of

behaviors of reactivity and regulation in infancy as a means of coping with environmental stressors is

crucial for mitigating risk for developing conduct problems with comorbid anxiety (e.g., Calkins, 2007).

Additionally, similar to prior empirical investigations (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Steinberg et al., 2009), the

degree of children's attention regulation difficulties in early childhood was similar between the

COMORBID and CP subgroups within this high-risk population.

Complex Interplay of All Domains of Risk by Toddlerhood. In alignment with multiple theoretical

models of developmental processes (e.g., Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998; Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994;

Rutter, 1990; Sameroff & Chandler, 1975; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984), these findings suggest that as children

gain more experiences with proximal contexts and acquire behavioral skills to manipulate their

environmental interactions, the interrelationships among multiple domains of risk will have increasing

predictive value for the development of negative mental health outcomes. In fact, by 24 months of age,

it was primarily the interrelationships among all three domains of risk that was associated with the

development of CP and COMORBID subgroups versus the COMPARISON subgroup. Specifically, children

with high pretask HPA activity along with greater demonstrations of approach tendencies were more

likely to develop CP subgroup membership as exposure levels of sensitive and responsive parenting

Page 78: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

70

increased. This combination of high approach tendencies and heightened HPA activity may result in

more opportunities to engage in problem behaviors of aggression and rule breaking. That is, heightened

activation of the HPA makes it more difficult for children to enact self-regulation skills and executive

processing (e.g., McBurnett et al., 1998). If this biological propensity is compounded with a tendency to

approach more situations which may require these regulation skills in interacting with others, then the

probability using aggression or antisocial behaviors to cope with the situation may be amplified.

Ultimately, the coupling of these two biological and behavioral risk factors may require more maternal

engagement and sensitivity as an attempt to maintain their children’s well being and mitigate behavior

problems (e.g., Rutter, 2007; Rutter & Silberg, 2002; Rutter et al., 1997; Hershberger, 1994).

Alternatively, children with low pretask HPA levels and high displays of irritability in toddlerhood

increased children’s probabilities for developing COMORBID subgroup membership as exposure levels to

intrusive parenting increased. There are many potential explanations for this unexpected finding; two

possibilities are discussed here. First, the blunted biological stress levels may indicate that these children

have reduced sensitivity to punishment (Beauchaine, 2001; Corr, 2004; Gray, 1987; review, van Goozen

et al., 2007); therefore, they are unable to learn that their behaviors of irritability may evoke increases

in intrusive parenting (Damasio, 2000). The increased negative interactions over time may lead to

increased bouts of aggression and specific forms of anxiety problems that may be more likely to be

associated with blunted HPA activity including post traumatic stress disorder (review, Elhert, Gaab, &

Heinrichs, 2001).

Second, and a more likely explanation in this author’s opinion, is that stress reactivity may

become more difficult to elicit as children get older due to increased behavioral coping mechanisms,

habituation to similar stress, and/or a potential stress hyporesponsive period in early childhood (review,

Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, 2009). In the current study, on average, children did not exhibit a

HPA response to the emotional tasks during the 24-month assessment. Together with the notion that

Page 79: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

71

children with low HPA activity have a greater propensity to exhibit a cortisol response to a subsequent

stressor via the law of initial values—i.e., the existence of a negative correlation between initial level

and the magnitude of change in reaction to a stressor (e.g., Ramsay & Lewis, 2003) – this implies that

children with these low pretask cortisol levels may be prone to heightened biological stress reactivity.

This heightened HPA activity may be associated with high irritability, and there is strong theoretical and

empirical support for intrusive parenting being part of the developmental processes underlying conduct

problems and comorbid anxiety in early childhood (Boivin et al., 1994; Farmer et al., 2002; Milich &

Landau, 1984). Therefore, with a more effective stressor, one may speculate that these children would

have displayed heightened stress reactivity in toddlerhood, which would have interacted with high

levels of irritability and intrusive parenting to influence the later development of co-occurring conduct

problems and anxiety. However, to verify or negate this conjecture, future studies with more effective

stressors in toddlerhood should be performed.

Prevention Implications

Many prevention and intervention programs that focus on mitigating risk for the development

of externalizing behaviors in childhood target multiple domains of risk, including adverse parenting

styles and risky behaviors (e.g., CPPRG, 1999; Olds, 2006; Webster-Stratton, 2000, 2005; Webster-

Stratton, Reid, & Hammond, 2001). Efficacy trials from a number of caregiver-focused interventions that

promote warm and consistent parental involvement, as well as children’s acquisition of appropriate

affective behavioral skills, have demonstrated decreases in aggressive behaviors during childhood and

adolescence (e.g., Dishion & Patterson, 1996; Old, 2002; Webster-Stratton, 1998), even in families with

high levels of accumulated risk (Hart & Risley, 1995; Lengua, 2002). Yet, in all studies, a significant

portion of children with conduct problems do not demonstrate therapeutic responses, and many

improvements are not maintained over long intervals (e.g., Forehand & Long, 1986; Webster-Stratton,

1990).

Page 80: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

72

Researchers who currently design and implement prevention programs for conduct problems

may desire a continued focus on improving all components of parenting practices and children’s

emotional reactivity and self-regulation skills. Yet, findings from the current study suggest that it may be

more efficacious to target the specific parenting and behavioral risk factors at key development time

points that possess the most explanatory power regarding the later development of conduct problems

with and without comorbid anxiety in early childhood. Additionally, the current study provides initial

support for the suggestion that incorporating the assessment of biological vulnerabilities to stress

responsivity along with the behavioral and parenting domains of risk may be critical for designing more

efficacious interventions in early childhood to meet the unique needs of children who may develop only

conduct problems compared to those who develop comorbid anxiety (e.g., Beauchaine et al., 2008;

Hodgins, 2007; van Goozen & Fairchild, 2008).

In this high-risk sample, results indicate that prevention programs in early infancy may benefit

from determining whether children have heightened biological stress responsivity. Heightened HPA

stress responsivity in infancy was associated with the later development of CP subgroup membership.

To prevent the attenuation of HPA reactivity that is proposed to transpire as a result of chronic exposure

to stress in the CP subgroup (e.g., Del Giudice et al., 2010; Susman, 2006), the current study advocates

that prevention programs focus on the consistent reduction of intrusive parenting styles starting in early

infancy. Researchers have speculated that, because of the heightened biological sensitivity to context in

these infants, changes to environmental conditions over prolonged periods of time (e.g., through the

reduction of exposure to intrusive parenting styles; Adam et al., 2007; Gunnar & Donzella, 2002; Teicher

et al., 2003) may prevent blunted HPA stress responsivity and/or allow their systems to revert back to

more moderate or heightened HPA functioning (Del Giudice et al., 2010). The possibility that this

reversion of biological stress responsivity may result in a reduction in aggressive behaviors was clearly

demonstrated in a recent experimental intervention (O’Neal et al., 2010). Specifically, children who

Page 81: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

73

changed from blunted to heightened cortisol responsivity in anticipation of social stressors showed

lower levels of aggressive behaviors during middle childhood, while those children who showed blunted

patterns pre- and post-intervention continued to demonstrate high levels of externalizing behaviors.

As children progress into later infancy, these results suggest sensitive and responsive parenting

may reciprocally exacerbate childrens’ propensity to engage in high social approach (sensation-seeking

behaviors), ultimately leading to CP subgroup membership at age 5. It does not seem reasonable to

decrease parenting styles of warmth and sensitivity. However, these findings may suggest that

prevention programs after the second year of life may need to focus on consistent limit-setting

parenting practices to decrease the amount of approach behaviors potentially related to greater

displays of externalizing behaviors (e.g., Bates et al., 1998). Future research that explores how parenting

practices interrelate with approach tendencies in early childhood to influence later conduct problems,

with and without comorbid anxiety, is warranted to determine the validity of such speculations.

Disparate from children who develop CP subgroup membership, the present study’s findings

indicate that preventative interventions for early childhood displays of co-occurring conduct problems

and anxiety would be most beneficial when targeting key developmental time points in later infancy and

toddlerhood. As previously discussed, children who display heightened biological stress responsivity

along with high approach tendencies may possess deficits in effortful inhibitory control or executive

processing (Hubbard et al., 2002; Tyson, 1998; van Goozen et al., 1998). Therefore, preventative

interventions may need to focus on enhancing children’s self-regulation skills and parents’ modeling of

appropriate emotional reactivity and regulation. In later infancy and toddlerhood, as with children who

are in the CP subgroup, infants who develop COMORBID subgroup membership display heightened

biological stress responsivity to intrusive parenting styles. Prevention programs should focus on the

reduction of intrusive parenting to decrease contextual stressors, which may help avert the further up-

regulation of their biological stress responsivity across development. This may ultimately aid in reducing

Page 82: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

74

the limbic system’s dominance over higher-order prefrontal systems under perceived threat, and

decreasing associated externalizing behavior problems.

Concluding Remarks on Prevention Implications. High attrition rates of families are thought to

partially explain the lack of intervention efficacy (Kaminski, Stormshak, Good, & Goodman, 2002; Reid &

Eddy, 2002). Focusing on selective biological vulnerabilities, affective behavioral risks, and adverse

parenting styles targeting key developmental time points may 1) ensure that parents and children who

do not complete the entire intervention get the information that targets their children’s specific

vulnerabilities and related risk factors, and 2) increase the likelihood of an immediate response (or

improvement) from the child, thereby reinforcing both the parents’ and children’s utilization of specific

techniques and their continued participation in the prevention program.

Limitations and Future Analyses

The inferences of the present study must be tempered by limitations associated with the

sample, subgroup identification, and other study procedures and measures. First, the non-urban,

predominantly low-income, White and Black children in the sample are an underrepresented population

in the developmental literature (Dill, 2001; Morris & Monroe, 2009). Although utilizing this population

adds to the developmental field’s knowledge base regarding this often neglected set of children and

families, there is limited generalizability beyond this population in the conclusions drawn from the

current analyses. Therefore, future analyses are needed with a more nationally representative sample of

both urban and non-urban children of different ethnicities from larger geographical regions to

determine if similar findings are revealed.

While conduct problem subgroups were identified utilizing a reliable and validated parent-

report measure of conduct problems and anxiety at 5 years of age (Goodman et al., 2000), there were

several limitations to the identification procedure. First, even though parent reports have been

successful in determining whether children show behavior symptoms associated with DSM IV criteria,

Page 83: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

75

the use of both parent and teacher reports of children’s behavioral outcomes at 5 years of age could

have reduced informant bias and led to more accurate assessments of these problems. Additionally, the

longitudinal design of the study, which allowed for the investigation of precursors in infancy and

toddlerhood for the identification of subgroups of children three years later, is an exceptional

contribution to the literature. However, children who demonstrate conduct problems with and without

anxiety may not demonstrate these patterns by early adolescence (Broidy et al., 2003; Loeber et al.,

1991; Moffitt, 1993; Patterson et al., 1992). In fact, a small portion of children (4-11%) demonstrated

this stability in externalizing problems from early childhood to adolescence. Thus, in order to identify

subgroups with more homogenous underlying biological vulnerabilities, these groups may need to be

formed in adolescence. Future investigations should identify children who have conduct problems with

and without comorbid anxiety using data from multiple informants during adolescence to determine

whether similar biological vulnerabilities, risky behaviors, and adverse parenting styles in early

childhood (and their interrelationships) predict the development of these subgroups.

Furthermore, the findings from this study demonstrate that there may still be a great deal of

within- and between-subgroup heterogeneity regarding precursors of these conduct problem

subgroups. For example, heightened biological stress responsivity and intrusive parenting interact to

predict the development of conduct problems with and without comorbid anxiety. Additionally, there

are potentially two distinct biological phenotypes – attenuation of HPA reactivity and consistently

blunted HPA activity – underlying the CP subgroup (Del Giudice et al., 2010; Susman, 2006). Thus, the

utilization of a bottom-up approach, wherein neurophysiological markers of stress responsivity in

infancy and toddlerhood informs the creation of distinct conduct problem subgroups (Gatzke-Kopp et

al., 2010), seems warranted in future analyses.

Pretask cortisol levels may represent a more “trait”-like indicator of heightened biological stress

responsivity or, as an alternative explanation, an anticipatory stress response (review, Kudielka,

Page 84: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

76

Hellhammer, & Wust, 2009). To determine whether the response was a “true” baseline or simply

anticipatory stress reactivity, children’s habituation to the same (or similar) stressors over multiple days

or weeks would have to be assessed. Determining whether the pretask level of HPA activity was “trait”-

like could be helpful when interpreting whether the interaction between heightened biological stress

responsivity and parenting styles differed for children in the CP subgroup (whose heightened HPA

activity may reflect HPA reactivity to the stressor) compared to those in the COMORBID subgroup

(whose heightened HPA activity may reflect high levels of anticipatory stress). Unfortunately, this is

beyond the scope of these analyses. Future research determining trait-like versus state-like biological

vulnerabilities across early childhood may be helpful in disentangling the developmental mechanisms

underlying conduct problems with and without comorbid anxiety.

As mentioned previously, the emotion-inducing tasks given to children at the 24-month

assessment failed to, on average, successfully elicit a biological stress response. This also led to

decreased variability in HPA reactivity in the population. Thus, the current study should be replicated

with a stress task that elicits greater variability in HPA reactivity in order to more accurately determine

how biological vulnerabilities in stress response relate to other domains of risk to influence the

development of conduct problem subgroups.

Finally, poor parenting practices such as inconsistent reinforcement and application of

consequences (e.g., Beauchaine & Neuhaus, 2008); limited modeling of empathy (Fowles & Dindo, 2006;

Hawes & Dadds, 2005); as well as deficits in cognitive functioning such as poor inhibitory control (e.g.,

Reid & Eddy, 2002) have been associated with biological vulnerabilities and later development of

conduct problems. Thus, additional risk factors in early childhood such as other parenting practices,

maternal depression, and cognitive functioning, should also be assessed as potential moderators of later

conduct problem development in this sample.

Conclusions and Contributions

Page 85: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

77

Researchers often call for an integrative approach, involving the assessment of

interrelationships among biological, behavioral, and parental domains of risk across early childhood, to

determine the developmental mechanisms underlying externalizing problems (Bronfenbrenner &

Morris, 1998; Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994; Rutter, 1990; Sameroff & Chandler, 1975; Shaw, Keenan, &

Vondra, 1994; Sroufe & Rutter, 1984). However, a dearth of empirical investigations utilizing this

approach remains in the developmental literature. Moreover, investigations of how interactions among

these domains of risk at key developmental time points in early childhood predict distinct phenotypes of

children who have conduct problems with and without comorbid anxiety are even more scarce.

The current investigation addressed this research gap and made three contributions to the

extant literature. First, in infancy, this study revealed different developmental pathways related to

children who have conduct problems with and without comorbid anxiety at 5 years of age. Specifically,

children who were characterized by heightened biological stress responsivity and exposed to intrusive

parenting are more likely to develop conduct problems without comorbid anxiety. Second, after the first

year of life, children’s behavioral reactivity and regulation are important in distinguishing the underlying

mechanisms associated with the development of CP, COMORBID, and COMPARISON subgroups at 5

years of age. Lastly, during toddlerhood, interrelationships among all three domains of risk―biological

stress responsivity, behavioral reactivity and regulation, and parenting styles―have the most

explanatory value when predicting the development of conduct problem subgroups. Overall, the current

study is a preliminary step toward explicating how the dynamic and complex interrelationships among

children's biological stress responsivity, children's behavioral reactivity and regulation, and exposure to

parenting styles in infancy and toddlerhood may lead to the development of conduct problems with and

without comorbid anxiety in early childhood. Taken together, these findings provide a preliminary

framework for the design of more targeted and efficacious preventative programs.

Page 86: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

78

REFERENCES

Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Quay, H. C. & Conners, C. K. (1991). National survey of problems

and competencies among four-to sixteen-year-olds: Parents' reports for normative and clinical

samples. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56, 5-120.

Adam, E., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Gunnar, M. R. (2007). Social regulation of the adrenocortical response to

stress in infants, children, and adolescents: implications for psychopathology and education. In

D. Coch, G. Dawson, K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Human Behavior, Learning, and the Developing Brain:

Atypical Development (pp. 264-304). New York: Guilford. 2nd ed.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the

strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bates, J. E., & Bayles, K. (1988). Attachment and the development of behavior problems. In: Belsky, J.;

Nezworski, T., (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 86-101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Ridge, B. (1998). Interaction of temperamental resistance to

control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Developmental

Psychology, 34, 982-995.

Bayley, N. (1969). Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York Psychological

Corporation, New York.

Beauchaine, T. P. (2001). Vagal tone, development, and Gray’s motivational theory: Toward an

integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development

and Psychopathology, 13, 183–214.

Beauchaine, T.P., Katkin, E.S., Strassberg, Z., & Snarr, J. (2001). Disinhibitory psychopathology in male

adolescents: Discriminating conduct disorder from attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

through concurrent assessment of multiple autonomic states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,

110, 610-624.

Page 87: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

79

Beauchaine, T. P., & Neuhaus, E. (2008). Impulsivity and vulnerability to psychopathology. In:

Beauchaine, TP.; Hinshaw, SP., (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology. (pp. 129-156).

Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Beauchaine, T. P., Neuhaus, E., Brenner, S. L., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2008). Ten good reasons to consider

biological processes in prevention and intervention research. Development and

Psychopathology, 20, 745-774.

Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental influence: An evolutionary argument.

Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182-186.

Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: An evolutionary hypothesis and some

evidence. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund Eds., Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology

and child development (pp. 139–163). New York: Guilford Press.

Berkowitz, L., (1993). Aggression, its causes, consequences, and control. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath.

Cognition, 57, 1-29.

Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and

Cognition, 55, 198-208.

Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing

of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 29, 491-498.

Blair, C., Granger, D., Kivlighan, K. T., Mills-Koonce, R., Willoughby, M., Greenberg, MT., Hibel, L. &

Fortunato, C.K. (2008). Maternal and child contributions to cortisol response to emotional

arousal in young children from low-income, rural communities. Developmental Psychology, 44,

1095-1109.

Page 88: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

80

Blair, R.J.R., Peschardt, K.S., Budhani, S., Mitchell, D.G.V., & Pine, D.S. (2006). The development of

psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 262-275.

Broidy, L.M., Nagin, D.S., Tremblay, R.E., Bates, J.E., Brame, B., Dodge, K.A., et al. (2003). Developmental

trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-

national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222-245.

Boivin, M., Poulin, F., & Vitaro, F. (1994). Depressed mood and peer rejection in childhood. Development

and Psychopathology, 6, 483-498.

Bourdon, K.H., Goodman, R., Rae, D.S., Simpson, G., & Koretz, D.S. (2005). The Strengths and

Difficulties Questionnaire: U.S. normative data and psychometric properties. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 557-564.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental

theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17,

271-301.

Bronfenbrenner, U. & Morris, P.A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon (Ed.)

Handbook of Child Psychology (5th ed., pp. 993-1028). New York: Wiley.

Bugental, D. B., Johnson, C., New, M., & Sylvester, J. (1998). Measuring parental attributions:

Conceptual and methodological issues. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 459-480.

Buss, K., & Goldsmith, H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal

dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359-374.

Calkins, S. D. (2007). The emergence of self-regulation: Biological and behavioral control

mechanisms supporting toddler competencies. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.),

Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations (pp.

261 – 284). New York: Guilford.

Page 89: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

81

Campbell, S. B., & von Stauffenberg, C. (2008). Child characteristics and family processes that predict

behavioral readiness for school. In A. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Early disparities in school

readiness: How families contribute to transitions into school (pp. 225-258).Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Capaldi, D., DeGarmo, D., Patterson, G. R., & Forgatch, M. (2002). Contextual risk factors across the

early life span and association with antisocial behavior. In J. B. Reid, R. Gerald, J. Snyder (Eds.)

Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for

intervention (pp. 123-145). Washington: APA.

Caron, C., & Rutter, M. (1991). Comorbidity in children psychopathology: Concepts, issues and research

strategies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1063-1080.

Chrousos, G. P. (1998). Stressors, stress, and neuroendocrine integration of the adaptive response. The

1997 Hans Selye Memorial Lecture. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 851, 311-335.

Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1992). The concepts of stress and stress system disorders: Overview of

physical and behavioral homeostasis. JAMA, 267, 1244-1252.

Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development

and Psychopathology, 6, 533–549.

Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C. M. (2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in

modern missing-data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330-351.

Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (1999). Initial impact of the Fast Track prevention trial for

conduct problems: I. The high-risk sample. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67,

631-647.

Corr, P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral

Reviews, 28, 317-332.

Page 90: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

82

Cox, M. J., Paley, B., Burchinal, M., & Payne, C. C. (1999). Marital perceptions and interactions across the

transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage & Family, 61, 611-625.

Crespi, E. J. & Denver, R. J. (2005). Ancient origins of human developmental plasticity. American Journal

of Human Biology, 17, 44-54.

Dallaire, D. H., & Weinraub, M. (2005). Predicting children's separation anxiety at age 6: The

contributions of infant–mother attachment security, maternal sensitivity and maternal

separation anxiety. Attachment & Human Development, 7, 393-408.

Damasio, A. R. (2000). A neural basis of sociopathy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 118-135.

Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation:

Perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 890-909.

Dearing, K. F., Hubbard, J. A., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Relyea, N., & Smithmyer, C. M. (2002).

Children’s self-reports about anger regulation: Direct and indirect links to social preference and

aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 308-336.

Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K.A., Bates, J.E., & Pettit, G.S. (1998). Multiple-risk factors in the

development of externalizing behavior problems: Group and individual differences.

Development and Psychopathology, 10, 469-493.

Del Giudice, M. J., Ellis, B. J., & Shirtcliff, E. A. (2010). The adaptive calibration model of stress

responsivity. Neuroscience & Behavioral Reviews, 35, 1562-1592.

Demaris, A. (1992). Logit modeling: practical applications. Sage University Paper Series on Quantitative

Applications in the Social Sciences, 07-086.Newbury Park, CA.

Derryberry, D. & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of

temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 631-650.

Dill, B. T. (2001). Rediscovering rural America. In Blau, J. R. (Ed.), Blackwell companions to sociology (pp.

196-210). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Page 91: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

83

Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, S. G. (1996). Preventive parenting with love, encouragement, and limits: The

preschool years. Eugene, OR: Castalia.

Dodge, K. A. (1991). The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression. In: Pepler D, Rubin

K, editors. The development and treatment for childhood aggression. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1991.

pp. 201–218.

Dodge, K.A., & Coie, J.D. (1987). Social information processing factors in reactive and proactive

aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-

1158.

Dodge, K.A., Coie, J.D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior in youth. In W. Damon

(Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and

personality development (6th ed., pp. 719-788). New York: Wiley.

Dodge, K. A., & McCourt, S. N. (2010). Translating models of antisocial behavioral development into

efficacious intervention policy to prevent adolescent violence. Developmental Psychobiology,

52(3), 277-285.

Ehlert, U., Gaab, J, & Heinrichs, M. (2001). Psychoneuroendocrinological contributions to the etiology of

depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and stress-related bodily disorders: The role of the

hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Biological Psychology, 57, 141-152.

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., & Reiser, M., (2001). The

relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem

behavior. Child Development. 72, 1112-1134.

Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T.L., Liew, J., Zhou, Q., Losoya, S.H., Reiser, M., & Cumberland, A.

(2009). Longitudinal relations of children’s effortful control, impulsivity, and negative

emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems.

Developmental Psychology, 45, 988-1008.

Page 92: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

84

Eysenck, M.W. (1977). Human memory: Theory, research and individual differences. Oxford: Pergamon.

Farmer, A. D., Bierman, K. L., & CPPRG (2002). Predictors and consequences of aggressive-withdrawn

problem profiles in early grade school. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31,

299-311.

Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Christ, M. A., & Hanson, K. (1990). Familial

risk factors to oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Parental psychopathology and

maternal parenting. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 49-55.

Frick, P. J., & White, S. F. (2008). Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for

developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 49, 359-375.

Fries, E., Hesse, J., Hellhammer, J. & Hellhammer, D. H. (2005). A new view on hypocortisolism.

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 1010-1016.

Forehand, R., & Long, N. (1986). A long-term follow-up of parent training participants, Association for

the Advancement of Behavior Therapy.

Fowles, D.C. (2000). Electrodermal hyporeactivity and antisocial behavior: Does anxiety mediate the

relationship? Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 177-189.

Fowles, D. C., & Dindo, L. (2009). Temperament and psychopathology: A dual pathway model. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 179-183.

Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Ram, N., Fortunato, C. K., & Wadlinger, H. (2010). A neurobiological approach to

addressing the phenotype problem in externalizing behavioral disorders. Annual Convention of

the Association of Psychological Science, Boston, MA.

Gendreau, P.L., & Archer, J. (2005). Hormones and the developmental origins of aggression. In: R. E.

Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 281-306).

New York: Guilford.

Page 93: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

85

Giancola, P., & Tarter, R. (1999). Executive cognitive functioning and risk for substance abuse.

Psychological Science, 10, 203-205.

Glassman, R. B. (1973). Persistence and loose coupling in living systems. Behavioral Science, 18, 83-98.

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586.

Goodman, R., Ford, T., Simmons, H., Gatward, R., & Meltzer, H. (2000). Using the Strengths and

Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample.

British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 534-539.

Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Granic, I., & Lamey, A. V. (2002). Combining dynamic systems and multivariate analyses to compare the

mother–child interactions of externalizing subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30,

265-283.

Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T., Fortunato, C. K., Harmon, A. G., Hibel, L. C., Schwartz, E. B., &

Whembolua, G. L. (2007). Integration of salivary biomarkers into developmental and

behaviorally oriented research: Problems and solutions for collecting specimens. Physiology

Behavior, 92, 583-590.

Granger, D., Weisz, J., & Kauneckis, D. (1994). Neuroendocrine reactivity, internalizing behavior

problems, and control-related cognitions in clinic referred children and adolescents. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 103, 267-276.

Granger, D. A., Weisz, J. R., McCracken, J. T., Ikeda, S. C., & Douglas, P. (1996). Reciprocal influences among

adrenocortical activation, psychosocial processes, and the behavioral adjustment of clinic-referred

children. Child Development, 67, 3250-3262.

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Page 94: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

86

Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., & Speltz, M. (1991). Emotional regulation, self control, and

psychopathology: The role of relationships in early childhood. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.),

Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dysfunction: Rochester symposium on

developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2, pp. 21-66). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P. H., & Thomas, C. W. (2003). Temperament. Infancy

through adolescence. New York: Kluwer Academic.

Gunnar, M. R. & Davis, E. P., (2003). The developmental psychobiology of stress and emotion in early

childhood. In I.B. Weiner (Series Ed.) & R.M. Lerner, M.A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry, (Vol. Eds.).

Handbook of Psychology, Vol. 6, Developmental Psychology (pp. 113-143).

Gunnar, M. R. & Donzella, B. (2002). Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development.

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 27, 199-220.

Gunnar, M. R., & Vazquez, D. M. (2001). Low cortisol and a flattening of expected diurnal rhythm:

potential indices of risk in human development. Developmental Psychopathology, 13, 515-538.

Gustafsson, P. E., Janlert, U., Theorell, T., & Hammarstrom, A. (2010). Life-course socioeconomic

trajectories and diurnal cortisol regulation in adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 613-

623.

Harmon, A. G., Hibel, L. C., Rumyantseva, O., & Granger, D.A. (2007). Measuring salivary cortisol in

studies of child development: Watch out- what goes in may not come out of saliva collection

devices. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 495-500.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American

children. Baltimore: Brookes.

Hawes, D. J., Brennan, J., & Dadds, M. R., (2009). Cortisol, callous-unemotional traits, and

pathways to antisocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22, 357-362.

Hawes, D. J., & Dadds, M. R. (2005). The treatment of conduct problems in children with callous-

Page 95: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

87

unemotional traits. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 737-741.

Hershberger, S. L. (1994). Genotype–environment interaction and correlation. In J. C. DeFries, R. Plomin,

& D. W. Fulker (Eds.), Nature and Nurture During Middle Childhood. (pp. 281–294). Malden, MA:

Blackwell.

Hibel, L. C., Granger, D. A., Kivlighan, K. T., Blair, C., & The Family Life Project Investigators

(2006). Individual differences in salivary cortisol: Effects of common over the counter

and prescription medications in infants and their mothers. Hormones and Behavior, 50,

293-300.

Hinshaw, S. P., & Anderson, C. A. (1996). Conduct and oppositional defiant disorders. In E. J. Mash & R.

A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (pp. 113–149). New York: Guilford Press.

Hodgins, S. (2007). Persistent violent offending: What do we know? Editorial. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 190, s12-s14.

Hodgins, S., de Brito, S., Simonoff, E., Vloet, T., & Viding, E. (2009). Getting the phenotypes right: An

essential ingredient for understanding aetiological mechanisms underlying persistent violence

and developing effective treatments. Frontiers in Behavioral Science, 3, 44-53.

Hosmer, D.W. Jr. & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. John Wiley & Sons, New York.

Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., & Dearing, K. F. (2002).

Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger: Relations to reactive

versus proactive aggression. Child Development, 73, 1101-1118.

Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2001). Parent–child interactions and anxiety disorders: An observational

study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1411-1427.

Johnson, S. L., Turner, R. J., & Iwata, N. (2003). BIS/BAS levels and psychiatric disorder: An

epidemiological study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 25-36.

Joseph, R. (1999). Environmental influences on neural plasticity, the limbic system, emotional

Page 96: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

88

development and attachment: A review. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 29, 189-208.

Kaminski, R. A. , Stormshak, E. A., Good, R. H., & Goodman, M. R. (2002). Prevention of substance abuse

with rural head start children and families: Results of project STAR. Psychology of Addictive

Behaviors, 16 (Supp. 14), S11-S26.

Kazdin, A. E. (1985). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. Homewood, IL: Dorsey

Press.

Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children: Current status and future directions.

Psychological Bulletin, 102, 187-203.

Keiley, M. K., Lofthouse, M., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). Differential risks of covarying

and pure components in mother and teacher reports of externalizing and internalizing behavior

across ages 5 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 267-283.

Kellam, S. G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., Dolan, L. J., Brown, C. H., Mayer, L. S., Rebok,

G. W., Anthony, J. C., Laudolff, J., Edelsohn, G., & Wheeler, L. (1991). Developmental

epidemiologically based preventive trials: Baseline modeling of early target behaviors

and depressive symptoms. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 563-584.

Khan, A. A., Jacobson, K. C., Gardner, C. O., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2005). Personality and

comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 190-196.

Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children:

Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68, 94-112.

Kochanska, G., Murray, K. & Coy, K. C. (1997). Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience in

childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 68, 263-277.

Kochanska, G., Tjebkes, T. L. & Forman, D. R. (1998). Children's emerging regulation of conduct:

Restraint, compliance, and internalization from infancy to the second year. Child Development,

69, 1378-1389.

Page 97: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

89

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., & Wust, S. (2009). Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing

determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34,

2–18.

Lahey, B. B., & Loeber, R. (1994). Framework for a developmental model of oppositional defiant

disorder and conduct disorder. In: Disruptive Behavior Disorders in Childhood, Routh DK, ed.

New York: Plenum, pp 139-180.

Lengua, L. J. (2002). The contribution of emotionality and self-regulation to the understanding of

children’s response to multiple risk. Child Development, 73,144-161.

Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. S. (1999). Effect of maternal soothing on infant stress response. Child

Development, 70, 11-21.

Lewis, M., & Thomas. D. (1990). Cortisol release in infants in response to inoculation. Child Development,

61, 50-59.

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1991). Differences and similarities

between children, mothers, and teachers as informants on disruptive child behavior. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 75 -95.

Loney, B. R., Butler, M. A., Lima, E. N., Counts, C. A., & Eckel, L. A. (2006). The relation between salivary

cortisol, callous-unemotional traits, and conduct problems in an adolescent non-referred

sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 30-36.

Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan

on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 434-445.

MacDonald, K. B., (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing, and the regulation of human evolved

predispositions. Psychological Review, 115, 1012-1031.

McBurnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Frick, P. F., Risch, C., Loeber, R., & Hart, E. L. (1991). Anxiety, inhibition, and

conduct disorder in children: II. Relation to salivary cortisol. Journal of American Academy of

Page 98: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

90

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 192-196.

McBurnett, K., Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., & Loeber, R. (2000). Low salivary cortisol and persistent

aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 38-43.

McBurnett, K., Swanson, J. M., Pfiffner, L. J., & Tamm, L. (1997). A measure of ADHD-related classroom

impairment based on targets for behavioral intervention. Journal of Attention Disorders, 2, 69-

76.

McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological

distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311-346.

Milich, R., & Dodge, K. A., (1984). Social information processing deficits in child psychiatric populations.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 471-490.

Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course persistent antisocial behavior: A

developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674-701.

Morris, J. E., & Monroe, C. R. (2009). Why study the U.S. South? The nexus of race and place in

investigating Black student achievement. Educational Researcher, 38, 21-36.

Nelson, C. A., & Carver, L. (1998). The effects of stress and trauma on brain and memory: a view from

developmental cognitive neuroscience. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 793-809.

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (NICHD ECCRN, 1999). Child care and mother-child interaction

in the first three years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399-1413.

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (NICHD ECCRN, 2005). Predicting individual differences in

attention, memory, and planning in first graders from experiences at home, child care, and

school. Developmental Psychology, 41, 99-114.

Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 47, 395-422.

Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., & Racine, Y. A. (1991). The epidemiology of antisocial behavior in childhood

Page 99: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

91

and adolescence. In D. J. Pepler & K. H. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of

childhood aggression (pp. 31–54). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., De Winter, A. F., Veenstra, R., & Ormel, J. (2004). Temperament profiles

associated with internalizing and externalizing problems in preadolescence. Development and

Psychopathology, 16, 421-440.

Olds, D.L. (2002). Prenatal and infancy home visiting by nurses: From randomized trials to community

replication. Prevention Science, 3, 153-172.

Olds, D.L. (2006). The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant

Mental Health Journal, 27, 5-25.

O’Neal, C.R., Brotman, L.M., Huang, K., Gouley, K.K., Kamboukos, D., Calzada, E.J., & Pine, D.S., (2010).

Understanding relations among early family environment, cortisol response, and child

aggression via a prevention experiment. Child Development, 81, 290-305.

Patterson, G.R., Reid, J.B., & Dishion, T.J. (1992). A social learning approach. Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR:

Castalia.

Porges, S.W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system.

International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 123-146.

Poulin, R., & Boivin, M. (2000). Reactive and proactive aggression: Evidence of a two-factor model.

Psychological Assessment, 12, 115-122.

Price, J. M., & Dodge, K. A., (1989). Reactive and proactive aggression in childhood: Relations to peer

status and social context dimensions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 455-471.

Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. In M.

Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255-277).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Quas, J. A., Hong, M., Alkon, A., & Boyce, W. T. (2000). Dissociations between psychobiologic reactivity

Page 100: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

92

and emotional expression in children. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 153-175.

Quay, H. C. (1993). The psychobiology of undersocialized aggressive conduct disorder: a theoretical

perspective. Developmental Psychopathology, 5, 165-180.

Raine, A. (2002). Biosocial studies of antisocial and violent behavior in children and adults: A review.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 311-326.

Raine, A., Venables, P.H., & Mednick, S.A., (1997). Low resting heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to

aggression at age 11 years: evidence from the Mauritius child health project. Journal of

American Academy in Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 1457-1464.

Ramsay, D., & Lewis, M. (2001). Temperament, stress, and soothing. In T. D. Wachs & D. Kohnstamm

(Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 23 – 41). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Reid, J.B., & Eddy, J.M. (2002). Interventions for antisocial behavior: Overview. In J. B. Reid, G. R.

Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental

analysis and model for intervention (pp. 173-192). Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Reid, J. B., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (2002). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A

developmental analysis and model for intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Illustrating the interface of family and peer relations through the

study of child maltreatment. Social Development, 3, 293-308.

Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Executive attention and effortful control: Linking

temperament, brain networks, and genes. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 2-7.

Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley.

Page 101: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

93

Rubin, K., Burgess, K., Dwyer, K., & Hastings, P. (2003). Predicting preschoolers’ externalizing behaviors

from toddler temperament, conflict, and maternal negativity. Developmental Psychology, 39,

164–176.

Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. In: Rolf, J.; Masten, AS.; Cicchetti,

D.; Nuechterlein, KH.; Weintraub, S., (Eds.). Risk and protective factors in the development of

psychopathology (pp. 181-214.) Cambridge; New York.

Rutter, M. (2007). Gene–environment interdependence. Developmental Science, 10, 12-18.

Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., Ormel, J., & Eaves, L. (1997).

Integrating nature and nurture: Implications of person–environment correlations and

interactions for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 335–

364.

Rutter, M. & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral

disturbance. Annual Review in Psychology, 53, 463-490.

Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E. B., Aase, H., & Russell, V. A. (2005). A dynamic developmental theory of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and

combined subtypes. Behavior & Brain Science, 28, 397-419.

Sameroff, A. J. (1990). Prevention of developmental psychopathology using the transactional model:

Perspectives on host, risk agent, and environment interactions. Paper presented at the

Conference on the Present Status and Future Needs of Research on Prevention of Mental

Disorders, Washington, DC.

Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In:

Horowitz, FD.; Hetherington, M.; Scarr-Salapatek, S.; Siegel, G., editors. Review of child

development research (pp. 187-243). University of Chicago Press; Chicago.

Schafer, J. L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London: Chapman & Hall.

Page 102: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

94

Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: our view of the state of the art. Psychological

Methods, 7, 147-77

Shaw, D. S., Keenan, K., & Vondra, J. I. (1994). The developmental precursors of antisocial behavior:

Ages 1-3, Developmental Psychology, 30, 355-364.

Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development,

55, 17-29.

Steinberg, L., Graham, S., O’Brien, L., Woolard, J., Cauffman, E., & Banich, M. (2009). Age differences in

future orientation and delay discounting. Child Development, 80, 28-44.

Stieben, M., Lewis, M. D., Granic, I., Zelazo, P. D., Segalowitz, S., & Pepler, D. (2007). Neurophysiological

mechanisms of emotion regulation for subtypes of externalizing children. Development and

Psychopathology, 19, 455-480.

Stifter, C. A., & Corey, J. M. (2001). Vagal regulation and observed social behavior in infancy. Social

Development, 10, 189-201.

Stifter, C. A., & Braungart, J. M. (1995). The regulation of negative reactivity in infancy: Function and

development. Developmental Psychology, 31, 448-455.

Stifter, C. A., Putnam, S., & Jahromi, L. (2008). Exuberant and inhibited toddlers: Stability of

temperament and risk for problem behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 401-421.

Stone, L. L., Otten, R., Engels, R. C. M. E., Vermulst, A. A., & Janssens, J. M. A. M. (2010). Psychometric

properties of the parent and teacher versions of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire for

4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology, 13, 254-274.

Susman, E. J. (2006). Psychobiology of persistent antisocial behavior: Stress, early vulnerabilities and the

attenuation hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 376-389.

Tarullo, A. & Gunnar, M. R. (2006). Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. In M. M. McCarthy

& G. deVries, (Eds.) Special Issue of Hormones & Behavior, 5, 632-639.

Page 103: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

95

Teicher, M., Andersen, S., Polcari, A., Anderson, C., Navalta, C., & Kim, D. (2003). The neurobiological

consequences of early stress and childhood maltreatment. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review,

27, 33-44.

Towe-Goodman, Stifter, Coccia, Cox, & The FLP Key Investigators (2011). Interparental aggression,

attention skills, and early childhood behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 23,

563-576.

Tremblay, R. E., & Nagin, D. S. (2005). The developmental origins of physical aggression in humans. In R.

E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup, & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 83 -106).

New York: Guilford Press.

Tyson, P. D. (1998). Physiological arousal, reactive aggression, and the induction of an incompatible

relaxation response. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 3, 143-158.

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397-409.

van Goozen, S. H. M., & Fairchild, G. (2008). How can the study of biological processes help design new

interventions for children with severe antisocial behavior? Development and Psychopathology,

20, 941-973.

van Goozen, S. H. M., Matthys, W., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Gispen-de Wied, C., Wiegant ,V., & Van

Engeland, H. (1998). Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular activity during stress in oppositional

defiant disorder boys and normal controls. Biological Psychiatry, 43, 531-539.

van Goozen, S. H. M., Fairchild, G., Snoek, H., & Harold, G. T. (2007). The evidence for a neurobiological

model of childhood antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 149-182.

Vernon-Feagans, L., Pancsofar, N., J., Willoughby, M., Odom, E., Quade, A, Cox, M, & The Family Life Key

Investigators (2008). Predictors of maternal language to infants during a picture book task in the

Page 104: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

96

home: Family SES, child characteristics and the parenting environment. Journal of Applied

Developmental Psychology, 29, 213-226.

Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R.E. (2002). Reactively and proactively aggressive children:

Antecedents and subsequent characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43,

495-505.

Vitaro, F., Gendreau, P. L., Tremblay, R. E., & Oligny, P. (1998). Reactive and proactive aggression

differentially predict later conduct problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1-9.

Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in Head Start children: Strengthening parent

competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 715–730.

Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). Enhancing the effectiveness of self-administered videotape parent training

for families with conduct problem children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18,

479-492.

Webster-Stratton, C. (2000). The Incredible Years training series bulletin. Washington, DC: US

Department of Justice, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.

Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M.J., & Hammond, M. (2001). Preventing conduct problems, promoting

social competence: A parent and teacher training partnership in Head Start. Journal of Clinical

Child Psychology, 30, 238-302.

Webster-Stratton, C. (2005). The Incredible Years: A training series for the prevention and

treatment of conduct problems in young children. In E. D. Hibbs, & P. S. Jensen (Eds.),

Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies for

clinical practice (2nd ed.) (pp. 507-555).

West, D. J., & Farrington, D. P. (1973). Who becomes delinquent? London: Heinemann.

Zahn-Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E.A., and Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders of childhood and adolescence:

Gender and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 275-303.

Page 105: UNLOCKING THE HETEROGENEITY OF EXTERNALIZING …

Curriculum Vita

Christine K. Fortunato Education 2011 The Pennsylvania State University University Park, PA M.S., Human Development and Family Studies, December 2008 Ph.D., December 2011 1995-1999 Clark University Worcester, MA B.A. Biology and Psychology, Cum Laude with Psychology Honors Honors & Awards 2010-2011 Kligman Dissertation Fellowship 2005-2007 Graduate Education Enhancement Fellowship

Selected Publications Beltzer, E., Fortunato, C. K., Guaderramma, M., Peckins, M. K., Garramone, B. M., & Granger, D. A.

(2010). Salivary flow and salivary alpha-amylase: Collection technique, duration, and oral fluid type. Physiology and Behavior, 101, 289-296.

Granger, D. A., Hibel , L. C., & Fortunato, C. K., & Kapelewski, C. H. (2009). Medication effects on salivary

cortisol: Tactics and strategy to minimize impact in behavioral and developmental science. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 1437-1448.

Fortunato, C.K., Dribin, A., Buss, K.A., & Granger, D.A. (2008). Salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol in

toddlers: Differential relations with affective behavior, Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 807-818. Blair, C., Granger, D.A., Kivlighan, K.T., Willoughby, M., Greenberg, M., Hibel, L.C., Fortunato, C. K., &

The Family Life Project Investigators (2008). Maternal and child contributions to cortisol response to emotional arousal in young children from low-income, rural communities. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1095-1101.

Harmon, A., Towe, N.R., Fortunato, C.K., & Granger, D.A. (2008). Differences in saliva collection location and disparities in baseline and diurnal rhythms of alpha-amylase: A cautionary note.

Hormones and Behavior, 54, 592-596. Granger, D.A., Fortunato, C., & Hibel, L.C. (2007). Salivary Hormones in Research and Diagnostics. In D.

Wong (Ed), Saliva Diagnostics, Blackwell Press.