Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    1/6

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    2/6

    In Vivo Distraction-coping 459levels of anxiety tem porarily induced by extem al stressors (state anxiety) and of more en-during levels of anxiety (trait anxiety) presumably related to personality characteristics(Spielberger, Gorsuch. & Lushene, 1970). Discriminant validit) has been established forboth the TAI and the STAI with respect to test-anxious individuals vs. non-test-anxiouspersons (Him le, Thyer, & Papsdorf, 1982; Thyer & Papsdorf, 1982). Because cogn itiverestructuring procedures formed a part of the core treatment that all 5s received, theRational Behavior Inventory (RBI) was administered to provide an index of therationality of the 5s* personal belief systems (Shorkey & Whiteman, 1977). The RBI isbased upon Ellis's (1973) system of rational emotive therapy and is a 37-item question-naire answered on a 4-point Likert scale with a range of scores from 0-38. Higher scoresare indicative of greater levels of rationality as defined in the rational emotive therapyliterature. The RBI has been found to have high levels of convergent (Himle et al.. 1^82.Thyer & Papsdorf, 1982; Thyer. Papsdort. & Kilgore, 1983) and discriminan t validity(Thver. Miller, Gordon, & Papsdorf, 1982).

    Each 5 also was administered a standardized anagram solution task and a psy-chomotor test of manual dexterity pre and post intervention. The anagram solution taskwas designed to provide a measure of cognitive performance under high distraction con-ditions and has been sho\Nn to be sensitive to differing levels of test anxiety (Papsdorf,Himle, McCann, & Thyer. 1982).

    The basic core treatment consisted of a package of procedures. To treat thephysiological arousal component of test anxiety, 5s in both treatment groups receivedidentical instructions in progressive relaxation training, modified from Bernstein andBorkovec (1973), and training in biofeedback-assisted hand temperature control accord-ing to the procedures outlined by Lake. Rainey. and Papsdorf (1979). To treat thecognitive component of test anxiety, three distinct cognitive behavioral interventionswere given to both groups. The first cognitive treatment was based upon proceduresdeveloped by Meichenbaum (1972) and was designed to give the 5s an awareness of therole that anxiety-eliciting thoughts have in the development and maintenance of test anx-iety. This was followed by training and learning to substitute positive, coping self-statements and imagery whenever such thoughts related to test anxiety were perceived.This was based upon procedures outlined by Meichenbaum and Turk (1976). Lastly,techniques based upon rational emotive therapy (Ellis. 1973) were used to enhance theoverall rationality of the 5's belief systems through the disputation of irrational beliefsrelated to the importance of test-taking and its consequen ces. The final compon ent of thecore treatment package consisted of traditional systematic desensitization. All 5s weretrained in the procedures outlined by Allen (1972) and progressed through a standardized12-item hierarchy composed of test-related stimuli. When an 5 indicated that a par-ticular hierarchy item e licited an anxiet) response, he or she was instructed to: (I ) ver-balize positive coping self-statements and to utilize imagery to reduce the anxiety; and (2)then to engage in covert reinforcing self-statements for doing so.All 5s also completed a lO-minute written test at the end of each therapy session,which contained verbal and quantitative questions similar in difficulty to those found mthe Graduate Record Examination. 5s assigned to the core treatment condition com-pleted these examinations in a quiet setting and were urged to utilize the coping skillstaught previously. 5s who had received additional training in distraction copingprocedures completed identical examinations while audio tapes of distracting stimuliwere played. 5s in this latter group were trained to practice self-instructions designed to

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    3/6

    460 Journai of Clinical Psychology. March. 1984. Vol. 40. No. 2As a result of this program, statistically significant reductions in test anxiety, stateand trait anxiety, increases in rationality, improvements in anagram solution time, andthe manual dexterity task all were observed posttreatment. There were no significantdifferences between the two experimental groups, which suggested that the distractioncoping procedure added little, if any, effectiveness to the core treatment. In order toassess the maintenance of these therapeutic gains, a 12-month follow-up evaluation wasconducted.

    M E T H O DSubjects and Procedure

    One year after the completion of treatment, 14 of the original 19 5s were contactedby mail for participation in the follow-up study. The 5s were mailed an evaluation packetthat consisted of the questionnaires utilized in the original study. It was not feasible toreadminister the anagram solution task and the psychomotor test because some of the 5shad moved from the University community. It was felt that the information from thequestionnaire, which measures four distinguishable constructs (test anxiety, state andtrait anxiety, and rationality), would be of value.The percentage (73.6%) of the 5s assessed at the 1-year follow-up in the presentstudy is comparable to the return rates from earlier long-term follow-up studies in thetreatment of test anxiety. For example, DefTenbacher and Michaels (1981) obtained areturn rate of 73.5% in their year-long follow-up study, while Allen and Desaulniers(1974) were able to obtain follow-up information on 64% of their 5s at 2-year follow-up.

    RESULTSBecause there were no significant differences between the core treatment group andthe distraction-coping training group at posttreatment on the dependent variables in theoriginal study, the follow-up data are reported for both groups in a combined form. Theoriginal sample consisted of 10 women and 9 men. of whom information was obtainedfrom 7 women and 7 men at follow-up. Chi square analyses showed that the return ratewas not related significantly to either 5 sex or treatment condition. Follow-uprespondents did not differ from non-respondents when both pre- and posttreatmentscores on the various questionnaires were examined. The mean scores and standarddeviations for all 5s who completed the pretreatment, posttreatment, and follow-upassessments are presented in Table 1. Post hoc comparisons for differences in the 5sscores between time periods are presented in Table 2.The reductions in test and trait anxiety scores observed at posttreatment in theoriginal study were found to persist 1 year after the 8-week treatment program. Test anxiety scores at follow-up were significantly lower than at posttreatment as well. The data

    TABLL IMeans and Standard Deviations Jor all vS\

    Completing the Extended Folloy\-up Evaluationi.\ 141

    MeasuresPretreatment\f SD

    PosttreatmentM SD

    Follow-upM SD

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    4/6

    In Vivo Distraction-coping 461for slate and trail anxiety, as well as for rationa lity, clearly are in the direction of greaterimprovements.

    T A B L E 2Post Hoc Comparisons Between T ime Periods^

    MeasuresTest AnxietyState AnxietyTrait AnxietyRational Beliefs

    Pre vs. Post/)

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    5/6

    462 Journal of Clinical Psychology. March, 1984, Vol. 40, N o. 2BIGLAN, A., VILLWOCK, C , & WICK, S . (1979). The feasibility o f a computer controlled program for thetreatment of lest anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy an d Experimental Psychiatry. 10. 47-49.BRUCH, M. (1978 ). Type of cognitive m odeling, imitation of modeled tactics, and modification of test anx-iety. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2. 147-164.COUNTS, D., HOLLANDSWORTH, J., & ALCORN, J. (1978). Uses of EMG biofeedback and cue controlledrelaxation in the treatment of test anxiety. Journal of Consulting and C linical Psychology. 2. 147-164.DEFFENBACHER, J., & MICHAELS, A. (198 1). Two self-control procedures in the reduction of targeted andnontargeted anxietiesa year later. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 28. 525-528.DOCTOR, R., & ALTMAN, R. (1969). Worry and emotionality as components of test anxiety: R eplication and

    further data. Psychological Reports, 24. 563-568.ELLIS , A. (1973). Humanistic psychotherapy: The rational emotive approach. New York: Julian Press.GOLDFRIED, M., & SOBOCINSKI, D. (1975). Effects of irrational beliefs on emotional arousal. yourna/o/Con-

    sulting and Clinical Psychology. 43, 504-510.HIMLE, D., THYER, B., & PAPSDORF, J. (1982). Relationships between rational beliefs and anxiety. Cognitive

    Therapy and Research. 6. 219-223.HOLROYD, K. (1976 ). Cognition and desensitization in the group treatment of test anxiety. Journal of Con-sulting and Clinical Psychology. 44, 991-1001.HUSSIAN, R. , & LAWRENCE, P. (1978 ). The reduction of test, state and trait anxiety by test specific and

    generalized stress innoculation training. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2. 25-37.LAKE, A., RAINEY, J.. & PAPSDORF, J. (1979). Biofeedback and rational emotive-therapy in the managementof migraine headache. Journal of Applied Beh avior Analysis. 1 2. 127-140.LIEBERT. R.. & MORRIS, L. (1967). Cognitive and emotional components of test anxiety: A distinction andsome initial data. Psychological Reports, 20. 975-978.MARLETT, N.. & WATSON. D. (1968). Test anxiety and immediate or delayed feedback in a test-likeavoidance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8. 200-203.MATTHEWS. A. (1978) . Fear reduction research and clinical phobias. Psychological B ulletin. 85. 390-404.MEICHENBAUM. D. (1972). Cognitive modification of test anxious college sludcnls. Journal of Consulting an

    Clinical Psychology, 39. 370-380.MEICHENBAUM. D.. & TURK. D. (1976). The cognitive-behavioral management of anxiety, anger, and pain.In P. Davidson (Ed.). Behavioral management of anxiety, depression and pain. New York:Brunner/Mazel.PAPSDORF, J.. HIMLE, D.. MCCANN, B., & THYER, B. (1982). Anagram solution time and effects of distrac-tion, sex differences, and anxiety. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 55. 215-222.SARASON. 1. (1975). Test anxiety and the self-disclosing coping model. Journal of Consulting and CiinicaiPsychology, 43, 148-153.SARASON, I.. & GANZER. V. (1961). Anxiety, reinforcement and experimental instructions in a free verbaliza-tion situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 65. 300-307.SHORKEY. C . & WHITEMAN. V. (1977). Development of the Rational Behavior Inventory: Initial validity and

    reliability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 37, 527-532.SPIEOLER. M.. MORRIS, L.. & LIEBERT, R. (1968). Cognitive and emotional com ponents of test anxiety:Temporal factors. Psychological Reports. 22. 451-456.SPIELBERGER, C. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety: Current trends intheory and research (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.SPIELBERGER. C. (1980). The Test Anxiety Inventory {TAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.SPIELBERGER, C . GO RSUCH, R. . & LUSHENE, R. (t970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo

    Alto. CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.THYER, B.. MILLER. E., GORDON, A., & PAPSDORF. J. (1982). Discriminant validity of the Rational Behavior

    Inventory with respect to social desirability. Psychological Reports. 51, 370.THYER, B.. & PAPSDORF, J. (1982). Discriminant and concurrent validity of two commonly used measures of

    test anxiety. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 42. 1197-1204.THYER, B.. PAPSDORF. J.. HIMLE. D., MCCANN, B.. CALDWELU S., 8L WICKERT. M . (1981 ). In vivo

  • 7/27/2019 Usmle-relat-papers; In Vivo Distraction-coping in the Rx of Test Anxiety

    6/6