51
PREFERENCE IS NOT COLOR BLIND 1 Preference is Not Color Blind: Disentangling Implicit Race and Color Effects Ioannis Kareklas Washington State University Robin A. Coulter University of Connecticut Frédéric F. Brunel Boston University Author Note Ioannis Kareklas, Department of Marketing, College of Business, Washington State University. Robin A. Coulter, Marketing Department, School of Business, University of Connecticut. Frédéric F. Brunel, Department of Marketing, School of Management, Boston University. Corresponding author: Ioannis Kareklas, Department of Marketing, College of Business, Washington State University, Todd Addition 375, PO Box 644730, Pullman, WA 99164-4730. E-mail: [email protected]

questromapps.bu.eduquestromapps.bu.edu/qstnet/Personal/Faculty/Publication/... · Web viewThe overall propensity to evaluate members of one’s own group more favorably than people

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

1

Preference is Not Color Blind:

Disentangling Implicit Race and Color Effects

Ioannis Kareklas

Washington State University

Robin A. Coulter

University of Connecticut

Frdric F. Brunel

Boston University

Author Note

Ioannis Kareklas, Department of Marketing, College of Business, Washington State University. Robin A. Coulter, Marketing Department, School of Business, University of Connecticut. Frdric F. Brunel, Department of Marketing, School of Management, Boston University.

Corresponding author: Ioannis Kareklas, Department of Marketing, College of Business, Washington State University, Todd Addition 375, PO Box 644730, Pullman, WA 99164-4730. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This research draws upon theoretical perspectives related to in-group favoritism and color symbolism to hypothesize effects of an individuals implicit color bias (i.e., preference for the color white vs. the color black) and implicit racial bias (i.e., preference for African American vs. Caucasian racial stimuli) on their implicit reactions to advertisements featuring endorsers of different races. Results from Implicit Association Test (IAT) assessments of participants color, racial, and advertising preferences indicate that both African American and Caucasian consumers exhibit an implicit preference for the color white (as compared to the color black), and that implicit color preference significantly influences both their racial preferences and advertising evaluations. These findings document that implicit color bias has a key role in how consumers react to endorsers of their own and other races, thus highlighting the need to remove its effect from evaluations of racial stimuli.

Preference is Not Color Blind: Disentangling Implicit Race and Color Effects

The overall propensity to evaluate members of ones own group more favorably than people belonging to other groups (a.k.a., in-group favoritism) is a generally prevalent and well-established social-psychological phenomenon (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971). In the consumer domain, one of the direct consequences of in-group favoritism is that consumers are likely to favor ad endorsers whom they perceive to belong to their own in-group over endorsers from other groups. Although this appears to be a robust finding, at least one of the possible bases for in-group identification, race, leads to more varied results.

Race (e.g., African American or Caucasian American) is a readily noticeable and salient characteristic of a spokesperson and can serve as a basis for in-group identification and persuasion (Spira & Whittler, 2004; Whittler & Spira, 2002). However, the body of research evidence provides mixed results on the effect of race on persuasion. On one end, research using explicit (i.e., self-report) measures has demonstrated that both African Americans and Caucasian Americans tend to respond more favorably to persuasive messages delivered by endorsers that belong to their own racial in-group (Schlinger & Plummer, 1972; Whittler, 1989; Simpson, Snuggs, Christiansen, & Simples, 2000). Yet, on the other end, psychology and marketing studies utilizing implicit measures have found that although Caucasians tend to exhibit pro-white (in-group) preferences, African Americans tend to not exhibit pro-black (in-group) preferences, and in some instances might even show pro-white preferences (Ashburn-Nardo, Knowles, & Monteith, 2003; Brunel, Tietje, and Greenwald 2004; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald, 2002; Smith-McLallen, Johnson, Dovidio, & Pearson, 2006). This lack of automatic preference for ones in-group is perplexing, especially in the context of automatic associations which are supposed to be free from social-desirability response biases and tap into automatic attitudes and beliefs (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995).

System justification theory (Jost & Banaji, 1994) has been used to explain the absence of automatic in-group preference for African American respondents. Researchers have proposed that a long history of discrimination can lead minority individuals to internalize negative attitudes toward their own group as a means of justifying the status quo (Rudman, Feinberg, & Fairchild, 2002), and that such attitudes tend to be non-conscious (Jost and Banaji 1994) and can be unearthed by implicit measures (Rudman, Feinberg, & Fairchild, 2002).

Although we believe in the merits of system justification theory and the evidence that has been offered in its support, we also believe that there exist at least one other possible explanation for the lack of pro-black automatic in-group preference among African Americans. Instead of offering a historical and sociological discrimination explanation, we propose that there is an even more fundamental explanation for these results.

In this research, we argue that individuals implicit preferences for the color white versus the color black may impact their evaluations of white versus black faces. We ground our expectations in work on color preference which has shown that in many cultures, the associations for the color white are positive, whereas the color black tends to be associated with more negative connotations (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006). Examples include cultural dress codes that dictate wearing white at weddings and black at funerals (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006), or the portrayal of villains in black hats and good guys in white clothing in traditional American Western films (Frank & Gilovich, 1988). Further, white is often used to connote decency and purity, whereas black is used to connote evil and disgrace (Longshore, 1979). Anthropologists have argued that the preference for the color white over black can be traced to ancient tribal fears for darkness, the night and the unknown versus the fondness for white which is linked to light, fire or the sun (Mead & Baldwin, 1971). In general, it has been argued that the preference for white over black is learned from early childhood, is culturally reinforced over time and is a more fundamental automatic preference than race preference (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006).

In this article, we seek to extend recent results that showed that for Caucasian respondents an automatic white color preference can be linked to an automatic white racial preference (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006). In particular, we include African Americans in our studies, and use the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) to investigate how automatic color preference is linked to automatic racial preference at a fundamental level. Furthermore, we explore how automatic color preference is linked to two marketing variables, automatic product preference and automatic spokesperson preference across both racial groups, by varying the color (i.e., black or white) of products, and the race (i.e., African American or Caucasian American) of spokespersons featured in advertisements.

In study 1, we show that both African Americans and Caucasian Americans have pro-white automatic color preferences and pro-white-colored product preferences, and that these overall implicit color preferences for the color white can be linked to implicit preferences for white products. In study 2, we replicate the pro-white color preference for both racial groups, but then we show that only Caucasian Americans respondents appear to have automatic racial and advertising spokesperson preferences which are consistent with an in-group favoritism explanation. We also show that for both groups, implicit color preferences are linked to implicit race and advertising spokespersons preferences. We then statistically remove the effect of the implicit color preference from the implicit race and advertising spokesperson preference measures and show that once this is done, both racial groups exhibit racial preferences for their own in-group. In particular, in contrast to many of the above mentioned studies, the African American respondents have a pro-black racial preference. Thus, we are able to show that automatic color preference and in-group-favoritism are directionally similar and additive in creating automatic racial and Aad preferences for Caucasian Americans. But, for African American respondents, automatic color preference and in-group-favoritism are countervailing forces and therefore allow us to explain why, in ours and many other studies, African American might at first glance appear to lack in in-group favoritism, this being due instead to the fact that as a group (like most other human beings) they prefer the color white over the color black.

Theoretical background

Implicit racial preference

A focal communicator characteristic of interest in persuasion research has been the perceived similarity between the endorsers featured in ads and the targeted consumers. Several studies employing explicit measurement techniques have shown that similar (vs. dissimilar) endorsers are more influential in changing recipient attitudes (Brock, 1965; Woodside & Davenport, 1974). The basis of this effect is in-group favoritism (Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971), which refers to the propensity to evaluate people who are perceived to belong to your own group more favorably than people belonging to other groups (Spira & Whittler, 2004). Hence, recipients are likely to favor endorsers whom they perceive to belong to their racial in-group over endorsers who belong to other groups. Racial groups represent pre-defined social categories, hence recipients share socially ascribed group membership with same-race endorsers, and are likely to exhibit in-group favoritism when evaluating ads featuring endorsers of their racial in-group compared to ads featuring out-group endorsers.

Race is a key attribute related to recipients perceptions of similarity between themselves and the endorsers featured in advertisements. Given that race is one of the most noticeable physical characteristics (especially in terms of skin color), it is likely to influence persuasion (Spira & Whittler, 2004), and is directly relevant to judgments of similarity. Extant research using explicit measures tends to affirm this assertion. As previously mentioned, several studies have shown that both African American and Caucasian consumers evaluate advertisements featuring same versus different-race endorsers more favorably (e.g., Schlinger & Plummer, 1972; Whittler, 1989).

However, studies using explicit measures suffer from response biases when the context is socially sensitive, as in the case of race, because explicit measures allow participants to consciously control their responses (Ashburn-Nardo et al., 2003). Extant studies utilizing implicit measures to examine racial associations have found that both Caucasians and African Americans tend to exhibit implicit pro-Caucasian association bias. For example, Nosek et al. (2002) found that although African Americans exhibited stronger explicit liking for their own racial group than Caucasians, participants of both racial groups exhibited a pro-Caucasian (vs. African American) implicit association bias.

Furthermore, research on the neural basis of social group processing has documented that indirect measures of race evaluation, such as the IAT, tend to correlate with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)-assessed activation of the amygdala. The amygdala is a subcortical structure involved in emotional learning and evaluation (Phelps et al., 2000), and is implicated in the automatic evaluation of social stimuli. These results, combined with previous investigations of intergroup attitudes, suggest that implicit negative associations toward a social group may result in an automatic emotional response when encountering members of that group. Lieberman et al. (2005, p. 722) posit that the amygdala activity that is associated with race-related processing may be a reflection of culturally learned negative associations regarding African American individuals. This explanation is consistent with system justification theory (Jost & Banaji, 1994) which has previously been used to explain why African Americans exhibited a lack of in-group preference in implicit advertising evaluations (Brunel et al., 2004).

System justification theory posits that people are motivated to believe in a just world, and that a history of discrimination can lead minority individuals to internalize negative attitudes toward their own group as a means of justifying the status quo (Rudman, Feinberg, & Fairchild, 2002). Jost and Banaji (1994) emphasize that such attitudes and the motive to sustain them are likely to be non-conscious. This explains why this phenomenon may not be picked up by self-report measures, but can be unearthed by implicit measures (Rudman, Feinberg, & Fairchild, 2002) which assess automatic associations that are believed to underlie nonconscious attitudes and beliefs (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). To summarize, extant research using implicit measures demonstrates a preference for Caucasian racial stimuli for both African Americans and Caucasians.

Implicit color preference

Researchers in psychology have theorized about an individuals color bias; that is, an implicit color preference for some colors over others (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006). Two theories have been advanced to explain the development of color preference in young children. The majority of researchers in this area believe that the emergence of color preference in young children is due to the cultural socialization of color symbolism. This theory suggests that children develop a pro-white/anti-black color preference through the verbal learning of color symbolism in their culture (Duckitt, Wall, & Pokroy, 1999). In other words, as a childs verbal comprehension develops, s/he learns to associate the color white with positive connotations and the color black with negative connotations. Williams, Tucker, and Dunham (1971) note that the color white has been used in religion, literature, and the mass media as a symbol of goodness, whereas the color black has been used as a symbol of badness. For example, the English language idiom black sheep is a derogatory term (because black wool is less valuable than white wool) used to describe an undesirable or disreputable member of a group. In contrast, the term white knight is used in the management literature to describe a friendly company that is invited by the target management to outbid an unwanted bidder, and thus protect the company from a hostile takeover (Jensen & Ruback, 1983). Frank and Gilovich (1988) observed that the terms Black Thursday (referring to Thursday, Oct 24, 1929 when stock values dropped leading to the Great Depression of the 1930s), being blacklisted or blackmailed all carry negative connotations.

Other researchers favor the early experience theory proposed by Williams and Morland (1976) as an explanation for the emergence of a pro-white/anti-black color preference in children. Early experience theory proposes that young children develop a preference for the color white as compared to the color black as a result of their early experiences with light and darkness. Specifically, as diurnal beings, we require light to interact effectively with our environment, and may find darkness to be disorienting and therefore intrinsically aversive (Williams, Boswell, & Best, 1975). Early experience theory suggests that a childs early experiences with the light of day and the darkness of the night lead her/him to develop a preference for light over darkness, which may then generalize to a preference for the color white as compared to the color black (Williams et al. 1975).

As noted, cross-cultural research indicates a prevalence of positive associations with the color white and negative associations with the color black (Adams & Osgood, 1973). Several studies have documented the existence of a pro-white/anti-black color preference using participants from varying social and ethnic backgrounds, indicating that this may be a pan-cultural phenomenon. For example, Adams and Osgood, (1973) used semantic differential scales to study the color preferences of adults and found that participants evaluated the color white more positively than the color black. Others have used an indirect measure called the Color Meaning Test (CMT II; Williams et al., 1975) to study color preference in children. Researchers using the CMT II have found support for a pro-white/anti-black color preference in children from varying cultures including: European-American (Boswell & Williams, 1975), African American (Williams & Rousseau, 1971), and bi-racial American (Neto & Paiva, 1998). Furthermore, a cross-cultural study investigating the affective meanings of color in 23 cultures found that the color black is viewed as the color of evil and death in almost all cultures (Adams & Osgood, 1973). Hence, we expect that:

H1:Caucasian and African American participants will exhibit an implicit color preference for

the color white as compared to the color black.

Relationship between color preference and product preference

Color is an important component of marketing communications, and its effects have been widely studied in the areas of advertising, packaging, and store design (Bellizzi, Crowley, & Hasty, 1983). While a large portion of color research on products by marketers has not been published due to competitive concerns (Bellizzi et al., 1983), extant published research has shown that effective use of color can attract attention (Lee & Barnes, 1989), and influence consumers perceptions and behaviors (Aslam, 2006).

Consumers deliberately choose product colors that complement their desired self-image (Madden et al., 2000). For example, consumers choose colors for their houses, cars, and clothes that are consistent with how they want to present themselves (Trinkaus, 1991). We anticipate that consumers implicit product preferences will be consistent with their implicit color preferences. As noted in the previous section, extant research suggests that consumers from varying backgrounds have a non-conscious preference for the color white over the color black. Hence, we expect that:

H2:Caucasian and African American participants will exhibit an implicit product preference for white as compared to black colored products.

H3:Implicit color preference will predict implicit product preference, such that participants who implicitly prefer the color white (black) will also tend to implicitly prefer white (black) colored products.

Relationship between color preference and racial preference

Research suggests that individuals develop a preference for the color white over the color black at an early age, and this contributes to the later development of racial preference (Duckitt, Wall, & Pokroy, 1999). Correlational studies have found a significant positive relationship between color and racial preferences, such that participants with a high degree of pro-white/anti-black color preference tend to evaluate dark-skinned people less favorably than light-skinned people (Williams, 1969; Boswell & Williams, 1975; Neto & Williams, 1997). Livingston and Brewer (2002) found that Caucasian participants made more negative associations with African Americans than Caucasians, especially for African Americans with prototypic features such as darker colored skin. Similarly, Maddox and Gray (2002) report that both Caucasian and African American participants exhibited stronger associations between stereotypic negative racial characteristics (e.g., being criminal, poor, or aggressive) and darker-skinned African Americans. Furthermore, experimental studies show evidence for a functional relationship between color and racial preferences (Williams et al., 1975). For example, Williams and Edwards (1969) used reinforcement procedures to weaken participants pro-white/anti-black color preferences, which subsequently led to a reduction in their pro-Caucasian/anti-Black racial evaluations, and later studies replicated these findings in different cultures and societies (Duckitt et al., 1999).

The aforementioned theory and research findings suggest that participants implicit color preferences will influence their implicit racial preferences and their implicit evaluations of advertisements that feature African American or Caucasian endorsers. Hence, we expect:

H4:Implicit color preference will predict implicit racial preference and implicit advertising

preference, such that participants who implicitly prefer the color white (black) will also tend to implicitly prefer Caucasian (African American) faces and advertisements featuring Caucasian (vs. African American) endorsers.

H5:Implicit racial preference will mediate the effect of implicit color preference on

participants implicit advertising preference.

Disentangling color preference from racial and advertising evaluations. As previously mentioned, studies using implicit measurement techniques such as the IAT generally find that both Caucasian and African American participants exhibit pro-Caucasian association bias when responding to racial stimuli (Nosek et al., 2002). This finding is incongruent with the vast majority of extant studies assessing participants advertising preferences in response to ads featuring in-group versus out-group models. We expect that the influence of color preference on racial evaluations may partially account for this disparity. Specifically, we disentangle the effect of participants implicit color preferences from their implicit racial and implicit advertising preferences, and test the following hypothesis:

H6:After controlling for color preference, both African American and Caucasian participants will exhibit implicit racial and advertising preferences in favor of their racial in-group.

Methodology

Recent research in consumer behavior and psychology has acknowledged that consumption behavior is frequently affected by cognitive processes outside conscious awareness and control (Bargh, 2002; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Moreover, even when research participants are aware of their attitudes, they may be unwilling to share them or may purposefully distort their answers to avoid embarrassment, especially when the topic of interest is socially sensitive in nature (Mick, 1996). Methodological advances in implicit measurement techniques have enabled researchers to use indirect measures to examine participants racial associations. Implicit measures provide an indirect estimate of the construct of interest, without directly asking the participant (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Prominent examples include projective techniques (Haire, 1950), various forms of priming (Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986), the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998), and the Go/No-Go Association Task (Nosek & Banaji, 2001).

In our research, we employ the IAT, a well-established and widely used technique for measuring implicit associations (Fazio & Olson, 2003), to explore whether both African American and Caucasian respondents exhibit an implicit color preference (ICP) for the color white as compared to the color black, and an implicit product preference (IPP) for white products as compared to black products. Furthermore, we examine whether participants implicit racial preference (IRP) may be partially explained by their implicit color preference (ICP). In addition, we investigate the effect of these two antecedents of attitude toward the ad (i.e., IRP and ICP) on participants implicit attitudes toward advertisements (IAad) featuring African American and Caucasian endorsers.

Study 1

Study 1 was designed to test the first three hypotheses. While extant research has shown that Caucasian participants exhibit an implicit color preference for the color white as compared to the color black (Smith-McLallen et al., 2006) replicating past studies using explicit measures, to the best of our knowledge, no other study has examined the color preferences of African American respondents using an implicit measure. Furthermore, we designed study 1 to test whether participants automatic color preferences are related to their automatic preference for white versus black colored products.

Participants, Materials and Procedure

We recruited Caucasian and African American participants from an online panel provider to participate in an IAT experiment. Following the recommendations of Greenwald, Nosek, and Banaji (2003), individual trial response latencies greater than 10,000 milliseconds, and data from participants who responded faster than 300 milliseconds on more than 10% of trials were eliminated. This yielded a total of 219 useable responses from 123 Caucasian (64 female and 59 male) and 96 African American (77 female and 19 male) respondents, with a mean age of 39 years old.

Participants completed two separate IAT procedures: a color IAT, and a product IAT. Each IAT consisted of seven blocks. Blocks four and seven (counterbalanced) were the measurement blocks. Within each of the three IAT tasks, the order in which participants encountered the black preference blocks (pairing black stimuli with pleasant words, and white stimuli with unpleasant words) and white preference blocks (pairing white stimuli with pleasant words, and black stimuli with unpleasant words) was counterbalanced. Categorization labels (e.g., Pleasant vs. Unpleasant) appeared on the top right and the top left of the computer screen, and a randomly selected stimulus (either an image or a word) from that IAT appeared in the middle of the screen. Participants were instructed to sort stimuli to the appropriate category as quickly as they could, while trying to minimize errors. Participants were instructed to press the D key on their computer keyboard if the correct category label for the target stimulus appeared on the left side of the screen, and the K key if the appropriate category label appeared on the right side of the screen. Whenever participants responded correctly, the next stimulus and category labels appeared; whenever participants responses were incorrect, a red X appeared and remained on the screen until the stimulus was correctly classified.

Stimuli development. All IAT stimuli used a gray background (RGB color code 127 127 127), which is exactly between the colors black and white in the color spectrum to avoid priming participants with either the color white or black. Additionally, we set the background color of all experimental procedures to the same color gray. The color IAT assessed participants implicit color preferences for the colors white and black, using pictures of black and white geometric shapes as color stimuli (see the Appendix for examples of the stimuli used in each IAT), and words with positive (e.g., happiness) and negative (e.g., misery) connotations as evaluative stimuli to represent the two attribute concepts (i.e., Pleasant and Unpleasant). The product IAT

Measurement of IAT effects. As IAT studies are based on latency measures, participants response latencies were recorded (in milliseconds) from the onset of each stimulus to its correct classification for the two measured blocks in each IAT. Mean preference scores were calculated using the improved D score algorithm (Greenwald et al., 2003). Specifically, D was scored so that higher numbers indicate a stronger association between pleasant words and Caucasian stimuli on the race IAT and advertising IAT, and white shapes on the color IAT. Therefore, a positive (negative) D implies that participants paired Caucasian (or white) stimuli with pleasant words and African American (or black) stimuli with unpleasant words quicker (slower) than they paired African American (or black) stimuli with pleasant words and Caucasian (or white) stimuli with unpleasant words. In contrast to the earlier conventional scoring procedure, the improved algorithm minimizes the effect of having previously completed one or more IATs on the calculated IAT scores (Greenwald et al., 2003), which is particularly important for the present experiment where participants were asked to complete three separate IAT procedures.

Results

Our findings for study 1, the mean D scores, 95% confidence intervals, and correlations for the two IAT tasks are presented in Table 1. In support of hypothesis 1, both Caucasians (ICP Mean d = .68, t(122) = 18.48, p