Written Report Kay Dean

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    1/8

    POLYTECHNIC UNIVERSITY OF THE PHILIPPINES

    COLLEGE OF EDUCATION

    STA. MESA, MANILA

    Observational LearningOperant Conditioning

    Written Report

    Prepared by:

    Jessa Wenica C. Pallomina

    BBTE IV1D

    Prepared for:

    Dr. Milagrina A. GomezDean, College of Education

  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    2/8

    Observational Learning Compared To Imitation

    Observational learning differs from Imitation in that it does not require a duplication ofthe behavior exhibited by the model. For example, the learner may observe an unwanted

    behavior and the subsequent consequences, and thus learn to refrain from that behavior. For

    example, Riopelle, A.J. (1960) found that monkeys did better with observational learning if theysaw the "tutor" monkey make a mistake before making the right choice. Heyes (1993)

    distinguished imitation and non-imitative social learning in the following way: imitation occurs

    when animals learn about behavior from observing conspecifics, whereas non-imitative sociallearning occurs when animals learn about the environment from observing others.

    Observational learning is presumed to have occurred when an organism copies animprobable action or action outcome that it has observed and the matching behavior cannot be

    explained by an alternative mechanism. Psychologists have been particularly interested in the

    form of observational learning known as imitation and in how to distinguish imitation from other

    processes. To successfully make this distinction, one must separate the degree to which

    behavioral similarity results from (a) predisposed behavior, (b) increased motivation resultingfrom the presence of another animal, (c) attention drawn to a place or object, (d) learning about

    the way the environment works, as distinguished from what we think of as (e) imitation (thecopying of the demonstrated behavior) (Zentall 2012).

    Observational learning

    Tomasello (1999) described various ways of observational learning without the process

    of imitation in animals: Exposure- Individuals learn about their environment with a closeproximity to other individuals that have more experience. For example, a young dolphin learning

    the location of a plethora of fish by staying nears its mother.

    Stimulus Enhancement - Individuals become interested in an object from watching

    others interact with it (Spence 1937).

    Increased interest in an object may result in object

    manipulation, which facilitates new object-related behaviors by trial-and-error learning.

    For example, a young killer whale might become interested in playing with a sea lion pup

    after watching other whales toss the sea lion pup around. After playing with the pup, the killerwhale may develop foraging behaviors appropriate to such prey. In this case, the killer whale did

    not learn to prey on sea lions by observing other whales do so, but rather the killer whale became

    intrigued after observing other whales play with the pup. After the killer whale became

    interested, then its interactions with the sea lion resulted in behaviors that provoked futureforaging efforts.

    Goal Emulation-Individuals are enticed by the end result of an observed behavior andattempt the same outcome but with a different method. For example, Haggerty (1909) devised an

    experiment in which a monkey climbed up the side of a cage, stuck its arm into a wooden chute,

    and pulled a rope in the chute to release food. Another monkey was provided an opportunity toobtain the food after watching a monkey go through this process on four separate occasions. The

    monkey performed a different method and finally succeeded after trial and error.

    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Imitationhttp://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Imitation
  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    3/8

    Peer model influences

    Observational learning is very beneficial when there are positive, reinforcing peer modelsinvolved. Although individuals go through four different stages for observational learning:

    attention; retention; production; and motivation, this does not simply mean that when an

    individual's attention is captured that it automatically sets the process in that exact order. One ofthe most important ongoing stages for observational learning, especially among children, is

    motivation and positive reinforcement.

    Performance is enhanced when children are positively instructed on how they can

    improve a situation and where children actively participate alongside a more skilled person.

    Examples of this are scaffolding and guided participation. Scaffolding refers to an expertresponding contingently to a novice so the novice gradually increases their understanding of a

    problem. Guided participation refers to an expert actively engaging in a situation with a novice

    so the novice participates with or observes the adult to understand how to resolve a problem.

    Observational learning across cultures

    Cultural variation can be seen in the extent of information learned or absorbed by

    children through the use of observation and more specifically the use of observation without

    verbal requests for further information. For example, children from Mexican heritage familiestend to learn and make better use of information observed during classroom demonstration thenEuropean heritage children.

    Another example is seen in the immersion, of children in some

    Indigenous communities of the Americas, into the adult world and the effects it has on

    observational learning and the ability to complete multiple tasks simultaneously.

    This might bedue to children in these communities having the opportunity to see a task being completed by

    their elders or peers and then trying to emulate the task. In doing so they learn to value

    observation and the skill-building it affords them because of the value it holds within theircommunity. This type of observation is not passive, but reflects the child's intent to participateor learn within a community.

    Indigenous Communities of the Americas

    Children observe elders, parents, and siblings completing tasks and learn to participate inthem as they grow. Observational opportunities tend to be more prominent in indigenous

    communities, because children integrate in adult activities. They are seen as contributors

    themselves and therefore they learn to observe multiple tasks being completed at once and can

    learn to complete a task, while still engaging with other community members without beingdistracted.

    The heightened value towards observation allows children to multi-task in activelyengage in simultaneous activities. The exposure to an uncensored adult lifestyle incorporating

    children allows them to observe and learn the different skills and practices that are valued in their

    communities.

    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Indigenous_peoples_of_the_Americashttp://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Indigenous_peoples_of_the_Americas
  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    4/8

    Children from indigenous heritage communities and backgrounds learn through

    observation, a learning strategy that can carry over into adulthood.

    Children of indigenous

    heritage communities commonly use contextual cues in their understanding and ideas. In anative Northern Canadian and an Indigenous Mayan community children often learn as third-

    party observers to stories and conversations by others.

    Indigenous communities utilize observational learning by providing more opportunity to

    incorporate children in everyday life, where observational learning is expected and may be more

    inherent to some cultures more than others. This integration of children into everyday life can beseen in some Mayan communities where children are given full access to community events,

    which allows observational learning to occur more often. Children in communities such as the

    Mazahua in Mexico are known to intensely observe ongoing activities.

    Within certain indigenous communities a characteristic of observational learning is that

    people do not typically seek out explanation beyond basic observation because they are

    competent in learning through observation. In a Guatemalan footloom factory amateur adult

    weavers observed skilled weavers over the course of weeks without questioning or being givenexplanations; the amateur weaver moved at their own pace and began when they felt confident.

    The framework of learning how to weave through observation can serve as a model thatparticular members or groups within a society use as a reference to guide their actions in

    particular domains of life.

    Indigenous parent's teaching styles are shaped by their influence with western schooling.

    In traditionalMayan families,given the difference of the education levels of mothers, those with

    more years of formal education often prompt children to take turns in learning to solve a

    problem, while mothers with less education facilitate open ended discussion with the children. Ina similar study among Mayan fathers and children showed that fathers with 03 years of

    education operated through a mix of observation and shared collaboration between adults andchildren, compared to the fathers with higher levels of education who structured a discussion.

    Observational Learning and Children with Autism

    There are not a lot of studies done on the acquisition of knowledge through observation,

    but there are none on observational learning in children with low-functioning autism accordingto Nadel, Aouka, Coulon, Grad-Vincendon, Canet, Fagard & Bursztejn (2011). This group of

    researchers set out to consider whether or not children with low-functioning autism are able to

    learn through observation only.

    They used two groups; children aged four to nine diagnosed with Autism Spectrum

    Disorder (ASD) and a control group of children. They further divided each group into two

    subgroups, based on developmental age (24 or 36 months). All four groups then receiveddifferent tasks that corresponded to their developmental age. The task involved a red box that

    contained candy, which the children tried to get. This study lasted nine daysduring which all

    the children were given the box on the first day and given time to try to get the object out. Theywere then shown a demonstration video twice but were not given the box after the demonstration

    http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Maya_peopleshttp://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Maya_peoples
  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    5/8

    to try to get the candy. The following day, the participants were only given the box with no video

    demonstrations to test for observational learning. This was repeated seven days later.

    Nadel et al. (2011) found that the children who did not have autism showed improvement

    after the first demonstration. The children of a younger developmental age improved one week

    later, and the older children improved after only one demonstration. Children with autismimproved after the second demonstration only.

    The authors believe this means that children with autism progress the same as typicalchildren, but take longer to learn. They also believe this means that children with autism can

    form motor representations for a task without prior experience, and that they can correct motor

    representations after previously not being able to do a task. The researchers argue that thedifference in the need for more demonstrations for children with autism was not due to a lack of

    attention but more as a result of an increased difficulty of creating a motor representation of an

    action that leads to a remote goal as opposed to an immediate goal.

    In free play, children with autism often display self-stimulatory behaviors that inhibitappropriate behaviors and decrease their ability to learn new behaviors. In one study, the

    researchers were looking to determine the effect of the observation of peers on appropriate toy-playing skills in autistic-like children. They also examined the effects training can have on

    autistic-like children, in the training situation and in a generalization setting. No study before this

    1986 work looked at the ability of autistic-like children to learn skills similar to those theylearned in training, by observing a peer in a non-training setting. This study strove for insight

    into the generalization of autistic-like children's skills through observational learning.

    Participants included three autistic-like boys with a mean chronological age of 4.4 and a

    mean age of 2.5. Six other boys with a mean age chronological age of 4.3 and a mean age of 3.2,

    and good receptive and language skills, served as peer models. Each participant took part in:

    A pretest to determine ten different toys that the child did not play with appropriately A baseline test of free play, where children played in a room where the ten toys

    determined in the pretest were present, as well as another peer

    Two training sessions, each followed by a generalization and maintenance conditionIn the first training session, the participant watched a peer (who they did not see in the

    baseline test) correctly play with a toy. The second training was the same as the first except that

    the participant was exposed to both a new peer model and a new toy. The amount of exposure to

    a modeled play task was the independent variable, and was manipulated through additional tasks,models, and settings. Tryon and Keane (1986) identified the dependent variables as the training

    task acquisition, generalization in free-play, and the frequency of both self-stimulatory behavior

    and imitative play behaviors.

    The authors found that all three autistic-like boys could learn to imitate the peer model,

    and could play with an unfamiliar toy in the training sessions by watching a peer model. In thefollowing generalization and maintenance sessions, all the autistic-like boys learned to play with

    the unfamiliar toy that they had not been trained in. The researchers noticed that the boys

  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    6/8

    decreased their self-stimulatory behaviors as a result of the imitative play learned in training. The

    authors suggested that enhanced imitation of play behaviors may have been due to the use of

    multiple peer models.

    Other human and animal behavior experiments

    When an animal is given a task to complete, they are almost always more successful after

    observing another animal doing the same task before them. Experiments have been conducted on

    several different species with the same effect: animals can learn behaviors from peers. However,there is a need to distinguish the propagation of behavior and the stability of behavior. Research

    has shown that social learning can spread a behavior, but there are more factors regarding how a

    behavior carries across generations of an animal culture.

    B.F. SKINNERS THEORY OF OPERANT CONDITIONING

    Derived from the theory of Thorndike, Skinner analyzed reinforcing stimuli based on the

    law of effect. Skinner also emphasized the effects of the subjects action among the causes of

    behavior.

    The operant conditioning theory is based on Skinners experiments with animals.

    Skinner uses the term operant behavior to refer to his idea that an organism has to do something

    in order to get a reward, that is, it must operate on its environment. His basic premise is that any

    organism (including man) tends to repeat what it was doing at the time its behavior was

    reinforced and that the task is a matter of baiting each step of the way, thus gradually leading the

    subject to the required performance.

    In one of Skinners well-known experiments, a hungry rat was placed in a box. Upon its

    accidental pressing of a lever in a box, it was rewarded with a food pellet which served as

    reinforcement to the pressing behavior. Reinforced thus, the rat kept on pressing that bar, this

    time no longer accidentally but intentionally. Skinner has shown that basic to operant

    conditioning is the use of reinforcement.

    Reinforcement is defined as any behavioral consequence that strengthens behavior. The

    reinforcement increases the likelihood of the recurrence of a particular type of response. By

    reinforcement, skinner refers to any event that increases the probability that a particular response

    will increase in frequency. Responses may be reinforced by the presentation (positive) orremoval (negative) of particular consequences. Thus, reinforcement may be positive or negative.

    The presentation of a positive reinforce increases the likelihood that a particular response will

    occur. The withdrawal of a negative reinforce will also increase the likelihood of occurrence of

    a desired response by presenting or withdrawing a positive or a negative reinforcer.

  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    7/8

    Positive and negative reinforcers influence behaviors in opposite ways. Positive

    reinforcers increase response frequency. Negative reinforcers strengthen behavior by their

    removal. That means termination of the reinforcing stimulus increases response frequency.

    To determine whether a particular event is reinforcing, observations are conducted. The

    frequency of a selected response is first observed and then made contingent on the response. Therate of responding with the added consequences is observed next. If the response frequency is

    increased, the selected event is therefore reinforcing.

    To be effective in altering behavior, reinforcement must be made contingent on the

    execution of appropriate responses. Reinforcement increases the rate of responding; however,

    elimination of the reinforcing consequence decreases the rate. This will eventually lead to

    extinction. In everyday life then, reinforcement is used to prevent the extinction of behavior.

    Primary and Secondary reinforcers are two types of reinforcement. There are some

    reinforcers that are innately reinforcing. They are powerful in increasing the chance that aparticular behavior will occur. These are called primary reinforcers.

    In primary reinforce, the increase in response rate occurs without training. Given the

    state of deprivation, primary reinforcers will alter the probability of responding. Sleep is

    reinforcing for a sleep-deprived person. Food and water also belong to this category of

    reinforcers.

    Secondary reinforcers or conditioned reinforcers influence behavior through training.

    These reinforcers are not innately reinforcing. This type of reinforcement is done specifically by

    developing associations with a primary reinforce. Their power to reinforce behavior is acquired.

    Money, grades, stars, and tokens are all secondary reinforcers.

    Primary and secondary reinforcers may have the same effectiveness depending on how

    they are used or managed in the conditioning process. Both types of reinforcers are most

    effective when they immediately follow the responses they are intended to increase.

    Other reinforcers, however, have a more general influence on behavior. Generalized

    reinforces can function under more than one set of circumstances through association with more

    than one primary reinforce (e.g., Money is a token associated with food, drink, and shelter).

    Attention, approval, congratulations, and peer approval are other types of generalized

    reinforcers.

    As in classical conditioning, a process related to operant conditioning is extinction.

    Extinction of a learned behavior in this case occurs as a result of its repetition while receiving no

    further reinforcement. This process of extinction may be employed in terminating an undesirable

    behavior.

  • 8/13/2019 Written Report Kay Dean

    8/8

    In contrast with reinforcers, punishment involves the withdrawal of a positive reinforcer

    or the addition of a negative reinforcer. Withdrawal of a positive reinforcer, or removing a

    pleasant experience, may involve removing TV watching privileges. Presenting an unpleasant

    event, on the other hand, or the addition of a negative reinforcer could be confining the child to

    his room.

    Skinner proposed that in terms of effect, punishment is not the opposite of reinforcement.

    For Skinner, punishment leads to 3 undesirable effects:

    1. Punished responses only disappear temporarily.2. Emotional predispositions such as guilt or shame may be conditioned through the use

    of punishment.

    3. Any behavior that reduces the aversive stimulation accompanying the punishmentwill be reinforced.

    For example, a child may lie to his mother that he is sick so that he can avoid going toschool, thus avoiding taking the test. The test which serves as an aversive stimulus will only

    reinforce the negative behavior of lying.

    The contribution of Skinners theory to learning involves the acquisition of complex

    behavior through the process of shaping.

    Shaping behavior is the acquisition of complex behaviors such as playing tennis, and

    solving problems. The procedure of first reinforcing responses that only resemble the desired

    response is referred to as reinforcing successive approximations. This calls for reinforcing

    behavior like kicking the ball when the child is just learning how to play soccer.

    The importance of shaping is that it can generate complex behaviors that do not occur

    naturally through shaping by a series of contingencies in a program. Each stage of the program

    evokes a response and also serves to prepare the organism to respond at some later point.

    Shaping is different from behavior modifications that occur with puzzles, mazes and

    other Pavlovian tasks. It does not entail trial and error at random points in the learning process.