Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    1/26

    This article was downloaded by: [189.170.4.149]On: 21 September 2012, At: 21:09Publisher: Psychology PressInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

    Cognitive NeuropsychiatryPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcnp20

    Is the Charcot and Bernard case(1883) of loss of visual imagery reallybased on neurological impairment?Stefano Zago

    a, Nicola Allegri

    a, Marta Cristoffanini

    a, Roberta

    Ferruccib, Mauro Porta

    c& Alberto Priori

    b

    aDipartimento di Neuroscienze ed Organi di Senso, Universit

    degli Studi di Milano, UOC di Neurologia Fondazione IRCCS CaGranda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, ItalybCentro Clinico per la Neurostimolazione, le Neurotecnologie

    e i Disordini del Movimento, Dipartimento di Neuroscienze edOrgani di Senso, Universit degli Studi di Milano, ItalycCentro Tourette e Malattie Extrapiramidali, IRCCS Ospedale

    Galeazzi di Milano, Italy

    Version of record first published: 23 May 2011.

    To cite this article:Stefano Zago, Nicola Allegri, Marta Cristoffanini, Roberta Ferrucci, MauroPorta & Alberto Priori (2011): Is the Charcot and Bernard case (1883) of loss of visual imageryreally based on neurological impairment?, Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 16:6, 481-504

    To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2011.556024

    PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

    Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

    This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

    The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. Theaccuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independentlyverified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions,claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused

    http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcnp20http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditionshttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2011.556024http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pcnp20
  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    2/26

    arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of thismaterial.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    3/26

    Is the Charcot and Bernard case (1883) of loss of visualimagery really based on neurological impairment?

    Stefano Zago1, Nicola Allegri1, Marta Cristoffanini1,

    Roberta Ferrucci2, Mauro Porta3, and Alberto Priori2

    1Dipartimento di Neuroscienze ed Organi di Senso, Universita degli

    Studi di Milano, UOC di Neurologia Fondazione IRCCS Ca Granda

    Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Italy2Centro Clinico per la Neurostimolazione, le Neurotecnologie e i

    Disordini del Movimento, Dipartimento di Neuroscienze ed Organi di

    Senso, Universita degli Studi di Milano, Italy3Centro Tourette e Malattie Extrapiramidali, IRCCS Ospedale

    Galeazzi di Milano, Italy

    Introduction. The Charcot and Bernard case of visual imagery, Monsieur X, is a

    classic case in the history of neuropsychology. Published in 1883, it has beenconsidered the first case of visual imagery loss due to brain injury. Also in recent

    times a neurological valence has been given to it. However, the presence ofanalogous cases of loss of visual imagery in the psychiatric field have led us to

    hypothesise functional origins rather than organic.

    Methods. In order to assess the validity of such an inference, we have compared the

    symptomatology of Monsieur X with that found in cases of loss of visual mental

    images, both psychiatric and neurological, presented in literature.

    Results. The clinical findings show strong assonances of the Monsieur X case with

    the symptoms manifested over time by the patients with functionally based loss of

    visual imagery.

    Conclusion. Although Monsieur Xs damage was initially interpreted as neurolo-

    gical, reports of similar symptoms in the psychiatric field lead us to postulate a

    functional cause for his impairment as well.

    Correspondence should be addressed to Stefano Zago, PhD, Dipartimento di Neuroscienze ed

    Organi di Senso, Universita degli Studi di Milano, UOC di Neurologia, Fondazione IRCCS Ca

    Granda, Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, via Francesco Sforza 35, 20122 Milano, Italy. E-mail:

    [email protected]

    We wish to thank David Noonan for help with the English text. Preliminary data were

    presented at the eighth annual meeting of the American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (AACN), Chicago, Illinois, 1719 June 2010.

    COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHIATRY

    2011, 16 (6), 481504

    # 2011 Psychology Press, an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

    http://www.psypress.com/cogneuropsychiatry http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2011.556024

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

    http://www.psypress.com/cogneuropsychiatryhttp://www.psypress.com/cogneuropsychiatry
  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    4/26

    Keywords: Charcot and Bernard; Depersonalisation; Derealisation; Functional;

    Loss of visual imagery; Monsieur X; Organic.

    INTRODUCTION

    Visual mental imagery is the ability to create, more or less faithfully, a similar

    perceptual visual representation of environments, people, objects, symbols,

    or scenes in absence of their physical presence. It is a function that plays an

    important role in humans mental life and that is variously used in

    representing for example the model of an organic molecule, a city route,

    the appearance of places in our childhoods or the faces of our loved ones.

    These examples reveal that visual imagery it is clearly a function that

    involves both cognitive and emotional aspects even if there is not always an

    emotional overlay in the acquisition and cognitive manipulation of visualinformation.

    Clinically, there is great evidence that visual imagery can be damaged by

    neurological lesions which compromise the ability to describe and draw

    familiar environments or objects from memory (e.g., Farah, 1984; Ganis,

    Thompson, Mast, & Kosslyn, 2003; Trojano & Grossi, 1994). Less known,

    but equally well documented, is the possibility that psychiatric patients

    should result in a similar loss of visual imagery. This condition has been

    documented in patients with depression and anxiety associated with

    depersonalisation/derealisation and, more rarely, with psychosis (e.g.,

    Reda, 1960; Schilder, 1928; Sierra & Berrios, 2001).The main aim of this paper is to revisit, in a critical vein, the Monsieur X

    case reported in 1883 by Charcot and Bernard, considered by neurological

    and neuropsychological literature as the first case of loss of visualisation and

    considered a standard citation in case reports or reviews of loss of visual

    imagery. In particular, on the basis of evidence of past and present

    psychiatric and neurological literature, we aim to assess if it is possible to

    put forward an alternative hypothesis to the neurological origins explanation

    which has been commonly and indeed recently posed (see Bartolomeo, 2008;

    Della Sala & Young, 2003; Goldenberg, 1993; Zeman et al., 2010).

    THE RENOWNED CASE OF MONSIEUR X

    The Monsieur X case was published in 1883 on Le Progres Medicalwith the

    title ofUn cas de suppression brusque et isolee de la vision mentale des signes

    et des objets (formes et couleurs) [A case of sudden and isolated suppression of

    the mental vision of signs and objects (forms and colours)] (Charcot &

    Bernard, 1883).Le Progres Medicalusually collected clinical cases examined

    by Charcot at the prestigious Parisian hospital, the Salpetriere. It is thanks

    482 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    5/26

    to the written report by Desire Bernard, an apprentice within his entourage,

    that the description of this patient has reached us.

    From the short account, Monsieur X was an entrepreneur born in

    Vienna, with good business acumen and was a cultured person, able tounderstand and express himself in his German mother tongue, Spanish,

    and French, and with a good knowledge of Latin and Greek (both ancient

    and modern), so much so that he was able to do business in the latter. He

    would read the works of Virgil and Homer, whose first books he knew very

    well and could finish lines from these when prompted with the beginnings.

    According to the same Monsieur X, before the onset of the symptomatol-

    ogy that led him to see professor Charcot, he enjoyed a formidable visual

    memory in recalling the features of faces, shapes, and the colours of

    objects, so as to use this ability as a mnemonic technique. However, by his

    own admission, his auditory-verbal and musical memory skills were lessremarkable. A year and a half before visiting Charcot, Monsieur X had

    presented a marked and persistent mood disturbance with states of

    agitation and restlessness linked to the possible risk of losing huge sums

    of money. In addition to the psychopathological symptomatology, there

    was sudden appearance of the well-known loss of the ability to recognise

    and to visualise both recent and past shapes and colours. Prodromic

    symptoms, which were somatic in nature, had not been noted during the

    visit to Charcot nor were other neurological signs present besides the visual

    imagery and recognition deficits. An ophthalmological examination carried

    out at the clinic by Dr. Parinaud had indicated significant myopia (7

    dioptres), but ocular lesions or defects of the visual field were excluded.

    The only meaningful data was represented by a light weakening of

    chromatic sensitivity in relation to the entire spectrum of colours. The

    onset and development of Monsieur Xs disturbance, over an 18 month

    period, are described both in the article and in a letter sent personally

    by Monsieur X to Charcot, dated 11 June 1883, attached to the

    original article. Charcot and Bernard described the onset of the disorder

    as follows:

    About a year and half ago he was assailed by a strong concern related to the

    payment of considerable credits of which seemed to him to be uncertain. He lost his

    appetite and was unable to sleep despite the fact that the problems were not serious

    enough to justify the disorders which manifested themselves. The disturbance had

    been very intense and showed no signs of diminishing, until one day Monsieur

    X was abruptly shaken at noticing in himself a deep change. At first he felt a

    complete loss and a violent contrast between the old and the new state that had

    taken place. For a moment Monsieur X believed he was threatened by a mental

    alienation, so much so that things around him appeared strange and new. He

    became anxious and irritable . . .

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 483

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    6/26

    Monsieur X himself noticed that he had become less sensitive to loss and

    suffering confessing he had not felt sadness at the death of a beloved relative.

    He recognised a strong change in his own personality and, as he himself

    pointed out:

    I think my existence has completely changed and obviously also my personality.

    Before, I used to be much more emotional and enthusiastic and I had a fervent

    imagination. Today, instead, I am calm, cold and my imagination does not allow me

    any more representation . . .

    Everything Monsieur X perceived seemed as though it were being observed

    for the first time, including monuments, streets and houses familiar to

    him. When he went down the streets of his native town, this gave him a

    strange feeling as if everything had assumed new forms. To the request to

    provide a description of his native citys main public square from memory, hestated:

    I know that it exists, but I am unable to imagine it nor can I tell you anything about

    it, I can tell you no more.

    Also the ability to recognise faces seemed to be disturbed. As Charcot and

    Bernard noted:

    Recently, in a public gallery, someone crossed his path to whom he immediately

    apologised but it was nothing more than his own image reflected in a mirror.

    Monsieur X was scared of finding himself in always new situations on a daily

    basis, pointing out:

    No longer being able to form images of what I see and having completely preserved

    my abstract memory, I feel every day a great astonishment in seeing for the first

    time things which should be very familiar for me.

    The visual recognition impairment, was apparently also extended to

    graphical signs. When he was asked to write the Greek alphabet, he omitted

    certain letters (u,w,s,z,8,c,l) that he was not then able to recognise and

    read when Charcot traced the form of these with his finger. Only after havingreproduced the letters with his own finger was Monsieur X able to interpret

    them, underlining, according to Charcot, a kind of compensation for mild

    alexia (ceciteverbale).

    He was not able to visualise his wifes or his childrens features, but the

    thing that depressed him most was the inability to visualise colours. All in

    all, Monsieur X described his loss of visual imagery as follows:

    . . . if you ask me to represent the towers of Notre-Dame, a sheep that is grazing or a

    ship that is sinking into the sea, I would answer that, even distinguishing perfectly

    484 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    7/26

    the three things and knowing very well what it concerns, they do not have any

    meaning for me from the point of view of inner vision.

    His graphical abilities were also changed. Before the illness, Monsieur Xused to reproduce meticulous drawings of the things that he observed,

    imprinting them in his memory. After the appearance of the disorder he was

    able to draw environments, objects, and faces using only notions that one

    might calltechnical. Invited to draw a minaret, Monsieur X reported that

    he knew that this constituted a high and quadrangular tower and traced a

    schematic design formed by a few vertical and horizontal lines on the sheet.

    He added:

    Do you want an arcade? I am about to trace it because I am aware that a round arch

    is a circumference, an ogive is formed from two arches that intersect one another at

    an acute angle. In spite of that I cannot imagine them at all and I cannot see themwith my mind.

    Monsieur X reported he had also changed his way of dreaming:

    . . . now I dream only words, where once instead, I had above all a visual perception

    of my dreams.

    The diminished ability of visual memory led him to have to rely on other

    kinds of memory in order to continue carrying out his own work activities.

    He was forced to read words and phrases aloud several times, resorting to

    repetition in order to memorise:Today I am forced to say to myself the things that I want to keep in my memory,

    whereas before I only needed to photograph them with my vision.

    After having widely described Monsieur Xs inability to conjure up mental

    visual images, Charcot and Bernard provided an explanation in neuropsy-

    chological terms of this deficit, assuming that the case represented an

    example of selective loss of visual memory linked to a hypothetical

    circumscribed cerebral lesion. This conception derives from the componen-

    tial memory theory proposed at the time by Theodule Ribot (1881). There is

    no information available about aetiology or lesion site, as the postmortemexamination*the only available method to locate cerebral lesions at the

    time*was apparently not performed.

    PUBLISHED CASES OF VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS AFTER THEMONSIEUR X CASE

    Since Charcot and Bernards pioneering study at the turn of the twentieth

    century, a number of cases similar to Monsieur X with an inability to

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 485

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    8/26

    visualise absent objects in their thoughts were reported in France, among

    whom the clinical contexts suggest a psychopathologic basis. In these

    patients, impaired visual images fall within a complex medical history in

    which a depressive and anxious symptomatology coexist with depersonalisa-tion and derealisation (Cotard, 1884; Deny & Camus, 1905; Dugas, 1898;

    Magalhaes Lemos, 1906; Picard, 1919; Wimmer, 1919).

    Depersonalisation and derealisation are both dissociative disorders

    having a strong affiliation with anxiety and depression and consist of a

    change of self-awareness or experience of the environment in clinical settings

    (Sierra, 2009). Both the disorders occur in healthy subjects as a response to

    stress, fatigue, or drug use and in those with psychiatric disorders where

    symptoms can persist for lengthy periods (Senior et al., 2001). From a

    neurobiological standpoint, depersonalisation has been characterised in

    terms of inhibitory and excitatory processes in which three cerebral areasplay a critical role: bilateral prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and cingulated

    cortex (Sierra & Berrios, 2001).

    From the 1950s and 1960s, a series of single cases began to appear in

    neurological/neuropsychological literature, in which the inability to generate

    visual mental images was linked to various neurological conditions such as

    head injury, vascular disorders, postsurgical complications, etc. Often, in

    these cases, visual imagery disorder is associated with visual recognition

    deficits due to focal lesion to the posterior cortical damage. Moreover, it

    should be noted that, although clearly organically damaged, an additional

    functional component is sometimes also invoked in some of these patients(e.g., Brain, 1954). However, the organic/functional question has never been

    explicitly raised either in the psychiatric or neurological fields.

    The same situation recurs subsequently in psychiatric literature starting

    from the 1960s where a series of cases of sudden loss of mental visual

    imagery appear again, apparently only in Italy (e.g., Amabile & Rizzo, 1966;

    Giacheddu, Amabile, & Sanarelli, 1968; Giannini & Loprieno, 1964;

    Giovanardi Rossi & Muscatello, 1968; Muscatello & Giovanardi Rossi,

    1967; Reda, 1960). In particular, Reda (1960) observed that visual imagery is

    a faded and vanishing copy of perception, a weakened and veiled reliving of

    sensations mainly characterised by fragility and uncertainty and particularlyvulnerable in states of poor affectivity and loss of emotion. The images, such

    as the faded copy of perception, underwent further opacity to the point of

    appearing even more distant and hazy, so distant to convince them they have

    lost the capacity for imagination. The patients who complained of loss of

    visual imagery had, according to Reda, anxiety attacks, different levels of

    depressive states, and, above all, depersonalisation and derealisation. These

    latter two aspects will be the key elements of the syndrome.

    To our knowledge, after the end of the 1960s no single case studies of

    selective visual imagery loss have been reported in psychiatric literature,

    486 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    9/26

    excluding among other things, an evaluation with modern neuropsycholo-

    gical tools. It is difficult to give an explanation for this gap but it should be

    remembered that, after the first group of French patients, over forty years

    passed before the publication regarding the Italian cases. You can seehistorical phases in which the attention on a phenomenon is strong, then

    reduces and fades from memory.

    Many years later, a new series of neurological cases of loss of visual

    imagery with confirmed focal cerebral lesions and a visual imagery

    psychometric evaluation were reported (e.g., Basso, Bisiach, & Luzzatti,

    1980; Deleval, de Mol, & Noterman, 1983; Riddoch, 1990).

    In our opinion, there are two categories of patients, functional and

    organic, with a visual imagery deficit but with distinctive clinical and

    pathophysiological features (see Tables 1 and 2).

    However, for a small number of patients a dichotomic framework isproblematic due to the overlapping of organic and functional factors (see

    Table 3). Once again we see, for the visual imagery function the same old

    organic/functional debate, that for over a century has been concerned with

    the loss of retrograde amnesia (Barbarotto, Laiacona, & Cocchini, 1996; de

    Renzi, Lucchelli, Muggia, & Spinnler, 1997; Serra, Fadda, Buccione,

    Caltagirone, & Carlesimo, 2007). Moreover, it has been demonstrated that

    in patients with retrograde amnesia, there can be a substantial overlap in

    terms of neuroanatomic dysfunctional locus noticeable when they undergo

    examinations using functional neuroimaging techniques (Markowitsch,

    2003). This is an observation that makes us reflect on the need to supposea dichotomy on a lesional basis rather than using the simple organic/

    functional distinction. In other words, it may be more correct to assume that,

    apart from structural neurological alterations which can be identified

    through traditional neuroradiological methods (CT, NMR), there are also

    others consisting in anomalous focal reactions of brain metabolism that can

    be documented only through neurofunctional imaging methods (PET, fMRI,

    or others). In this sense, the organic or functional disorders represent two

    sides of the same coin as highlighted by Markowitsch (1996).

    MONSIEUR X: AN ORGANIC OR FUNCTIONAL CASE?

    As seen in Table 1, in our opinion Monsieur X must be included among

    those patients with a functional disturbance. Such positioning finds

    justification after comparing the symptomatology of this patient with those

    cases of a neurological and functional nature presented in literature, and also

    through critical rereading of the symptoms he complained of.

    From Table 1 it can be clearly deduced that patients identified with a

    functional origin constantly present a symptomatology characterised by

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 487

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    10/26

    TABLE 1

    Summary of case reports of patients with a loss of visual imagery on a

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditions

    Brain

    damage

    Otherneurological

    deficits

    Visualperception

    of dreams

    Charcot & Bernard,

    Case Monsieur X

    1883 M (?) anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation (?),

    derealisation (?)

    * * loss of the visu

    element of

    dreaming

    Cotard,

    Case 1

    1884 M 68 anxiety, depression, psychotic

    disorder, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Cotard,

    Case 2

    1884 M 40 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Dugas,

    Case M.

    1898 M (?) apathy, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Deny & Camus,

    Case 1

    1905 F 47 anxiety, psychotic disorder,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Magalhaes Lemos,

    Case 1

    1906 F 36 anxiety, depression, psychotic

    disorder, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * preserved

    Picard,

    Case 1

    1919 F 47 anxiety, depression

    hypochondria, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    11/26

    TABLE 1 (Continued)

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditions

    Brain

    damage

    Other

    neurological

    deficits

    Visual

    perception

    of dreams

    Wimmer,

    Case 2

    1919 F 55 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * progressive

    reduction of

    visual imagery

    Hartmann,

    Case Auguste W

    1922 M 35 anxiety, depersonalisation * facial

    paresthesias

    preserved

    Ackner,

    Case NP

    1954 F 42 depression, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Reda,

    Case NF

    1960 F 33 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Reda,

    Case RA

    1960 F 57 anxiety, depression, psychotic

    disorder (?), depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Reda,

    Case NA

    1960 F 27 anxiety, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Reda,

    Case BD

    1960 F 37 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    12/26

    TABLE 1 (Continued)

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditions

    Brain

    damage

    Other

    neurological

    deficits

    Visual

    perception

    of dreams

    Giannini & Loprieno,

    Case FJ

    1964 F 42 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Amabile & Rizzo,

    Case 1

    1966 M 37 anxiety, depression,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Muscatello &

    Giovanardi Rossi,

    Case R Anna

    Maria

    1967 F 35 anxiety (?), depression,

    psychotic disorder,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * dream feeling

    without visual

    images

    Muscatello &

    Giovanardi Rossi,

    Case P Renata

    1967 F 23 anxiety (?), depression,

    psychotic disorder,

    depersonalisation, derealisation

    * * dream feeling

    without visual

    images

    Muscatello &

    Giovanardi Rossi,

    Case C Elvira

    1967 F 31 anxiety, depression, psychotic

    disorder (?), depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * restriction of

    visual dream

    imagery

    Muscatello &

    Giovanardi Rossi,

    Case M Corinna

    1967 F 60 anxiety, depression, psychotic

    disorder, depersonalisation,

    derealisation

    * * no informatio

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    13/26

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    14/26

    TABLE 2

    Summary of case reports of patients with a loss of visual imagery on a

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitatingstressful event

    Neuropsychiatricconditions Brai n damage

    Other neurologicaldeficits

    Visual

    perception ofdreams

    Stengel,

    Case 1

    1948 M 63 (?) * (?) right hemianopia no

    information

    Stengel,

    Case 2

    1948 M 68 (?) depression (?) right hemianopia no

    information

    Boyle & Nielsen,

    Case ES

    1954 M 31 * bilateral occipital (?) left hemiparesis,

    papilledema

    no

    information

    Macrae & Trolle,Case 1

    1956 M 32 * bilateral parietal (?) none preserved (?)

    Beyn and Knyazeva,

    Case Ch

    1962 39 * bilateral occipital right hemianosia,

    right superior

    quadrantopia

    no

    information

    Basso et al.,

    Case MG

    1980 M 63 * left mesial occipital

    lobe, part of left

    temporal

    juxtaventricular

    structures, possible

    inclusion of the

    hippocampus, upper

    left cerebellar

    hemisphere

    decreased density

    right hemiparesis,

    slight right motor,

    right homonymus

    hemianopia

    loss of the

    visual

    element of

    dreaming

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    15/26

    TABLE 2 (Continued)

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditions Brain damage

    Other neurological

    deficits

    Visual

    perception of

    dreams

    Deleval et al.,

    Case Ch. T

    1983 M 35 aphaty parieto-occipito-

    temporal with left

    occipital lesion

    penetrating into deep

    posterior angular

    gyrus

    right homonymous

    hemianopia, right

    hemiparesis

    mentioned

    absence of

    imagery even

    prior to

    neurological

    damage

    Levine et al.,

    Case 1

    1985 M 18 * right anterior

    temporal, right

    inferior frontal, left

    temporo-occipital

    superior left

    homynimous

    quadrantopia,

    partial right

    homonymus superior

    quadrantopia

    no

    information

    Levine et al.,

    Case 2

    1985 M 43 * bilateral parieto-

    occipital

    right homonymous

    hemianopia, right

    hemiparersis, right

    hemianesthesia

    no

    information

    Grossi et al.,

    Case AP

    1986 M 56 * left temporo-

    occipital

    right hemiparesis,

    right superior

    quadrantopia

    no

    information

    Chiacchio et al.,Case BL 1987 M 58 *

    left occipital r ight homonymushemianopia noinformationDownloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    16/26

    TABLE 2 (Continued)

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditions Brain damage

    Other neurological

    deficits

    Visual

    perception of

    dreams

    Farah et al.,

    Case RM

    1988 M 64 * left occipital and

    medial temporal

    regions

    right homonymus

    hemianopia

    no

    information

    Riddoch,

    Case DW

    1990 M 71 * left temporo-parietal right-sided

    hemiplegia, right

    homonymus

    hemianopia

    no

    information

    Goldenberg,

    Case K. Qu

    1992 M 83 * left temporo-

    occipital

    right homonymus

    quadrantopia

    no

    information

    Mehta et al.,

    Case MS

    1992 M 42 * bilateral temporo-

    occipital, enlarged

    ventricles,

    parahippocampal

    gyrus and fourth

    temporal gyrus, left

    mesial temporo-

    occipital

    left-sided

    homonymus

    hemianopia

    no

    information

    Ogden,

    Case MH

    1993 M 24 * medial bilateral

    occipital lobes,

    dilatation of both

    occipital horns

    transitory cortical

    blindness

    loss of the

    visual

    element of

    dreaming

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    17/26

    TABLE 2 (Continued)

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitating

    stressful event

    Neuropsychiatric

    conditi ons Brain damage

    Other neurological

    deficits

    Visual

    perception of

    dreams

    Zago & Policardi,

    Case ZB

    1996 M 67 * left fronto-temporo-

    parieto-occipital,

    cortical, and

    subcortical

    slight right

    hemiparesis

    no

    information

    o

    Zago et al.,

    Case

    Michelangelo

    1997 M 44 * temporal bilateral none no

    information

    a

    Moro et al.,

    Case 1

    2008 F 29 * left middle and

    inferior temporalgyrus

    none no

    information

    o

    ac

    Moro et al.,

    Case 2

    2008 M 23 * left temporo-

    occipital, left medial

    and superior parietal

    lobe

    none no

    information

    o

    a

    c

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    18/26

    TABLE 3

    Summary of case reports of patients with a loss of visual imagery with u

    Author/s Year Sex Age

    Trauma/

    precipitatingstressful event

    Neuropsychiatricconditions Brain damage

    Other

    neurologicaldeficits

    Visual perceptionof dreams

    Typ

    visuad

    Brain,

    Case 1

    1954 M 36 nevrotic traits (?) probably frontal

    damage

    none loss of the visual

    element of

    dreaming

    objects

    faces

    Brain,

    Case 2

    1954 M 50 depression frontal none no information objects

    faces

    Botez et al.,

    Case 1

    1985 M 38 none right hemisphere (?),

    corpus callosum (III

    posterior) (?),

    dysgenesis (?)

    none restriction of

    visual dream

    imagery

    objects

    faces

    Solms,

    Case 1

    1997 F 26 depersonalisation

    (?)

    arterio-venous

    malformation in the

    right parietal lobe

    left

    hemiparesis,

    somotosensory

    defect, left

    field visual loss

    loss of the visual

    element of

    dreaming

    objects

    Zeman et al.,

    Case MX

    2010 M 65 none mild hypodensity of

    the white matter, slight

    fronto-temporal

    atrophy estimated

    within normal limits

    none initial loss of the

    visual element of

    dreaming with

    partial recovery

    after 1 year

    subjec

    visual

    not co

    psycho

    tests

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.149]at21:0921September2012

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    19/26

    anxiety, depression, depersonalisation with derealisation and sometimes

    psychotic features. Some of these features are clearly present in Monsieur X.

    In fact, financial worries caused in him a state of anguish leading to obvious

    symptoms of depression, anxiety, and an unreal view of the world congruentwith derealisation and probable depersonalisation. In particular, the

    derealisation feature can in itself justify the subjective sense of strangeness

    and unfamiliarity in the external visual perception manifested by him.

    Therefore, although he seems to reflect a dysfunctional pattern, widely

    documented in neurological literature, of a deficit of visual mental imagery

    accompanied by visual recognition disturbances (visual agnosia, proso-

    pagnosia, alexia), an alternative hypothesis is that the misrecognitions of

    Monsieur X can be justified by a disorder of derealisation with depersona-

    lisation.

    In fact, it is worth noting that certain cases of loss of mental vision withdepersonalisation present this disorder. Again, Reda (1960), in relation to a

    patient of his stated:RA goes to the market and everything appears to her

    new and artificial; the houses of cardboard, the chalk fruit; she takes in her

    hand a tomato, she looks at it curiously, and even recognising it perfectly

    says to herself: BB How strange these tomatoes are . BB Do you see

    that house before us? the same patient says to me BB I see it as if were

    of cardboard, according to me it is only the facade without anything in it

    . A patient studied by Giacheddu et al. (1968) states that: objects no

    longer have any shape or defined colours; it is as if things were made of wood

    and the people were statues. Everything has changed. . .

    I am unable toexplain to myself why.

    Also the deficits of recognition of faces and letters, present in Monsieur

    X, can be interpreted as the consequence of a functional reaction. In the very

    brief passage describing the face recognition disorder (see earlier), there is a

    clear resemblance to some patients with depersonalisation (e.g., Ackner,

    1954; Mayer-Gross, 1935). For example, Mayer-Grosss patient stated that:

    When looking in a mirror I have to keep telling myself the reflection is

    mine. Its like looking at another person. Likewise, Ackner (1954) wrote:

    Miss P; aged 28, suffering from depressive state, was so struck by her

    strange reflection in the mirror that for short while she was convinced thatshe appeared peculiar and that other people must be laughing at her and

    talking about her.

    Similarly, the same argument could be extended to Monsieur Xs

    recognition disorder regarding the letters of the Greek alphabet. Despite

    not having been described in the depersonalisation context, it is worth noting

    that reading disorders have been reported in a psychopathological context

    (e.g., Dinshaw & Lishman, 1984; Saunders & Barker, 1972).

    It is important to state that Monsieur X continued to carry out his

    commercial transactions profitably, even if resorting to alternative strategies

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 497

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    20/26

    to that of visual mental imagery. This appears unusual as if effectively an

    agnosic/prosopagnosic impairment with an associated mild reading disorder

    were present.

    The proposal that depersonalisation/derealisation disorders can interferewith the visual imagery process is also supported by some group studies.

    Schilder (1928) had observed that patients with a depersonalisation

    condition:characterize their imagery as pale, colourless and some complain

    they have altogether lost the power of imagination. Shorvon, Hill, Burkitt,

    and Halstead (1946) mentioned a study in which imagery tests were made on

    16 cases of depersonalisation and reported that a few patients complained of

    genuine loss of visual imagery. More recently, Lambert et al. (2001), based

    on the examination of 28 patients with depersonalisation/derealisation,

    found a significant level of complaints of imagery deficit in this group,

    compared to the healthy subjects on the Vividness of Visual ImageryQuestionnaire (VVIQ; Marks, 1973) and the Vividness of Movement

    Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ; Issac, Marks, & Russell, 1986). Sierra and

    Berrios (2001), in a historical analysis of the 200 cases of depersonalisation

    disorder in the medical literature since 1898, showed that complaints about

    inability to evoke images occurred with a frequency of 52.6% in the pre-1946

    cases (phenomenological period) and 18.8% in the post-1946 cases (empirical

    period).

    It is also of interest that Simeon et al. (2000) in a PET study with subjects

    with depersonalisation found a pattern of altered metabolic activity,

    primarily in the posterior cortex including left occipital area 19, an areatraditionally considered critical for image generation process (Farah, 1984;

    Nielsen, 1946). Left temporo-occipital activation in visual imagery has also

    been demonstrated in a series of SPECT, PET, and fMRI studies (e.g.,

    DEsposito et al., 1997; Goldenberg et al., 1989; Goldenberg, Steiner,

    Podreka, & Deecke, 1992; Roland & Guylas, 1994).

    An additional element that characterises psychiatric patients with

    depersonalisation/derealisation is the emphatic self-reporting of the loss of

    mental vision, an element present in Monsieur X but rare in neurological

    patients (e.g., Nielsen, 1955: for a loss of visual imagery in dreaming and,

    partially, Case K. Qu of Goldenberg, 1992: for drawing from memory).Interestingly, in some patients the reporting of mental imagery deficit does

    not seem to correspond to a deficit found in nonsubjective measure of

    imagining capacity (e.g., drawing from memory, description from memory).

    This is an intriguing aspect already highlighted by Shorvon et al. (1946), and

    reported in some single case reports (Amabile & Rizzo, 1966; Giannini &

    Loprieno, 1964; Reda, 1960; Zeman et al., 2010).

    Another discordant aspect in the case of Monsieur X is the absence of

    deficit of the visual field*which can usually be found in neurological

    patients with posterior lesions*while only an undefined slight decrease

    498 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    21/26

    in colour perception was detected. Also Monsieur Xs dream abnormality

    appears unusual in that he lost only the visual aspects of dreaming. Solms

    (1997), in his personal examination of a series of 361 neurological cases,

    found only two cases of nonvisual dreams, and another 15 cases reported inliterature from 1883 to 1991 (including the Charcot & Bernard case). It is

    worth noting that in one case, personally observed by Solms, he suspected a

    depersonalisation disorder (included in Table 3, doubtful cases). Of the 15

    cases found in literature we have considered three as doubtful and have

    included them in Table 3.

    In our review of the seven psychiatric cases in which dream loss is

    reported, four presented a nonvisual dream loss and two had normal dream

    recall. Out of the four neurological cases, in which dreams recall is

    mentioned, two showed a global loss, one preserved it, and one indicated

    an absence of dream recall before neurological damage. Unfortunately, in themajority of the other cases, both psychiatric and neurological, there is no

    mention of dreaming.

    Finally, a functional origin (hysterical) of Monsieur Xs deficit had

    been suggested by Sigmund Freud who, on having the opportunity to

    personally examine Monsieur X during the months spent at the

    Salpetriere with Charcot, suspected a neurosis (Schilder, 1951). More

    recently, Young and van de Wal (1996), in the first volume of Classic

    Cases in Neuropsychology, also raised doubts about the aetiology of this

    case, being surprised at the fact that Charcot had not taken into account

    the hypothesis of a hysterical form, despite being the greatest expert at

    the time in the field.

    UNSOLVED ASPECTS WITH REGARD TO THEORGANIC/FUNCTIONAL DISTINCTION

    Undoubtedly, the organic/functional distinction is a difficult subject both for

    the researchers of the past and for those today (Goldenberg, 2002). This is

    particularly true in cases in which organic and functional aspects are

    combined or when the structural and functional imaging examinations are

    difficult to interpret. Examples regarding the loss of visual mental imagery

    are found among cases described by Botez, Olivier, Vezina, Botez, and

    Kaufman (1985), Brain (1954), and Zeman et al. (2010). In particular, Case

    MX, reported by Zeman et al. (2010), who abruptly lost the ability to form

    visual images, showed the absence of a relevant organic cause on MRI but a

    significantly reduced activity within the posterior region network on a visual

    imagery fMRI task, as opposed to an increased frontal activation compared

    to controls.

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 499

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    22/26

    To further complicate this framework, there is evidence of disorders

    classically interpreted in psychopathological terms which can also occur as

    secondary to neurological conditions such as epilepsy or head injuries. This

    is precisely the case of the phenomena of depersonalisation and derealisation(Kenna & Seldman, 1965; Sierra, Lopera, Lambert, Phillips, & David, 2002).

    Moreover, it must be pointed out that the advent of refined functional

    imaging techniques have radically changed the approach to dysfunctional

    neural mechanisms of psychopathologic disorders. From indications of a

    vague and imprecise organicism we have moved to a clear physiopathological

    orientation. This aims to define brain metabolic-functional modifications

    also in functional cases, traditionally considered lacking in documentable

    structural or neurochemical brain correlate (Markowitsch, 1996, 2003;

    Tramoni et al., 2009). This deals with functional alterations, potentially of

    a reversible nature, that mimic true organic damage.

    Despite the above specifications and limitations, we believe it is possible

    to help the clinician to position patients in a functional or organic form of

    loss of visual imagery. Table 4 summarises the main discriminating

    TABLE 4

    Criteria for distinguishing between a functional or an organic loss of visual imagery

    Main features in patients with loss of visual imagery

    Psychogenic basis Organic basis

    Emphatic complaint about visual imagery

    deficits

    Rare complaint about visual imagery deficits

    Psychological stressors and/or traumas in past

    or recent medical history

    Lack of relevant psychological stressors and/or

    traumas in past or recent medical history

    Relevant depressive and anxious

    symptomatologies

    Lack of relevant depressive and anxious

    symptomatologies or, at the most, presence of

    reactive symptomatology of neurological

    disorder

    Presence of depersonalisation/derealisation Lack of depersonalisation/derealisation

    Psychotic disorders Lack of psychotic symptoms

    Lack of structural alterations as seen by staticneuroradiological examination (CT, NMR)

    Neuropsychological deficits of visual recognition(visual agnosia, prosopagnosia,

    achromatopsia, alexia)

    Possible presence of metabolic-functional

    modifications as detected by functional

    examinations (PET, fMRI, and others)

    Presence of structural alterations (commonly

    involving posterior areas) as detected by

    neuroradiological examination (CT, NMR)

    Evidence of metabolic-functional

    modifications as per functional

    examinations (PET, SPECT, fMRI, and

    others) congruent with detected neurological

    lesions

    500 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    23/26

    characteristics that in our opinion can help modern clinicians to differentiate

    functional or organic forms of loss of visual imagery.

    CONCLUSIONS

    The Charcot and Bernard case of visual imagery loss is a classic in clinical

    neuropsychology. Although a neurological valence has been attributed to

    this case for more than a century, the presence of analogous unknown

    European cases of loss of mental imagery in the psychiatric field, has led us

    to consider the hypothesis of a functional origin rather than organic. In

    order to assess the validity of such inference, we compared the symptoma-

    tology of Monsieur X with that found in cases of loss of visual mental

    images, both psychiatric and neurological, presented in the literature. Thecomparison highlights strong assonances of Monsieur X with those

    symptoms manifested by the patients with functionally based loss of visual

    imagery, and provides support to the hypothesis of a psychogenic base origin

    of the disorder. Such an assumption is formulated in the awareness that the

    gap between organic and functional closes when we consider functional

    neuroimaging evidence. This demonstrates that both conditions are able to

    alter*even if in a qualitatively different way*circumscribed cerebral areas

    selectively, hence causing specific cognitive deficits.

    Manuscript received 12 July 2010Revised manuscript received 7 January 2011

    First published online 23 May 2011

    REFERENCES

    Ackner, B. (1954). Depersonalisation. II. Clinical syndromes. Journal of Mental Science, 100,

    854872.

    Amabile, G., & Rizzo, P. (1966). Perdita della visione mentale. Contributo clinico. Rivista

    Sperimentale di Frenatria, 90, 11561163.

    Barbarotto, R., Laiacona, M., & Cocchini, G. (1996). A case of simulated, psychogenic or focal

    pure retrograde amnesia: Did an entire life become unconscious? Neuropsychologia, 34,

    575585.

    Bartolomeo, P. (2008). The neural correlates of visual mental imagery: An ongoing debate. Cortex,

    44, 107108.

    Basso, A., Bisiach, E., & Luzzatti, C. (1980). Loss of mental imagery: A case study.

    Neuropsychologia, 18, 435442.

    Beyn, E. S., & Knyazeva, G. R. (1962). The problem of prosopagnosia. Journal of Neurology,

    Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 25, 154158.

    Botez, M. I., Olivier, M., Vezina, J. L., Botez, T., & Kaufman, B. (1985). Defective revisualization:

    Dissociation between cognitive and imaginistic thought case report and a short review of the

    literature. Cortex, 21, 375389.

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 501

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    24/26

    Boyle, J., & Nielsen, J. M. (1954). Visual agnosia and loss of recall. Bulletin of Los Angeles

    Neurological Society, 19, 3942.

    Brain, R. W. (1954). Loss of visualization. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 47,

    288290.

    Charcot, M., & Bernard, D. (1883). Un cas de suppression brusque et isolee de la vision mentale

    des signes et des objets (formes et couleurs). Progres Medicale, 11, 568571.

    Chiaccio, L., Angelini, R., Orsini, A., & Grossi, D. (1987). Riconoscimento di figure di oggetti in

    prospettiva insolita e immagine mentale. Rivista di Neurologia, 57, 316320.

    Cotard, D. (1884). Perte de la vision mentale dans la melanconie ansieuse.Archives de Neurologie,

    7, 289295.

    Deleval, J., de Mol, J., & Noterman, J. (1983). La perte des images souvenirs. Acta Neurologica

    Belgica, 83, 6179.

    Della Sala, S., & Young, A. W. (2003). Quaglinos 1867 case of prosopoagnosia. Cortex, 39,

    533540.

    Deny, G., & Camus, P. (1905). Une forme dhypocondrie aberrante due a la perte de la coscience du

    corps. Revue Neurologique, 9, 461467.

    De Renzi, E., Lucchelli, F., Muggia, S., & Spinnler, H. (1997). Is memory loss without anatomical

    damage tantamount to a psychogenic deficit? The case of pure retrograde amnesia.

    Neuropsychologia, 35, 781794.

    DEsposito, M., Deter, J. A., Aguirre, G. K., Stallcup, M., Aslop, D. C., Tippet, L. J., & Farah,

    M. J. (1997). A functional MRI study of mental image generation. Neuropsychologia, 35,

    725730.

    Dinshaw, R., & Lishman, W. A. (1984). Seizures, dyslexia, and dysgraphia of psychogenic origin.

    Archives of Neurology, 41, 889890.

    Dugas, L. (1898). Un cas de depersonnalisation. Revue Philosophique,14, 500507. [Translated into

    English by Sierra, M., & Barrios, G. E. in Un cas de depersonnalisation.History of Psychiatry,

    7, 451461.]

    Farah, M. J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis. Cognition,

    18, 245272.

    Farah, M. J., Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). A case study of mental imagery deficit. Brain

    and Cognition, 8, 147164.

    Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., Mast, F. W., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2003). Visual imagery in cerebral

    visual dysfunction. Neurological Clinics of North America, 21, 631646.

    Giacheddu, M., Amabile, G., & Sanarelli, L. (1968). La perdita della visione mentale. Annali di

    Neurologia e Psichiatria, 62, 139162.

    Giannini, A., & Loprieno, M. (1964). Contributo allo studio del fenomeno della perdita della

    visione mentale. Rassegna di Studi Psichiatrici, 53, 293299.

    Giovanardi Rossi, P., & Muscatello, C. F. (1968). I disturbi dellesperienza del tempo nella

    schizofrenia iniziale con particolare riferimento alla perdita della visione mentale. Psichiatria

    Generale e dell eta Evolutiva, 6, 391404.

    Goldenberg, G. (1992). Loss of visual imagery and loss of visual knowledge: A case study.

    Neuropsychologia, 30, 10811099.

    Goldenberg, G. (1993). The neural basis of mental imagery. Baillieres Clinical Neurology, 2,

    265286.

    Goldenberg, G. (2002). Disentangling the organic from the psychic was as difficult in 1910 as

    it is today*a commentary on Liepmann (1910). Cortex, 38, 631633.

    Goldenberg, G., Podreka, I., Steiner, M., Willmes, K., Suess, E., & Deecke, L. (1989). Regional

    cerebral blood flow patterns in visual imagery. Neuropsychologia, 27, 641664.

    Goldenberg, G., Steiner, M., Podreka, I., & Deecke, L. (1992). Regional cerebral blood flow

    patterns related to verification of low- and high-imagery sentences. Neuropsychologia, 30,

    581586.

    502 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    25/26

    Grossi, D., Orsini, A., Modafferi, A., & Liotti, M. (1986). Visuoimaginal constructional apraxia:

    On a case of selective deficit of imagery. Brain and Cognition, 5, 255267.

    Hartmann, H. (1922). Ein Fall von Depersonalisation. Zeitschrift fur die gesamte Neurologie und

    Psychiatrie, 74, 593601.

    Issac, A., Marks, D. F., & Russell, D. G. (1986). An instrument for assessing imagery of movement:

    The vividness of movement imagery questionnaire (VMIQ). Journal of Mental Imagery, 10,

    2330.

    Kenna, J. C., & Sedman, G. (1965). Depersonalisation in temporal lobe epilepsy and the organic

    psychoses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 293299.

    Lambert, M. V., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L., Sierra, M., Hunter, E., & David, A. S. (2001). Visual

    imagery and depersonalisation. Psychopathology, 34, 259264.

    Levine, D. N., Warach, J., & Farah, M. (1985). Two visual systems in mental imagery: Dissociation

    of what and where in imagery disorders due to bilateral posterior cerebral lesions.

    Neurology, 35, 10101018.

    Macrae, D., & Trolle, E. (1956). The defect of function in visual agnosia. Brain, 79, 94110.

    Magalhaes Lemos [initial not reported]. (1906). Perte de la vision mentale des objets (formes et

    couleurs) dans la melancolie anxieuse. Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 6, 631.

    Markowitsch, H. J. (1996). Organic and psychogenic retrograde amnesia: Two sides of the same

    coin? Neurocase, 2, 357371.

    Markowitsch, H. J. (2003). Psychogenic amnesia. Neuroimage, 20, S132S135.

    Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Journal of

    Psychology, 64, 1724.

    Mayer-Gross, W. (1935). On depersonalisation.British Journal of Medical Psychology,15, 103122.

    Metha, Z., Newcombe, F., & de Haan, E. (1992). Selective loss of imagery in a case of visual

    agnosia.Neuropsychologia, 30, 645655.

    Moro, V., Berlucchi, G., Lerch, J., Tomaiuolo, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2008). Selective deficit of mental

    visual imagery with intact primary visual cortex and visual perception.Cortex, 44, 109118.

    Muscatello, C. F., & Giovanardi Rossi, P. (1967). Perdita della visione mentale e patologia

    dellesperienza temporale. Giornale di Psichiatria e Neuropatologia, 95, 765790.

    Nielsen, J. M. (1946). Agnosia, apraxia, aphasia. New York, NY: Hafner.

    Nielsen, J. M. (1955). Occipital lobes, dreams and psychosis. Journal of Nervous and Mental

    Disease, 121, 3032.

    Ogden, J. A. (1993). Visual object agnosia, prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, loss of visual imagery,

    and autobiographical amnesia following recovery from cortical blindness: Case M.H.

    Neuropsychologia, 31, 571589.

    Picard, R. (1919). Contribution a letude de la perte de la vision mentale chez les melancolique.

    Unpublished Doctorat en Medecine thesis, Imprimerie Centrale de LOuest, Paris.

    Reda, G. (1960). Perdita della visione mentale e depersonalizzazione nella psicosi depressiva. In

    C. Fazio (Ed.), Le sindromi depressive (pp. 95114). Turin, Italy: Edizioni Minerva Medica.

    Ribot, T. (1881). Les maladies de la memoire. Paris, France: Felix Alcan.

    Riddoch, M. J. (1990). Loss of visual imagery: A generation deficit. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 7,

    249273.

    Roland, P. E., & Gulyas, B. (1994). Visual imagery and visual representation. Trends in

    Neuroscience, 17, 281287.

    Saunders, W. A., & Barker, M. G. (1972). Dyslexia as cause of psychiatric disorder in adults.British

    Medical Journal, 4, 759761.

    Schilder, P. (1928). Depersonalization. In Introduction to Psychoanalytic Psychiatry (Nervous

    Mental Disease Monographs, Vol. 50, p. 120).

    Schilder, P. (1951). Brain and personality. New York, NY: International Universities Press.

    Senior, C., Hunter, E., Lambert, M., Medford, N. C., Sierra, M., Phillips, M. L., & David, A. S.

    (2001). Depersonalisation. The Psychologist, 14, 128132.

    CHARCOT AND BERNARD AND VISUAL IMAGERY LOSS 503

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2

  • 8/12/2019 Zago Stefano Et Al - Is Charcot and Bernard Case of Loss of Visual Imagery_neurological Impairment

    26/26

    Serra, L., Fadda, L., Buccione, I., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. A. (2007). Psychogenic and

    organic amnesia: A multidimensional assessment of clinical, neuroradiological, neuropsycho-

    logical and psychopathological features. Behavioual Neurology, 18, 5364.

    Shorvon, H. J., Hill, J. D. N., Burkitt, E., & Halstead, H. (1946). The depersonalization syndrome.

    Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 39, 779792.Sierra, M. (2009). Depersonalization: A new look at a neglected syndrome. Cambridge, UK:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Sierra, M., & Berrios, G. E. (2001). The phenomenological stability of depersonalization

    comparing the old with the new. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 629636.

    Sierra, M., Lopera, F., Lambert, M. V., Phillips, M. L., & David, A. S. (2002). Separating

    depersonalisation and derealisation: The relevance of the lesion method. Journal of

    Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72, 530532.

    Simeon, D., Guralnik, O., Hazlett, E. A., Spiegel-Cohen, J., Hollander, E., & Buchsbaum, M. S.

    (2000). Feeling unreal: A PET study of depersonalization disorder. American Journal of

    Psychiatry, 157, 17821788.

    Solms, M. (1997). The neuropsychology of dreams: A clinico-anatomical study. Mahwah, NJ:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Stengel, E. (1948). The syndrome of visual alexia with colour agnosia.Journal of Mental Science,

    94, 4658.

    Tramoni, E., Aubert-Khalfa, S., Guye, M., Ranjeva, J. P., Felician, O., & Ceccaldi, M. (2009).

    Hypo-retrieval and hyper-suppression mechanisms in functional amnesia. Neuropsychologia,

    47, 611624.

    Trojano, L., & Grossi, D. (1994). A critical review of mental imagery defects. Brain and Cognition,

    24, 213243.

    Wimmer, A. (1919). Sur letat de conscience pendant la stupeur. Les psychomecanismes du delire

    de negation de Cotard. Annales Medico-Psychologiques, 11, 1444.

    Young, A. W., & van de Wal, C. (1996). Charcots case of impaired imagery. In C. Code, C. W.

    Wallesh, Y. Joanette, & A. R. Lecours (Eds.), Classic cases in neuropsychology(Vol. 1, pp. 31

    44). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Zago, S., & Policardi, E. (1996). Perdita di immagini mentali visive: studio di un caso.

    Neuropsychologia Latina, 2, 29.

    Zago, S., Policardi, E., Burgio, F., & Sartori, G. (1997). Visual imagery in a category-specific

    impaired patient. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 195202.

    Zeman, A. Z., Della Sala, S., Torrens, L. A., Gountouna, V. E., McGonigle, D. J., & Logie, R. H.

    (2010). Loss of imagery phenomenology with intact visuo-spatial task performance: A case of

    blind imagination. Neuropsychologia, 48, 145155.

    504 ZAGO ET AL.

    Downloadedby[189.170.4.14

    9]at21:0921September201

    2