21
THE JOURNAL OF FINANCE VOL, L, NO, 2 • JUNE 1995 Performance Persistence STEPHEN J. BROWN and WILLIAM N. GOETZMANN* ABSTRACT We explore performance persistence in mutual funds using absolute and relative benchmarks. Our sample, largely free of survivorship bias, indicates tbat relative risk-adjusted performance of mutual funds persists; however, persistence is mostly due to funds that lag the S&P 500. A probit analysis indicates that poor perfor- mance increases the probability of disappearance. A year-by-year decomposition of tbe persistence effect demonstrates that the relative performance pattern depends upon the time period observed, and it is correlated across managers. Consequently, it is due to a common strategy tbat is not captured by standard stylistic categories or risk adjustment procedures. A NUMBER OF EMPIRICAL studies demonstrate that the relative performance of equity mutual funds persists from period to period. Carlson (1970) finds evidence that funds with ahove-median returns over the preceding year typically repeat their superior performance. Elton and Gruher (1989) cite a 1971 Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) study that indicates similar persistence in risk-adjusted mutual fund rankings. Lehmann and Modest (1987) report some evidence of persistent mutual fund alphas, and Grinblatt and Titman (1988, 1992) show that the effect is statistically significant. Goetzmann and Ihbotson (1994) conclude that the performance persistence phenomenon is present in raw and risk-adjusted returns to equity funds at observation intervals from one month to three years. In an in-depth study focused on growth funds, Hendricks, Patel, and Zeckhauser (1993) show that the performance persistence phenomenon appears robust to a variety of risk-adjustment measures. All of these studies lend strong support to the conventional wisdom that the track record of a fund manager contains information about future performance. ' Brown is from the Leonard Stern School of Business, New York University. Goetzmann is from tbe Scbool of Management, Yale University, We thank Bent Christiansen, Edwin Elton, Mark Grinblatt. Martin Gruber, Gur Huberman, Roger Ibbotson, Philippe Jorion, Robert Ko- rajczyk, Josef Lakonisbok, Jayendu Patel, Nadav Poles, Stepben Ross, William Scbwert, Erik Sirri, an anonymous referee, and editor Rene Stulz for belpful comments. We thank participants in the following conferences and workshops for their input: the 1993 Western Finance Associa- tion Meetings, tbe October 1993 NBER program on asset pricing, the 1994 AFA session on tbe bebavior of institutional investors, and finance workshops at Columbia Business School. The University of Texas, the University of Illinois at Champagne, and tbe Yale School of Manage- ment. We also thank Nadav Peles for programming assistance and Stephen Livingston for data collection. We thank Ihbotson Associates and Morningstar, Inc., for providing data used in the analysis. All errors are tbe sole responsibility of the authors, Roger Ibbotson contributed substantially to the preliminary version of this article. Tbis article was formerly circulated as "Attrition and Mutual Fund Perfornnance," 679

Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

 

Citation preview

Page 1: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

THE JOURNAL OF FINANCE • VOL, L, NO, 2 • JUNE 1995

Performance Persistence

STEPHEN J. BROWN and WILLIAM N. GOETZMANN*

ABSTRACT

We explore performance persistence in mutual funds using absolute and relativebenchmarks. Our sample, largely free of survivorship bias, indicates tbat relativerisk-adjusted performance of mutual funds persists; however, persistence is mostlydue to funds that lag the S&P 500. A probit analysis indicates that poor perfor-mance increases the probability of disappearance. A year-by-year decomposition oftbe persistence effect demonstrates that the relative performance pattern dependsupon the time period observed, and it is correlated across managers. Consequently,it is due to a common strategy tbat is not captured by standard stylistic categoriesor risk adjustment procedures.

A NUMBER OF EMPIRICAL studies demonstrate that the relative performance ofequity mutual funds persists from period to period. Carlson (1970) findsevidence that funds with ahove-median returns over the preceding yeartypically repeat their superior performance. Elton and Gruher (1989) cite a1971 Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC) study that indicates similarpersistence in risk-adjusted mutual fund rankings. Lehmann and Modest(1987) report some evidence of persistent mutual fund alphas, and Grinblattand Titman (1988, 1992) show that the effect is statistically significant.Goetzmann and Ihbotson (1994) conclude that the performance persistencephenomenon is present in raw and risk-adjusted returns to equity funds atobservation intervals from one month to three years. In an in-depth studyfocused on growth funds, Hendricks, Patel, and Zeckhauser (1993) show thatthe performance persistence phenomenon appears robust to a variety ofrisk-adjustment measures. All of these studies lend strong support to theconventional wisdom that the track record of a fund manager containsinformation about future performance.

' Brown is from the Leonard Stern School of Business, New York University. Goetzmann isfrom tbe Scbool of Management, Yale University, We thank Bent Christiansen, Edwin Elton,Mark Grinblatt. Martin Gruber, Gur Huberman, Roger Ibbotson, Philippe Jorion, Robert Ko-rajczyk, Josef Lakonisbok, Jayendu Patel, Nadav Poles, Stepben Ross, William Scbwert, ErikSirri, an anonymous referee, and editor Rene Stulz for belpful comments. We thank participantsin the following conferences and workshops for their input: the 1993 Western Finance Associa-tion Meetings, tbe October 1993 NBER program on asset pricing, the 1994 AFA session on tbebebavior of institutional investors, and finance workshops at Columbia Business School. TheUniversity of Texas, the University of Illinois at Champagne, and tbe Yale School of Manage-ment. We also thank Nadav Peles for programming assistance and Stephen Livingston for datacollection. We thank Ihbotson Associates and Morningstar, Inc., for providing data used in theanalysis. All errors are tbe sole responsibility of the authors, Roger Ibbotson contributedsubstantially to the preliminary version of this article. Tbis article was formerly circulated as"Attrition and Mutual Fund Perfornnance,"

679

Page 2: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

680 The Journal of Finance

In this article, we explore the phenomenon of performance persistence inequity mutual funds using a sample that contains defunct as well as surviv-ing funds. The sample shows that poorly performing funds disappear morefrequently from the mutual fund universe, suggesting that selection biasconcerns can be relevant to mutual fund performance studies. We documentperformance persistence on this broad sample and show that it is robust toadjustments for risk. We find that much of the persistence is due to fundsthat repeatedly lag passive benchmarks. Though most previous studies haveaggregated the results from different time periods in order to increase thepower of tests designed to identify performance persistence, we find it in-structive to break the analysis down on a year-by-year basis. This temporaldisaggregation provides some clues regarding the source of the persistencephenomenon. While most years winners and losers repeat, occasionally theeffect is dramatically reversed. These reversals suggest there are two possiblereasons for persistence. First, persistence is correlated across managers.Consequently, it is likely due to a common strategy that is not captured bystandard stylistic categories or risk adjustment procedures. Second, whilelosing funds have an increased probability of disappearance or merger, not allof them are eliminated. The market fails to fully discipline underperformers,and their presence in sample contributes to the pattern of relative persis-tence. The implications of our results for investors are that the persistencephenomenon is a useful indicator of which funds to avoid. However, evidencethat the pattern can be used to beat absolute, risk-adjusted benchmarksremains weak. Future research should address the issues of cross-fundcorrelation and the persistence of poor performers. This article is organizedas follows. Section I describes the database and the method of data collection.Section II considers the determinants of fund disappearance. Section IIIreports the results of the performance persistence tests. Section IV concludes.

I. Data

The Weisenberger Investment Companies Service reports information aboutvirtually all publicly offered open-end mutual funds on an annual basis. Datawere collected by hand from their Mutual Funds Panorama, a data section inWeisenberger (1977 through 1989), for the years 1976 through 1988, for allfirms listed as common stock funds, or those specialty funds that invested incommon stock (typically sector funds). For each fund, we record the name asit appeared that year, the year of origin, the fund objective, the net assetvalue at the end of the year, the net asset value per share at the beginning ofthe period, the twelve-month percentage change in net asset value per shareadjusted for capital gains distributions, the income return, the capital gainsdistributions, and the expense ratio. We calculate the total return inclusive ofcapital appreciation, income, and capital gains distributions. In some cases,one or more of these data were not reported, and this prevents total return

Page 3: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 681

calculations.^ We assign a unique number to each fund. Footnote at the end ofthe Panorama section indicate merged funds and name changes of funds.When one fund was merged into another, the acquired fund is deemed tohave disappeared, while the acquiring fund is deemed to have continued inoperation. Though Weisenberger seeks to collect information on all open-endedmutual funds, they may not have included small funds, or funds for whichtheir data were incomplete. Since reporting to Weisenberger is at least inpart discretionary, the data base may not be completely free of survivorshipbias. In addition, by using only those funds for which an annual return mayhe calculated, we omit all funds that existed for less than one year. Wetherefore exclude from the sample the year that funds do poorly and merge orfail.^ Despite these limitations, the Weisenberger database that we haveassembled allows us to directly examine the process of fund attrition.

Table I reports the number of funds extant each year and breaks them intofive categories by objective. The sample ranges from 372 funds in 1976 to 829funds in 1988, with most of the growth occurring in the period 1983 to 1988.This breakdown by category shows the evolution of funds' styles throughtime, in a way that a backward-looking sample could not. Notice that in theearly years of the sample, the Maximum Capital Gain category representednearly 30 percent of the extant mutual funds. Presumably, these are fundsthat invested in high growth potential, or high price/earnings ratio (P/E)stocks. By the end of the sample, the popularity of this investment style hadshrunk by half, when measured by number of funds. Table I also indicatesthe capitalization by category. The Weisenberger equity mutual fund uni-verse grew from $37.2 billion in 1976 to $166.5 billion in 1988.

Table II reports the equally-weighted and value-weighted mean each yearfor the whole sample and for those funds that survived for the entire sample.The returns to the S&P 500 and the Vanguard Index Trust (an S&P Indexfund) are reported for purposes of comparison. Note that the average returnsfor mutual funds differ significantly from year to year from the benchmarks.In 1979, for instance, mutual funds appear to substantially outperform theS&P 500. In most years of the 1980s they substantially underperform. Thedeviation of the aggregate mutual fund performance index from the S&P 500may be due to differences in composition. S&P 500 returns lagged small stockreturns for the years 1977 through 1983, and they exceeded small stockreturns for the period 1984 through 1987. This is similar to the mutual fund

The fund returns are calculated as follows:

^NAV, D,Return, = NAV, ,

where ANAV, is the change in net asset value per share adjusted to eapital gains distributionsas reported by Weisenberger, D, is the dollar-denominated investment income per share at time(, and NAV, _ [ is the net asset per share in the preceding period.

^ The Elton et al. (1993) study takes great care to avoid this source of survivorship bias bycomputing returns for a buy-and-hold position in mutual funds from 1964 through 1984,accounting for merger terms of funds that are combined with other funds.

Page 4: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

682 The Journal of Finance

3 d cf cB

.2 S 3

£ c

0 _

0 a c

5 3

P CO C

I/I-an3

[>:.

o

o

£

Page 5: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 683

Table II

Annual Summary Statistics For Equity Mutual FundsExtant in 1988 represents the sample of funds that were existing in 1988, EW refers to equallyweighted mean, and VW refers to value-weighted mean. The value-weighted mean is calculatedby the capitalization of the fund at the beginning of the period. Vanguard represents theVanguard Market Index Trust. Me lns are calculated over the period 1977 through 1987.

Year

197719781979198019811982198319841985198619871988

Mean

Whole Sample

EWMean

0.0150.1080.2920.334

-0.0170.2330.214

-0.0240.2670.1580.0220.133

0.145

VWMean

-0.0280.1020.2610.326

- 0.0400.2200.212

-0.0190.2700.1640.0280.139

0.136

Extant in

EWMean

0.0290.1250.3060.351

-0.0170.2480.224

-0.0220.2730.1620.0240.133

0.153

1988

VWMean

-0.0250.1020.2600.321

-0.0360.2230.218

-0.0160.2720.1650.0300.139

0.138

CJoneBy

EWMean

-0.0100.0750.2610.287

-0.0150.1750.159

-0.0390.2150.113

- 0.021NA

0.109

1988

VWMean

-0.0400.1030.2640.356

- 0.0660.1950.141

-0.0610.2520.148

-0.078NA

0.110

Benchmarks

S&P500

- 0.0720.0660.1840.324

- 0.0490.2140.2250.0630.3220.1850.0520.168

0.140

Van-guard

-0.0780.0580.1780.312

-0.0520.2000.2120.0600.3080.1790.0510.161

0.132

pattern. Mutual funds exceeded the S&P 500 for the period 1977 through1982, and then lagged over the period 1983 through 1988.'

The effect of survivorship upon the estimated annual return to investmentin mutual funds is not trivial. Tahle II divides the whole sample into twocategories, those funds that had disappeared by 1988, and those funds thatwere extant in 1988. The equally-weighted average of defunct funds is belowthe equally-weighted average of the entire sample for every year since 1981.This is not true for the value-weighted indices, which track more closely. Theimplication is that most of the difference is due to the attrition of small fundsthat performed poorly and were shut down or merged into other funds. Wefind the difference hetween returns composed of the entire sample andreturns composed of funds extant in 1988 to be 0.8 percent per year. Whenreturns are scaled by capitalization, however, the margin is much lower: 0.2percent per year.'* This is not surprising, since we expect larger funds to havea higher probability of survival, and thus weigh more heavily in the meancalculation.

• Lakonishok, Shleifer, and Vishny (1992) find a similar result for pension funds and alsoattribute it to a small firm effect.

* These results are consistent with those reported in Grinblatt and Titman (1988) and Malkiel(1995).

Page 6: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

684 The Journal of Finance

II. Fund Disappearance

The Weisenberger database allows us to examine the factors contributingto fund disappearance. Mutual funds typically disappear as a result of beingterminated or merged into other funds. As a result, fund disappearance is amanagement decision, which is presumably based upon fund profitability,and ultimately upon consumer demand. Studies of consumer behavior andthe dollar flows into mutual funds indicate that investors select funds on thebasis of past performance.^ We also expect the age of the fund to influenceconsumer response to returns, since older funds have a longer track recordfrom which to infer differential performance. Age and fund size may alsointeract with track record as a determinant of survival. A thorough descrip-tion of the process that governs fund survival would require publicly unavail-able revenue and expense data. Thus, we estimate a reduced form model thatcaptures the major factors contributing to the decision to close or merge afund.

The first specification models disappearance as a function of relative return(i.e., fund return in the year less average fund return that year), relative size(fund size less average fund size that year), expense ratio, and age (expressedin years since fund inception). The results are reported in Table III. Allvariables are significant at the 95 percent level, and only one, the expenseratio, increases the probability of disappearance. The signs on the relativereturn coefficient are consistent with previous research into customer re-sponse to investment performance. The probit shows that poor performers notonly lose customers, but also have a higher probability of disappearing. Thenegative coefficient on relative size indicates that the bigger the fund, theless likely it is to disappear. Of course, it is difficult to separate this effectfrom past performance, since a good track record attracts customers. Thepositive coefficient on expense indicates that funds with higher expenseratios also have a higher probability of disappearance. This is consistent withthe existence of some fixed operating costs for funds—if all costs werevariable, companies would have little incentive to shut even the small fundsdown. It is also interesting to note the negative coefficient on age. Youngerfunds clearly have a higher probability of disappearance.

In the second model we include additional variables that might explainfund disappearance. These are the lagged relative return, relative new money,and relative new money lagged.^ New money is included because we conjec-ture that the decision to close the fund depends upon customer response toreturns, rather than returns themselves. In addition, we consider otherinteraction terms among variables. For instance, it seems plausible that largefunds might be less susceptible to closure as a result of poor performance orshorter histories. In addition, older funds with poor returns might be lesslikely to close than younger ones. To account for these effects, we specifyinteraction terms between relative return and age, relative return and rela-

"SeePatel, Hendricks, and Zeckhauaer (1990), Kane, Santini, and Aher (1991), Ippolito (1992),Lakonishok, Shleifer. and Vishny (1992), and Sirri and Tufano (1992), for examples.

^ New money is calculated as: NM, = [NAV, - (1 + r,)NAV,_^]/NAV^^ ,.

Page 7: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 685

,2 "V fl « ' •¥ 01 -C

> :2

01 in - - (DCD 00 Ol (Nf-i (N 1-i NIN — ^ --^

qd oII

o O) « t> oqw CS3 d d 6

O (D O) CTJ ' t CJlO ^ O O ID O^ o p o c-q od d d d d dI I I

O O (N CO •-(

CO in m -H (N inC O O OS O Oq p p -H ^ pd d d d d d

I I I

O O Q9 P 9•odd

00 OCO Oao -H

I I I

COod1

ood

ot ^od1

CO

GOod

cd

CO t j " T-H

o •"• 5o ^ cd o d o1

o

IN (Mf CsCO " *

o o oI I

1

34.JOJk>

>

> .

coE>c

>

> i

coE>0)c

>

>.caE>c

>

1

N

'S0)>

' S -"C OS

.1 5 5 .1

a o ) ^ ^ a)J2i2 m w

Page 8: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

686 The Journal of Finance

tive size, and age and relative size. The results of this specification aresomewhat surprising. Relative returns and lagged relative returns are bothsignificant predictors, but new money is not. The sign on new money indi-cates that past positive inflows decrease the probability of closure, althoughthe effect is weak. The third specification shows that eliminating returns andreplacing them with lagged values of new money makes the first lag of newmoney a significant predictor of disappearance, but not earlier lags. This mayhe due to the fact that fund managers care about total growth, rather thannew money, or it may be due to our inability to adequately model the pastand future interactions between returns and new money. The coefficients onthe interaction terms among the other variahles all proved to be insignifi-cantly different from zero.

The fourth model includes longer lags for returns. The rationale for consid-ering longer lags is the possibility that the extended track record contributesto the closure decision. When we include three years of past relative returnsin the model, we find all three years to be significant or near-significantpredictors of fund disappearance. The coefficients on past relative returnslagged one and two years are of the same sign as the relative returns in thefirst model, and a bit less that half of the magnitude. In other words, poorperformance in earlier years is not as powerful a factor in fund disappear-ance, but certainly is an important one.

The results of the probit analysis suggest that past performance overseveral years is a major determinant of fund disappearance. Surprisingly, netfund growth, at least as we have defined it, contributes only marginally toprediction of fund disappearance. Other variables clearly play a role inpredicting fund closure. Size and age are negatively related to fund disap-pearance, and expense ratio is positively related to fund disappearance.

III. Repeat Performers

Following Brown et al. (1992) and Goetzmann and Ibbotson (1994), wetrack the evolution of the mutual fund universe using a nonparametricmethodology based upon contingency tables. Table IV reports the frequencycounts for each year. The table identifies a fund as a winner in the currentyear if it is above or equal to the median of all funds with returns reportedthat year. The same criterion is used to identify it as a winner or loser for thefollowing period. Thus, Winner-Winner (WW) for 1976 is the count of thewinners in 1976 that were also winners in 1977. The same principle definesthe other categories. Winner-Gone indicates the number of winners thatdisappeared from the sample in the following period. No Data indicates thatWeisenberger listed the fund for the following period, but was unable tocollect data necessary to calculate a total return. New Fund indicates thenumber of new funds that appear in that year. Cross-Product Ratio reportsthe odds ratio of the number of repeat performers to the number of those thatdo not repeat; that is, {WW*LL)/(WL*LW). The null hypothesis that perfor-mance in the first period is unrelated to performance in the second periodcorresponds to an odds ratio of one. In large samples with independent

Page 9: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 687

observations, the standard error of the natural log of the odds ratio is wellapproximated.'

Brown et al. (1992) show that fund attrition and cross-fund dependenciestend to bias the cross-product ratio test toward rejection." The degree of thisbias in the cross-product ratio test is dependent both upon the correlationstructure of the mutual fund universe and upon the attrition rate. To addressthis problem, we bootstrap the distribution of the odds ratio conditional uponan actual correlation matrix of mutual fund returns, and upon attrition ratesfor winners and losers. In effect, we replicate the effect of fund attrition,cross-sectional dependencies, and heteroskedasticity upon the test statistics.^

In Table IV we report the test statistic for the odds ratio test, as well as itsbootstrap probability value, conditional upon the sample correlation matrix offund returns and the observed attrition rates. Under both the biased and thecorrected hypothesis tests, we find that seven years (eight years for thebias-corrected distributions) of the sample indicate significant positivepersistence, and two years indicate significant negative persistence. Thebootstrapped probability values generally agree with the theoretical distri-bution of the test statistics and alternative procedures designed to addresssurvivorship.'"

^ This is given as:

\og(odds ratio) W,W W.L L,W L,L

See Christensen (1990) p. 40, for instance.Patel and Zeckhauser (1992) also investigate the distribution of the performance persistence

test statistics under the alternative of a performance threshold. They observe that there aresome interesting consequences to dividing the samples up into octiles each period, rather thaninto halves. Their simulations yield an interesting result. Performance may reverse for thepoorest performing group of managers who survive both periods.

To simulate the distribution of the log odds ratio, we used a de-meaned two-year sample ofmonthly mutual fund returns (obtained from Morningstar, Inc.) over the period 1987 to 1988,from which we selected a sample without replacement, of size corresponding to tbe sum of WW,LW, WL, and LL for the given year. We simulated return series through randomization over thedimension of time, and eliminated the appropriate number of funds in the appropriate category.For losing funds, we assume that the poorest performers would be eliminated. This represents aconservative approach, since it maximizes any potential bias of the sort reported in Brown et al.(1992). We calculate the odds ratio for the 2 x 2 table of winners and losers, and the likelihoodratio statistic for the contingency table including funds that disappeared. Tbis is performed 100times to generate simulated distributions that correspond to the null hyopthesis of no perfor-mance persistence, conditional upon a typical correlation structure, fund variances, and actualattrition rates. The simulated distributions are used for comparison to the actual statistics. Notethat this procedure relies upon a variance-covariance matrix that is singular, since there aremore securities than time periods. Thus, we are unable to completely address the issue of thesampling error of the statistic.

For comparison to the simulated distributions of cross-product ratios, we applied standardlimited dependent variable procedures to tbe problem of estimating year by year cross-sectionalrelationships between returns, conditional upon survival. Using the inverse Mills ratio estimatedfrom tbe probit regression helps explain some differential in annual performance, but does littleto affect the general performance persistence results.

Page 10: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

688 The Journal of Finance

0.-5 a i. *i ii ^

I M0)

Si

3C

sffiO

ndi

S

"3u«

m

o

n .+i

.J2 03 •"• - , <u

I il•2 ° S

=2 n «

.5 ffi

•c

o o .2

^1£o

OOOOOO'-'CTOOOO

- t in (D C- m COI

, - . O O Tf ^t tp

W m ^ t D O t N - H r H - H t N t M O

Page 11: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 689

The disaggregation reveals some interesting features of the persistencephenomenon. We find evidence of significant persistence seven or eight out oftwelve years. While persistence is more common, it is important to emphasizethat reversal also occurs. One of the years that indicated a significantreversal pattern was 1987. Winning funds in 1987 tended to he losing fundsin 1988. Malkiel (1995) finds reversals in two of the years following oursample period, which suggests that the probability of reversal is high andconfirms that the strongest evidence for repeat performance is over the late1970s and early 1980s.

The reversals also indicate that persistence is correlated across managers.This is important because it tells us that persistence is probably not due toindividual managers selecting stocks that are overlooked or ignored by othermanagers. Whatever the cause of winning, it is evidently a group phe-nomenon. While this correlation in persistence is consistent with recentlyidentified herding behavior among equity fund managers (see Grinblatt,Titman, and Wermers (1994)), it is also consistent with correlated dynamicportfolio strategies, such as portfolio insurance (see Connor and Korajczyk(1991)).

A. Risk Adjustment

One possible explanation for the secular trend in performance persistenceis that systematic risk differs across managers. The annual frequency of theWeisenberger data makes it difficult to use traditional risk adjustments. Toaddress this prohlem, we use the Morningstar monthly database for theperiod 1976 through 1988 for a subset of the funds in the Weisenbergersample. By merging these two datasets, we obtain fund characteristics aswell as monthly return data for a substantial subset of the mutual funduniverse. We use this merged database to model fund betas and residualerrors as linear functions of other mutual fund characteristics. We specifya traditional single index model, as well as the Elton et al. (1993) three-index model, and report estimates for the coefficients and residual errors inTable V.'' Beta and residual risk measures differ significantly according tofund classification, prior year size and expense ratios, and period sinceinception of the fund. The R^ indicates that the model performs well, andrankings of three index beta measures by fund characteristic correspond tothose reported by Elton et al. (1993) on the basis of annual data. We then usethe estimated coefficients from this model to extrapolate beta and residualrisk measures on the basis of characteristics of all funds in the Weisenbergersample.

In the table, the Growth fund classification represents the base case. The linear model isdescribed in the notes to Table V. It was estimated via OLS and GLS, the latter being used tocorrect for heteroskedasticity. Results for the two models were nearly indistinguishable, and wereport the GLS estimates. The estimation of forecasting of mutual fund betas and factorsensitivities as a function of observable factors is a topic of on-going research. See, for instance,Ferson and Schadt (1995).

Page 12: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

690 The Journal of Finance

Hi C

CO iJ

OS ; = •" ^

•ffl i ' i i

en

T: oi aj

o 2

X —

"SJSo

3n

c

3OH

ca3c:0

•"3T 3^Q

CJO.01

-4-)

(04 J

bB

c

lorn

C£3O

'SIU

O.

'Cen

-o

c3

<*--T3CcU

c.2in

£i

le r

eg

c

stim

2

C XI

5i o

• w

E X1-. ra _

B T I<U > Xi

O O ir. y^ i : 3 «

^ ^ 'C i3

G m £

.3"S

C 3

o s7 i= E

- " £

be ,3 -5 ..2

pa

o

CQ

Sfi c i - p cvi - p p p p p• q - d d d d d d d d dII I I I I II

o t N T r m O O O Op c p -^ p p P P p po o o ' o o c o o o oI I I I

p p p Po o o o o o d o o o

p p p p p po o o o o c o o o o

p p ^o o o o o o o o d o

11.2 S

S ^ S - . E . S

in "

CO ^

d QJ' c.

CM• < * •

II

d Q

IM

- p - l

Q

Page 13: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 691

The persistence tests for risk-adjusted returns are represented in Table VI.Risk adjustment does not appear to affect the pattern of persistence. In part,this may be due to the fact that systematic risk differences across managersas estimated by the model are not great. Depending upon which risk adjust-ment measures are used, we find that from five to seven years show evidenceof significant persistence, and, as with the raw returns, 1980 to 1981 and1987 to 1988 show evidence of a significant reversal in the pattern.

Brown et al. (1992) suggest using an appraisal ratio (the alpha measuredin standard deviation units) to measure persistence because, in the presenceof survivorship, ex post superior returns and alphas appear positively relatedto idiosyncratic risk. Scaling hy this risk reduces the bias. The table reportstests using the single index and multiple index appraisal ratio. Neitherappears to reduce the evidence for persistence. When single index appraisalratios are used as opposed to Capital Asset Pricing Model (CAPM) alphas, theperformance persistence pattern changes marginally, hut the changes are notstatistically significant.

An alternative way of adjusting for risk is to identify funds according totheir style category. Is the repeat performance in the sample driven by thestyles effect, or hy individual fund deviations from the style average? Weaddress this question by examining the performance persistence pattern ofdeviations from the average style return. The last panel in Table VI showsthat the pattern of performance persistence is little affected by subtracting offthe style benchmark. Clearly, if the Weisenberger style codes are meaningful,then the performance persistence in the sample is not driven by picking thewinning management style each year. The fund reversals are due to correla-tion across fund managers; however, convential stylistic classifications fail tocontrol for this correlation.

B. Absolute Benchmarks

In this section, we consider the effect of redefining winner as a fund thatexceeds an absolute, rather than a relative benchmark. The simplest bench-mark is the one that would have been most familiar to industry participantsover the period of our study: the S&P 500. Figure 1 shows the effect ofredefining a winner as a mutual fund that beat the total return of the S&P500 in a given year. Notice that the absolute repeat-winner and repeat-loserpattern follows its own trend through time that closely matches the relativesuccess of mutual funds reported in Table I. Over the second half of thesample, repeat-losers dramatically dominate. When results are aggregatedacross years, most of the persistence phenomenon is due to repeat-losersrather than to repeat-winners.

Table VI reports the result of using a risk-adjusted absolute benchmark.When a winner is defined as a fund with a positive alpha, the aggregatepersistence pattern is only slightly affected. However, on a desegregatedbasis, we find that much of the effect is due to only a few statisticallysignificant years in the sample period. Five of the years show significant

Page 14: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

692 The Journal of Finance

q M

ir.)•-<

53 0

'-*

49 0

C^O)

08 0

CO

ac

in(N

90 0

28 1

o

30 1

>—1

IM

40 0

in

97 0

00

26 1

CO

27 1

60 0

I I

^ C O - ^ t N C O t X ) O C Q

d c - i > - " — i i - i c ^ [ N d

Page 15: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 693

400

1B76 1977 T07B 1870 1080 10B1 1982 1983 1984 1885 1980 1987Ono-Yaar Taat, Bgglnning In Yamr

Lagana

• loMr-loaor •'.-'.{M l loaar-wlnnar \ |

winnar-loswwinnar-wtnnar

Figure 1. Frequency of Repeat Losers and Winners. The figure shows the effect ofdefining a winner as a mutual fund that beat the total return to the S&P 500 in a given year.The bars indicate the number of winning and losing funds each year that were winners or losersin the following year. Winner-Winner indicates a fund whose return exceeded the S&P 500 year.

positive persistence, two of the years show significant negative persistence,and five are ambiguous. While not reported in the table, we find that addingback expenses to returns also makes little difference in the overall persis-tence results for growth funds—consistent losers are not simply those withhigher fees. The net effect of redefining winner in absolute terms is to reducethe reliability of the persistence effect for mutual funds. It matters littlewhether more sophisticated measure of absolute performance, such as multi-ple-factor appraisal ratios or positive alphas, are used.

C. Investment Implications

Can the persistence effect be used to earn excess risk-adjusted returns? Inother words, can it be used to beat the market? Insight into this question canbe gained by considering gradations of performance finer than the binaryWinner-Loser classification we have used to this point. In Table VII we reportaverage realized second year returns and alphas to a portfolio strategy wherewe invest an equal amount in funds that fall in each octile by total return inthe first year. At the aggregate level, the results correspond to those reportedin the earlier studies: top-octile performers do well, and bottom octile per-formers do poorly. It is interesting to note that these results are not sensitiveto the choice of benchmark; we show results for the CAPM alpha computedusing total returns on the S & P 500 Index, and for an equally weightedaverage of mutual funds in the sample (assuming, in this case, that the betas

Page 16: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

694 The Journal of Finance

inlO

d

• ^

COiri

INO

d

CDCD

d•-H

d

inin

d

Bes

t

CO

;D

to

CO

Wor

a(B

est)

8.70

10.1

7

CM(Nt-

coinCD

6.48

rHIOIO

4.41

7.63

17.4

8

0000Tf

00OD

13.1

5

to

CM1-H

1.21

0.04

SO

T

o

orH

SO

T

1-H

o

1-H

q

8.62

4.64

inCD^^

O)cno

1.04

T-H

oc13

1.14

(2.2

9(1

.46)

.75)

o

.51)

o

(0.5

9)01

)o1

0.76

)

8.70

4.29

inCOT-H

CDCDO

09

0

t-

o1

1.47

(2.1

5(1

.73)

CO

Io

(66

0)

sCDO1

1.83

)

COIN

03t-Oi r1-H

o cCO rH

1 1

in c-cc ^d d

I 1

00 t CO « o:"^ 00 Ol Oi tor-i OS d CO -H

O CO

I I

cc o u

Page 17: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 695

p.

5

<:£> ^ T-i CN

0 0 0)

91-H

01COO)

69.73

CO

oCD

1457

COOl

rH

CO

C^

.58

18toCOCD

55

in

37

CO

COCO

o

01

o

37

CM

rH00

in

oin

98

rH

1

CDino1

mCOCO1

50

00

t

c;

o00

19

CO1

Ol

oCO1

O)

CO98

CMOSCM

CD00CO1

35

01

COCO

Ol

1

20-14

.-H

CD

1

CMCO

1

11

Ol O) CM t>

IM CO

CO CM• oCO CD

CO

CO

01

CM

1-H

O1

68

CDCOCJl

rHCO

CO

1

CMCDCM

1

CO

CO

in

72

CM

1

CO

rH

oCD

CO

1

00t>

56

1

o

1

1-H

in

1

CO

65

in

1

inOl1

CDCD

CO

1

01CO1

48

o

CM

o

34

•f m CO lo

01 01 01 01om (75 m

nn 00m

Page 18: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

696 The Journal of Finance

of rank portfolios are all unity). Similar results are found for the three indexbenchmark, as well as using the S & P 500 total return as a benchmark.

We also report the effect of a simulated strategy of buying winners andshorting losers, indicated as the Best-Worst column in the top panel. We findthat, if such a strategy were feasible, its benefits depend considerably uponthe poor returns of those funds in the lowest octile. What happens if weeliminate these "bottom feeders" from the sample? When the persistent losersare eliminated, where persistent loser is defined as being in the lowest octileof two-year returns, the mean excess return to the strategy is positive, butinsignificant.^^ The effect upon earlier tests of excluding bottom feeders isreported in the final two columns of Table VI. The significance is practicallyeliminated when the persistent losers are eliminated from the sample, andwinners are defined as those with positive alphas or positive appraisal ratios.

Of course, investors are concerned with risk as well as return. In the toppanel of Table VII, the average betas for the lowest and highest octiles arepractically the same, but the annual standard deviation of returns to theoctile portfolios differ considerably. Chasing winners is clearly a volatilestrategy. While one might argue that investors are unconcerned with totalrisk because it is diversifiable, the strong correlation of winning funds notedin the earlier section suggests that diversification is not consistent withpicking a portfolio of winners. Differences in risk are clearly evident in theannual decomposition of octile portfolio returns. While performance is corre-lated across all eight groups, the track record of the top octile is the mostvariable. It is interesting to note that, because of the high relative volatility oftop octile funds, when they fail, they fail dramatically. For instance, the topoctile performers in 1980 ended up in the bottom octile in 1981. Again in1987, the top octile funds ended up in the bottom octile in 1988.

Regardless of the risk and return characteristics of chasing winners, thepenalties to holding funds in the lowest octile are unambiguous. Precedingyear performance appears to be an excellent predictor of negative alphas.Positive alphas were obtained in only three of the twelve years in the samplefor the lowest octile. How can it be so easy to use simple historical informa-tion to identify a dominated asset in an efficient financial market? Theanswer seems to be the inability of investors to short most losing mutualfiinds. Investors can respond to poor performance by reclaiming shares, butnot by arbitrage. The table focuses upon performance measures, but is silent

^ Excluding those managers who perform poorly in aggregate over the two-year period does ofcourse lead to an upward bias in excess returns; however, there is no reason to expect that it willaffect relative returns. We verify this conclusion in two ways. First, we perform a simulation,reported in earlier versions of this article, in which we eliminate the lowest octile of two-yearperformers. We find that the two-year performance cut failed to induce differential performanceacross the octiles. An imposition of a 10 percent cut on the simulated sample based uponone-year returns does increase the lowest octile returns as expected. Second, we eliminate losersbased upon a two-year period preceding the final year for which the evaluation is made. Thisalternative definition of bottom feeder only slightly increases the returns to the Best-Worststrategy.

Page 19: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

Performance Persistence 697

about the money invested in the lowest octile funds. Evidence form Goetz-mann and Peles (1993) suggests that only two to three percent of mutualfund investors hold shares in this bottom octile.

IV. Conclusion

Our study of a relatively survivorship-bias-free data set of equity mutualfunds allows us to examine mutual fund performance with a database thatlargely controls for survivorship bias. We report basic summary measures forthe Weisenberger equity fund universe. These directly show the magnitude ofsurvival bias in mutual fund samples observed ex post. They also documentthe evolution of fads in the mutual fund industry. An analysis of the factorscontributing to the disappearance of funds shows that a poor track record isthe strongest predictor of attrition. Size, age, and the fund's expense ratio arealso important. Attrition is negatively related to the relative growth of thefund assets due to new share purchases, but not strongly so.

The primary focus of the article is upon the issue of performance persis-tence. Our study takes a different tack from earlier researchers who identi-fied the existence of repeat winners. By desegregating the persistence testson an annual basis, we find that the phenomenon is strongly dependent uponthe time period of study. This result is validated by Malkiel's (1995) analysisthat extends the period of observation. Perhaps more significant than theuncertainty about repeat performance is the correlation across managers thatis implied by the periods when the pattern is reversed. This suggests thatfuture investigation of the persistence effect should concentrate upon asearch for common management strategies. Recent candidates for suchstrategies include dynamic rebalancing of the type proposed by Connor andKorajczyk (1991), trend-chasing, identified by Grinblatt, Titman, and Werm-ers (1993), and common conditioning upon macroeconomic variables, sug-gested by Ferson and Schadt (1995).

Using methods designed to control for the survivorship bias identified byBrown et al. (1992), and a substantially larger database than previouslyassembled, we find clear evidence of relative performance persistence. In-vestors can use historical information to beat the pack. Evidence that histori-cal information can be used to earn returns in excess of ex ante benchmarks,such as the S & P 500, positive appraisal ratios, and positive alphas is weakerand depends upon the time period of analysis.

An analysis of the risk and return characteristics of chasing the winnerssuggests that, while it is a positive alpha strategy, it also has a high level oftotal risk. Because of the correlation across winning funds, this total risk isnot diversifiable, and thus it matters to risk-averse investors. Indeed thecorrelation of winning strategies suggests the possibility that winning fundsare loading up on a macroeconomic factor, unassociated with the majorcomponents of equity returns, that may be priced. It is clear from theseresults that the nature of mutual fund persistence is more complicated thanprevious researchers, including the current authors, have understood. These

Page 20: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann

698 The Journal of Finance

issues are fertile ground for future inquiry regarding the basis for correlatedactive strategies among fund managers and motives for fund mergers.

REFERENCES

Brown, Stephen J., William N. Goetzmann, Roger G. Ihbotson, and Stephen A. Ross, 1992,Survivorship bias in performance studies. Review of Financial Studies 5, 553-580.

Carlson, Robert S., 1970, Aggregate performance of mutual funds. Journal of Financial andQuantitative Analysis S, 1-32.

Christensen, Ronald, 1990, Log-Linear Models (Springer-Verlag, New York).Connor, Gregory, and Robert Korajczyk, 1991, The attributes, behavior, and performance of U.S.

mutual funds. Review of Quantitative Finance and Accounting 1, 5-26.Elton, Edwin J., and Martin J. Gruber, 1989, Modern Portfolio Theory and Investment Manage-

ment (John Wiley and Sons, New York).Elton, Edwin J., Martin J. Gruber, Sanjiv Das, and Matthew Htavka, 1993, Efficiency with costly

information: A reinterpretation of evidence for managed portfolios. Review of FinancialStudies 6, 1-22.

Ferson, Wayne, and Rudi Schadt, 1995, Measuring fund strategy and performance in changingeconomic conditions. Working paper. University of Washington School of Business Adminis-tration.

Goetzmann. William N., and Roger G. Ibbotson. 1994, Do winners repeat? Patterns in mutualfund performance. Journal of Portfolio Management 20, 9-17.

Goetzmann, William N., and Nadav Peles, 1993, Cognitive dissonance and mutual fund in-vestors. Working paper, Columbia Business School.

Grinblatt, Mark, and Sheridan Titman 1988, The evaluation of mutual fund performance: Ananalysis of monthly returns. Working paper. The John E. Anderson Graduate School ofManagement at UCLA.

Grinblatt, Mark, and Sheridan Titman, 1992, Performance persistence in mutual funds. Journalof Finance 47, 1977-1984.

Grinblatt, Mark, Sheridan Titman, and Russell Wermers, 1994. Momentum strategies, portfolioperformance and herding. Working Paper, Johnson School of Management, UCLA.

Hendricks, Darryl. Jayendu Patel, and Richard Zeckhauser, 1993, Hot hands in mutual funds:The persistence of performance, 1974-1988, Journal of Finance 48, 93-130.

Ippolito, Richard A., 1992, Consumer reaction to measures of poor quality: Evidence from themutual fund industry. Journal of Law and Economics 35, 45-70.

Kane, Alex, Danilo Santini, and John W. Aber, 1991, Lessons from the growth history of mutualfunds, Working paper, Boston University.

Lakonishok, Josef, Andrei Shleifer, and Robert Vishny, 1992, The structure and performance ofthe money management industry, Brookings Papers on Economic Activity: Microeconomics339-391.

Lehmann, Bruce N., and David Modest, 1987, Mutual fund performance evaluation: A compari-son of benchmarks and a benchmark of comparisons. Journal of Finance 21, 233-265.

Malkiel, Burton, 1995, Returns from investing in equity mutual funds 1971 to 1991, Journal ofFinance 50, 549-572.

Patel, Jayendu, and Richard J. Zeckhauser, 1992, Survivorship and the 'u' shaped pattern ofresponse. Review of Eeonomics and Statistics, Forthcoming.

Patel, Jayendu, Richard J. Zeckhauser, and Daryl Hendricks, 1990, Investment flows andperformance: Evidence from mutual funds, cross border investments and new issues, inRichard Satl, Robert Levitch, and Rama Ramachandran, Eds.: Japan, Europe and theInternational Financial Market.';: Analytical and Empirical Perspectives (Cambridge Univer-sity Press, New York).

Sirri, Erik, and Peter Tufano, 1992, The demand for mutual fund services by individualinvestors. Working paper. Harvard Business School.

Weisenberger Inc., 1977 through 1989, Investment Companies (Weisenberger Investment Com-panies, New York).

Page 21: Performance persistence brown_goetzmann