5
Comprehension Questions in Bahasa Indonesia and English Textbooks [email protected] SMPN 1 Cinangka Questions in their while or post reading activities are mostly used to check the students’ comprehension about texts. They ou ght to challenge all levels of the students’ comprehension: literal, inferential and evaluative. They are mostly written after te xts on textbooks. However, not all textbooks take those levels of comprehension into account. This study investigated what levels of comprehension were challenged in the Bahasa Indonesia and English textbooks for Junior High School students and how teachers treat the questions in text books. It was found that in the Bahasa Indonesia textbook, 19.9 % of questions challenge literal comprehension level, 45.9% check inferential level and 34% exercise evaluative one. In the English textbooks, 62.9 % challenge the students’ literal comprehension level and 32.7 train inferential and only 4.2% exercise evaluative ones. It was also found th at most teachers treated the questions as they were. They scarcely modified them. These findings show in the textbooks the comprehension levels are not evenly challenged. The fact that literal level was not much tested In Bahasa Indonesia might lead students guess the content of the texts without reading. Meanwhile in English textbooks, neglecting evaluative comprehension level might lead students to be uncritical. Therefore, it is recommended that textbook writers as well as teachers should be more cautious in generating questions so that all levels of comprehension are well exercised. In many language lessons, textbooks help teachers teach intensive reading. This lesson is aimed to help students to comprehend texts (Aebersold and Field 1997 in Hedgcock and Ferris, 2009). They work simultaneously with the same texts and activities like re-reading the texts by answering comprehension questions (Hedgcock and Ferris 2009 p. 172). The questions function to check students’ understanding (Nunan and Lamb, 1996 in Yang 2010). The students as readers are more likely to understand what they read when they are asked comprehension questions about the texts and receive immediate feedback about their answers (Bassel and Rasinki, 2008 p. 20). Comprehension questions are expected not only to facilitate but also to challenge at least three levels of comprehension: literal, inferential and evaluative or critical. Literal comprehension is level of understanding of a text wherein a reader has access and can recognize and recall details in the text (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p. 87). It requires recognition and recall of ideas, information and happening explicitly stated in the reading selection (Clymer, 1968 in Hudson, 2007 p. 85). The examples of them are finding date of flight; who is …; what/where did she/he …; what happened when or during …; find out the differences between … and …? Inferential or interpretative comprehension is the level of understanding wherein readers can read meanings which are not directly stated on the texts (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p. 17; Alexander, 1989; Burnes & Glenda, 1985 in Setiadi, 2010 p. 92; Briskin, 2005). This level requires the orchestration and the manipulation of information from the text as well as within the readers (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p. 17) The questions are, for example, how did she converse with ; what was the weather like; do you think …; what will happen next etc. Evaluative or critical comprehension refers to the understanding of a text which requires readers to use an adequately developed knowledge base (Carr and Thompson, 1996) and new information (Briskin, 2005) including prior knowledge, intuition, and imagination to make hypotheses (Pennel, 2002); to draw conclusions, to make reasonable predictions, connections between conclusions and critical judgments about what texts (Keene and Zimmerman, 1997 in Pennel, 2002; Clymer 1968 in Hudson, 2007 pp. 84-5; Brasell & Rasinski, 2008 p. 17). The examples of the questions are what strange ideas did have? why was true?; how do you feel about this character?; what do you think …’s attitude?

Comprehension questions in textbooks

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Comprehension questions in textbooks

Comprehension Questions in Bahasa Indonesia and English Textbooks

[email protected]

SMPN 1 Cinangka

Questions in their while or post reading activities are mostly used to check the students’ comprehension about texts. They ought

to challenge all levels of the students’ comprehension: literal, inferential and evaluative. They are mostly written after texts on

textbooks. However, not all textbooks take those levels of comprehension into account. This study investigated what levels of

comprehension were challenged in the Bahasa Indonesia and English textbooks for Junior High School students and how teachers

treat the questions in text books. It was found that in the Bahasa Indonesia textbook, 19.9 % of questions challenge literal

comprehension level, 45.9% check inferential level and 34% exercise evaluative one. In the English textbooks, 62.9 % challenge

the students’ literal comprehension level and 32.7 train inferential and only 4.2% exercise evaluative ones. It was also found that

most teachers treated the questions as they were. They scarcely modified them. These findings show in the textbooks the

comprehension levels are not evenly challenged. The fact that literal level was not much tested In Bahasa Indonesia might lead

students guess the content of the texts without reading. Meanwhile in English textbooks, neglecting evaluative comprehension

level might lead students to be uncritical. Therefore, it is recommended that textbook writers as well as teachers should be more

cautious in generating questions so that all levels of comprehension are well exercised.

In many language lessons, textbooks help teachers teach intensive reading. This lesson is aimed to help

students to comprehend texts (Aebersold and Field 1997 in Hedgcock and Ferris, 2009). They work

simultaneously with the same texts and activities like re-reading the texts by answering comprehension

questions (Hedgcock and Ferris 2009 p. 172). The questions function to check students’ understanding

(Nunan and Lamb, 1996 in Yang 2010). The students as readers are more likely to understand what they

read when they are asked comprehension questions about the texts and receive immediate feedback about

their answers (Bassel and Rasinki, 2008 p. 20).

Comprehension questions are expected not only to facilitate but also to challenge at least three levels of

comprehension: literal, inferential and evaluative or critical.

Literal comprehension is level of understanding of a text wherein a reader has access and can recognize

and recall details in the text (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p. 87). It requires recognition and recall of ideas,

information and happening explicitly stated in the reading selection (Clymer, 1968 in Hudson, 2007 p.

85). The examples of them are finding date of flight; who is …; what/where did she/he …; what happened

when or during …; find out the differences between … and …?

Inferential or interpretative comprehension is the level of understanding wherein readers can read

meanings which are not directly stated on the texts (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p. 17; Alexander, 1989;

Burnes & Glenda, 1985 in Setiadi, 2010 p. 92; Briskin, 2005). This level requires the orchestration and

the manipulation of information from the text as well as within the readers (Brasel and Rasinski, 2008, p.

17) The questions are, for example, how did she converse with …; what was the weather like; do you

think …; what will happen next etc.

Evaluative or critical comprehension refers to the understanding of a text which requires readers to use an

adequately developed knowledge base (Carr and Thompson, 1996) and new information (Briskin, 2005)

including prior knowledge, intuition, and imagination to make hypotheses (Pennel, 2002); to draw

conclusions, to make reasonable predictions, connections between conclusions and critical judgments

about what texts (Keene and Zimmerman, 1997 in Pennel, 2002; Clymer 1968 in Hudson, 2007 pp. 84-5;

Brasell & Rasinski, 2008 p. 17). The examples of the questions are what strange ideas did … have? why

was … true?; how do you feel about this character?; what do you think …’s attitude?

Page 2: Comprehension questions in textbooks

Based on the categories above, the questions on Bahasa Indonesia textbooks from one publisher and

English textbooks from two publishers for grade 7, 8 and 9 junior high schools were categorized. Only the

questions written under/after the texts were analyzed. Four teachers were interviewed.

From Bahasa Indonesia textbooks, it was found that the questions of inferential comprehension

dominated. In grade 7’s textbook, among 90 questions from 19 texts, there were 21 questions (23%)

drilling literal comprehension, 38 questions (42%) challenging inferential comprehension and 31

questions (34%) exercising evaluative comprehension level. In grade 8’s, among 98 questions in 17 texts,

12 questions (12%) challenging literal comprehension, 46 (47%) drilling inferential and 40 questions

(41%) exercising evaluative comprehension level. In grade 9’s, among 123 questions in 19 texts, 29

questions (24%) exercising literal comprehension, 59 questions (48%) practicing inferential

comprehension and 35 (28 %) questions challenging evaluative comprehension. To get the picture of

overall distribution, see the table below.

The Comprehension Questions In Bahasa Indonesia Textbooks

This finding shows that inferential questions are the most frequently exercised. This means that the

textbooks trained students to be good at inferring which is important for reading (Mickulecky, 1990).

However, the literal comprehension was seldom challenged. This could be risky since the students may

guess the answer without active reading. They might be reluctant to read the text and rely their answer on

their prediction, instead.

On the contrary, from the two English textbooks, it was found that the questions about literal

comprehension dominated in all grades. In grade 7’s, among 104 questions in 21 texts, there were 81

questions (78%) exercising literal, 21 questions (20%) challenging inferential and 2 (2%) questions

checking evaluative comprehension level. In grade 8’s, among 323 questions in 58 texts, there are 182

questions (56%) challenging literal, 121 questions (37%) examining inferential and 9 questions (6 %)

training evaluative comprehension. Finally, in grade 9’s, among 193 questions in 50 texts, 193 questions

(65%) challenging literal comprehension, 95 questions (32%) training inferential and 9 questions (3 %)

exercising evaluative comprehension level. The table below the ratio

-

10.00

20.00

30.00

40.00

50.00

Ina Book 7

Ina Book 8

Ina Book 9

Overall

Literal

Inferential

Evaluative

Page 3: Comprehension questions in textbooks

The Comprehension Questions in English Textbooks

These findings show that the questions in the textbooks mostly challenge literal comprehension. This

means that the students were mostly exercised to recognize and recall of ideas, information and happening

explicitly stated in the texts (Clymer, 1968 iin Hudson, 2007 p. 85; Briskin, 2005;). These are in line with

what Myhil et. al. (2006) claims that factual questions are the most common ones. These also confirm the

statement of Muhamad (1999) that teachers tend to ask their students mainly literal comprehension

questions.

This implies that the students were not challenged to have better comprehension. Their evaluative

comprehension is not frequently exercised. This is parallel to the claim that the emphasis of lesson was

reading and doing exercises leading to rote-learning, which could have negative impact on the students’

meaningful learning, and critical thinking has never been the object of learning (Mok, J. 2010). When the

students’ critical thinking is not exercised, they do not have the ability to take control of their conscious

thought processes. With this, they risk being controlled by the ideas of others.

In addition, based on the interview, it was found that teachers tend to utilize the questions as they were

written. They modify or add more questions only when the students do not understand the questions,

when the time for teaching the texts were still long or when other language skills, speaking or writing, are

trained. This confirms the claim that most class activities were dominated by textbooks: it was a textbook

which determines what the lecturer and students do (Zohrabi, M. 2011).

In summary, this study concludes that the questions in Bahasa Indonesia and English textbooks different

emphasis in challenging students’ comprehension levels. The former contained 19.9% literal, 45.9%

inferential and 34% evaluative comprehension questions. The latter contained 62.9 % literal, 32.7% and

only 4.2% evaluative comprehension questions. The fact that literal level was not much tested In Bahasa

Indonesia might lead students guess the content of the texts without reading. Meanwhile in English

textbooks, neglecting evaluative comprehension might lead students to be uncritical. It was also found

that most teachers utilized the questions as written and scarcely modified them. So, the questions on the

textbooks determined what was exercised in many classrooms.

Therefore, it is recommended that both textbook writers and teachers should be more cautious in

generating questions, to include all levels of comprehension evenly. Reading teacher should modify or

add questions to challenge all the comprehension levels.

-

20.00

40.00

60.00

80.00

Literal

Inferential

Evaluative

Page 4: Comprehension questions in textbooks

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Aebersold, J.A., and Field, M.L. (1997). From reader to reading teacher: Issues and strategies for second language

classroom. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. in Hedgcock and Ferris, 2009

Alexander , E. (1989). Teaching Reading Illinois. London: Scott, Foresman and Company.

Brasell, D. & Rasinski, T. (2008). Comprehension that Works: Taking Students Beyond Ordinary Understanding to

Deep Comprehension. Huntington Beach: Shell Education.

Briskin, L. (2005). A Guide To Active Reading And Asking Questions: A Handout For Students. Online article.

Available at www.yorku.ca/laps/sosc/Foundations/documents/ActiveReading.pdf . Accessed on February

15. 2012.

Burnes, D & Page, Glenda. (1985). Insight and Strategies for Teaching Reading. Sydney: Harcourt Brace

Jovanovich. In Setiadi, R. 2010. Self-Efficacy in Indonesian Literacy Teaching Context: A Theoretical and

Empirical Perspective. Bandung: Rizki Press.

Carr, S. C. and Thompson, B. (1996). The Effects of prior knowledge and schema activation strategies on the

inferential reading comprehension of children with and without learning disabilities. Learning Disability

Quarterly. 19, 48 - 61.

Clymer, T. (1968). ‘What is “reading”?’: some current concepts. 67th Yearbook of National Society for the Study of

Education, 1968, 7-29. In Hudson, T. (2007). Teaching Second Language Reading. Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Hudson, T. (2007). Teaching Second Language Reading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hedgecock, J.S. and Ferris, D. R. (2009). Teaching Readers of English: Students, Texts and Contexts. New York:

Routledge.

Keene, E. O., & Zimmerman, S. (1997) Mosaic of thought: Teaching reading comprehension in a reader’s

workshop. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. In Pennel, D. (2002). Explicit Instruction for Implicit Meaning:

Strategies for Teaching Inferential Reading Comprehension. Available at education.wm.edu accessed on

November 12, 2012.

Mikulecky, B.S. (1990). A Short Course in Teaching Reading Skills. USA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company

Inc.

Mok, J. 2010 The New Role of English Language Teachers: Developing Students’ Critical Thinking in Hongkong

Secondary School Classroom. ASIAN EFL Journal. 12 (2). P. 262-287

Muhamad, A. (1999). What Do We Test When We Test Reading Comprehension? The Internet TESL Journal, Vol.

V, No. 12, December 1999 available at http://iteslj.org/Techniques/Nunn-Interacting.html accessed on

February 5th

, 2012.

Myhil, D. Jones, S & Hopper, R. 2006. Talking, Listening, Learning: Effective Talk in Primary Classrooms.

Maidenhead, UK: Open university Press. in Yang, C. C. R. (2010). Teacher questions in Second Language

Classrooms: An Investigation of three case studies. Asian EFL Journal. 12 (1) pp. 181-201)

Nunan, D & Lamb, C. 1996. Self-Directed Teacher. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. In Yang, C. C. R,

2010. Teacher questions in Second Language Classrooms: An Investigation of three case studies. Asian

EFL Journal. 12 (1) pp. 181-201

Page 5: Comprehension questions in textbooks

Pennel, D. (2002). Explicit Instruction for Implicit Meaning: Strategies for Teaching Inferential Reading

Comprehension. Available at education.wm.edu accessed on November 12, 2012.

Setiadi, R. (2010). Self-Efficacy in Indonesian Literacy Teaching Context: A Theoretical and Empirical Perspective.

Bandung: Rizki Press.

Yang, C. C. R, 2010. Teacher questions in Second Language Classrooms: An Investigation of three case studies.

Asian EFL Journal. 12 (1) pp. 181-201.

Zohrabi, M. 2011. An Evaluation of Class Activities and Exercises in ELT Classroom for General Purpose Course.

Canadian Center of Science and Education. 4 (1). 141-151. In Proquest Documents: English Language

Teaching. 84.