23
DIRECT PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY* L. EDWARD WELLS Illinois State University JOSEPH H. RANKIN Eastern Michigan University Until recently, “direct controls” by parents have been summarily dis- missed by delinquency researchers as theoretically and empirically unim- portant. Although prior research indicates that various measures of direct parental controIs (e.g.. the amount of time spent interacting with parents) are related to delinquency, the correlations are uniformly weak and often not signijicant. However, when the term “direct control” is reconceptual- ized to include specific components-normative regulation, monitoring, and punishment-the results indicate that direct controls by parents have as great an impact on delinquency as that of %direct controls” orparen- tal “attachments.” Further, the results suggest that the form of the rela- tion between direct controls and delinquency is not simple, direct, and linear. Depending on which specijic component of direct control is examined, its relationship to delinquency may be either linear or non- linear, positive or inverse. Despite a sizable body of empirical research extending across various aca- demic disciplines, the causal connection between the family and delinquency remains ambiguous and equivocal (Wells and Rankin, 1985, 1986). This ambiguity may reflect a number of considerations. One problem is the com- mon difficulty in delinquency research of trying to compare the empirical results of studies that use diverse measures of delinquency (e.g., a single global index versus multiple specific indices), since family variables may relate to some particular kinds of delinquency but not to all (Rankin, 1983). Comparing data collected from different sources (i.e., self-report surveys ver- sus official records) is also uncertain, given the restrictive and selective sam- pling involved in both official records of delinquency (i.e., the underestimation of relatively minor and infrequent offenders) and self- reported delinquency (i.e., the underestimation of the very serious and chronic offenders) that can bias the research results (see Cernkovich et al., 1985)’ Another difficulty in attempting to make conceptual and empirical sense of * An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 38th annual meeting of the 1. No methodology is without biases. For the purpose of studying family variables, American Society of Criminology in Atlanta, Georgia, 1986. CRIMINOLOGY VOLUME 26 NUMBER 2 1988 263

Direct parental contraol

  • Upload
    nyi-maw

  • View
    312

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

 

Citation preview

Page 1: Direct parental contraol

DIRECT PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY*

L. EDWARD WELLS Illinois State University

JOSEPH H. RANKIN Eastern Michigan University

Until recently, “direct controls” by parents have been summarily dis- missed by delinquency researchers as theoretically and empirically unim- portant. Although prior research indicates that various measures of direct parental controIs (e.g.. the amount of time spent interacting with parents) are related to delinquency, the correlations are uniformly weak and often not signijicant. However, when the term “direct control” is reconceptual- ized to include specific components-normative regulation, monitoring, and punishment-the results indicate that direct controls by parents have as great an impact on delinquency as that of %direct controls” orparen- tal “attachments.” Further, the results suggest that the form of the rela- tion between direct controls and delinquency is not simple, direct, and linear. Depending on which specijic component of direct control is examined, its relationship to delinquency may be either linear or non- linear, positive or inverse.

Despite a sizable body of empirical research extending across various aca- demic disciplines, the causal connection between the family and delinquency remains ambiguous and equivocal (Wells and Rankin, 1985, 1986). This ambiguity may reflect a number of considerations. One problem is the com- mon difficulty in delinquency research of trying to compare the empirical results of studies that use diverse measures of delinquency (e.g., a single global index versus multiple specific indices), since family variables may relate to some particular kinds of delinquency but not to all (Rankin, 1983). Comparing data collected from different sources (i.e., self-report surveys ver- sus official records) is also uncertain, given the restrictive and selective sam- pling involved in both official records of delinquency (i.e., the underestimation of relatively minor and infrequent offenders) and self- reported delinquency (i.e., the underestimation of the very serious and chronic offenders) that can bias the research results (see Cernkovich et al., 1985)’

Another difficulty in attempting to make conceptual and empirical sense of

* An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 38th annual meeting of the

1. No methodology is without biases. For the purpose of studying family variables, American Society of Criminology in Atlanta, Georgia, 1986.

CRIMINOLOGY VOLUME 26 NUMBER 2 1988 263

Page 2: Direct parental contraol

264 WELLS AND RANKIN

the existing literature is the plethora of family variables empirically related to delinquency. These include (a) structural variables, such as broken homes, family size, and birth order (e.g., Gove and Crutchfield, 1982; Rahav, 1981; Wilkinson et al., 1982); (b) psychological or attitudinal measures of parental identification and attachment (e.g., Hirschi, 1969; McCord, 1979; Nye, 1958); and (c) measures of controls and restrictions over children’s behaviors, including car ownership, freedom to date, amount of time spent with peers, and type of punishmentheward used to enforce these restrictions (e.g., Bahr, 1979; Gold and Reimer, 1975; Nye, 1958; Welsh, 1976).

Not only is the list of possible family variables long, but their causal con- nections to delinquency may be explained by a variety of competing concep- tual models, including social control and containment theories, strain theories, social learning theory, and various syntheses of strain and control theories (e.g., Bahr, 1979; Conger, 1980; Elliott et al., 1985; Johnson, 1979; Geismar and Wood, 1986; Weis and Hawkins, 1981). Probably the most widely cited and tested of these theories is Hirschi’s (1969) “social bond” version of control theory, which depicts the family as the major source of attachment to the conventional social order. The emphasis on testing Hir- schi’s control model (because it is relatively explicit, well developed, and ame- nable to empirical test) has turned attention away from earlier and more inclusive conceptualizations of the family in social control terms, such as Nye (1958). Recent considerations of control theory (e.g., Hirschi, 1983; Patter- son, 1980; Wilson and Herrnstein, 1985) indicate that a more complete view of social control is necessary. The aim in this paper is to test empirically the relation between delinquency and a somewhat forgotten concept in control theory-what Nye (1958) calls “direct controls.”

SOCIAL CONTROL THEORIES

A number of control perspectives explain delinquency (or more accurately, they explain conformity) by variations in juveniles’ “ties” to the conventional social order (e.g., Briar and Piliavin, 1965; Hirschi, 1969; Nye, 1958; Reck- less, 1967; Reiss, 1951). According to Hirschi’s (1969) model, these ties (or

~~~

however, it is advantageous to examine self-reports rather than officially recorded delin- quency, for at least two reasons. First, a correlation between certain family variables and officially recorded delinquency may be a statistical artifact-a consequence of differential law enforcement or reactions to children from “problem” as opposed to “normal” home settings (see Rankin, 1983; Wells and Rankin, 1985). Second, given the infrequency of the very serious and chronic offender (estimated to be less than 2%-3% of the population by Hindelang et al. [1979]), it would be difficult to test social control (or any other) theory by examining only chronic offenders. Either exceptionally large samples or samples that are heavily stratified on the basis of known correlates of serious delinquency would be required for such tests (Hagan et al., 1985). Thus, for our purposes, self-reports offer the less offen- sive biases.

Page 3: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 265

“bonds”) have four components: attachment, involvement, belief, and com- mitment. These components are positively related to conformity (and to each other) and are thought to have independent effects on delinquency. The two components most closely related conceptually to parental controls are attach- ment and involvement.

“Attachment” refers to the strength of one’s ties to parents, peers, and school. The premise is that individuals who are not strongly attached to others are also insensitive to their opinions. Thus, they are not bound by others’ norms and are free to deviate. On the other hand, those juveniles who closely identify with (are close to or have positive affect for) their parents are more likely to take their feelings and opinions into account when and if a delinquent act is contemplated. As such, attachment is essentially a social- psychological concept, involving the motivational value of social approval and imitation.

A juvenile may also be so “involved” in conventional activities that he or she could not even find the time for delinquent activities. The idea here is that if juveniles can be kept busy and “off the streets,” then they will not have time to get into trouble. Almost by definition, a juvenile who is studying, playing baseball, or spending time with family in other conventional activities (e.g., picnics, vacations, watching television) is not violating the law.

Nye’s (1958) depiction of control theory is more inclusive, identifying four types of social control: (1) direct control, based on the application (or threat) of punishments and rewards to gain compliance with conventional norms; (2) indirect control, primarily based on affectional attachment to or identifica- tion with conventional persons (especially parents); (3) internalized control, based on the development of autonomous patterns of conformity located in the individual personality, self-concept, or conscience; and (4) control over opportunities for conventional and deviant activities whereby compliance results from restricted choices or alternatives. Although direct restraints can take many forms (e.g., formal laws or informal customs) and can be imposed or enforced by a variety of individuals in different social roles (e.g., police, teachers, peers), Nye viewed the family as the closest and most important agency of direct control.

Hirschi’s (1 969) concepts of attachment and belief correspond conceptually to Nye’s concepts of indirect and internalized controls, except that Hirschi locates the conscience in the bond to others rather than making it part of the individual personality. Hirschi’s concept of involvement and Nye’s concept of direct control have some conceptual overlap, but they clearly are not iden- tical. Although the premise behind involvement is “time” (“idle hands are the devil’s workshop”), direct controls are more concerned with the physical restriction and surveillance of behaviors. Increased involvement in conven- tional activities does tend to increase the accessibility of youthful behavior to

Page 4: Direct parental contraol

266 WELLS AND RANKIN

monitoring and supervision by parents, but it includes other elements of con- trol as well. The notion of involvement is conceptually more ambiguous and complex.

Both Nye and Hirschi argued that for adolescent children, the utility of direct monitoring and supervision by parents is probably limited, because youths in this age period are relatively autonomous from their parents and more involved in peer networks. Although Nye did find an inverse relation between self-reported delinquency and a number of measures of parental monitoring, he suggested that its potential for control was limited (Nye, 1958:

it is effective only when the child can expect to be detected in the delin- quent act, is actually within the physical limits of the house, or is other- wise under the surveillance of adults. Since there are many times when the child is outside the sphere of direct control, it cannot be effective by itself.

The implication is that the major impact of the family will be through other forms of control-principally, indirect ones.

Similarly, Hirschi (1969: 88) argued that direct parental controls offer lit- tle effect in controlling delinquent behaviors beyond that already offered through attachments or indirect controls:

Since most delinquent acts require little time, and since most adolescents are frequently exposed to situations potentially definable as opportunities for delinquency, the amount of time spent with parents would probably be only a minor factor in delinquency prevention. So-called “direct con- trol” is not, except as a limiting case, of much substantive or theoretical importance. The important consideration is whether the parent is psy- chologically present when temptation to commit a crime occurs.

7):

RESEARCH ON DIRECT CONTROLS Earlier research supports the view that adolescent behaviors are generally

not affected by direct parental controls. Items measuring the amount of time talking with parents, engaging in various activities with parents, and working around the house indicate the degree to which juveniles will be within the sphere of direct parental control. Such variables are inversely related to delinquency but uniformly weak and not statistically significant (e.g., Hirschi, 1969: 88, note 17). Similarly, the correlations between various indicators of involvement (e.g., after-school jobs, sports activities, club memberships, hob- bies, extracurricular school activities) and delinquent behavior reveal few sta- tistically reliable relations (e.g., Hirschi, 1969; Rankin, 1977). Measures of adolescent freedom and autonomy (e.g., frequency of dating, number of eve- nings “out” per week, availability of a car, freedom to dress as desired) show similar patterns of association with delinquency. Statistically significant

Page 5: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 267

bivariate correlations are generally but not always in the predicted direction (e.g., Nye, 1958).

STRUCTURAL VARIABLES

Other empirical evidence on direct control by parents is more indirect and inferential, relying on proxy variables, such as broken homes, mother’s employment, and family size, as substitute indicators of (the loss of) direct control. For instance, the recent literature on broken homes and delinquency is generally consistent with the views of Nye and Hirschi, based on the assumption that loss of one (or more) parents results in less direct control over adolescent behaviors. Recent reviews (e.g., Rankin, 1983; Rosen and Neilson, 1982; Wells and Rankin, 1985) suggest that, except for relatively trivial offenses (e.g., running away and truancy), the relation between broken homes and delinquency is negligible.

Despite the purportedly harmful consequences of mother’s employment on delinquency, there is little evidence of such effects. Most studies have found little or no association (e.g., Riege, 1972; Roy, 1963), particularly when other family variables confounded with employment are controlled. The Gluecks (Glueck and Glueck, 1957), for example, found that mother’s employment had no effect on delinquency when the mother was able to arrange for the supervision and care of her children. When Nye (1958) controlled for back- ground factors, such as socioeconomic status, mother’s education, family size, and broken homes, the relation between mother’s employment and delin- quency was statistically unreliable. Hirschi (1969: 237) found a difference in delinquency (defined as two or more self-reported acts) of only 4% when comparing boys from homes in which the mother was employed full time to homes in which the mother was a housewife (20% and 16%, respectively). In part, these negligible effects may be explained by recent studies in the mar- riage and family literature, which indicate that differences in the time spent on child care between working and nonworking mothers are small (e.g., Coser, 1982; Lotz et al., 1985).

One consistent research finding is the positive association between family size and delinquency (see Hirschi, 1985). A variety of theoretical processes can be hypothesized to explain this relationship (see Loeber and Stouthamer- Loeber, 1986): (a) parents with larger families have more difficulty in disci- plining and supervising their children (i.e., less “direct” control); (b) a greater economic strain is placed on parents with larger families, leading to a greater risk of poverty and overcrowding; (c) parents with larger families are more likely to delegate child rearing to older siblings, who may not have the requi- site skills for such a task; and (d) larger families may increase the risk of exposure to a delinquent sibling. As with the “broken home” and “working mother” variables, however, the association between family size and delin- quency is exceedingly small (Rosen, 1985).

Page 6: Direct parental contraol

268 WELLS AND RANKIN

In sum, although broken homes, working mothers, and large families have provided convenient ideological targets, their actual empirical impacts on delinquency appear negligible. The difficulty in generalizing from these find- ings, however, is that such variables involve structural conditions that are only inferentially and indeterminately tied to direct control. At best, they provide very crude indicators of the loss of direct parental control.

RECENT RESEARCH

More recently, however, a few delinquency researchers appear to have “rediscovered” direct parental controls. In a study that assessed the relative explanatory power of key elements of four theories of delinquency, Smith and Paternoster (1987) included two measures of parental supervision (“How often do your parents know ‘where you are’lwhom you are with’ when you are away from home?”) as reflective of social control theory’s bond of family attachment. Cernkovich and Giordano (1987) used a similar measure to assess the relative impact of various family elements on delinquency, and Hagan et al. (1985) examined comparable parental measures to test a “power- control” theory of delinquency.

These three studies all found statistically significant relations between direct parental controls and delinquency. In fact, based upon the size of the correlations, the effect of direct controls on delinquency was just as great (if not greater) as that of indirect controls or attachments (see Cernkovich and Giordano, 1987). This finding is somewhat unexpected given (a) the concep- tual and theoretical statements by Nye (1958) and Hirschi (1969) dismissing the importance of direct controls, (b) the earlier empirical research that found only small or statistically unreliable relationships between various measures of direct parental controls and delinquency, and (c) these more recent empiri- cal studies examined only one aspect of direct parental controls (i.e., supervi- sion). As we now suggest, there are at least three conceptually distinct aspects of direct parental controls.

A RECONCEPTUALIZATION

What conceptual significance do these results have for understanding the utility of direct parental control over juvenile misbehaviors? Are direct con- trols theoretically and empirically irrelevant, as suggested by Nye and Hir- schi? Or, are the effects of direct controls still relevant but not in the simple, linear, and bivariate form assumed in earlier studies?

Attempts to answer such questions must address both conceptual and empirical issues. Conceptually, a fuller explication of the notion of direct control is required-what it includes and what its conceptual structure might be. Indeed, the conceptual and empirical distinction between direct and indi- rect parental controls is often confused and blurred (e.g., see Rosen, 1985;

Page 7: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 269

Wilson and Herrnstein, 1985). Drawing upon research and theory in devel- opmental psychology (e.g., Patterson, 1980, 1982), we suggest that what is generally termed “direct parental control” actually has three basic compo- nents: normative regulation, monitoring, and punishment.2

First, normative regulation refers to the process by which parents specify the rules, constraints, and criteria for their children’s behavior. Parents may determine, for example, what activities children can engage in, who their friends are, how they dress, and so on. This part of “laying down the law” is an explicit specification of parental expectations and restrictions.

A second component of direct control is monitoring children’s behaviors for compliance or noncompliance. This involves parents’ surveillance or supervision of children’s activities, such as checking to see if family rules are followed. Some parents simply do not make their children accountable for their behaviors. For example, parents may not supervise their children’s rec- reational activities, inquire about their homework, enforce a regular bedtime hour, or care about their choice of friends (Nye, 1958). In short, parents may spend insufficient time interacting with and monitoring their children’s behaviors, thereby making it easier for them to engage in delinquent behav- iors. In a major review and reanalysis of research that examined family cor- relates of juvenile misconduct, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber ( 1986) found nine studies in which lack of parental supervision was significantly (but weakly) related to delinquency. A more recent study by Cernkovich and Giordano (1987), however, suggests that the effect of parental supervision (as measured by the size of the correlation coefficients) is at least as strong as that for family attachments or indirect controls.

The third component of direct parental control is discipline or punish- ment-applying negative (unwanted) sanctions to misbehavior and deviation. This involves the consequences administered by parents for rule violations. The last part of “laying down the law” seems to involve “picking up the rod.”

One complication is that punishment is not a single, unitary variable, but rather reflects multiple component issues, such as severity, contingency, fre- quency, consistency, perceived fairness, and so on. Also, the term “punish- ment” has been used to denote a variety of disciplinary practices, including paddling and hitting, yelling and scolding, and the withdrawal of affection and privileges. As Walters and Grusec’s (1977) review indicates, punishment

2. What we call “direct parental control” basically involves the insfrumenful control of children’s behaviors through the use of rewards and punishments. It is “coercive” and mostly negative, aimed at inhibiting undesired responses. Although it seems equivalent with what frequently is termed “parental discipline,” we prefer the term “direct control” as more neutrally descriptive. In common usage, the term “discipline” has a more ambiguous reference, including a variety of meanings and connotations. In this analysis, we use the term “discipline” to refer more specifically to the administration of punishment.

Page 8: Direct parental contraol

270 WELLS AND RANKIN

as a social scientific concept is a much more complex idea than ordinary usage of this term suggests.

Although the traditional child development literature consistently reports a positive relation between punishment and children’s antisocial behaviors (see Patterson, 1980), the association is not strong. Loeber and Stouthamer- Loeber’s (1986) review, for example, found only a very weak and often non- significant relation between physical punishment and children’s aggression. Moreover, none of these studies analyzed “normal” (e.g., random) samples of children.

The relation (or lack of it) between punishment and delinquency also presents a problem in causal interpretation. Given that delinquent children commit punishable behavior more often than nondelinquents, the same rate of punishment would produce a higher absolute frequency of punishment for delinquents than for nondelinquents. In fact, the more frequent punishment of delinquents may actually reduce their rate of misconduct. Because their rate of misconduct is already high, however, it may appear that more fre- quent punishment actually causes more frequent delinquency. “Thus, mea- suring the amount of one reinforcer (e.g., physical punishment) without taking into account the other reinforcers at work (the context of reinforce- ment) can easily lead to erroneous conclusions” (Wilson and Herrnstein, 1985: 228).

Which aspects of punishment are causally important? Patterson (1980, 1982) argues that it is not the frequency or the severity of punishment, but rather its consistency and contingency that affects the content of children’s behaviors. Parents of misbehaving children lack the requisite skills to know how to punish effectively. Thus, punishment may occur (even frequently and vigorously), but it results in little control over misbehavior. Undoubtedly, there are additional conceptual distinctions that can be made. At present, there are few clear predictions regarding which distinct elements of “punish- ment” are likely to involve important differences in effect.

In empirical terms, this review indicates that there is actually little “hard” data regarding the impact of direct parental control over delinquency. Existing research is sketchy and based on a loose assortment of presumed measures of direct control. To some degree, this lack of systematic data is due to a deficiency in conceptual explication. Without a clear specification of the essential ideas, it is difficult to know what is a good measure; without good measures, it is difficult to collect informative data. Our aims in this analysis are expository and exploratory: to indicate conceptually what the relevant content of direct parental control variables might be and to examine empirically what relationships they have with delinquency.

Page 9: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 271

METHODS

THE SAMPLE

This study analyzes the Youth in Transition panel data originally collected by Bachman (Bachman, 1970; Bachman et al., 1978). The data set includes four waves of surveys administered to a nationwide sample of boys who were in the 10th grade in 1966. The first survey was given to respondents at the beginning of their sophomore year in high school, and the remaining surveys were administered during the spring of the following years in school and 1 year after high school. A 1.5-year interval separates the first and second waves, in the panel and a 1-year interval separates the remaining waves. The sample employs a multistage sampling procedure: weighted cluster sampling in the first stage, with high schools as sampling units; simple random sam- pling of boys within each school in the second stage. A total of 2,277 boys were selected into the sample list, 2,213 of whom (over 97%) agreed to par- ticipate in the initial survey. With each wave in the panel, the number of respondents was reduced; however, 85.2% of the first wave were present in the second wave, and 73.2% were still in the sample at the fourth wave. Surveys in the initial wave were administered to groups of respondents in school settings; subsequent surveys were administered individually in neutral sites (away from school and home).

This analysis uses only the first and second waves in the panel, i.e., the surveys during the sophomore and junior years in high school. This corre- sponds to the point in adolescence (15 to 17 years old) at which delinquent activities are most frequent and problematic. These data include 1,886 respondents (82.8% of the original sample list), although the number used in specific analyses may be less due to missing information on some variables. For a fuller description of procedures on sampling and data collection, see Bachman (1970) or Bachman et al. (1978).

MEASUREMENT OF VARIABLES

Four variables were used to measure the multidimensional content of direct parental control. These variables were selected to correspond closely to the dimensions of direct control of children by parents described earlier-regula- tion, monitoring, and punishment. The specific variables include: (1) regula- tion/restriction (a summary index of five items indicating the degree to which parents decided their sons’ friends and activities); (2) strictness (a two-item index of how strict respondents rated their parents to be); (3) punishment contingency (a one-item measure of how frequently parents ignored rather than punished wrongdoing); and (4) punitiveness (a summary index of four

Page 10: Direct parental contraol

272 WELLS AND RANKIN

items rating how vigorously and frequently parents punished their sons, rang- ing from yelling to hitting). All of these measures are based on sons’ (respon- dents’) ratings of parental behavior during the first panel survey, when the respondents were high school sophomores.

Although nominally based on face validity, these measures of parental con- trol correspond closely to measurements used in other studies of family disci- pline (see Cernkovich and Giordano, 1987; Johnson, 1979; Miller et al., 1986; Norland et al., 1979; Smith and Paternoster, 1987). Reliability estimates (alpha coefficients of internal consistency) for the family control variables are .55 for regulation/restriction, .55 for strictness, and .73 for punitiveness.

Six variables are used to measure involvement in juvenile delinquency. Each of the indices is an average score, constructed by adding the scores from each individual delinquency item and dividing that sum by the total number of items answered by the respondent.3 Four of these variables index specific types of delinquent activity: (1) school delinquency (a seven-item scale reflect- ing serious misconduct at school and truancy); (2) theft-vandalism (a nine- item scale reflecting property offenses, such as theft, trespassing, damaging property, setting fires); (3) assault-threat (an eight-item scale reflecting offenses against persons, such as hitting, fighting, or strong-arming, as well as carrying weapons); and (4) trouble with parents (a three-item index reflecting “family delinquency,” such as running away from home and physically fight- ing with parents). A fifth variable, trouble with police, involves a single item asking whether the youth had gotten into trouble with the police or authori- ties (self-reported). This latter measure corresponds to the type of events included in officially recorded data on delinquency. The sixth variable meas- ures total delinquency and involves a summary (average) score of 26 assorted delinquency items. All the delinquency items were measured during the sec- ond panel survey (at the end of the 1 lth grade) and cover self-reported inci- dents during the 1.5-year interval between the first and second surveys. Thus, the measurement of delinquent behaviors is temporally subsequent to the measurement of direct parental control variables.

The content of the separate delinquency scales is based on earlier research by Gold (1 966) and corresponds closely to the categories of delinquency sug- gested in other studies (e.g., Elliott and Huizinga, 1983; Hindelang et al., 1981; Snyder and Patterson, 1987). Although information on the internal

3. The use of an averaged index score is statistically equivalent to a summed total score, being simply a linear transformation of the total sum, i.e., multiplied by the constant (l/N). However, use of an average score allows for easier and more direct comparisons between indices based on different numbers of component items. Also, an average score allows computation of an index when information may be missing on one of the component items, since the summary value of the index does not depend strictly on the number of items used to compute it.

Page 11: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 273

consistency of these scales is not available, test-retest reliabilities (using corre- lations between first and second surveys) are: .82 for school delinquency, .54 for thefthandalism, .49 for assaulthhreat, and .69 for total delinquency.4

RESULTS

First, we examined the form of the association between direct control and delinquency to determine whether the relationship is linear or nonlinear. All direct control measures were coded as three-category variables, and mean levels of delinquency were compared across categories using ANOVA proce- dures. This provided both an estimate of the strength of association between parental control and delinquency variables as well as a test of the linearity of their correlation.5

For one measure of direct control (perceived strictness of parents), the association with delinquent behaviors is clearly nonlinear (see Figure 1). That is, medium levels of perceived parental strictness show the lowest levels of self-reported delinquency, but low and high strictness result in higher delinquency. The relationship with delinquency is generally linear for the three other types of direct parental controls, although the differences for reg- ulation/restriction are rather weak for most forms of delinquency and incon- sistent in direction (see Figure 2). The two other control variables, representing components of punishment (contingency and punitiveness), both show appreciable and consistent associations with all forms of delinquent behavior (see Figures 3 and 4).

The patterns shown in Figures 1 through 4 hold rather consistently across different indicators of delinquent activity. The one exception to this trend occurs for regulation/restriction, where the sign of the correlation changes for two forms of delinquency-trouble with parents (reflecting mainly run- ning away behaviors) and assaulthhreat. These types of delinquency may reflect expressive, hostile, rebellious responses to parental attempts at regula- tion. Indeed, for 15-year olds (such as the respondents in our sample), strong parental control may be interpreted as an intrusion on their privacy and inde- pendence, thus prompting selective “acting out” forms of misbehavior rather than generalized delinquency. However, because the levels of correlation for

4. Information on individual delinquent acts is not available (in order to maintain confidentiality of individual answers); only summary index scores are provided for those items that have clearly illegal content. Thus, we could not compute the inter-item correla- tions needed for estimating alpha coefficients of reliability. However, using the stability coefficients reported in Bachman et al. (1978: 292), we estimated reliabilities using the correlations between delinquency scores in the first and second surveys through the estima- tion strategy suggested by Heise (1969) for panel data.

We used the BREAKDOWN procedure in SPSSX. This computes both r and eta as correlation coefficients and computes an F-test (for differences between r and eta) to test statistically for nonlinearity.

5.

Page 12: Direct parental contraol

214

.I

v) W L. 0 u vr

3 c W 2 0- .r

5 . W 0.

W

m

2

WELLS AND RANKIN

Figure 1 Average Delinquency Scores by Levels o f S t r ic tness

Tota l Del

School Del

T r b l e w/ Po l ice

b Assaul t /Threa t

T r b l e w/ Parents

Low Med High

Level o f Parental S t r i c t n e s s

Page 13: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 275

.7

.6

.5

v) aJ ; .4 v)

3 aI 3 0- c .r

2.3 aJ oi rn L Q > U

. 2

.1

0

Figure 2 Average Delinquency Scores by Levels o f Regula t ion /Rest r ic t ion

I

School Del

,

As s au 1 t/ Th rea t

T r b l e w/ Parents

Low k d H i ah Level o f Parental Regula t ion /Rest r ic t ion

Page 14: Direct parental contraol

276 WELLS AND RANKIN

regulation/restriction with delinquency are quite low, we must be cautious in their interpretation.

To test for the possibility that the relation between parental controls and delinquent behaviors may be spurious (reflecting earlier involvement in delin- quency), we repeated the analyses separately for groups high and low in prior delinquency. Although effect sizes were attentuated slightly due to more restricted variance in the dependent variable, the basic patterns persist and do not depend on prior delinquent involvement.

Two patterns of special interest should be noted. One is the direction of the relationship between punitiveness and delinquent behaviors, which is a positive association (see Figure 4). More vigorous (frequent or severe) punish- ment is associated with higher levels of delinquency. This pattern persists even when prior levels of delinquency are controlled (not shown here); thus, the correlation is not an artifact of confused causal order. The second pattern of interest is the nonlinear association between perceived parental strictness and delinquency-medium strictness results in the lowest levels of delin- quency (see Figure 1). These patterns indicate that the relation between direct controls and delinquency is variable (albeit in consistent ways). The effects of “more control” on curbing delinquency do not follow a simple and unitary pattern.

Our initial results suggest that the form of the relationship is dependent on which aspect of control is being examined. For some dimensions, the relation is nonlinear and cannot be estimated well by ordinary correlational proce- dures (e.g., multiple regression, path analysis, LISREL) that assume linear relations between variables. For those variables that do have linear relation- ships with delinquency, the direction of the relation varies among different components of control.

A second question concerns the magnitude of the relationships between direct parental controls and delinquency. Are they large enough to be statis- tically reliable? Which variables have the greatest impact on delinquency? Table 1 displays the coefficients of correlation between the four measures of direct control and six delinquency items, using Pearson’s r to measure linear associations and eta to measure nonlinear associations. For three of the four control variables (i.e., strictness, punishment contingency, and punitiveness), the coefficients are moderate (mostly between . 1 and .2) and statistically sig- nificant. For regulation/restriction, the coefficients (eta’s) are not statistically reliable. Of the four measures of direct parental control, punitiveness is the most strongly and consistently related to delinquency. One indicator of delin- quency (trouble with police) relates less consistently with all the direct con- trol measures. This latter variable (involving only a single-item measure of a rare occurrence) has more restricted variance than the other delinquency indicators, which constrains the size of its correlations.

When all the measures of direct control are simultaneously examined in a

Page 15: Direct parental contraol

.7

.6

.5

v) W

U v,

& .4

3 r W

0 c a

.I- - A . 3 % 4 L W > 4

.2

.1

a

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 277

Figure 3

Average Delinquency Scores by Levels o f Punishment Contingency

School Dei

T r b i e w/ P o l i c e

Theft/Vandal i sm

LOW bled High

Level o f Punishment Contingency

Page 16: Direct parental contraol

278

.7

.6

.5

La W

0 u ln

L .4

E c 2 (3 c

W

aJ

.r c

a . 3 m m

W > U

. 2

. 1

c

WELLS AND RANKIN

Figure 4 Average Delinquency Scores by Levels o f Punitiveness

/

Low Med H i qh

Level o f Parental Punitiveness

Page 17: Direct parental contraol

Tabl

e 1.

C

orre

latio

ns B

etw

een

Dir

ect

Pare

ntal

Con

trol

s an

d D

elin

quen

t B

ehav

iors

cd

Dire

ct C

ontro

l V

aria

bles

m

5 R

egul

atio

n/

Puni

shm

ent

Del

inqu

ency

R

estri

ctio

n St

rictn

ess

Con

tinge

ncy

Puni

tiven

ess

-

R

Mea

n -

SD

$ (r

) (4

(r

) (4

To

tal D

elin

quen

cy

- .07*

.09*

-.1

1**

.17*

* .2

2 1.

58

.5 1

Thef

tfla

ndal

ism

- .0

7 .0

9*

- .08*

.11*

* .1

6 1.

41

.57

Ass

ault/

Thre

at

.oo

.lo*

-. 12

**

.16*

* .1

9 1.

24

.44

Trou

ble

wfl

aren

ts

.03

.08*

-.11*

* .1

8**

.21

1.12

.3

9 Sc

hool

Del

inqu

ency

- .0

6 .1

1*

-. 1

4**

.16*

* .2

3 1.

54

.64

Trou

ble

w/P

olic

e - .0

5 .0

5 - .0

6 .1

1**

.13

1.46

.9

0 ~

~~

p <

.01

** p

< .0

01

(e)

indi

cate

d et

u co

effic

ient

s fo

r th

at c

olum

n (n

onlin

ear

rela

tions

hips

). (r

) in

dica

tes

Pear

son

r co

effic

ient

s for

tha

t co

lum

n (li

near

rel

atio

nshi

ps).

Ns

for

the

coef

ficie

nts r

ange

bet

wee

n 1,

561

and

1,83

0, r

efle

ctin

g pa

irw

ise d

elet

ion

of m

issi

ng d

ata

n

0

ga

0 * Z U 5 5:

Page 18: Direct parental contraol

280 WELLS AND RANKIN

multiple regression (see column 5 of Table l), the combined (multiple) corre- lations with delinquent behavior are statistically significant (at the .01 level or beyond) but not especially large. This pattern is generally consistent with multiple R’s reported in similar large-scale survey data of self-reported delin- quency (e.g., Johnson, 1979; Norland et al., 1979; Wiatrowski et al., 1981).

When all direct control variables are included simultaneously in a multiple regression, the same patterns of relationship hold as in the bivariate analyses, except that the (partial) correlation between punishment contingency and delinquency is reduced appreciably. As an additional comparison, the pat- tern of relationships between direct control variables and delinquent behav- iors remains fairly constant even when four measures of “indirect parental control” (i.e., attachment to parents, communication with parents, percep- tions that parents care, and parental support) are included in the multiple regression. The magnitudes of the direct control coefficients are reduced only slightly when indirect control variables are introduced, and the basic patterns of associations remain unchanged.

DISCUSSION

Our results indicate that measures of direct parental control do relate in consistent ways to the occurrence of delinquent behavior. Thus, such vari- ables should not be summarily dismissed as theoretically and empirically irrelevant, as suggested by earlier research. Indeed, despite Hirschi’s (1985) apparent change of heart, parental “functioning” or “child-rearing’’ variables are rarely highlighted in delinquency theory and research. Instead, “indirect controls” (Nye, 1958) or parental “attachments” (Hirschi, 1969) are viewed as the more critical variable in controlling delinquent behaviors. Our research suggests, however, that even when controlling statistically for the effects of “attachments,” direct parental controls are significantly related to various measures of delinquency. Direct controls by parents are at least as effective (based on the magnitude of correlation coefficients) as measures of indirect controls or attachments (also, see Cernkovich and Giordano, 1987).

Our results also indicate that the form of the relation between direct con- trol and delinquency is not simple, direct, and linear. Conceptually, “direct control” over delinquent behavior is a complex, multidimensional set of processes in which distinct component dimensions of control operate in very different ways. In some cases, parental attempts to control children have a nonlinear relation to delinquency; either too much or too little control leads to greater frequency of delinquent behavior. In other cases, the relation is generally linear. The direction of such relationships is also variable, however. Some kinds of control increase delinquency, and other kinds inhibit it. Only one indicator of delinquency (trouble with police) relates less consistently with all the direct control measures. This variable may be more indicative of

Page 19: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 28 1

social circumstances and police reactions than it is of delinquent behavior. As a variable indicating an uncommon event, it also has rather restricted vari- ance, which limits the size of its correlations.

Such conclusions are consistent with recent conclusions drawn in other areas of social and psychological research concerning the issue of social con- trol. For instance, recent work in experimental and developmental psychol- ogy (e.g., Walters and Grusec, 1977) has modified earlier ideas about the supposed ineffectiveness of punishment as a way to shape behavior and learn- ing. According to this research, punishment works, but its effects are not generally simple and linear; some punishment is often effective but more is not always better. In similar terms, Miller et al. (1986) found a nonlinear relation between adolescents’ perceptions of parental strictness/rules and adolescent sexual experience (intercourse). Sexual permissiveness was highest among adolescents who perceived their parents as not being strict at all and having few rules, lowest among those who perceived their parents as moder- ately strict, and intermediate among those who perceived their parents to be very strict and to have many rules. Rollins and Thomas (1979) reached a similar conclusion in their view of over 230 studies of parental control and support: very low and very high levels of parental control were least effective in obtaining desired behaviors from children. Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber (1986) also concluded that both strict and punitive as well as lax and erratic disciplinary styles are related to child conduct problems (including delin- quency). The conclusion in this literature is that after a generation or more of neglect (due more i-o ideological than empirical factors), new research is needed to detail what kinds of punishment work, under what conditions, and with what effects, as well as to elaborate the dimensional structure of punishment.

The situation in the sociology of delinquency is remarkably similar. Earlier dismissal of direct parental control was not grounded in a clear empirical demonstration of its disutility, but rather in an ideological preference for other forms of social control. At this point, researchers cannot say they know very much about the operation of direct social control; much of what is “known” is based on indirect evidence or borrowed from other fields of study (e.g., Hirschi, 1983; Wilson and Herrnstein, 1985). The comparative impact of direct versus indirect forms of control have yet to be elaborated or tested. Also unclear are the probable interdependencies between direct and indirect control that condition their effects. Subsequent research might explore, for instance, the hypothesized interaction (e.g., Hirschi, 1983) between direct parental control and attachment, where effective parental discipline is condi- tional on a strong parent-child bond. Our aim here is to begin an examina- tion in that direction and to suggest its utility to the sociology of delinquency.

Page 20: Direct parental contraol

282 WELLS AND RANKIN

REFERENCES

Bachman, Jerald 1970 Youth in Transition. Vol. 11: The Impact of Family Background and

Intelligence on Tenth-Grade Boys. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Institute for Social Research.

Bachman, Jerald, Patrick OMalley, and Lloyd Johnston 1978 Youth in Transition. Vol. VI: Adolescence to Adulthood-Change and

Stability in the Lives of Young Men. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Institute for Social Research.

Bahr, Steven 1979 Family determinants and effects of deviance. In Wesley Burr, Reuben Hill,

F. Ivan Nye, and Ira Reiss (eds.), Contemporary Theories About the Family. New York: Free Press.

Briar, Scott and Irving Piliavin 1965 Delinquency, situational inducements, and commitments to conformity.

Social Problems 13: 35-45.

Cernkovich, Stephen and Peggy Giordano 1987 Family relationships and delinquency. Criminology 25: 295-32 1.

Cernkovich, Stephen, Peggy Giordano, and Meredith Pugh 1985 Chronic offenders: The missing cases in self-report delinquency research.

The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 76: 705-732.

Conger, Rand 1980 Juvenile delinquency: Behavior restraint or behavior facilitation? In Travis

Hirschi and Michael Gottfredson (eds.), Understanding Crime: Current Theory and Research. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.

Coser, Rose Laub 1982 The American family: Changing patterns of social control. In Jack Gibhs

(ed.), Social Control: Views from the Social Sciences. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.

Elliott, Delbert and David Huizinga 1983 Social class and delinquent behavior in a national youth panel. Criminology

21: 149-177.

Elliott, Delbert, David Huizinga, and Suzanne Ageton 1985 Explaining Delinquency and Drug Use. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.

Geismar, Ludwig and Katherine Wood 1986 Family and Delinquency: Resocializing the Young Offender. New York:

Human Sciences Press.

Glueck, Sheldon and Eleanor Glueck 1957 Working mothers and delinquency. Mental Hygiene 41: 327-352.

Gold, Martin 1966 Undetected delinquent behavior. Journal of Research on Crime and

Delinquency 3: 27-46.

Page 21: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 283

Gold, Martin and David Reimer Changing patterns of delinquent behavior among Americans 13 through 16 years old: 1967-72. Crime and Delinquency Literature: Abstracts 17: 483- 517.

1975

Gove, Walter and Robert Crutchfield

Hagan, John, A.R. Gillis, and John Simpson

1982 The family and juvenile delinquency. Sociological Quarterly 23: 301-3 19.

The class structure of gender and delinquency: Toward a power-control theory of common delinquent behavior. American Journal of Sociology 9 0

1985

1,15 1-1,178.

Heise, David 1969 Separating reliability and stability in test-retest correlation. American

Sociological Review 34: 93- 10 1.

Correlates of delinquency: The illusion of discrepancy between self-report and official measures. American Sociological Review 44: 995- 1,014. Measuring Delinquency. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.

Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press. Crime and family policy. In Ralph Weisheit and Robert Culbertson (eds.), Juvenile Delinquency: A Justice Perspective. Prospect Heights, 111.: Wave- land. Crime and the family. In James Q. Wilson (ed.), Crime and Public Policy. San Francisco: ICS Press.

Hindelang, Michael, Travis Hirschi, and Joseph Weis 1979

198 1

Hirschi, Travis 1969 1983

1985

Johnson, Richard Juvenile Delinquency and Its Origins. New York: Cambridge University Press.

1979

Loeber, Rolf and Magda Stouthamer-Loeber 1986 Family factors as correlates and predictors of juvenile conduct problems and

delinquency. In Michael Tonry and Norval Morris (eds.), Crime and Justice: An Annual Review of Research. Vol. 7. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Lotz, Roy, Eric Poole, and Robert Regoli

McCord, Joan

1985 Juvenile Delinquency and Juvenile Justice. New York: Random House.

1979 Some child-rearing antecedents of criminal behavior in adult men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 1,477-1,486.

Parental discipline and control in relation to adolescent sexual attitudes and behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family 48: 503-512.

Norland, Stephen, Neal Shover, William Thornton, and Jennifer James

Miller, Brent, J. Kelly McCoy, Terrance Olson, and Christopher Wallace 1986

1979 Intrafamily conflict and delinquency. Pacific Sociological Review 22: 223- 240.

Nye, F. Ivan 1958 Family Relationships and Delinquent Behavior. New York: John Wiley.

Page 22: Direct parental contraol

WELLS AND RANKIN

Patterson, Gerald 1980 Children who steal. In Travis Hirschi and Michael Gottfredson (eds.),

Understanding Crime: Current Theory and Research. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage. Coercive Family Process: A Social Learning Approach. Vol. 3. Eugene, Oreg.: Castalia.

Family size and delinquency. Sociology and Social Research 66: 42-51.

Investigating the interrelations among social control variables and conform- ity. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 67: 470-480.

1982

Rahav, Giora 1981

Rankin, Joseph 1977

1983 The family context of delinquency. Social Problems 30: 466-479.

Reckless, Walter

Reige, Mary

1967 The Crime Problem. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

1972 Parental affection and juvenile delinquency in girls. British Journal of Criminology 12: 55-73.

Reiss, Albert, Jr. 1951 Delinquency as the failure of personal and social controls. American

Sociological Review 16: 196-207.

Parental support, power, and control techniques in the socialization of children. In Wesley Burr, Reuben Hill, F. Ivan Nye, and Ira Reiss (eds.), Contemporary Theories about the Family. Vol. 1. New York: Free Press.

1985 Family and delinquency: Structure or function? Criminology 23: 553-573.

Rollins, Boyd and Darwin Thomas 1979

Rosen, Lawrence

Rosen, Lawrence and Kathleen Neilson Broken homes. In Leonard Savitz and Norman Johnston (eds.), Contempo- rary Criminology. New York: John Wiley.

1982

Roy, Prodipto 1963 Adolescent roles: Rural-urban differences. In F. Ivan Nye and Lois

Hoffman (eds.), The Employed Mother in America. Chicago: Rand McNally.

The gender gap in theories of deviance: Issues and evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 24: 140-172.

Family interaction and delinquent behavior. In Herbert C. Quay (ed.), Handbook of Juvenile Delinquency. New York: John Wiley.

Smith, Douglas and Raymond Paternoster 1987

Snyder, James and Gerald Patterson 1987

Walters, Gary and Joan Grusec

Weis, Joseph and J. David Hawkins

1977 Punishment. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

198 1 Preventing Delinquency. National Institute for Juvenile Justice and Delin- quency Prevention, U.S. Department of Justice. Washington, D.C.: Govern- ment Printing Office.

Page 23: Direct parental contraol

PARENTAL CONTROLS AND DELINQUENCY 285

Wells, L. Edward and Joseph Rankin 1985

1986

Broken homes and juvenile delinquency: An empirical review. Criminal Justice Abstracts 17: 249-272. The broken homes model of delinquency: Analytic issues. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 23: 68-93.

Severe parental punishment and delinquency: A developmental theory. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 5 : 17-21.

Wiatrowski, Michael, David Griswold, and Mary Roberts Social control theory and delinquency. American Sociological Review 46:

Welsh, Ralph S. 1976

198 1 525-541.

Wilkinson, Karen, B. Grant Stitt, and Maynard Erickson

Wilson, James Q. and Richard Herrnstein

1982

1985 Crime and Human Behavior. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Siblings and delinquent behavior. Criminology 20: 223-239.

L. Edward Wells. Assistant Professor of Criminal Justice, Illinois State University. Current research interests include social psychological models of delinquency and crime, juvenile victimization, and family factors as they relate to delinquency.

Joseph H. Rankin. Ph.D. in 1978 from the University of Arizona. Currently, Associate Professor of Sociology at Eastern Michigan University. Major research interest includes the etiological study of delinquency and behavioral problems in children, especially as they relate to the family.