63
Language learning mo-va-on in Asia Ma#hew Apple Ritsumeikan University Terry Fellner Saga University Dexter Da Silva Keisen University Drawbacks with exis-ng theories and possible solu-ons

Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Languagelearningmo-va-oninAsia

Ma#hewAppleRitsumeikanUniversityTerryFellnerSagaUniversityDexterDaSilvaKeisenUniversity

Drawbackswithexis-ngtheoriesandpossiblesolu-ons

Page 2: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Languagelearningmo-va-oninAsia

Ma#hewAppleRitsumeikanUniversityTerryFellnerSagaUniversityDexterDaSilvaKeisenUniversity

Drawbackswithexis-ngtheoriesandpossiblesolu-ons

Page 3: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Defini-ons

“[M]oBvaBoncanbedefinedasthedynamicallychangingcumulaBvearousalinapersonthatiniBates,directs,coordinates,amplifies,terminates,andevaluatesthecogniBveandmotorprocesseswherebyiniBalwishesanddesiresareselected,prioriBsed,operaBonalised,and…actedout.”

(Dörnyei&O#o,1998,p.65)

Whatis“mo-va-on”?

Page 4: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Defini-ons

Mo-va-onexplains… !

-whyaparBcularacBvity? -howlongtheywillpersist? -whatefforttheyinvestinit?!

Page 5: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Defini-ons

Mo-va-onexplains… !

-why? -howlong? -howhard?!

Page 6: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Defini-ons

Mo-va-onexplains… !

-why? -howlong? -howhard?!

ChoicePersistenceEffort

Page 7: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

PublishedinOctober2013Studies:•MulBpleanalysistechniques•MulBpleeducaBonallevels•PrimaryfocusonPossibleL2Selves!Overviews•AnoverviewofwhytherearesomanystudiesonmoBvaBoninJapan (E.Ushioda)•ExaminingtheuseofSelf-determinaBontheoriesincross-culturalcontexts(K.Noels)•InternaBonalposture,communiBesofpracBce(T.Yashima)•Comparingteachers’,researchers’,andteachereducators’viewsofmoBvaBon (Y.Nakata)

Page 8: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2SelvesandMoBvaBonsinAsianContexts!Japan MalaysiaChina(PR) IndonesiaChina(HongKongSAR) IndiaChina(Taiwan) VietnamSouthKorea Philippines!Winter2016/Spring2017

Page 9: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Overview

1. MoBvaBonaltheories

2. Assessingappropriateness

3. Flawsincurrentmodelsandapproaches

4. Apost-paradigm,suprathemaBc

approach

Page 10: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Overview

1. MoBvaBonaltheories

2. Assessingappropriateness

3. Flawsincurrentmodelsandapproaches

4. Apost-paradigm,suprathemaBc

approach

Page 11: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 12: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 13: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheoriesSocioeduca-onalmodel(Gardner,2010)Influences

mo=va=onalintensity (persistenteffort)

desiretolearn(desire)a?tudestowardL2learning(posiBveaffect)

integra=venessintegraBveorientaBon

interestinforeignlanguagesaotudestowardL2communitya?tudestowardlearningsitua=on

(instrumentality)

Aspects

Page 14: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 15: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 16: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on

SelfDetermina-onTheory(Deci&Ryan,2000)

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Cogni-veEvalua-onTheory(CET)

OrganismicIntegra-onTheory(OIT)

Page 17: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Cogni-veEvalua-onTheory(CET)

feelingcompetentandasenseofcontrol=

leadtointernalizingofexternalrewardsandgoals

SelfDetermina-onTheory(Deci&Ryan,2000)

Page 18: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on1.Mo-va-onaltheories

OrganismicIntegra-onTheory(OIT)

aseriesofsteps/categories=

!showthedegreetowhichmoBvaBonisself-directed

SelfDetermina-onTheory(Deci&Ryan,2000)

Page 19: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on

SelfDetermina-onTheory(Deci&Ryan,2000)

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Mo-va-onisenhancedby: 1.Autonomy -capacitytochoosetoengagein– certainacBviBes

! 2.Competence -abilitytocarryoutanacBvityand torisetoachallenge!

3.Relatedness -feelingofsecuritybetweenlearner andothers(family,friends,teacher,

classmates)

Page 20: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 21: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 22: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

“Possible”or“imagined”selves

• IdealL2Self theselfonedesirestobecome

• OughttoL2Selftheselfoneisobligedtobecome

• L2learningexperiencesituaBon-specific

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem(Dörnyei,2005)

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Page 23: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

“Self-congruency”-guidespushustoward/away

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem(Dörnyei,2005)

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

PeoplearemoBvatedtoreducethediscrepancybetweenwhotheycurrentlyareandwhotheywanttobe/donotwanttobe(Higgins,1987)IdealL2Self>Ought-toL2Self

Page 24: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 25: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

Page 26: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

DynamicSystemsTheory(DST)!

ChaosTheory!

ComplexSystemsTheory(CST)

Page 27: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)—Mo-va-onisindividual dependentuponsocialcontextinterac-onsamongindividual/situa-on specificeventsas“triggers”

!—Mo-va-onisdynamic respondstoaUractors,partofasystembothstableandunpredictable

Page 28: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Overview

1. MoBvaBonaltheories

2. Assessingappropriateness

3. Flawsincurrentmodelsandapproaches

4. Apost-paradigm,suprathemaBc

approach

Page 29: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

2.AssessingAppropriateness

InAsiancontexts…

• Englishasrequiredsubject• Englishas“greatdivider”ofsociety• LackofL2community(varyingdegrees)

• Geo-poliBcal/historicalcontext

Page 30: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

30

Much research on human behaviour and psychology assumes that everyone shares most fundamental cognitive

and affective processes, and that findings from one population apply across the board. A growing body of evidence suggests that this is not the case.

Experimental findings from several disci-plines indicate considerable variation among human populations in diverse domains, such as visual perception, analytic reasoning, fairness, cooperation, memory and the herit-ability of IQ1,2. This is in line with what anthropologists have long suggested: that people from West-ern, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) socie-ties — and particularly American undergraduates — are some of the most psychologically unusual peo-ple on Earth1.

So the fact that the vast majority of studies use WEIRD participants presents a challenge to the under-standing of human psychology and behaviour. A 2008 survey of the top psychology journals found that 96% of subjects were from Western industrialized countries — which house just 12% of the world’s population3. Strange, then, that research articles routinely assume that their results are broadly representative, rarely adding even a cautionary footnote on how far their findings can be generalized.

The evidence that basic cognitive and motivational processes vary across populations has become increasingly difficult to ignore. For example, many studies have shown that Ameri-cans, Canadians and western Europeans rely on analytical reasoning strategies — which separate objects from their contexts and rely on rules to explain and predict behaviour — substantially more than non-Westerners. Research also indi-cates that Americans use analytical thinking more than, say, Europeans. By contrast, Asians tend to reason holistically, for example by con-sidering people’s behaviour in terms of their situation1. Yet many long-standing theories of how humans perceive, categorize and remember emphasize the centrality of analytical thought.

It is a similar story with social behaviour related to fairness and equality. Here, research-ers often use one-shot economic experiments such as the ultimatum game, in which a player

decides how much of a fixed amount to offer a second player, who can then accept or reject this proposal. If the second player rejects it, neither player gets anything. Participants from industrialized societies tend to divide the money equally, and reject low offers. Peo-ple from non-industrialized societies behave differently, especially in the smallest-scale non-market societies such as foragers in Africa and horticulturalists in South America, where peo-ple are neither inclined to make equal offers nor to punish those who make low offers4.

Recent developments in evolutionary biology, neuroscience and related fields sug-gest that these differences stem from the way in which populations have adapted to diverse culturally constructed environments. Ama-zonian groups, such as the Piraha, whose languages do not include numerals above three, are worse at distinguishing large quan-tities digitally than groups using extensive counting systems, but are similar in their abil-ity to approximate quantities. This suggests the kind of counting system people grow up with influences how they think about integers1.

Costly generalizationsUsing study participants from one unusual population could have important practical consequences. For example, economists have been developing theories of decision-making incorporating insights from psychology and social science — such as how to set wages — and examining how these might translate into policy5. Researchers and policy-makers should recognize that populations vary con-siderably in the extent to which they display certain biases, patterns and preferences in economic decisions, such as those related to optimism1. Such differences can, for example,

affect the way that experienced investors make decisions about the stock market6.

We offer four suggestions to help put theories of human behaviour and psychology on a firmer empirical footing. First, editors and reviewers should push researchers to support any generalizations with evidence. Second, granting agencies, reviewers and editors should give researchers credit for comparing diverse and inconvenient subject pools. Third, granting agencies should prioritize cross-disciplinary, cross-cultural research. Fourth, researchers

must strive to evaluate how their findings apply to other populations. There are several low-cost ways to approach this in the short term: one is to select a few judiciously chosen populations that provide a ‘tough test’ of universality in some domain, such as societies with limited count-ing systems for testing theories about numerical cognition1,2.

A crucial longer-term goal is to establish a set of principles that researchers can use to distinguish variable from universal aspects of

psychology. Establishing such principles will remain difficult until behavioural scientists develop interdisciplinary, international research networks for long-term studies on diverse populations using an array of methods, from experimental techniques and ethnography to brain-imaging and biomarkers.

Recognizing the full extent of human diver-sity does not mean giving up on the quest to understand human nature. To the contrary, this recognition illuminates a journey into human nature that is more exciting, more complex, and ultimately more consequential than has previously been suspected Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan are in the Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada. Joseph Henrich is also in the Department of Economics.e-mail: [email protected]. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X (2010).2. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725 (2010).3. Arnett, J. Am. Psychol. 63, 602–614 (2008).4. Henrich, J. et al. Science 327, 1480–1484 (2010).5. Foote, C. L., Goette, L. & Meier, S. Policymaking Insights from

Behavioral Economics (Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, 2009).6. Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z. Y. & Guo, T. Y. J. Behav. Decis. Making 21,

399–413 (2008).

Most people are not WEIRD To understand human psychology, behavioural scientists must stop doing most of their experiments on Westerners, argue Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan.

GRA

CIA

LAM

29

Vol 466|1 July 2010

OPINION

© 20 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved10

2.AssessingAppropriateness

Page 31: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

31

Much research on human behaviour and psychology assumes that everyone shares most fundamental cognitive

and affective processes, and that findings from one population apply across the board. A growing body of evidence suggests that this is not the case.

Experimental findings from several disci-plines indicate considerable variation among human populations in diverse domains, such as visual perception, analytic reasoning, fairness, cooperation, memory and the herit-ability of IQ1,2. This is in line with what anthropologists have long suggested: that people from West-ern, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) socie-ties — and particularly American undergraduates — are some of the most psychologically unusual peo-ple on Earth1.

So the fact that the vast majority of studies use WEIRD participants presents a challenge to the under-standing of human psychology and behaviour. A 2008 survey of the top psychology journals found that 96% of subjects were from Western industrialized countries — which house just 12% of the world’s population3. Strange, then, that research articles routinely assume that their results are broadly representative, rarely adding even a cautionary footnote on how far their findings can be generalized.

The evidence that basic cognitive and motivational processes vary across populations has become increasingly difficult to ignore. For example, many studies have shown that Ameri-cans, Canadians and western Europeans rely on analytical reasoning strategies — which separate objects from their contexts and rely on rules to explain and predict behaviour — substantially more than non-Westerners. Research also indi-cates that Americans use analytical thinking more than, say, Europeans. By contrast, Asians tend to reason holistically, for example by con-sidering people’s behaviour in terms of their situation1. Yet many long-standing theories of how humans perceive, categorize and remember emphasize the centrality of analytical thought.

It is a similar story with social behaviour related to fairness and equality. Here, research-ers often use one-shot economic experiments such as the ultimatum game, in which a player

decides how much of a fixed amount to offer a second player, who can then accept or reject this proposal. If the second player rejects it, neither player gets anything. Participants from industrialized societies tend to divide the money equally, and reject low offers. Peo-ple from non-industrialized societies behave differently, especially in the smallest-scale non-market societies such as foragers in Africa and horticulturalists in South America, where peo-ple are neither inclined to make equal offers nor to punish those who make low offers4.

Recent developments in evolutionary biology, neuroscience and related fields sug-gest that these differences stem from the way in which populations have adapted to diverse culturally constructed environments. Ama-zonian groups, such as the Piraha, whose languages do not include numerals above three, are worse at distinguishing large quan-tities digitally than groups using extensive counting systems, but are similar in their abil-ity to approximate quantities. This suggests the kind of counting system people grow up with influences how they think about integers1.

Costly generalizationsUsing study participants from one unusual population could have important practical consequences. For example, economists have been developing theories of decision-making incorporating insights from psychology and social science — such as how to set wages — and examining how these might translate into policy5. Researchers and policy-makers should recognize that populations vary con-siderably in the extent to which they display certain biases, patterns and preferences in economic decisions, such as those related to optimism1. Such differences can, for example,

affect the way that experienced investors make decisions about the stock market6.

We offer four suggestions to help put theories of human behaviour and psychology on a firmer empirical footing. First, editors and reviewers should push researchers to support any generalizations with evidence. Second, granting agencies, reviewers and editors should give researchers credit for comparing diverse and inconvenient subject pools. Third, granting agencies should prioritize cross-disciplinary, cross-cultural research. Fourth, researchers

must strive to evaluate how their findings apply to other populations. There are several low-cost ways to approach this in the short term: one is to select a few judiciously chosen populations that provide a ‘tough test’ of universality in some domain, such as societies with limited count-ing systems for testing theories about numerical cognition1,2.

A crucial longer-term goal is to establish a set of principles that researchers can use to distinguish variable from universal aspects of

psychology. Establishing such principles will remain difficult until behavioural scientists develop interdisciplinary, international research networks for long-term studies on diverse populations using an array of methods, from experimental techniques and ethnography to brain-imaging and biomarkers.

Recognizing the full extent of human diver-sity does not mean giving up on the quest to understand human nature. To the contrary, this recognition illuminates a journey into human nature that is more exciting, more complex, and ultimately more consequential than has previously been suspected Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan are in the Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada. Joseph Henrich is also in the Department of Economics.e-mail: [email protected]. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X (2010).2. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725 (2010).3. Arnett, J. Am. Psychol. 63, 602–614 (2008).4. Henrich, J. et al. Science 327, 1480–1484 (2010).5. Foote, C. L., Goette, L. & Meier, S. Policymaking Insights from

Behavioral Economics (Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, 2009).6. Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z. Y. & Guo, T. Y. J. Behav. Decis. Making 21,

399–413 (2008).

Most people are not WEIRD To understand human psychology, behavioural scientists must stop doing most of their experiments on Westerners, argue Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan.

GRA

CIA

LAM

29

Vol 466|1 July 2010

OPINION

© 20 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved10

2.AssessingAppropriateness

Page 32: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

32

Much research on human behaviour and psychology assumes that everyone shares most fundamental cognitive

and affective processes, and that findings from one population apply across the board. A growing body of evidence suggests that this is not the case.

Experimental findings from several disci-plines indicate considerable variation among human populations in diverse domains, such as visual perception, analytic reasoning, fairness, cooperation, memory and the herit-ability of IQ1,2. This is in line with what anthropologists have long suggested: that people from West-ern, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) socie-ties — and particularly American undergraduates — are some of the most psychologically unusual peo-ple on Earth1.

So the fact that the vast majority of studies use WEIRD participants presents a challenge to the under-standing of human psychology and behaviour. A 2008 survey of the top psychology journals found that 96% of subjects were from Western industrialized countries — which house just 12% of the world’s population3. Strange, then, that research articles routinely assume that their results are broadly representative, rarely adding even a cautionary footnote on how far their findings can be generalized.

The evidence that basic cognitive and motivational processes vary across populations has become increasingly difficult to ignore. For example, many studies have shown that Ameri-cans, Canadians and western Europeans rely on analytical reasoning strategies — which separate objects from their contexts and rely on rules to explain and predict behaviour — substantially more than non-Westerners. Research also indi-cates that Americans use analytical thinking more than, say, Europeans. By contrast, Asians tend to reason holistically, for example by con-sidering people’s behaviour in terms of their situation1. Yet many long-standing theories of how humans perceive, categorize and remember emphasize the centrality of analytical thought.

It is a similar story with social behaviour related to fairness and equality. Here, research-ers often use one-shot economic experiments such as the ultimatum game, in which a player

decides how much of a fixed amount to offer a second player, who can then accept or reject this proposal. If the second player rejects it, neither player gets anything. Participants from industrialized societies tend to divide the money equally, and reject low offers. Peo-ple from non-industrialized societies behave differently, especially in the smallest-scale non-market societies such as foragers in Africa and horticulturalists in South America, where peo-ple are neither inclined to make equal offers nor to punish those who make low offers4.

Recent developments in evolutionary biology, neuroscience and related fields sug-gest that these differences stem from the way in which populations have adapted to diverse culturally constructed environments. Ama-zonian groups, such as the Piraha, whose languages do not include numerals above three, are worse at distinguishing large quan-tities digitally than groups using extensive counting systems, but are similar in their abil-ity to approximate quantities. This suggests the kind of counting system people grow up with influences how they think about integers1.

Costly generalizationsUsing study participants from one unusual population could have important practical consequences. For example, economists have been developing theories of decision-making incorporating insights from psychology and social science — such as how to set wages — and examining how these might translate into policy5. Researchers and policy-makers should recognize that populations vary con-siderably in the extent to which they display certain biases, patterns and preferences in economic decisions, such as those related to optimism1. Such differences can, for example,

affect the way that experienced investors make decisions about the stock market6.

We offer four suggestions to help put theories of human behaviour and psychology on a firmer empirical footing. First, editors and reviewers should push researchers to support any generalizations with evidence. Second, granting agencies, reviewers and editors should give researchers credit for comparing diverse and inconvenient subject pools. Third, granting agencies should prioritize cross-disciplinary, cross-cultural research. Fourth, researchers

must strive to evaluate how their findings apply to other populations. There are several low-cost ways to approach this in the short term: one is to select a few judiciously chosen populations that provide a ‘tough test’ of universality in some domain, such as societies with limited count-ing systems for testing theories about numerical cognition1,2.

A crucial longer-term goal is to establish a set of principles that researchers can use to distinguish variable from universal aspects of

psychology. Establishing such principles will remain difficult until behavioural scientists develop interdisciplinary, international research networks for long-term studies on diverse populations using an array of methods, from experimental techniques and ethnography to brain-imaging and biomarkers.

Recognizing the full extent of human diver-sity does not mean giving up on the quest to understand human nature. To the contrary, this recognition illuminates a journey into human nature that is more exciting, more complex, and ultimately more consequential than has previously been suspected Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan are in the Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4, Canada. Joseph Henrich is also in the Department of Economics.e-mail: [email protected]. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X (2010).2. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. Behav. Brain Sci.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725 (2010).3. Arnett, J. Am. Psychol. 63, 602–614 (2008).4. Henrich, J. et al. Science 327, 1480–1484 (2010).5. Foote, C. L., Goette, L. & Meier, S. Policymaking Insights from

Behavioral Economics (Federal Reserve Bank of Boston, 2009).6. Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z. Y. & Guo, T. Y. J. Behav. Decis. Making 21,

399–413 (2008).

Most people are not WEIRD To understand human psychology, behavioural scientists must stop doing most of their experiments on Westerners, argue Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine and Ara Norenzayan.

GRA

CIA

LAM

29

Vol 466|1 July 2010

OPINION

© 20 Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved10

WesternEducatedIndustrializedRichDemocraBc

2.AssessingAppropriateness

Page 33: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

2.AssessingAppropriatenessAbsolu=st —moBvaBonaltheoriescanbeapplieduniversally(eBcapproach)

Rela=vist—onlywithintheindigenousorlocalframesofreference—nogeneralisaBonpossibleacrossculturalcontexts(emicapproach)

Universalist—someaspectsareuniversal,butthesocioculturalcontextisalsocrucial (Zusho&Clayton,2011)

Absolu=st

Rela=vist

Universalist

Page 34: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

2.AssessingAppropriateness

Absolu=st Rela=vistUniversalist

SocioeducaBonal ComplexDynamicSystems

Self-determinaBon

L2MoBvaBonalSelfSystem

Page 35: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Overview

1. MoBvaBonaltheories

2. Assessingappropriateness

3. Flawsincurrentmodelsandapproaches

4. Apost-paradigm,suprathemaBc

approach

Page 36: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Socioeduca-onalmodel!

Self-determina-ontheory(SDT)!

L2Mo-va-onalSelf-System!

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)!

**Note:ThisisplayingDevil’sAdvocate…

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

Page 37: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromhardsciencesUsedtomathemaBcallypredictbothlinearandnonlinearsystems

!Ouenreferredtosupportclaimsthat-individualsareunpredictable-findingscannotbegeneralizedacrosscontexts-modelscannotpredictpa#erns-staBsBcsareuseless

Page 38: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromhardsciencesUsedtomathemaBcallypredictbothlinearandnonlinearsystems

!Ifeveryoneisuniqueandunpredictable…!—thenwhybotherconductresearch?whybotherreadwhatotherswrite?

Page 39: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Complex/DynamicSystems(CDS)

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromhardsciencesUsedtomathemaBcallypredictbothlinearandnonlinearsystems

!IsthisamodelofmoBvaBon,oranapproachtoresearchmethodology?

Page 40: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!L2experiences—notaselfconceptIdealL2Self—>whatdo/canstudentsimagine?Ought-toL2Self—>Isthisthesameas“feared”?

Page 41: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!Ought-toL2Self—Englishisarequiredcourse…so…—Thisisthe*current*self—Ouencombinedwithfamily/socialexpectaBons(esp.Confucian-influenced)

Page 42: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!IdealL2Self—The“idealL2self”asfutureuserofEnglish……whousesEnglish?—naBvespeakers…whoarenaBvespeakers?—whitemaleUS

Page 43: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!IdealL2Self—CanaspeakerofanotherlanguagebeacceptedashavinganL2selfbyL1speakers? Russian.Bantu.Urdu.German.Japanese.

Page 44: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!IdealL2Self—Selfispartlyself-determined,partlyother-determined

—Caughtupinissuesofethnicity,religion,gender,history,poliBcs…

—whenistheL1selfstable?theL2?Proficiency?

Page 45: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

L2Mo-va-onalSelfSystem

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

OriginallyfromcogniBvepsychology-Idealself,fearedself,probableself!Isthisreally“moBvaBon”?!Orapre-exisBngcondiBonleadingtomoBvaBngsituaBonsorfactors?

Page 46: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Self-determina-ontheory

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

Assumeschoice—Whatifthereisnone?—Whatifstudentshavetostudy?—WhatifthegoalisgraduaBon?orajob?travel?!—Whatifthereischoice,butthereisalsooverwhelmingpressure(societal,monetary)?

Socioeduca-onalmodel

Page 47: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Self-determina-ontheory

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

AssumestheexistenceofanL2community—Whatifthereisnonephysicallypresent?—WhatiftheL2communityistoolargetobedefined?

—WhatiftheL2communityisessenBalizedas“theOther”?

Socioeduca-onalmodel

Page 48: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Self-determina-ontheory

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

WhatkindofcommunicaBonisneeded?!“Theconstruct,IntegraBveness,reflectsagenuineinterestinlearning…forthepurposesofcommunicaBngwithmembersoftheotherlanguagecommunity…

Socioeduca-onalmodel

Page 49: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Self-determina-ontheory

3.Flawsincurrentmodels/approaches

“Intheextreme,thismightinvolvecompleteiden-fica-onwithandmembershipintheothercommunity…inlessextremeformitmightsimplyreflectawillingnesstoincorporatebehavioralpaUernsintheformofthelanguagefromtheothergroup…”(Gardner,2010,p.88)

Socioeduca-onalmodel

Page 50: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

50

[In the global age, the world is your rival]

LinguisBcimperialism?“FearofanEnglishPlanet.”

Page 51: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Overview

1. MoBvaBonaltheories

2. Assessingappropriateness

3. Flawsincurrentmodelsandapproaches

4. Apost-paradigm,suprathema-c

approach

Page 52: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

4.Apost-paradigm,suprathema-capproach

Universalistperspec=veonL2mo=va=on!—Allmodelsarewrong,butsomeareuseful—Allapproacheshavesomevalue,butnoneperfectlyexplaineverything—EBcandemicapproachesaretwocomplementarysides ofthesamecoin,andarebothnecessary—Individualwithinhisorherculturalcontextwhichcanalsoreflectsimilarpa#ernsinotherindividualsandcontexts

Absolu=st Rela=vistUniversalist

Page 53: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

4.Apost-paradigm,suprathema-capproach

QuantumCogni-on

An(old)newapproachthatcanexplain:—howstudyparBcipantscanholdcontradictoryviewsofthesameissueortopic

—howusingthesamequesBonnairecanreturndifferentresults

—howreorderingquesBonnaireorinterviewitemscanleadtodifferentresults

Page 54: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Complementarity—itemsarenotindependent;influenceeachother!Superposi=on—possiblefortwodifferingopinionstobeheld!Entanglement—measuringonepartaffectsanotherinasystem

4.Apost-paradigm,suprathema-capproach

QuantumCogni-on

Page 55: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

MoBvaBonisnotastatebuta!

dynamiccumula-veacEveprocess

thatexplains…! -why? -howlong? -howhard?

Page 56: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Thefinalword…

“Rememberthatallmodelsarewrong;thepracBcalquesBonishowwrongdotheyhavetobenottobeuseful.”

(Box,1987,p.74)

Page 57: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Thankyou!Ma#hewApple mapple@fc.ritsumei.ac.jpRitsumeikanUniversityTerryFellnerSagaUniversityDexterDaSilvaKeisenUniversity

Page 58: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Upcoming

L2SelvesandMoBvaBonsinAsianContexts!Japan MalaysiaChina(PR) IndonesiaChina(HongKongSAR) IndiaChina(Taiwan) VietnamSouthKorea!Winter2016/Spring2017

Page 59: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel

IntegraBvemoBvaBonIntegraBveorientaBonIntegraBveness

InstrumentalmoBvaBonInstrumentalorientaBonInstrumentality

v

“NoL2targetculture”—>“Therefore,nointegraBvemoBvaBon”

Page 60: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

“NoL2targetculture”—>“Therefore,nointegraBvemoBvaBon”

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Socioeduca-onalmodel

IntegraBvemoBvaBonIntegraBveorientaBonIntegraBveness

InstrumentalmoBvaBonInstrumentalorientaBonInstrumentality

vX

Page 61: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on

SelfDetermina-onTheory

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

IntrinsicmoBvaBon!

—internal—thinginitself

ExtrinsicmoBvaBon!

—external—rewards

vX

Page 62: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

Developmentofinternaliza-onoftheautonomousself !!

Text

Text

IdenBfiedRegulaBon

TIntrinsicMo-va-on

IntegratedRegulaBon

ExternalRegulaBon

AmoBvaBont

IntrojectedRegulaBon

(Ryan & Deci, 2002)

1.Mo-va-onaltheories

Page 63: Language Learning Motivation in Asia

1.Mo-va-on1.Mo-va-onaltheories

—feelingofconnectedness=!helpsinternalizaBonofexternalmoBvaBngfactors

SelfDetermina-onTheory(Deci&Ryan,2000)