85
CHAPTER 2: VISUAL ELEMENTS OF ART

Chapter 2

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

 

Citation preview

Page 1: Chapter 2

CHAPTER 2:

VISUAL ELEMENTS OF ART

Page 2: Chapter 2

Professor:Course/Section:

I found I could say things with color and shapes that I couldn’t say in any

other way – things I had no words for.

–Georgia O’Keeffe

Page 3: Chapter 2

The Visual Elements of Art:

• Color• Shape• Line• Light• Value• Texture • Space• Time• Motion

Page 4: Chapter 2

The Language of Art

• With the “Language of Art,” we are able to communicate thoughts and feelings about our visual and tactile experiences in our world

Page 5: Chapter 2

Visual Elements of Art

• Also called the Plastic Elements of art. • Art selects a medium

– Drawing, painting, sculpture, architecture, photography, textiles, ceramics, etc..

• Then they use the visual elements to express themselves in the chosen medium.

Page 6: Chapter 2

Principles of Design:

• Unify• Balance• Rhythm• Scale• Proportion• Etc..

Page 7: Chapter 2

LINE…

• …is the simplest and also the most complex of the elements of art.

• …serves as the basic building block for all art

• …has the capacity to evoke thoughts and emotions

Page 8: Chapter 2

Definition of Line

• In Geometry - “A line is made up of an infinite number of points and the the shortest distance between two points is a straight line.”

• In art - A line is a moving dot.

Page 9: Chapter 2

Characteristics of a Line

• Measure - its length and width. • Expressive qualities

Page 10: Chapter 2

Expressive Qualities of Line

• “Line may be perceived as delicate, tentative, elegant, assertive, forceful, or even brutal.”

Page 11: Chapter 2

Fig 2-2 JACKSON POLLOCK. Number 14: Gray (1948). Enamel and gesso on paper. 223/4” x 31”.

Page 12: Chapter 2

Lines can be…

• Straight • Curved• Vertical• Horizontal• Diagonal• Zigzagged…

Page 13: Chapter 2

Types of Line

• Contour Lines - Created by the edge of things.

• Actual lines - Are connected and continuous.

• Implied lines - completed by the viewer. • Psychological lines - A line created by a

mental or perceptual connection. (Ex: When a character of figure points or looks at another.)

Page 14: Chapter 2

Fig 2-4 A, B, and C Actual line (A) versus two kinds of implied lines, one formed by dots (B) and the other formed by psychologically connecting the edges of a series of straight lines (C).

Page 15: Chapter 2

More about line…

• “Edges are perceived because the objects differ from the background in value, texture or color.”

• Shading creates or models roundness.

• “One of the hallmarks of Renaissance painting is the use of implied lines to create or echo the structures of the composition.”

Page 16: Chapter 2

Figure 2.5, p.30: LEONARDO DA VINCI. Madonna of the Rocks (c. 1483). Oil on panel, transferred to canvas. 78 1⁄2” x 48”.Figure 2.6, p30: The pyramidal structure of the Madonna of the Rocks.

Page 17: Chapter 2

Figure 2.7 p. 31. EMILY MARY OSBORNE. Nameless and Friendless (1834 - ?) Oil on Canvas. 34” x 44”.

Page 18: Chapter 2

Functions of Line

1. To Outline and Shape2. As Form 3. To Create Depth and Texture4. To Suggest Direction and

Movement

Page 19: Chapter 2

Figure 2.8, p.31: RIMMA GERLOVINA AND VALERIY GERLOVIN. Madonna and Child (1992). Chromogenic print.

To Give Outline and Shape

Page 20: Chapter 2

Figure 2.9, p.31: ELIZABETH CATLETT. Sharecropper (1968). Color linocut. 26” x 22”.

To Create Depth and Texture

Page 21: Chapter 2

Ways to create Texture

1. Modeling - the creation of the illusion of roundness or 3d through the use of light and shadow.

2. Stippling - the use of a pattern of dots that thickens and thins.

3. Hatching - using a series of closely spaces parallel lines to achieve shading.

4. Cross-Hatching - a series of lines that run in a different direction and cross each other.

Page 22: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-12 Illusion of three-dimensionality.

Page 23: Chapter 2

Figure 2.11, p.32: SANDRO BOTTICELLI. The Birth of Venus (c. 1482). Oil on canvas. 5’8 7⁄8” x 9’1 7⁄8”.

To Suggest Direction and Movement

Page 24: Chapter 2

What lines imply

• Horizontal lines - suggest stability• Vertical lines - defy gravity and

suggest assertiveness.• Diagonal lines - imply movement

and directionality.

Page 25: Chapter 2

SHAPE, VOLUME AND MASS

SHAPE

Page 26: Chapter 2

SHAPE

• Has many definitions• In art - “shapes are defined as the

areas within a composition that have boundaries separating them from what surrounds them; shapes make those areas distinct.”

• Shape can also be communicated through patches of color and texture.

Page 27: Chapter 2

Fig. 12 JACOB LAWRENCE. Harriet Tubman Series, No. 4 (1939 - 1940) Casein tempera on gessoed hardboard. 12” x 17 7/8”.

Page 28: Chapter 2

Figure 2.14, p.34: HELENE BRANDT. Mondrian Variations, Construction No. 3B with Four Red Squares and Two Planes (1996). Welded steel, wood, paint. 22” x 19” x 17”.

The word FORM - is often used to speak about shapes in sculpture and

architecture - 3D works of art.

Page 29: Chapter 2

Volume refers to the mass or bulk of a 3D work. It is the amount of space

it contains.

Fig. 2-15. GERRIT RIETVELDT. Schroeder House, Utrecht. (1924).

Page 30: Chapter 2

Mass - In 3D art, the mass of an object refers to its bulk.

Fig. 2-16 RACHEL WHITEREAD. Holocaust Memorial, Vienna (2000).

Page 31: Chapter 2

Actual Mass versus Implied Mass

• Actual mass occupies three-dimensional space and has measurable volume and weight

• Implied mass creates the illusion of possessing volume, having weight and occupying three-dimensional space

Page 32: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-17 MARK TANSEY. Landscape (1994). Oil on Canvas. 181.6cm x 365.8 cm.

Page 33: Chapter 2

Types of Shapes

• 1. Geometric shapes - Are regular and precise. Ex: rectangles and circles.– Straight (rectilinear) – Curved (curvilinear)

• 2. Organic shapes -have a natural appearance.– Biomorphic shapes– Amorphous shapes

Page 34: Chapter 2

Fig. 18 DAVID SMITH. Cubi XVIII (1964). Polished stainless steel. 9’7 3/4” x 5” x 1’ 9 3/4”.

Geometric Shapes

Page 35: Chapter 2

Figure 2.21, p.39: FRANK GEHRY. Guggenheim Museum, Bilbao, Spain (1997).

Organic Shapes

Figure 2.19, p.37: FRANK GEHRY. Guggenheim Museum, Bilbao, Spain (1997).

Page 36: Chapter 2

Compare and Contrast

Picasso and Colescott

Rectilinear forms versus curvilinear forms presented

by two artists

Page 37: Chapter 2

Figure 2.20, p.38: PABLO PICASSO. Les Demoiselles d’Avignon (1907). Oil on canvas. 8’ x 7’8”.

Page 38: Chapter 2

Figure 2.21: ROBERT COLESCOTT. Les Demoiselles d’Alabama: Vestidas (1985). Acrylic on canvas. 96” x 92”.

Page 39: Chapter 2

Biomorphic Shapes

• Are said to have a form like a biological entity.

• (From the Greek word morphe.)

Page 40: Chapter 2

Figure 2.22, p.39: ELIZABETH MURRAY. Tangled (1985–1990). Oil on shaped canvas with wood. 83 1/2 x 66 x 19 in.

Page 41: Chapter 2

Positive and Negative Shapes

• Positive shapes - the objects or figure that the viewer focuses on.

• Negative shapes - the empty space (or the space filled with other imagery) left over in the piece.

Page 42: Chapter 2

Figure-Ground Terminology

• Figure - ground relationship - the relationship between the positive and negative shapes in a piece.

• Figure - ground reversals - when the positive and negative shapes in a piece can be reversed or are ambiguous.

• “We tend to perceive things in context.”

Page 43: Chapter 2

Fig. 26 A Rubin Vase.

Page 44: Chapter 2

Shape as Icon

• Some shapes carry with them immediate associations.

• Ex:• Christian Cross• Jewish Star of David• Chinese yin yang.• “Shape s a powerful visual element, and

the representation of shape is a powerful design tool.”

Page 45: Chapter 2

Figure 2.28, p.42: EDWARD STEICHEN. Rodin with His Sculptures “Victor Hugo” and “The Thinker” (1902). Carbon print, toned.

Page 46: Chapter 2

LIGHT AND VALUE

• Visible light is the part of the spectrum of electromagnetic energy that we can see.

• “Without light there is no art.”

Page 47: Chapter 2

Value

• The value of a color of a surface is its lightness or darkness.

• Value contrast - the degrees f difference between shades of gray.

• Drawing objects or figures with a high value contrast makes them easy to see.

• Value pattern describes the variation in light and dark within a composition.

Page 48: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-33 Value contrast.

Page 49: Chapter 2

Chiaroscuro

• The gradual shifting from light to dark through a successive gradation of tones across a curved surface.

Page 50: Chapter 2

Figure 2.35, PIERRE-PAUL PRUD’HON. La Source (c. 1801). Black and white chalk on gray paper. 21 3/16 x 15 5/18 in.

Page 51: Chapter 2

Descriptive and Expressive Properties

of Value• Values - blacks, grays and whites• May be used to describe objects• Or may be used to evoke

emotional response in the viewer.

Page 52: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-37 p.46 LORRAINE O’GRADY Mlle Bourgeoise Noire Goes to the New Museum (1981).

Page 53: Chapter 2

COLOR

• Language connects emotion with color.

• Color can trigger emotional response in the observer.

Page 54: Chapter 2

Psychological Dimensions of Color: Hue,

Value, and Saturation• Hue - a term for the family of color. • Cool - colors on the green-blue side

of the color wheel. • Warm - colors on the yellow-orange-

red side of the color wheel. • Saturation - the pureness of the color• Shades - adding black to a hue. • Tints - adding white to a hue.

Page 55: Chapter 2

Additive and Subtractive Colors

• Additive color - mixing light. • Subtractive color - mixing pigments. • Primary colors - Color that can not be derived

from the mixing of other colors.– Red– Yellow– Blue

• Secondary colors - created from the overlap or mixing of 2 primary colors. – Orange– Green– Violet.

Page 56: Chapter 2

Complementary versus Analogous Colors

• In pigments, the primary colors are red, yellow and blue.

• They can not be produced from mixing other colors.

• Tertiary colors - created by mixing pigments or primary and secondary colors.

• Analogous colors- Hues that lie next to each other on the color wheel.

• Complementary colors - colors that lie across from one another on the color wheel.

Page 57: Chapter 2

Local versus Optical Color

• Local Color - the hue of an object as created by the colors its surface reflects under normal lighting condition.

• Optical color - our perceptions of color, which can vary with lighting conditions.

Page 58: Chapter 2

Figure 2.46, p.51: CLAUDE MONET. Haystack at Sunset near Giverny (1891). Oil on canvas. 28 7⁄8” x 36 1⁄2”.

Page 59: Chapter 2

Figure 2.49, p.51: VINCENT VAN GOGH. The Night Café (1888). Oil on canvas. 27 1⁄2” x 35”.

Page 60: Chapter 2

Color as Symbol

• We link mood with color.• Feelings and behavior can be

symbolized with colors• The symbols and meanings of

colors are culture specific.

Page 61: Chapter 2

Texture

• Texture – Derived from the Latin word for “weaving”– Used to describe the surface character of

things through the sense of touch. – An artist can emphasize of distort texture of

an object to evoke emotional response in the viewer.

• Impasto - a think buildup of paint on the surface of the canvas.

Page 62: Chapter 2

Figure 2.49, p.53: LEON KOSSOFF. Portrait of Father, No. 2 (1972). Oil on board. 60” x 36”.

Page 63: Chapter 2

Types of Texture

• Actual Texture - is tactile, texture you can touch. – Example: impasto (The most common type

of texture used in painting.)

• Visual Texture - simulated texture. It looks like a texture but can’t really be felt. – Example: Trompe l’oeil a French word (and

style of painting) that means to trick the eye.

• Subversive Texture - Texture chosen or created by the artist to subvert or undermine our ideas about the objects they depict.

Page 64: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-52 P. 54 RACHEL RUYSCH. Flower Still Life (after 1700) Oil on Canvas. 29 3/4” x 23 7/8”.

Page 65: Chapter 2

Figure 2.53, p.55: DAVID GILHOOLY. Bowl of Chocolate Moose (1989). Ceramic. 10” x 6” x 7” (25.4 cm x 15.2 cm x 17.8 cm).

Page 66: Chapter 2

Figure 2.55 p.56 MERET OPPENHEIM. Object (1936). Fur covered cup, sauser, and spoon. Overall height: 2 7/8 in.

Page 67: Chapter 2

SPACE

• Objects exist in Three-dimensional space.

• Some art is truly 3D like sculpture and architecture.

• And some art just tries to depict space on a 2D surface.

Page 68: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-56 Overlapping circles and arcs.

Overlapping

• You can create the illusion of depth by overlapping objects.

Page 69: Chapter 2

Figure 2.58, p.58: NI ZAN. Rongxi Studio (Late Yuan/Early Ming dynasty, 1372 CE). Hanging scroll; ink on paper. H: 29 1⁄4”

Relative Size and Linear Perspective

• The furthers objects are from us the smaller the look.

• Things that are closer to us look larger and things that are further away look smaller.

• Artist use different techniques like relative size and linear perspective to create the illusion of depth in a piece of art.

Page 70: Chapter 2

The Illusion of Depth

• Vanishing point - The point at which parallel lines cone together, or converge.

• Horizon - the line where the line of sight stops and on which the artist often places the vanishing point.

• Vantage point - where (or the height) the viewer is looking from.

• One-point perspective - when parallel lines in a picture come together at one point, the vanishing point, on the horizon line.

• Two-point perspective - when parallel lines in a picture come together at 2 different points on the horizon line.

Page 71: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-64 RAFFAELLO SANZIO (CALLED RAPHAEL). PHILOSOPHY, OR SCHOOL OF ATHENS (1509-1511).

Fig. 2-65 Perspective in School of Athens.

Page 72: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-66 GUSTAVE CAILLEBOTTE. Paris Street: Rainy Day *1877). Oil on Canvas. 83 1/2” 108 1/4”.

Fig. 2-67. Perspective in Caillebottoes’s Paris Street: Rainy Day.

Page 73: Chapter 2

Atmospheric Perspective

• (Also called aerial perspective.)

• Texture gradient - closer objects are perceived as having rougher or more detailed surfaces.

• Brightness gradient - distant objects are less intense.

Page 74: Chapter 2

Figure 2.69, p.61: SYLVIA PLIMACK MANGOLD. Schunnemunk Mountain (1979). Oil on canvas. 60” x 80 1⁄8”.

Page 75: Chapter 2

Time and Motion

• Actual Motion:– Kinetic Art - art that moves.

Example: Mobiles– Photography

Page 76: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-70 p. 62 ALEXANDER CALDER. Untitled (1972). East Building mobile.

Page 77: Chapter 2

Implied Motion

• Stopped Time - a style of art that “stops time” in order to imply motion.

• Time implied & Motion Implied - Some works try to imply that motion or time has occurred.

Page 78: Chapter 2

Figure 2.71, p.63: GIANLORENZO BERNINI. Apollo and Daphne (1622–1624). Marble. 7’6”.

Page 79: Chapter 2

The Illusion of Motion

• There is a difference between implied motion and the illusion of motion.

• One implies that the motion has already occurred and the other implies that the motion is happening right now.

• Examples:• Early photographic experiments of multiple

exposures of motion.• The blurring of shapes and the repetition of

linear patterns blurring the contours of a figure. • Blurring outlines to create the illusion of motion.• Op Art !

Page 80: Chapter 2

Figure 2.74, p.64: THOMAS EAKINS. Man Pole Vaulting (c. 1884). Photograph.

Page 81: Chapter 2

UMBERTO BOCCIONI. Dynamism of a Soccer Player (1913). Oil on canvas. 6’4 1⁄8” x 6’7 1⁄8”.

Page 82: Chapter 2

BERNHARD JOHANNES AND ANNA BLUME. Kitchen Tantrums (1986–1987). Photo-piece. 51 1⁄8” x 35 7/8”.

Page 83: Chapter 2

Op Art

• Op Art - Optical Art, is based on creating optical sensations of movement through the repetition and manipulation of color, shape, and line.

• Afterimage - when we look at a color for a long period of time and then look away you may briefly see the opposite color due to fatigue of the cornea in the eyes.

Page 84: Chapter 2

Other Ways of Creating the Illusion of Motion

• Cinematography and video• Stroboscopic motion• (Real movement involves illusion)

Page 85: Chapter 2

Fig. 2-76. P.65 BRIDGET RILEY. Gala. (1974). Acrylic on canvas. 5’ 2 3/4” square.