11
2 CAN ANATOMICALLY-MODERN HUMANS BE USED AS ANALOGUES FOR NEANDERTAL FORAGING PATTERNS? Benjamin Collins Introduction Current frameworks and models for interpreting past foragers’ subsistence patterns have been constructed through extending ethnoarchaeological and ethnographic observations of modern peoples (Binford, 1978; David and Kramer, 2001; Hudson, 1993; Kelly, 2007). A strong critique of this approach during the 1970s and 1980s (Gould, 1978, 1980; Wobst, 1978) has led to both a refinement in the extent to which modern foraging behaviour is extended to past foragers, and more accurate interpretations of past foraging behaviours (Wylie, 1985, 2002). These models have also been used to interpret the behaviours that are preserved in archaeological and zooarchaeological assemblages attributed to other hominins, perhaps uncritically so. Neandertals present an interesting case, as they are the most closely related hominin to anatomically-modern humans (AMH). This close relationship is demonstrated by both genetic (Green, et al., 2006, 2010; Krause, et al., 2007; Noonan, et al., 2006) and morphological research (Conroy, 2005; Harvarti, et al., 2004; Trinkaus, 2007), with the genetic data indicating that the two populations diverged roughly 400,000 years ago and were capable of interbreeding. However, it has been strongly contested that Neandertals lack the cognitive complexity associated with AMH (Barnard, et al., 2007; Klein, 2008, 2009; Mellars, 2005, 2006; Wynn and Coolidge, 2004). Are interpretive models based on studies of AMH foragers therefore suitable for interpreting Neandertal foraging behaviours? This question will be addressed by assessing the arguments for differences and similarities between Neandertal and AMH cognition, with a focus on the archaeological record, biological differences and cognitive models. A recent study will also be discussed to demonstrate how the ethnographic record can be used to infer Neandertal behaviour. AMH and Neandertal archaeological assemblages The archaeological record has been extensively used to argue for significant cognitive differences between Within the past 25 years, ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of modern foragers have been critically used to create and refine models for inferring past human foraging behaviours. These models have also been extended to interpret the foraging behaviours of other hominins. However, critical evaluations of whether ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH are relevant to other hominins are rare. This article assesses whether the foraging patterns of Neandertals, our closest relatives, can be accurately inferred when using models derived and refined from studies of anatomically-modern human (AMH) foragers. It is suggested that Neandertal foraging patterns can be accurately inferred when using optimal foraging models derived from behavioural ecology and refined with ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH. However, potentially symbolic or cultural aspects of Neandertal foraging behaviours are contended to be beyond the scope of these models, as the differences between AMH and Neandertal cognition are yet to be understood.

Can anatomically modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns?

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

2

CAN ANATOMICALLY-MODERN HUMANS BE USED AS ANALOGUES FOR NEANDERTAL FORAGING PATTERNS?

Benjamin Collins

IntroductionCurrent frameworks and models for interpreting past foragers’ subsistence patterns have been constructed through extending ethnoarchaeological and ethnographic observations of modern peoples (Binford, 1978; David and Kramer, 2001; Hudson, 1993; Kelly, 2007). A strong critique of this approach during the 1970s and 1980s (Gould, 1978, 1980; Wobst, 1978) has led to both a refinement in the extent to which modern foraging behaviour is extended to past foragers, and more accurate interpretations of past foraging behaviours (Wylie, 1985, 2002). These models have also been used to interpret the behaviours that are preserved in archaeological and zooarchaeological assemblages attributed to other hominins, perhaps uncritically so.

Neandertals present an interesting case, as they are the most closely related hominin to anatomically-modern humans (AMH). This close relationship is demonstrated by both genetic (Green, et al., 2006, 2010; Krause, et al., 2007; Noonan, et al., 2006) and morphological research (Conroy, 2005; Harvarti, et al., 2004; Trinkaus, 2007),

with the genetic data indicating that the two populations diverged roughly 400,000 years ago and were capable of interbreeding. However, it has been strongly contested that Neandertals lack the cognitive complexity associated with AMH (Barnard, et al., 2007; Klein, 2008, 2009; Mellars, 2005, 2006; Wynn and Coolidge, 2004).

Are interpretive models based on studies of AMH foragers therefore suitable for interpreting Neandertal foraging behaviours? This question will be addressed by assessing the arguments for differences and similarities between Neandertal and AMH cognition, with a focus on the archaeological record, biological differences and cognitive models. A recent study will also be discussed to demonstrate how the ethnographic record can be used to infer Neandertal behaviour.

AMH and Neandertal archaeological assemblagesThe archaeological record has been extensively used to argue for significant cognitive differences between

Within the past 25 years, ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of modern foragers have been critically used to create and refine models for inferring past human foraging behaviours. These models have also been extended to interpret the foraging behaviours of other hominins. However, critical evaluations of whether ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH are relevant to other hominins are rare. This article assesses whether the foraging patterns of Neandertals, our closest relatives, can be accurately inferred when using models derived and refined from studies of anatomically-modern human (AMH) foragers.

It is suggested that Neandertal foraging patterns can be accurately inferred when using optimal foraging models derived from behavioural ecology and refined with ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH. However, potentially symbolic or cultural aspects of Neandertal foraging behaviours are contended to be beyond the scope of these models, as the differences between AMH and Neandertal cognition are yet to be understood.

2. Can anatomically-modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns? 9

Neandertals and AMH, a position that stems from the ‘symbolic explosion’ associated with the Upper Palaeolithic cultural tradition (UP, approximately 40 kya to 10 kya) (Klein, 2009; Mellars, 2006). The UP is represented by the appearance of new lithic technologies and material culture (see Table 1) that are either absent or rare during the Middle Palaeolithic (MP, approximately 250 kya to 40 kya) (Mellars, 1996, 2005). Material symbolic expression and complex behaviour are considered a uniquely AMH phenomenon, as AMH are argued to be exclusively associated with the UP (Klein, 2008, 2009; Mellars, 2005, 2006). Neandertals are considered to be solely represented by MP material culture in Europe and only contentiously associated with several transitional industries that have UP characteristics, such as blades and personal ornaments (Bar-Yosef and Kuhn, 1999; d’Errico, 2003; d’Errico, et al., 1998; Mellars, et al., 2007; Zilhao, et al., 2007).

The paucity of symbolic expression recovered during the MP has led to Neandertals typically being considered cognitively inferior to AMH. However, d’Errico and colleagues (2003, 2009), amongst others (Speth, 2004; Wolpoff, et al., 2004), have noted that there are sporadic and increasing indications of Neandertal symbolic behaviour during the MP. For example, over 40 MP sites yield evidence of pigment use, with over 500 pieces of ochre being recovered from Pech de l’Azé I and IV in France (Soressi, et al., 2007). Incised bone fragments are also becoming more common, as are personal adornments, such as shell beads and worked animal teeth (Burke and d’Errico, 2008; Davies and Underdown, 2006; d’Errico, et al., 2003, 2009; Zilhao, et al., 2010). In fact blades, once used as an indicator of UP AMH archaeological assemblages, are now frequently found in both MP and African Middle Stone Age (MSA) contexts (Bar-Yosef and Kuhn, 1999; d’Errico and Stringer, 2011; McBrearty and Brooks, 2000).

Comparisons between MP and UP faunal assemblages also provide an interesting perspective. UP AMH assemblages are argued to contain a diverse range of fauna, indicating broad spectrum foraging patterns that incorporate all aspects of the environment, including small and hard to catch prey, aquatic resources, seasonal prey and large, dangerous prey

(Klein, 2001; Mellars, 1996, 2005; Stiner and Munro, 2002). This has been contrasted with the MP, during which Neandertal faunal assemblages appear to be composed predominately of large herbivores Hockett and Haws, 2005; Patou-Mathis, 2000; Richards and Trinkaus, 2009).

Isotopic studies comparing Neandertals and UP AMH have frequently distinguished between the two groups, with Neandertals displaying specialised, terrestrial carnivore diets and UP AMH broader diets that included aquatic resources (Richards, et al., 2001; Richards and Trinkaus, 2009). Furthermore, a recent study involving a Neandertal skeleton recovered from an underwater coastal context did not give any indication of an aquatic dietary component, furthering the specialised, terrestrial carnivore hypothesis (Hublin, et al., 2009).

Other zooarchaeological studies contest this hypothesis and instead suggest that some Neandertal faunal assemblages indicate a broad spectrum diet, similar to UP AMH (Bar-Yosef, 2004; Stringer, et al., 2008). These studies have more closely related the past environmental conditions to the faunal assemblages and found that Neandertals living in warmer, non-steppe environments were acquiring aquatic and small prey, and using plant resources (Adler, et al., 2006; Blasco, 2008; Blasco and Fernandez Peris, 2009; Henry, et al., 2011; Lev, et al., 2005; Madella, et al., 2002; Stringer, et al., 2008). Furthermore, several zooarchaeological studies have demonstrated continuity in subsistence strategies across the terminal MP and early UP in steppe environments (Adler, et al., 2006; Grayson and Delpech, 2003; Morin, 2008).

Neandertal zooarchaeological assemblages can be argued to indicate that Neandertals were able to forage in a similar manner to AMH. However, there are still some important differences regarding the social and cultural aspects of MP and UP foraging. As noted above, the UP is characterised by symbolic expression, which is also a strong feature of modern hunter-gatherer subsistence (Bird-David, 1999; Brightman, 2002; Ingold, 2000; Willerslev, 2007). This is indicated by UP cave art, zoomorphic figures and modified faunal remains, such as pierced animal tooth pendants. Evidence for symbolic aspects of Neandertal foraging is sparse and represented by a small number of perforated animal teeth and some modified bone in late MP or controversial transitional assemblages (Burke and d’Errico, 2008; d’Errico, et al., 2009; Mellars, et al., 2007; Zilhao, et al., 2007).

The similarities and differences between Neandertal and AMH foraging patterns necessitates careful consideration, as the zooarchaeological assemblages suggest that Neandertals and AMH both potentially foraged in a similar fashion. However, the absence of a substantial symbolic component associated with the Neandertal assemblages suggests some cognitive differences may have been present.

Table 2.1. The major archaeological differences between the UP and MP (Mellars, 1996, 2005; Klein, 2009). Note that symbolic artefacts include person, portable and parietal art

Upper Palaeolithic Middle Palaeolithic Blade-focused industries Flake-focused industries Complex spatial patterning Simple spatial patterning Symbolic artefacts common Symbolic artefacts rare Broad spectrum subsistence strategies

Narrowly-focused subsistence strategies (focus on medium to large herbivores)

Elaborate burials Simple burials

Benjamin Collins10

AMH and Neandertal biologyThere are several physiological characteristics that are argued to differ between AMH and Neandertals. Most important for this discussion are developmental growth rates and skull morphology.

Developmental growth rates are thought to have differed substantially between Neandertals and AMH and potentially reflect different life cycles, social structures and group mobilities (Bird and O’Connell, 2006; Conroy, 2005; O’Connell, et al., 1999; Pettit, 2000). However, Ponce de Leon and colleagues (2008) have recently argued that despite Neandertal brains being larger than those of AMH, both finish growing at around the same age. The authors suggest that Neandertals had similarly slow or possibly even slower life histories. An interesting caveat to this study is the contention that the smaller AMH brain was re-organised to become more efficient, possibly resulting in a competitive advantage over the larger and more costly Neandertal brain (Ponce de Leon, et al., 2008, p. 13767).

Neandertals and AMH exhibit significant differences in the external shapes of their skulls, with Neandertals generally demonstrating greater robusticity, especially in the brow ridge and occipital regions. This results in a platycephalic, or long and low-shaped Neandertal cranial vault, in contrast to the globular-shaped vault characteristic of AMH. AMH also generally display more gracile cranial features and a pronounced chin (Conroy, 2005; Stringer and Gamble, 1993; Schwartz and Tattersall, 2000; Trinkaus, 2007).

Differences in the external shape of the brain case have been used to infer variation in internal brain morphology, particularly in the frontal cortex and occipital regions (Wynn and Coolidge, 2004). The frontal lobes and frontal portion of the brain are argued to play a significant role in speech and language production, which is an important condition for AMH cognition (Amanti and Shallice, 2007; Davies and Underdown, 2006; d’Errico, et al., 2009). Similarly, the temporal-parietal area differs in both hominins and is also argued to play a major role in the ability to symbolise and for abstract thought (Henshilwood and Dubreuil, 2011; Wynn et al., 2009). If different brain morphologies existed and had functional consequences, Neandertal communication and cognitive abilities may have been, at the very least, qualitatively different from AMH.

Weaver and colleagues (2007) have reached an interesting conclusion in their comparison of Neandertal and AMH cranial differences. The authors sought to determine whether the external morphological variation present in AMH and Neandertal crania was the result of genetic drift or natural selection. Weaver and colleagues (2007) concluded that they could not reject their null hypothesis: morphological differences between AMH and Neandertal crania were the result of genetic drift and not natural selection. However,

there are two caveats to this study: it tested for natural selection’s role in producing variation and not maintaining the same general shape and functional morphology; and it focused on the external morphology only, and not the internal morphology, which has a greater bearing on cognition.

Roseman and colleagues (2011) have also compared human and Neandertal cranial morphology. In this study, the authors compared the patterns of cranial integration, or the covariance of specific cranial traits, between W.W. Howell’s collection of AMH cranial data and data from a set of 20 Neandertal skulls. Using these data, the authors demonstrated that AMH and Neandertals share a similar pattern of cranial integration for roughly three-quarters of the 37 traits studied and differed significantly for several others, specifically those related to the frontal and occipital bones. The authors conclude that their results indicate that the two hominins reacted differently to evolutionary forces (Roseman, et al., 2011).

Davies and Underdown (2006, p. 149), however, have noted issues with ‘paleo-phrenological’ approaches, as external differences in cranial morphology have not been shown to elicit significant cognitive differences amongst AMH. Two studies directly support this argument, with Bookstein and colleagues (1999) and Semendeferi and Damasio (2000) both demonstrating that despite significant differences in external cranial morphology amongst several hominoid species, frontal lobe morphology has remained relatively stable over time. This conclusion has been supported by other palaeo-neurological research that has also failed to detect significant differences between the neural development of Neandertals and AMH (Holloway, 1995; Semendeferi, 2001).

However, recent research by Bruner (2010), has demon-strated that the parietal portions of the skull and the parietal lobes of the brain in AMH are distinct from other hominins. AMH generally have larger parietals enabling parietal lobes that that are larger and more integrated with other parts of the brain. Bruner (2010) notes that the parietal lobes are involved with the interpretation of external spatial environments and its integration with inner perception, for example internal mental images, imagined worlds, thought experiments and abstract representations. These are all associated with enhanced working memory (Wynn and Coolidge, 2004; and see below) and modern cognition (Henshilwood and Dubreuil, 2011), and may represent different modes of cognition between AMH and Neandertals.

Modelling Neandertal and AMH cognitionWithin the past decade, Enhanced Working Memory (EWM) has been a very influential concept for describing the evolution of AMH cognition (Wynn and Coolidge, 2004). Wynn and Coolidge (2004) in particular have developed a

2. Can anatomically-modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns? 11

model that uses both archaeological research and cognitive neuropsychology to posit an understanding of AMH and past hominin cognitions (Davidson, 2010).

Wynn and Coolidge (2004; Coolidge and Wynn, 2005) use the concept of working memory to propose a model that differentiates between AMH and Neandertal cognitions (Baddeley, 2001; Baddeley and Logie, 1999). Working memory is described as a tripartite cognitive system consisting of a phonological storage system, related to speech information; a visual-spatial sketchpad, relating to visual and spatial information; and a central executive that is responsible for maintaining attention and decision-making, and is located in the frontal lobes (Wynn and Coolidge, 2004, p. 469).

The authors propose that differences in working memory can be used to distinguish between Neandertal and AMH cognitions and that these differences are reflected in the archaeological record. To briefly summarise their model, Wynn and Coolidge (2004) suggest that a genetic mutation in AMH approximately 50 kya created an ‘enhanced’ working memory capacity and modern cognitive abilities, a position also advanced by Klein (1995, 2001, 2008, 2009). Enhanced working memory (EWM) is reflected by an increase in the capacities of the central executive and an expanded phonological storage system. Together, these enhancements are argued to have resulted in greater native and fluid intelligences, better attention spans and the development of the subjunctive, or ‘what if?’ mode of speech. Wynn and Coolidge (2004; Coolidge and Wynn, 2005) further argue that this would have facilitated thought experiments, increased innovative potential, and created introspection, all of which are strongly related to increased cultural complexity and symbolic expression (see Rossano (2009) for a discussion of EWM’s role in symbolic ritual).

Introspection and self-reflection are argued to be particularly important and uniquely human, as in conjunction with the subjunctive mode of speech, they establish the conditions for reflecting on past experiences and extending them to the future. Wynn and Coolidge (2004) argue that AMH cognition is thereby reflected in the establishment of highly abstract, symbolic culture, illustrated by the UP in Europe and Later Stone Age in Africa. And that Neandertals, while demonstrating complex and very similar cognitions to AMH, lacked EWM and were therefore limited in their innovative and symbolic capacities.

Much of the EWM model has been constructed using studies of individuals with frontal lobe damage, as this is the suggested site of the central executive portion of working memory. Wynn and Coolidge (2004; Coolidge and Wynn, 2007) are careful to note criticisms that brain-damaged AMH are not the best models for Neandertals (Beaman, 2007). However, they contend that the research on impaired frontal lobe neural structures, in combination with the archaeology, provides a heuristic window into the Neandertal mind.

Other recent models of Neandertal-AMH cognition largely draw upon the EWM concept as discussed above (Amati and Shallice, 2007; Barnard, et al., 2007; Cole, 2009; Davidson, 2010; Henshilwood and Dubreuil, 2011; Rossano, 2009). These models all form the same conclusion: AMH display more complex cognitive abilities, reflected by a fluid intelligence that is ascribed to an internal dialogue. This dialogue facilitates complex reflection on the past and future, and provides the foundation for an enhanced innovative capacity (see also Mithen, 1996).

The archaeology, zooarchaeology and cognitive models have all pointed to both broad similarities and particular differences in Neandertal and AMH cognition. These differences are posited to be in the AMH capacity for abstract thought and internal reflection manifested through symbolic expression. However this position has been contested with the presence of some symbolic artefacts associated with Neandertal occupations (d’Errico, 2003; d’Errico and Vanhaeren, 2007; Zilhao, 2006; Zilhao et al., 2010), as well as the genetic closeness of Neandertals to AMH (Green, et al., 2010).

While Neandertal expressions of symbolism are not as ubiquitous as those associated with UP AMH, some similarities arise when they are compared to contemporaneous AMH from the African MSA. The MSA and MP display similarities in lithic technology, as well as with the paucity of symbolic materials. On this basis, Neandertals and MSA AMH were thought to be cognitive ‘equals’, with AMH cognition and behaviour only appearing with the transition to the Later Stone Age around 40 kya to 50 kya (Klein, 2001; Mellars, 2006; Willoughby, 2007).

However, recent excavations focusing on the African MSA have provided substantial archaeological material that can be related to symbolism and complex cognition. McBrearty and Brooks (2000) provided a summary of some of these materials over a decade ago and demonstrated how the African MSA’s material culture argued for the presence of modern cognition. More recently, excavations from Blombos Cave (Henshilwood, et al., 2002, 2009), Pinnacle Point (Marean, 2010; Marean, et al., 2007) and Sibudu Cave (Wadley, 2007, 2010a, 2010b; Wadley and Plug, 2006; Wadley, et al., 2009) have all demonstrated archaeological remains that are used to argue for complex symbolic (modern) behaviour (see Table 2). Furthermore, the Still Bay (c.73 kya to c.70 kya) and Howieson’s Poort (c. 65 kya to c. 59 kya) lithic industries are argued to bear strong resemblance to those from the LSA and UP (Jacobs, et al., 2008a, 2008b). These similarities include styles that span large geographical regions and are temporally limited; and the frequent occurrence of microliths and backed tools in the Howieson’s Poort (Wadley, 2007; Lombard, 2011; Villa, et al., 2009; Wurz, 1999). Both industries are associated with symbolic expressions and are preceded and followed by hiatuses.

Benjamin Collins12

The saltational occurrences of symbolic behaviour during the MSA have recently been characterised as demographic pulses, with larger populations consisting of well-connected sub-populations capable of information sharing displaying more complex behaviour, followed by smaller, less well-connected populations displaying less complex behaviour. Both the greater population size and the ability to share information are argued to facilitate novel and more complex cultural innovations to be acquired, spread and maintained (Powell, et al., 2009; Richerson, et al., 2009). The much used example of the decrease in complexity of the lithic technology used by the Tasmanian Aborigines fits well with this argument (Henrich, 2004).

Taking into account the hypothesised low population densities for Neandertals during the MP, may explain why symbolic expression is rare and patchy during this period and increases substantially in the archaeological record with the transition to the UP, which is associated with the arrival of AMH and increased demographic pressure (Adler, et al., 2006; d’Errico, 2003; d’Errico, et al., 2003; Hockett and Haws, 2005; Zilhao, 2006). d’Errico and Stringer (2011) have recently contested that the patchy appearance of complex, ‘modern’, behaviours during the MP may be linked to climatic and environmental variation. In this regard, symbolic or ‘modern’ behaviours appear as adaptations to eco-cultural niches, which contract and expand through time based on demographic and environmental change.

A complicated picture arises in trying to understand Neandertal cognition and behaviour. What can be said is that despite differences reflected in the archaeological record, Neandertals are not, to put it bluntly, stupid and must have been quite similar to AMH in order for interbreeding to occur (Green, et al., 2010). Potential differences in cognition should then be considered to reflect cultural variation, rather than different levels of cognitive complexity. This conclusion merits further consideration for the extent to which frameworks and models used for understanding AMH foraging behaviours can be extended to Neandertals.

Interpreting Neandertal behaviourAnalogical reasoning is fundamental for the construction of archaeological models and interpretive frameworks, whether explicitly acknowledged or not (Wylie, 1985, 2002). Ethnoarchaeology contributes insight into past behaviours by studying modern peoples and the use of analogical extension, either directly (formal analogic reasoning), or more commonly indirectly (relational analogic reasoning) (David and Kramer, 2001). Through combining ethnoarchaeological studies with explanatory frameworks from evolutionary theory, behavioural ecology can be used to construct models for interpreting past subsistence practices, specifically those that have arisen from optimal foraging theory (Bird and O’Connell, 2006; Lupo, 2007; Kelly, 2007; Winterhalder and Smith, 2000). These models were developed in the biological sciences, and have been borrowed to interpret human behaviour.

Optimality models are, however, fundamentally reductionist and, as such, necessarily limit the number of variables that are taken into account. This approach often yields false hypotheses (see Sih and Christensen (2001) for a review), which can act as heuristics for further research. Archaeologists have therefore used ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological research to refine these optimality models and make them more applicable to understanding both present and past hunter-gatherers’ foraging decisions (for example: Broughton, 2002; Faith, 2008; Grayson and Delpech, 1998, 2003; Morin, 2008; Nagaoka, 2002, 2005). In this regard, can optimal foraging models refined using ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH be used to infer Neandertal subsistence strategies?

This question depends strongly on two factors: the extent to which AMH and Neandertals are cognitively similar and the extent to which the subsistence behaviours being studied can be related to differences in cognition. The above comparison of Neandertal and AMH cognitions, based on the biological and archaeological information, as well as the proposed cognitive models has elicited a complicated picture.

Table 2.2. Recent Middle Stone Age finds that demonstrate symbolism and complex cognition

Artefact How it demonstrates symbolic thought References Engraved ochre and ostrich eggshell

Intentionally engraved abstract design that has no functional purpose. Possibly art.

Henshilwood et al., 2002, 2009; MacKay and Welz, 2008; Texier, et al., 2010

Shell beads No functional purpose. Used as pendants for displaying personal and group identity.

Henshilwood, et al., 2004; d'Errico, et al., 2005, 2008

Compound tools Multi-component, compound tools require complex mental preparation and planning.

Wadley, 2010b; Haidle, 2010; Reuland, 2010

Trapping and snaring Requires ability to make traps and snares, and long-term planning and scheduling to place them correctly and remember to check them

Wadley, 2010a; Clark and Plug, 2008

Ochre use Ochre could be used as adhesives for multi-component tools, and for symbolic painting of body and objects

Watts, 2010; Wadley, et al., 2009;

Marine resource use Requires specialised technology and long-term scheduling Marean, 2010; Marean, et al., 2007; Henshilwood, et al., 2002

2. Can anatomically-modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns? 13

Genetics, parts of the endocranial skull morphology and the faunal assemblages indicate a substantial degree of cognitive similarity between the two hominins. However, substantial differences are found in material symbolic expressions, ubiquitously associated with UP AMH, but only sparsely so with Neandertals. While this difference may be related to differences in demographic intensity (d’Errico and Stringer, 2011), it has also led to models proposing that Neandertals lacked complex, abstract thoughts and internal dialogues (Barnard, et al., 2007; Wynn and Coolidge, 2004). Cognitive models and the archaeology therefore necessitate a consideration of the validity for extending AMH foraging behaviours to Neandertals.

Taking the above into account requires that any attempt to infer more cultural aspects of Neandertal subsistence must necessarily be met with caution. If Neandertals are characterised by a different cognitive system (which appears likely, although these differences may be more akin to cultural variation than cognitive complexity), then they have no modern analogue (Barnard, et al., 2007). This weakens any attempt to infer the social and cultural contexts surrounding Neandertal foraging behaviours, especially since such contexts have been strongly linked ethnoarchaeologically to AMH forager decision making (Egeland and Byerly, 2005; Lupo, 2006; 2007). However, behavioural ecology and optimal foraging models can be used to infer the less cultural aspects of Neandertal foraging behaviours.

Behavioural ecology and optimal foraging models employ an evolutionary framework that focuses on fundamental behavioural patterning conditioned by natural selection (Bird and O’Connell, 2006; Winterhalder and Smith, 2000). Optimal foraging models, such as the diet-breadth model, patch-choice model and central-place foraging model (see Kelly, 2007, pp. 73–110), used in a reflexive manner can be used to establish a link between AMH and Neandertal behaviour, especially considering these models’ reductionist tendencies. This reductionism is what gives optimal foraging models their analytical power (Bettinger, 2009), as they do not incorporate the symbolic aspects of foraging, which differ immensely between different cultures and more than likely between Neandertals and AMH (Kelly, 2007; Ingold, 2000).

Dusseldorp (2012) has recently provided a detailed study of several Neandertal faunal assemblages from Europe. This study used optimal foraging models, specifically the diet-breadth model to assess the relationship between climate change and changes in Neandertal foraging strategies in northern European steppe and forest environments. Through analysing the species that were hunted and their relative abundances, Dusseldorp was able to demonstrate that solitary species were focused upon during the warmer periods, while gregarious species were focused on during colder periods.

Dusseldorp (2012) uses ethnographic and ethnoarch-aeological information to explain these foraging strategies. Modern hunter-gatherers will tend to aggregate and increase group size when large herds of animals are present, and disperse and decrease group size when they are not (Kelly, 2007). These strategies are related to the amount of animal biomass present in the landscape with the presence of large gregarious animals increasing the animal biomass and therefore being able to provide more food for more people.

Using the ethnographic information to interpret the archaeological record, it can be inferred that Neandertals were engaging in similar foraging strategies as AMH. Neandertals specifically exploited large herd animals when they were abundant during colder and more open conditions. To efficiently exploit these species requires large group sizes, which is reflected in the archaeology from these sites.

When the climate and environment changed to warmer and more forested conditions, herd animals were not ranked as highly and solitary species were more intensively exploited. This change in foraging strategy is linked to a decrease in group size as the available animal biomass decreased and could not support large groups.

Dusseldorp’s (2012) study demonstrates how aspects of the ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological records can be used to infer some Neandertal behaviours, such as the relationship between foraging strategies and group size. However, these records cannot be extended to infer the social and cultural changes that accompanied these behavioural shifts and further reinforces the limits to which AMH behaviours can be used to understand those of other hominins.

ConclusionWylie (1985, 2002) and others contend that strong relational analogies are constructed using multiple cables or arguments that all point to the same conclusion (Dominguez-Rodrigo, 2008; Gifford-Gonzalez, 1991; Stahl, 1993). An interpretive framework using behavioural ecology and optimal foraging models supports this contention, as it is based on fundamental behavioural similarities patterned by natural selection (Bettinger, 2009; Bird and O’Connell, 2006). This framework is strengthened by the addition of ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological studies of AMH foraging patterns, and environmental and other contextual information.

The crucial issue considered by this paper is whether these models, refined with ethnoarchaeological data from AMH and used for AMH, are applicable to Neandertals, who are considered to be equipped with a unique cognitive structure. As optimal foraging models largely focus on basic, evolutionary-patterned foraging behaviours, and Neandertal zooarchaeological assemblages closely resemble those of AMH (Adler, et al., 2006; Grayson and Delpech, 2003; Morin, 2008), it is argued that they can be reliably extended

Benjamin Collins14

to interpret Neandertal faunal assemblages. This conclusion was furthered by discussing Dusseldorp’s (2012) research on the relationship between Neandertal group size, climate and environmental change and changes in foraging strategies.

The strength of the behavioural ecology interpretive framework is argued to weaken when ‘higher-order’ (Hawkes, 1954) cultural and social interpretations are attempted, stemming from possible differences between AMH and Neandertal cognitions. AMH demonstrate internal, reflexive dialogues that facilitate abstract, symbolic thoughts and behaviours. These complex cognitive abilities have been argued by some to be absent in Neandertals, thereby impeding the interpretative scope of their social and cultural behaviour (Barnard, et al., 2007; Klein, 2008; Wynn and Coolidge, 2004). This position however, is strongly contested (d’Errico, 2003; d’Errico and Vanhaeren, 2007; Zilhao, 2006).

Analogical reasoning, ethnoarchaeology, behavioural ecology and cognitive modelling have all been considered to establish the basis for and limits of an approach to inferring Neandertal foraging behaviours from faunal assemblages. The above do not consist of the only aspects contributing to a sound interpretation, with taphonomy, palaeoecology, the palaeoenvironment and the archaeology all being extremely important. These data and other potential sources of information should be continually considered, as it will facilitate a better understanding of Neandertal behaviour and the development of hominin cognition.

AcknowledgementsI would like to thank Lee Broderick, Dr Steven Churchill, Dr Lyn Wadley, Dr Manuel Dominguez-Rodrigo and an anonymous reviewer for their constructive comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, as well as comments from the Archaeology Graduate Students Writing Group from McGill University. I would also like to acknowledge financial support from a Social Sciences and Humanities Research (SSHRC) Doctoral Fellowship and the Hominid Dispersals Research Cluster.

BibliographyAdler, D., Bar-Oz, G., Belfer-Cohen, A. and Bar-Yosef, O., 2006.

Ahead of the Game: Middle and Upper Palaeolithic Hunting Behaviours in the Southern Caucasus. Current Anthropology 47, 89–118. doi:10.1086/432455

Amati, D. and Shallice, T., 2007. On the Emergence of AMH. Cognition 103, 358–385.

Baddeley, A., 2001. Is Working Memory still Working? American Psychologist 11, 851–864.

Baddeley, A. and Logie, R., 1999. Working Memory: The Multi-Component Model, in: Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (eds) Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and

Executive Control, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 28–61.

Barnard, P., Duke, D., Byrne, R. and Davidson, I., 2007. Differentiation in Cognitive and Emotional Meanings: An Evolutionary Analysis. Cognition and Emotion 21, 1155–1183. doi:10.1080/02699930701437477

Bar-Yosef, O., 2004. Eat What is There: Hunting and Gathering in the World of Neanderthals and their Neighbours. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 14, 333–342. doi:10.1002/oa.765

Bar-Yosef, O. and Kuhn, S., 1999. The Big Deal about Blades: Laminar Technologies and Human Evolution. American Anthropologist 101, 322–338. doi:10.1525/aa.1999.101.2.322

Beaman, C., 2007. Modern Cognition in the Absence of Working Memory: Does the Working Memory Account of Neandertal Cognition Work? Journal of Human Evolution 52, 702–706.

Bettinger, R., 2009. Hunter-Gatherer Foraging: Five Simple Models. Eliot Clinton Publications, New York.

Binford, L., 1978. Nunamiut Ethnoarchaeology. Academic Press, New York.

Bird, D. and O’Connell, J., 2006. Behavioural Ecology and Archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Research 14, 143–188. doi:10.1007/s10814-006-9003-6

Bird-David, N., 1999. “Animism” Revisited: Personhood, Environment, and Relational Epistemology. Current Anthropology 40, S67–S91. doi:10.1086/200061

Blasco, R., 2008. Human Consumption of Tortoises at Level IV of Bolomor Cave (Valencia, Spain). Journal of Archaeological Science 35, 2839–2848.

Blasco, R. and Fernandez Peris, J., 2009. Middle Pleistocene bird consumption at Level XI of Bolomor Cave (Valencia, Spain). Journal of Archaeological Science 36, 2213–2223. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2008.05.013

Bookstein, F., Schäfer, K., Prossinger, H., Seidler, H., Fieder, M., Stringer, C., Weber, G., Arsuaga, J.-L., Slice, D., Rohlf, F., Recheis, W., Mariam, A. and Marcus, L., 1999. Comparing Frontal and Cranial Profiles in Archaic and Modern Homo using Morphometric Analysis. Anatomical Record (New Anatomist) 257, 217–224. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0185(19991215)257:6<217::AID-AR7>3.0.CO;2-W

Brightman, R., 2002. Grateful Prey: Rock Cree Human-Animal Relationships, Canadian Plains Research Center, University of Saskatchewan, Regina.

Broughton, J., 2002. Prey Spatial Structure and Behaviour Affect Archaeological Tests of Optimal Foraging Models: Examples from the Emeryville Shellmound Vertebrate Fauna. World Archaeology 34, 60–83. doi:10.1080/00438240220134269

Bruner, E., 2010. Morphological Differences within the Parietal Lobes of the Human Genus: A Neurofunctional Perspective. Current Anthropology 51, S77–S88. doi:10.1086/650729

Burke, A. and D’Errico, F., 2008. A Middle Palaeolithic Bone Tool from Crimea (Ukraine). Antiquity 82, 843–852. doi:10.1017/S0003598X00097611

Clark, J. and Plug, I., 2008. Animal Exploitation Strategies during the South African Middle Stone Age: Howiesons Poort and Post-Howiesons Poort Fauna from Sibudu Cave. Journal of Human Evolution 54, 886–898. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.12.004

Cole, C., 2009. People Transforming Information – Information

2. Can anatomically-modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns? 15

Transforming People: What Neanderthals can Teach Us. Proceedings of the Annual Society for Information Sciences and Technology 45, 1–10. doi:10.1002/meet.2008.1450450201

Conroy, G., 2005. Reconstructing Human Origins, 2nd ed., W.W. Norton, New York.

Coolidge, F. and Wynn, T., 2005. Working Memory, its Executive Function, and the Emergence of AMH Thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 15, 5–26. doi:10.1017/S0959774305000016

Coolidge, F. and Wynn, T., 2007. The Working Memory Account of Neandertal Cognition – How Phonological Storage Capacity may be Related to Recursion and the Pragmatics of Modern Speech. Journal of Human Evolution 52, 707–710. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.01.003

David, N. and Kramer, C., 2001. Ethnoarchaeology in Action, Cambridge University Press, New York.

Davidson, I., 2010. The Archaeology of Cognitive Evolution. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Sciences 1, 214–229. doi:10.1002/wcs.40

Davies, R. and Underdown, S., 2006. The Neandertals: A Social Synthesis. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 16, 145–164. doi:10.1017/S0959774306000096

d’Errico, F., 2003. The Invisible Frontier: A Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioral Modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology 12, 188–202. doi:10.1002/evan.10113

d’Errico, F. and Hombert, J.-M., 2009. Becoming Eloquent: Advances in the Emergence of Language, Human Cognition, and Modern Cultures. John Benjamins, Philadephia PA.

d’Errico, F. and Stringer, C., 2011. Evolution, Revolution or Saltation Scenario for the Emergence of Modern Cultures?. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences 366, 1060–1069. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0340

d’Errico, F. and Vanhaeren, M., 2007. Evolution or Revolution? New Evidence for the Origin of Symbolic Behaviour in and out of Africa, in: Mellars, P., Boyle, K., Bar-Yosef, O. and Stringer, C. (eds) Rethinking the Human Revolution, MacDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Cambridge, pp. 275–286.

d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A.-M., Soressi, M., Bresson, F., Maureille, M., Nowell, A., Lakarra, J., Backwell, L. and Julien, M., 2003. Archaeological Evidence for the Emergence of Language, Symbolism, and Music – An Alternative Multidisciplinary Perspective. Journal of World Prehistory 17, 1–70. doi:10.1023/A:1023980201043

d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M. and van Niekerk, K., 2005. Nassarius kraussianus Shell Beads from Blombos Cave: Evidence for Symbolic Behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution 48, 3–24. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.002

Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., 2008. Conceptual Premises in Experi-mental Design and their Bearing on the use of Analogy: An Example from Experiments on Cut Marks. World Archaeology 40, 67–82. doi:10.1080/00438240701843629

Dusseldorp, G., 2012. Studying Prehistoric Hunting Proficiency: Applying Optimal Foraging Theory to the Middle Palaeolithic and Middle Stone Age. Quaternary International 252, 3–15. doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2011.04.024

Egeland, C. and Byerly, R., 2005. Application of Return Rates to Large Mammal Butchery and Transport Among Hunter-

Gatherers and its Implications for Plio-Pleistocene Hominid Carcass Foraging and Site Use. Journal of Taphonomy 3, 135–157.

Faith, J., 2008. Eland, Buffalo, and wild Pigs: Were Middle Stone Age Humans Ineffective Hunters? Journal of Human Evolution 55, 24–36. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.11.005

Gifford-Gonzalez, D., 1991. Bones are not Enough: Analogues, Knowledge and Interpretive Strategies in Zooarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 10, 215–254. doi:10.1016/0278-4165(91)90014-O

Gould, R., 1978. Beyond Analogy in Ethnoarchaeology, in: Gould, R. (ed.) Explorations in Ethnoarchaeology, University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque NM, pp. 249–293.

Gould, R., 1980. Living Archaeology, Cambridge University Press, New York.

Grayson, D. and Delpech, F., 1998. Changing Diet Breadth in the Early Upper Palaeolithic of Southwestern France. Journal of Archaeological Science 25, 1119–1129. doi:10.1006/jasc.1998.0339

Grayson, D. and Delpech, F., 2003. Ungulates and the Middle-to-Upper Palaeolithic Transition at Grotte XVI (Dordogne, France). Journal of Archaeological Science 30, 1633–1648. doi:10.1016/S0305-4403(03)00064-5

Green, R., Krause, J., Ptak, S., Briggs, A., Ronan, M., Simons, J., Du, L., Egholm, M., Rothberg, J., Paunovic, M. and Pääbo, S., 2006. Analysis of One Million Base Pairs of Neanderthal DNA. Nature 444, 330–336. doi:10.1038/nature05336

Green, R., Krause, J., Briggs, A., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., Patterson, N., Li, H., Zhai, W., Fritz, M., Hansen, N., Durand, E., Malaspinas, A., Jensen, J., Marques-Bonet, T., Alkan, C., Prüfer, K., Meyer, M., Burbano, H., Good, J., Schultz, R., Aximu-Petri, A., Butthof, A., Höber, B., Höffner, B., Siegemund, M., Weihmann, A., Nusbaum, C., Lander, E., Russ, C., Novod, N., Affourtit, J., Egholm, M., Verna, C., Rudan, P., Brajkovic, D., Kucan, Z., Gušic, I., Doronichev, V., Golovanova, L., Lalueza-Fox, C., de la Rasilla, M., Fortea, J., Rosas, A., Schmitz, R., Johnson, P., Eichler, E., Falush, D., Birney, E., Mullikin, J., Slatkin, M., Nielsen, R., Kelso, J., Lachmann, M., Reich, D. and Pääbo, S., 2010. A Draft Sequence of the Neandertal Genome. Science 328, 710–722. doi:10.1126/science.1188021

Haidle, M., 2010. Working-Memory Capacity and the Evolution of Modern Cognitive Potential: Implications from Animal and Early Human Tool Use. Current Anthropology 51, S149–S166. doi:10.1086/650295

Harvarti, K., Frost, S. and McNulty, K., 2004. Neanderthal Taxonomy Reconsidered: Implications of 3D Primate Models of Intra- and Interspecific Differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101, 1147–1152. doi:10.1073/pnas.0308085100

Hawkes, C., 1954. Archaeological Theory and Method: Some Suggestions from the Old World. American Anthropologist 56, 155–168. doi:10.1525/aa.1954.56.2.02a00660

Henrich, J., 2004. Demography and Cultural Evolution: How Adaptive Cultural Processes can Produce Maladaptive Losses: The Tasmanian Case. American Antiquity 69, 197–214. doi:10.2307/4128416

Henry, A., Brooks, A. and Piperno, D., 2011. Microfossils in

Benjamin Collins16

Calculus Demonstrate the Consumption of Plants and Cooked Foods in Neanderthal Diets (Shanidar III, Iraq, Spy I and II, Belgium). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, 486–491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016868108

Henshilwood, C. and Dubreuil, B., 2011. The Still Bay and Howieson’s Poort, 77–59 ka: Symbolic Material Culture and the Evolution of the Human Mind During the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology 52, 361–400. doi:10.1086/660022

Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F. and Watts, I., 2009. Engraved Ochres from the Middle Stone Age Levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 57, 27–47. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.01.005

Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K. and Jacobs, Z., 2004. Middle Stone Age Shell Beads from South Africa. Science 304, 404. doi:10.1126/science.1095905

Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G., Mercier, N., Sealy, J., Valladas, H., Watts, I. and Wintle, A., 2002. Emergence of AMH Behaviour: Middle Stone Age Engravings from South Africa. Science 295, 1278–1280. doi:10.1126/science.1067575

Hockett, B. and Haws, J., 2005. Nutritional Ecology and the Human Demography of Neandertal Extinction. Quaternary International 137, 21–34. doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2004.11.017

Holloway, R., 1995. Toward a Synthetic Theory of Brain Evolution, in: Changeux, J.-P. and Chavaillon, J. (eds) The Origin of the Human Brain, Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp. 42–60.

Hublin, J.-J., Weston, D., Gunz, P., Richards, M., Roebroeks, W., Glimmerveen, J. and Anthonis, L., 2009. Out of the North Sea: The Zeeland Ridges Neandertal. Journal of Human Evolution 57, 777–785. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.09.001

Hudson, J., 1993. From Bones to Behaviour: Ethnoarchaeological and Experimental Contributions to the Interpretation of Faunal Remains. Center for Archaeological Investigations, Carbondale IL.

Ingold, T., 2000. The Perception of the Environment. Routledge, New York.

Jacobs, Z., Wintle, A., Duller, G., Robert, R. and Wadley, L., 2008. New Ages for the Post-Howiesons Poort, Late and Final Middle Stone Age at Sibudu Cave, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science 35, 1790–1807. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.028

Jacobs, Z., Robert, R., Galbraith, R., Deacon, H., Grün, R., Mackay, A., Mitchell, P., Vogelsang, R. and Wadley, L., 2008. Ages for the Middle Stone Age of Southern Africa: Implications for Human Behaviour and Dispersal. Science 322, 733–735. doi:10.1126/science.1162219

Kelly, R., 2007. The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter-Gatherer Lifeways. Percheron Press, New York.

Klein, R., 1995. Anatomy, Behaviour, and Modern Human Origins. Journal of World Prehistory 9, 167–198. doi:10.1007/BF02221838

Klein, R., 2001. Southern Africa and Modern Human Origins. Journal of Anthropological Research 57, 1–16.

Klein, R., 2008. Out of Africa and the Evolution of Human Behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 17, 267–281. doi:10.1002/evan.20181

Klein, R., 2009. Darwin and the Recent African Origin of Modern Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 16007–16009. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908719106

Krause, J., Lalueza-Fox, C., Orlando, L., Enard, W., Green, R., Burbano, H., Hublin, J.-J., Hänni, C., Fortea, J., de la Rasilla, J., Bertranpetit, J., Rosas, A. and Pääbo, S., 2007. The Derived FOXP2 Variant of Modern Humans was Shared with Neandertals. Current Biology 17, 1908–1912. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008

Lev, E., Kislev, M. and Bar-Yosef, O., 2005. Mousterian Vegetal Food in Kebara Cave, Mt. Carmel. Journal of Archaeological Science 32, 475–484. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2004.11.006

Lombard, M., 2011. Quartz-Tipped Arrows Older than 60 ka: Further Use-Trace Evidence from Sibudu, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science 38, 1918–1930. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2011.04.001

Lupo, K., 2006. What Explains the Carcass Field Processing and Transport Decisions of Modern Hunter-Gatherers? Measures of Economic Anatomy and Zooarchaeological Skeletal Part Representation. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 13, 19–66. doi:10.1007/s10816-006-9000-6

Lupo, K., 2007. Evolutionary Foraging Models in Zooarch-aeological Analysis: Recent Applications and Future Challenges. Journal of Archaeological Research 15, 143–189. doi:10.1007/s10814-007-9011-1

Mackay, A. and Welz, A., 2008. Engraved Ochre from a Middle Stone Age Context at Klein Kliphuis in the Western Cape of South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science 35, 1521–1532. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.10.015

Madella, M., Jones, M., Goldberg, P., Goren, Y. and Hovers, E., 2002. The Exploitation of Plant Resources by Neanderthals in Amud Cave (Israel): The Evidence from Phytolith Studies. Journal of Archaeological Science 29, 703–719. doi:10.1006/jasc.2001.0743

Marean, C., 2010. Pinnacle Point Cave 13B (Western Cape Province, South Africa) in Context: The Cape Floral Kingdom, Shellfish, and Modern Human Origins. Journal of Human Evolution 59, 425–443. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.07.011

Marean, C., Bar-Matthews, M., Bernatchez, J., Fisher, E., Goldberg, P., Herries, A., Jacobs, Z., Jerardino, A., Karkanas, P., Minichillo, T., Nilssen, P., Thompson, E., Watts, I. and Williams, H., 2007. Early Human use of Marine Resources and Pigment South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature 449, 905–909. doi:10.1038/nature06204

McBrearty, S., and Brooks, A., 2000. The Revolution that wasn’t: a New Interpretation of the Origin of Modern Human Behaviour. Journal of Human Evolution 39, 453–563. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0435

Mellars, P., 1996. The Neandertal Legacy, Princeton University Press, Princeton.

Mellars, P., 2005. The Impossible Coincidence. A Single-Species Model for the Origins of Modern Human Behaviour in Europe. Evolutionary Anthropology 14, 12–27. doi:10.1002/evan.20037

Mellars, P., 2006. Why did Modern Human Populations Disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 Years Ago? A New Model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103, 9381–9386. doi:10.1073/pnas.0510792103

Mellars, P., Gravina, B. and Ramsey, C., 2007. Confirmation of Neanderthal/Modern Human Interstratification at the Chatelperronian Type-Site. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, 3657–3662. doi:10.1073/pnas.0608053104

Mithen, S., 1996. The Prehistory of the Mind: A Search for the

2. Can anatomically-modern humans be used as analogues for Neandertal foraging patterns? 17

Origins of Art, Religion, and Science. Thames and Hudson, London.

Morin, E., 2008. Evidence for Declines in Human Population Densities during the Early Upper Paleolithic in Western Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105, 48–53. doi:10.1073/pnas.0709372104

Nagaoka, L., 2002. The Effects of Resource Depression on Foraging Efficiency, Diet Breadth and Patch Use in Southern New Zealand. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21, 419–442. doi:10.1016/S0278-4165(02)00008-9

Nagaoka, L., 2005. Declining Foraging Efficiency and Moa Carcass Exploitation in Southern New Zealand. Journal of Archaeological Science 32, 1328–1338. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2005.04.004

Noonan, J., Coop, G., Kudaravalli, S., Smith, D., Krause, J., Alessi, J., Cheng, F., Platt, D., Pääbo, S., Pritchard, J. and Rubin, E., 2006. Sequencing and Analysis of Neanderthal Genomic DNA. Science 314, 1113–1118. doi:10.1126/science.1131412

O’Connell, J., Hawkes, K., Blurton Jones, N., 1999. Grand-mothering and the Evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution 36, 461–485. doi:10.1006/jhev.1998.0285

Patou-Mathis, M., 2000. Neandertal Subsistence Behaviours in Europe. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 10, 379–395. doi:10.1002/1099-1212(200009/10)10:5<379::AID-OA558>3.0.CO;2-4

Pettitt, P., 2000. Neanderthal Lifecycles: Development and Social Phases in the Lives of the Last Archaics. World Archaeology 31, 351–366. doi:10.1080/00438240009696926

Ponce de Leon, M., Golovanova, L., Doronichev, V., Romanova, G., Akazawa, T., Kondo, O., Ishida, H. and Zollikofer, C., 2008. Neanderthal Brain Size at Birth Provides Insight into Human Life History. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105, 13764–13768. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803917105

Powell, A., Shennan, S. and Thomas, M., 2009. Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behaviour. Science 324, 1298–1301. doi:10.1126/science.1170165

Reuland, E., 2010. Imagination, Planning, and Working Memory: The Emergence of Language. Current Anthropology 51, S99–S110. doi:10.1086/651260

Richards, M. and Trinkaus, E., 2009. Isotopic Evidence for the Diets of European Neanderthals and Early Modern Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 16034–16039. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903821106

Richards, M., Pettitt, P., Stiner, M. and Trinkaus, E., 2001. Stable Isotope Evidence for Increasing Dietary Breadth in the European mid-Upper Paleolithic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98, 6528–6532. doi:10.1073/pnas.111155298

Richerson, P., Boyd, R. and Bettinger, R., 2009. Cultural Inno-vations and Demographic Change. Human Biology 81, 211–235. doi:10.3378/027.081.0306

Roseman, C., Weaver, A. and Stringer, C., 2011. Do Modern Humans and Neandertals have Different Patterns of Cranial Integration? Journal of Human Evolution 60, 684–693. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.04.010

Rossano, M., 2009. Ritual Behaviour and the Origins of Modern Cognition. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 19, 243–256. doi:10.1017/S0959774309000298

Schwartz, J. and Tatersall, I., 2000. The Human Chin Revisited: What is it and Who has it? Journal of Human Evolution 38, 367–409. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0339

Semendeferi, K., 2001. Before or after the Split? Hominid Neural Specialisations, in: Nowell, A. (ed.) In the Mind’s Eye, Ann Arbor MI, pp. 107–120.

Semendeferi, K. and Damasio, H., 2000. The Brain and its Anatomical Subdivision using Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Journal of Human Evolution 38, 317–332. doi:10.1006/jhev.1999.0381

Sih, A. and Christensen, B., 2001. Optimal Diet Theory: When Does it Work, and When and Why does it Fail? Animal Behaviour 61, 379–390. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1592

Soressi, M., Jones, H., Rink, W., Maureille, B. and Tillier, A.-M., 2007. The Pech-de-l’Azé I Neandertal Child: ESR, Uranium Series and AMS 14C dating of its MTA Type B Context. Journal of Human Evolution 52, 455–466. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.11.006

Speth, J., 2004. News Flash: Negative Evidence Convicts Neanderthals of Gross Mental Incompetence. World Arch-aeology 36, 519–526. doi:10.1080/0043824042000303692

Stahl, A., 1993. Concepts of Time and Approaches to Analogical Reasoning in Historical Perspective. American Antiquity 58, 235–260. doi:10.2307/281967

Stiner, M. and Munro, N., 2002. Approaches to Prehistoric Diet Breadth, Demography, and Prey Ranking Systems in Time and Space. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 9, 181–214. doi:10.1023/A:1016530308865

Stringer, C. and Gamble, C., 1993. In Search of the Neanderthals: Solving the Puzzle of Modern Human Origins. Thames and Hudson, London.

Stringer, C., Finlayson, C., Barton, N., Fernandez-Jalvo, Y., Caceres, I., Sabin, R., Rhodes, E., Currant, A., Rodriguez-Vidal, J., Giles-Pacheco, F. and Riquelme-Cantal, J., 2008. Neanderthal Exploitation of Marine Mammals in Gibraltar. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805474105

Texier, J.-P., Porraz, G., Parkington, J., Riguaud, J.-P., Poggenpoel, C., Miller, C., Tribolo, C., Cartwright, C., Coudenneau, A., Klein, R., Steele, T. and Verna, C., 2010. A Howiesons Poort Tradition of Engraving Ostrich Eggshell Containers dated to 60,000 years ago at Diepkloof Rockshelter, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 6180–6185. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913047107

Trinkaus, E., 2007. European Early Modern Humans and the Fate of Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, 7367–7372. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702214104

Villa, P., Soressi, M., Henshilwood, C. and Mourre, V., 2009. The Still Bay Points of Blombos Cave (South Africa). Journal of Archaeological Science 36, 441–460. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2008.09.028

Wadley, L., 2007. Announcing a Still Bay Industry at Sibudu Cave. Journal of Human Evolution 52, 681–689. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.01.002

Wadley, L., 2010a. Were Snares and Traps used in the Middle Stone Age and does it matter? A Review and a Case Study from Sibudu, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 58, 179–192. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.10.004

Benjamin Collins18

Wadley, L., 2010b. Compound-Adhesive Manufacture as a Behavioural Proxy for Complex Cognition in the Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology 51, S111–S119. doi:10.1086/649836

Wadley, L. and Plug, I., 2006. Sibudu Cave: Background to the Excavations, Stratigraphy and Dating. South African Humanities 18, 1–26.

Wadley, L., Hodgskiss, T. and Grant, M., 2009. Implications for Complex Cognition from the Hafting of Tools with Compound Adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, 9590–9594. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900957106

Watts, I., 2010. The Pigments from Pinnacle Point Cave 13B, Western Cape, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 59, 392–411. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.07.006

Weaver, T., Roseman, C. and Stringer, C., 2007. Were Neandertal and Modern Human Cranial Differences produced by Natural Selection or Genetic Drift? Journal of Human Evolution 53, 135–145. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.03.001

Willerslev, R., 2007. Soul Hunters: Hunting, Animism, and Personhood among the Siberian Yukaghirs, University of California Press, Berkeley.

Willoughby, P., 2007. The Evolution of Modern Humans in Africa: A Comprehensive Guide, Altamira Press, Lanham.

Winterhalder, B. and Smith, E., 2000. Analyzing Adaptive Strategies: Human Behavioural Ecology at Twenty-Five. Evolutionary Anthropology 9, 51–72. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(2000)9:2<51::AID-EVAN1>3.0.CO;2-7

Wobst, H., 1978. The Archaeo-Ethnography of Hunter-Gatherers or the Tyranny of the Ethnographic Record in Archaeology. American Antiquity 43, 303–309. doi:10.2307/279256

Wolpoff, M., Mannheim, B., Mann, A., Hawks, J., Caspari,

R., Rosenberg, K., Frayer, D., Gill, G. and Clark, G., 2004. Why not the Neandertals? World Archaeology 36, 527–546. doi:10.1080/0043824042000303700

Wurz, S., 1999. The Howieson’s Poort Backed Artefacts from Klaises River: An Argument for Symbolic Behaviour. South African Archaeological Bulletin 54, 38–50. doi:10.2307/3889138

Wylie, A., 1985. The Reaction against Analogy. Advances in Archaeological Theory and Method 8, 63–111.

Wylie, A., 2002. Thinking from Things. Essays in the Philosophy of Archaeology, University of California Press, Berkeley.

Wynn, T. and Coolidge, F., 2004. The Expert Neandertal Mind. Journal of Human Evolution 46, 467–487. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.01.005

Wynn, T., Coolidge, F. and Bright, M., 2009. Hohlenstein-Stadel and the Evolution of Human Conceptual Thought. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 00, 73–83. doi: 10.1017/S0959774309000043

Zilhao, J., 2006. Neandertals and Modern Humans Mixed, and it Matters. Evolutionary Anthropology 15, 183–195. doi:10.1002/evan.20110

Zilhao, J., D’Errico, F., Bordes, J.-G., Lenoble, A., Texier, J.-P. and Riguaud, J.-P., 2007. Grotte des Fées (Châtelperron): History of Research, Stratigraphy, Dating and Archaeology of the Châtelperronian Type-Site. PaleoAnthropology, 1–42.

Zilhao, J., Angelucci, D., Badal-Garcia, E., d’Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., Douka, K., Higham, T., Martinez-Sanchez, M., Montes-Bernadez, R., Murcia-Mascaros, S., Perez-Sirvent, C., Roldan-Garcia, C., Vanhaeren, M., Villaverde, V., Wood, R. and Zapata, J., 2010. Symbolic use of Marine Shells and Pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107, 1023–1028. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914088107