37
1 THE GREENING OF IDENTITY: THREE ENVIRONMENTAL PATHS Simon Gottschalk Department of Sociology University of Nevada, Las Vegas 4505 Maryland Parkway Las Vegas, NV 891545033 Published in Studies in Symbolic Interaction – Vol. 24, FALL 2001 (pp. 245271)

The Greening of Identity: Three Environmental Paths

  • Upload
    unlv

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

1

     

 

 

THE  GREENING  OF  IDENTITY:  

THREE  ENVIRONMENTAL  PATHS    

 

 

Simon  Gottschalk  

Department  of  Sociology  

University  of  Nevada,  Las  Vegas  

4505  Maryland  Parkway  

Las  Vegas,  NV  89154-­‐5033  

 

Published  in  Studies  in  Symbolic  Interaction  –  Vol.  24,  FALL  2001  (pp.  245-­‐271)  

 

 

 

 

 

 

2

(1)  INTRODUCTION    

We  argue  that  the  individual  in  society,  his  or  her  subjectivity,  sense  of  selfhood,  and  experience  of   a   life  world,   all   have  an  ecological   dimension.  We  all   experience  our  selves  as  being   in  a  relationship  with  an  ecology,  and  we  all  express  our  selves   in  a  conversation  with  this  web  of  connections.  We  orient  ourselves  to  nonhuman  others  as  well   as  human  others   and   reference  groups.  We  are   all   essentially   grounded   in,  and  bonded  to,  a  nonhuman  world…Life  and  meaning  are  fundamentally  ecological.  This   has   radical   implications   for   the   ways   we   theorize   society,   culture,  communication,  and  the  self  (Jagtenberg  and  McKie  1997,  pp.  122-­‐123).    On  reconnâit   la  vraie   rationalité  à   sa  capacité  de   reconnaître   ses   insuffisances   [You  can   tell   true   rationality  by   its   ability   to   acknowledge   its  own  blind   spots]   (Morin  &  Kern  1993,  p.  188  -­‐-­‐  my  translation).    The  problem  of  how  to  transmit  our  ecological  reasoning  to  those  whom  we  wish  to  influence  in  what  seems  to  be  an  ecologically  “good”  direction  is  itself  an  ecological  problem  (Bateson  1975,  p.  504).    

  Located  at  the  fertile  intersection  where  a  postmodern  symbolic  interactionism  meets  

ecological  thought,  this  paper  invites  the  reader  to  consider  the  ecological  dimension  of  identity  

by  exploring  three  paths  which  might  hopefully  guide  this  endeavor.  Before  proceeding  any  

further,  let  me  emphasize  that  I  do  not  provide  here  absolute  solutions  or  final  answers  to  the  

complex  topics  of  identity  and  ecology.  My  aims  are  more  modest  and  consist  in  calling  the  

reader’s  attention  to  recent  insights  in  eco-­‐theories  of  identity  and  to  encourage  symbolic  

interactionists  and  others  to  further  develop  them.    

  The  issue  of  identity  has  admittedly  become  increasingly  complex  in  the  wake  of  the  post-­‐

structural  turn,  a  turn  which  has  produced  a  bewildering  number  of  arguments  about  the  

meaning  and  very  existence  of  this  slippery  concept  (Gergen  2000,  1991,  Hall  1996).  Rather  

than  offering  grand  conclusions  about  this  voluminous  and  multidisciplinary  literature,  I  will  

begin  by  suggesting  that  any  theoretical  statement  about  identity  -­‐-­‐the  present  one  included—

ultimately  articulates  and  reproduces  subjective  experiences,  cultural  assumptions,  ideological  

3

orientations,  matters  of  ability  and  choice.  As  Jagtenberg  and  McKie  (1997,  p.  124)  put  it,  in  the  

final  analysis,  statements  about  identity  always  communicate  about  “who  we  think  we  are”.  

And  since  pronouncements  about  who  we  think  we  are  vary  so  greatly  across  time  and  space,  it  

would  seem  prudent  to  approach  any  statement  about  identity  equipped  with  both  flexible  

intellectual  parameters  and  critical  self-­‐reflexivity.  Thus,  for  example,  while  we  are  busy  trying  

to  rationally  support  or  categorically  reject  the  idea  of  an  ecological  self  who  is  in  constant  

interaction  with  an  autonomous  environment,  we  tend  to  overlook  that,  for  most  of  history  and  

in  most  cultures,  such  an  idea  was  understood  as  plain  common  sense  (see  Egri  1997,  Gottlieb  

1996).  Bateson  (1975,  p.  484),  for  example  remarks  that:  

Anthropologically,   it  would  seem  from  what  we  know  of  the  early  material,  that  man  in  society  took  clues  from  the  natural  world  around  him  and  applied  those  clues  in  a  sort  of  metaphoric  way  to  the  society  in  which  he  lived.  That  is,  he  identified  with  or  empathized  with  the  natural  world  around  him  and  took  that  empathy  as  a  guide  for  his  own  social  organization  and  his  own  theories  of  his  own  psychology.    

As  French  sociologists  Morin  and  Kern  (1993,  p.  61)  also  remind  us,  “while  the  mythology  of  

every  civilization  has  located  the  human  world  squarely  in  nature,  Homo  occidentalis  was,  up  

until  the  middle  of  the  20th  century,  completely  oblivious  and  unconscious  of  his  cosmic  and  

terrestrial  identity.”  (my  translation)  From  an  ecological  perspective  then,  it  is  not  the  

ecological  self  which  should  be  approached  as  a  bizarre  and  odd  phenomenon  requiring  

extensive  theoretical  justification  and  compelling  empirical  evidence.  Rather,  what  really  begs  

analysis  is  a  self  and  a  discourse  of  the  self  which  ignore  or  deny  this  ecological  dimension.  As  

ecopsychologists  suggest  (and  the  argument  is  rather  seductive),  spending  more  than  90%  of  

our  lives  indoors,  we  might  have  developed  “indoors  thinking”  (Cohen  1998)  -­‐-­‐  particular  ways  

4

of  knowing  which  literally  frame  our  experiences  and  theories  of  identity,  as  well  as  our  

theories  of    the  world  around  and  within  us.    

  More  problematically  however,  since  “who  we  think  we  are”  not  only  shapes  our  

experiences  of  identity  but  also  inevitably  guides  our  daily  practices,  acknowledging  the  

ecological  dimension  of  identity  –or  failing  to  do  so—has  real  consequences  which  reach  well  

beyond  the  symbolic  realm  of  theory,  the  concrete  walls  of  academia,  the  printed  page,  and  our  

relatively  short  individual  biographies.  Whether  we  perceive  them  as  real  or  don’t.  Accordingly,  

while  an  indoors  epistemology  is  (philosophically,  historically,    psychologically)  a  fascinating    

development  in  the  history  of  human  consciousness,  it  is  also  a  dangerously  maladaptive  one  in  

need  of  urgent  transgression.  To  quote  Bateson  again,  (1975,  p.  485  &  487):  

Epistemological   error   is   all   right,   it’s   fine   up   to   the   point   at   which   you   create   around  yourself  a  universe  in  which  that  error  becomes  immanent  in  monstrous  changes  of  the  universe   that   you   have   created   and   now   try   to   live   in…I   believe   that   this   massive  aggregation  of  threats  to  man  and  his  ecological  system  arises  out  of  errors  in  our  habits  of  thought  at  deep  and  partly  unconscious  levels.    

Lately,  a  small  but  growing  number  of  social  scientists  of  various  persuasions  have  begun  to  

give  the  natural  environment  increasing  attention.  But  while  the  multiplying  number  of  eco-­‐

sociological  journals,  organizations,  sections,  books,  courses  and  programs  attest  to  a  (still  pale)  

“greening”  of  sociology,  this  ecological  turn  has  remained  largely  ignored  in  theories  of  identity  

(postmodern  ones  included),  1  or  constructed  in  problematic  ways.  In  most  of  our  writings,  it  

seems,  the  environment  is  either  ignored,  marginalized  as  the  backdrop  against  which  

important  human  actions  take  place,  or  reduced  to  social  construction.  For  example,  although  

usually  flexible,  dynamic,  and  willing  to  face  new  intellectual  challenges  head  on,  postmodern  

5

symbolic  interactionism  has  remained  largely  hesitant  to  acknowledge  the  important  insights  

various  ecological  theories  could  contribute  to  its  distinctive  approach  to  social  phenomena.  

Accordingly,  since  the  concept  of  identity  is  so  central  to  symbolic  interaction    theory,  the  topic  

of  an  ecological  identity  seems  to  constitute  a  particularly  strategic  point  of  entry,  a  fruitful  

terrain  where  those  theories  can  converse  and  inform  each  other.  In  Jagtenberg  and  McKie’s  

(1997,  p.  125)  words:  

  The  idea  of  an  ecological  self  is  manifestly  not  simply  a  problem  for  deep  ecologists  and       New  Age  philosophers.  The  whole  modernist  paradigm  of  self-­‐perception—its  glorification     in  individualisms  that  cut  across  gender,  class,  and  ethnic  sensibilities—is  challenged  by  any     broadening  and  recentering  of  the  self  across  nonhuman  territories.    Pushed  beyond  the  topic  of  identity,  this  argument  also  suggests  that  since  nature  exists  

beyond  our  social  constructions,  it  constitutes  a  challenging  new  intellectual  terrain,  a  new  

perspective    from  which  we  can  re-­‐assess  our  theoretical  assumptions.  

 

(2)  IDENTITY  AFTER  POST-­‐STRUCTURALISM  

For  most  of  its  history,  psychology  has  located  the  me  within  human  persons  defined  by  their  physical  skin  and  their  immediate  behavior.  The  subject  was  simply  the  me  in  my  body  and  in  my  relations  with  other  subjects.  Over  the  past  20  years  all  this  has  been   scrutinized,   dismantled,   and   even   junked.   Postmodernism   has   deconstructed  continuity,  self,   identification,   identity,  centrality,  gender,   individuality.  The  unity  of  the  self  has  fallen  before  the  onslaught  of  multiple  personalities  (Hillman  1995,  p.  xx).    The  self,  however,  like  nature  and  the  universe,  is  a  category  that  is  never  exhausted  of  meaning  (Jagtenberg  ands  McKie  1997,  p.  125).    We  can  decide  to  limit  [our  self]  to  our  skin,  our  person,  our  family,  our  organization,  our   species   ...   The   ecological   self,   like   any   notion   of   selfhood,   is   a   metaphoric  construct  and  a  dynamic  one.  It  involves  a  choice  (Macy  1994,  p.  293  &  298).    

  The  post-­‐structural  intervention  in  the  social  sciences  has  profoundly  challenged  our  

theories,  and  such  challenges  have  encouraged  symbolic  interactionists,  critical  social  

6

psychologists  and  others  to  develop  new  understandings  of  identity.  Following  the  post-­‐

structuralist  dismantling  of  the  Cartesian  subject  and  its  positing  of  the  continuous  discursive  

construction  of  identity  across  multiple  positions,  many  theorists  are  increasingly  asking  

themselves  “whether  the  individual  self,  sui  generis,  actually  exists”  (Spears  1997,  p.  15;  see  

also  Davis  1999,  Featherstone  1999,  Gergen  2000,  1999,  Grodin  and  Lindloff  1996,  Hall  1996,  

Kvale  1992).  Suggesting  that  a  surface  analysis  is  best,  many  reject  depth-­‐theories  of  drives,  

inner-­‐structures,  personality  traits  and  attitudes,  or  conclude  that  the  subject  is  best  left  

untheorized  altogether.  Instead,  they  approach  identity  as  an  ongoing  narrative  

accomplishment—stories  which  organize  our  everyday  experiences  and  structure  our  

accounts—and  propose  that  the  focus  of  inquiry  should  switch  from  inner  psychological  

processes  to  discursive  practices.  For  example,  in  his  introduction  to  Questions  of  Cultural  

Identity,  Stuart  Hall  (1996,  p.  4)  remarks  that  

actual  identities  are  about  questions  of  using  the  resources  of  history,  language  and  culture  in  the  process  of  becoming  rather  than  being:  not  “who  we  are”  or  “where  we   came   from”,   so   much   as   what   we   might   become,   how   we   have   been  represented  and  how  that  bears  on  how  we  might  represent  ourselves….not  the  so-­‐called  return  to  roots  but  a  coming  to  terms  with  our  “routes”.      

Although  in  general  agreement  with  Hall’s  essay,  his  dismissing  of  roots  in  favor  of  routes  is  

unfortunate  as  it  reproduces  problematic  assumptions.  More  precisely  a  project  of  

identification  which  ignores  our  ecological  roots  -­‐-­‐and  eschews  ecological  thinking  generally—

while  remaining  focused  on  discursive  resources  will  remain  intellectually  stimulating  but  

inevitably  incomplete.  From  an  ecological  point  of  view,  we  should  first  acknowledge  our  roots  

in  the  natural  environment  before  proceeding  any  further  onto  the  routes  we  might  follow  

across  postmodern  landscapes.  While  identities  are  constructed  with  “the  resources  of  history,  

7

language  and  culture,”  they  must  still  be  rooted  within  natural  environments.  In  other  words,  

while  subject  positions  change  across  time,  space,  and  interactional  contexts,  our  positioning  as  

biological  “natural”  organisms  remains  universal  and  constant.    

  Additionally,  while  this  dismantling  of  the  modern  subject  has  produced  far-­‐reaching  and  

liberating  insights  about  the  importance  of  discursive  practices  in  the  constitution,  

reproduction  and  fragmentation  of  identity,  it  has  also  led  many  to  a  sense  of  political  paralysis,  

cynicism,  abstinence  and  a  reluctance  to  formulate  a  solid  epistemological  position.  Decrying  

this  situation,  Spears  (1995,  pp.  17-­‐19)  argues  that  

we  need  a  theory  of  the  self   to  know  which  side  we  are  on,  both  epistemologically  and   politically.   Epistemologically,   we   need   to   know   who   we   are   in   order   to   act...  Selfhood  therefore  provides  perspective  and  a  sense  of  identity  which  are  necessary  for  conscious  agency...A  theory  of  the  subject  in  these  terms,  then,  would  seem  to  be  an  important  ingredient  for  a  critical  social  psychology  if  it  is  to  have  bite,  and  allow  us  to  descend  from  the  fence...Formulating  a  theory  of   the  subject  provides  agents  and  agency  that  can  be  the  vehicles  of  resistance  and  change.      

Motivated  partly  by  the  desire  to  contribute,  however  modestly,  to  the  greening  of  sociology,  

this  paper  does  not  claim  to  have  found  final  answers  to  the  question  of  ecological  identity,  but  

invites  readers  to  approach  the  project  of  an  ecological  identity  along  three  paths  

(ecopsychology,  ecological  symbolic  interactionism,  and  deep  ecology)  and  to  explore  the  

insights  these  paths  reveal.  Following  Bateson’s  position  (1975,  p.  505)  that  “the  ecological  

ideas  implicit  in  our  plans  are  more  important  than  the  plans  themselves…”,  this  paper  is  

openly  partisan,  and  my  wandering  along  these  three  paths  is  thus  also  a  means  to  other  ends.  

These  are:  (1)  to  problematize  the  anthropocentric  bias  in  post-­‐structuralism,  (2)  to  promote  an  

ecological  dimension  to  the  projects  of  identity-­‐construction  and  identity-­‐theory,  and  (3)  to  

advocate  ecological  thinking.      

8

(3)  BEYOND  TEXTUALISM  AND  ANTHROPOCENTRISM  

How  do  we  speak  of   that  which   is  not   reducible   to   the  mode   in  which  we  speak—both  acknowledging  the  mode  in  which  we  speak  and  that  which  asserts  itself  apart  from  having  a  “voice”?  There  is  an  earth  after  all.  Species  do  die  out.  Rains  do  come  down.   Toxic  waters   do   damage.   Organisms   do   attach   to   place   (Bertland   and   Slack  1994,  p.  2).    Anthropocentrism   means   human   chauvinism.   Similar   to   sexism,   but   substitute  human  race  for  man  and  all  other  species  for  woman...When  humans  investigate  and  see  through  their  layers  of  anthropocentric  self-­‐cherishing,  a  most  profound  change  in  consciousness  begins  to  take  place  (Macy  1994,  p.  292).    Clearly,  even  without  the  stimulus  of  new  social  movements,  the  symbolic  interaction  that   occurs   between   self   and   other   often   involves   nonhuman   life   forms   and   their  ecologies.   Yet   we   have   still   to   develop   the   theoretical   tools   that   will   allow   an  ecological   self   to   be   adequately   expressed   in   social   theory   and   general   philosophy  (McKie  and  Jagtenberg  1997,  p.  25).    

  One  first  limitation  ecological  thinkers  (Shepard  1996,  Spretnak  1991,  for  example)  charge  

post-­‐structuralism  with  is  its  excessive  textualism—the  emphasis  on  the  textual  construction  of  

identity,  society,  and  reality.  Accordingly,  the  very  mentioning  of  the  environment  in  the  

context  of  post-­‐structuralism  is  problematic  since,  from  such  a  perspective,  the  environment  is  

but  textual  constructions  and  can  only  have  meaning  through  the  various  discourses  we  deploy  

about  it  —from  Congressional  Acts  to  Earth  First!  newsletters.  In  other  words,  Nature  is  here  

just  “nature”.  From  an  ecological  perspective,  however,  such  an  approach  to  the  environment  is  

not  significantly  different  from  a  modern  one.  Thus,  while  a  modern-­‐materialistic  discourse  

positions  nature  as  the  alien  Other  of  Western  culture  and  imposes  a  fundamental  hierarchy  

between  the  two,  thereby  promoting  violent  domination  and  exploitation  (the  “mine  and  

dump”  view),  the  post-­‐structuralist  one  also  attempts  to  colonize  and  silence  nature,  but  by  

reducing  it  to  a  mere  effect  of  human  symbolic  activity.  While  the  first  approach  constructs  

nature  as  a  warehouse  of  raw  resources  for  economic  exploitation,  the  second  one  reduces  it  to  

9

a  warehouse  of  raw  resources  for  symbolic  manipulation.  In  their  respective  ways,  though,  both  

approaches  deny  nature  an  autonomous  existence,  a  will  of  its  own,  an  essential  value  

independent  of  human  needs  -­‐-­‐whether  economic  or  symbolic.  As  Shepard  remarks  in  this  

context:    

the   genuinely   innovative  direction  of  our   time   is   not   the   final   surrender   to   the  anomie  and  meaninglessness  or  the  escape  to  fantasylands  but  in  the  opposite  direction—toward  affirmation  and  continuity  with  something  beyond  representation...To  argue  that  because  we  interpose  talk  or  pictures  between  us  and  this  shared  immanence,  that  it  therefore  is  meaningless,  contradicts  the  testimony  of  life  itself  (1996,  pp.  160-­‐162).      

Thus,  while  the  deconstructionist  impulse  would  “dispute  the  existence  of  any  independent  

reality  beyond  the  stories  we  tell,  or  at  least  ones  we  can  get  at  in  any  meaningful  sense”  

(Spears  1995,  p.  5),  a  more  realist  argument  would  advance  that  if  “reality  is  always  constructed  

by  its  users,  this  does  not  necessarily  mean  that  reality...is  only  socially  constructed.”  Quoting  

Mead,  Weigert  (1997,  p.  39  &  47)  reminds  us  that            

meaning  is  in  nature…Natural  meanings  always,  everywhere,  and  necessarily  ground  social   and   symbolic   meanings.   Social   meanings   exist,   as   it   were,   only   within   a  spacesuit   or   spaceship,   that   is,   within   a   narrow   band   of   the   tremendous   range   of  physico-­‐chemico-­‐bio   variations   that   exist   in   interaction   in   the   cosmos...Recognizing  these   realist   limits  prevents  analysts   from  a   linguistic   turn   into   the   symbolic   fallacy  that   reduces   meaning   to   semiotic   structures,   contingent   cultures,   or   virtual  universes...    

For  ecofeminist  Spretnak  (1991)  also,  our  enthusiasm  with  deconstruction  has  led  us  to  a  rather  

bleak  intellectual  dead-­‐end  which  conceals  the  idea  that  there  is  something  beyond  and  outside    

our  texts.  To  state  the  obvious,  all  human  acts  (and  hence  all  social  constructions)  always  

require  certain  objective  conditions  which  are  simply  physico-­‐bio-­‐chemical,  and  which  we  

systematically  fail  to  take  into  consideration  when  discussing  identity,  its  dispersion,  

fragmentation,  and  discursive  construction.  Although  our  symbolic  constructions  vary  widely  

10

over  time  and  across  space,  our  essential  need  for  fresh  water,  a  fragile  mixture  of  gases,  2  

protection  against  extreme  temperatures,  and  constant  access  to  other  organisms  we  

transform  as  food  does  not.  As  deep  ecologists  and  ecopsychologists  ceaselessly  remind  us,  we  

are  not  only  in  nature  but  we  are  still  –organically-­‐-­‐  of  nature,  from  nature,  in  a  certain  sense  

we  are  nature.  Like  every  other  organism,  we  are  suspended  in  webs  of  complex  ecological  

processes  before  we  are  suspended  in  webs  of  complex  symbolic  meanings,  and  as  Weigert  

(1997)  astutely  remarks,  we  can  never  be  certain  that  the  meanings  we  weave  about  these  

processes  really  capture  what  they  are  about.      

    Anthropocentrism,  a  second  limitation  of  post-­‐structuralism  derives  from  and  reinforces  its  

textualism.  More  specifically,  while  the  daring  and  welcome  post-­‐structuralist  deconstruction  of  

Euro-­‐phallo-­‐andro-­‐logo-­‐helio-­‐hetero-­‐centric  discourses  has  radically  transformed  our  

understanding  of  central  sociological  concepts  and  has  informed  new  and  important  political  

projects,  it  has  remained  generally  reluctant  to  tackle  anthropocentrism  -­‐-­‐the  meta-­‐narrative  

which  spawned  all  these  discourses  in  the  first  place.  In  other  words,  by  positioning  humans  

and  their  discursive  practices  at  the  center  and  horizon  of  its  project,  the  post-­‐structuralist  

deconstructive  élan  reaffirms  anthropocentrism  and  human  exemptionalism.  While  post-­‐

structuralism  has  deconstructed  Man,  it  has  not  decentered  the  human.  As  a  result,  modernist  

hierarchies  distinguishing  human  from  nonhuman  and  culture  from  nature  quietly  return  

through  the  back  door  (and  between  the  lines)  of  even  the  most  radical  deconstructionist  work  

(see  Cheney  1995,  Michael  1997,  Spretnak  1991).  Post-­‐structuralism  is  certainly  neither  the  sole  

nor  first  discourse  guilty  of  anthropocentrism,  but  given  its  attention  to  the  power  of  

11

metanarratives  in  both  the  constriction  and  construction  of  cognitive  maps,  this  silence  about  

anthropocentrism  is,  to  say  the  least,  surprising.  For  deep  ecologist  Shepard  (1996,  p.  160-­‐161),  

the  assumption  that  there  is  nothing  outside  the  text  is  itself  “the  articulation  of  the  profound  

arrogance  of  humanism  which  fails  to  be  critically  reflexive  with  regard  to  this  meta-­‐narrative”.  

As  Jagtenberg  and  McKie  conclude  (1997,  p.  127),  “in  the  final  analysis,  neither  materialist  

dialectics  nor  poststructural  textual  analysis  are  ecological  theory—they  are  resolutely  human  

centered…”  

    Ecological  theorists  submit  that,  among  other  effects,  excessive  textualism  and  stubborn  

anthropocentrism  inhibit  our  progress  towards  a  necessary  social,  intellectual  and  psychological  

(r)evolution  by  (a)  encouraging  an  emotional  and  moral  autism  vis-­‐à-­‐vis  the  natural  

environment,  (b)  limiting  our  understanding  of  identity  as  solely  an  effect  of  textual  and  social  

constructions,  and  (c)  reproducing  problematic  binarisms.  Accordingly,  by  transgressing  these  

obstacles,  an  ecocentric  perspective  could  (a)  cultivate  qualitatively  different  emotional  and  

moral  responses  towards  the  natural  environment,  (b)  ground  or  at  least  align  our  continuous  

project  of  identity  more  firmly  (and  more  humbly)  with/in  the  natural  environment  rather  than  

in  social  and  textual  constructions,  and  (c)  nurture  new  forms  of  critical  thought,  validate  

radically  new  “ways  of  knowing”,  and  transcend  long-­‐established  but  inappropriate  

dichotomies.  As  they  also  suggest,  these  developments  will  then  hopefully  guide  our  daily  

practices  as  well  as  inspire  life-­‐enhancing  social  and  political  projects  (see  Zimmerman  1994).  

Summarizing  these  ideas,  Jagtenberg  and  McKie  (1997,  pp.  123-­‐124)  explain  that:  

An   ecocentric   shift   also   encourages   new   transgressive   thoughts—such   as   the   idea  that   identity  and  self-­‐awareness  are  ecological   in  essence.  Self   can  now  be  seen  as  socially   constructed   and   sustained   in   community   with   an   enormous   number   of  interconnected  others  along  with  their  ecologies  and  habitats.  As  soon  as  community  

12

is  extended  beyond  the  human  sphere,  a  number  of  significant  barriers  are  crossed:  The   dualisms   of   nature-­‐culture,   reason-­‐emotion,  mind-­‐matter,  male-­‐female   are,   in  practice,  all  transgressed  by  life  and  nature  itself.    

For  many  ecological  thinkers,  the  development  of  an  ecological  identity  constitutes  one—some  

say  the  most—important  step  in  this  evolution  from  an  anthropocentric  to  an  ecocentric      

perspective.  

 (4)  BRINGING  IDENTITY  DOWN  TO  EARTH:  THREE  PATHS    

Ecological  consciousness,  ecosophy,  the  ecological  self,  the  ecological  unconscious:  these  are  just  a  few  of  the  metaphorical  terms  that  have  been  used  to  formulate  an  epistemology  of  mind  and  ecosystem...In  other  words,  they  offer  a  new  synthesis  of  knowledge,   based   on   a   comprehensive   reappraisal   of   various   normative   views   of  the  world  (Thomashow  1995,    pp.  18-­‐19).    It  is  important  to  look  at  ideas  we  hold  about  the  self  in  a  changing  world  because  our   notions   of   self   and   the   symbols  we   deploy  will   be   of   direct   relevance   to   the  worlds  we  build   in   the   future.   The  way  we   refashion  our  ecosystems  will   emerge  fundamentally   from   the   structurally   constrained   use   of   our   symbols   of   self-­‐expression.   The   breadth   and   diversity   of   all   that   we   identify   with   self   and  subjectivity  will  clearly  determine  our  ability  to  deal  with  difference,  otherness,  and  multiplicity...  (Jatenberg  and  McKie  1997,  p.  148).    

  The  past  two  decades  or  so  have  witnessed  a  small  but  inspiring  number  of  works  scattered  

across  the  human  sciences  which  have  been  developing  the  idea  and  project  of  an  ecological  

identity.  As  many  note,  this  ecological  identity  constitutes  an  important  process  that  fosters      

significantly  different  ways  of  relating  to  the  environment  (and  of  relating  per  se)  and  an  

inspiring  source  of  environmental  activism  (see  especially  Ingalsbee  1996,  Thomashow  1995l,  

Weigert  1997).  Although  the  precise  features  of  this  identity,  self  or  consciousness  are  still  

much  contested  in  different  branches  of  ecological  thought,  most  agree  that  it  is  characterized  

by  several  important  tendencies.  Synthesizing  findings  generated  through  a  variety  of  

experiments,  therapeutic  encounters,  theoretical  developments,  and  pedagogical  practices,  

13

several  ecotheorists  (Cahalan  1995,  Fox  1990,  Greenway  1995,  Harper  1995,  Sewall  1995,  

Thomashow  1995)  advance  that  ecologically-­‐informed  shifts  in  the  definition/experience  of  

identity  often  produce  significant  and  enduring  changes  in  individuals’  experiences  of  self,  and  

of  human  and  nonhuman  others.  Characterized  by  mutuality,  reciprocity,  cooperation,  

compassion,  a  nurturing  ethic,  complementarity,  empathy,  the  experience  of  permeable  

boundaries  between  inner  and  outer  processes,  and  feelings  of  solidarity  with  both  human  and  

non-­‐humans,  such  tendencies  not  only  seem  inherently  desirable  and  adaptive,  but  on  a  more  

theoretical  level,  seem  especially  resonant  with  the  project  of  an  “affirmative”  (Rosenau  1992)  

postmodern  symbolic  interactionism,  and  a  spiritually-­‐inclined  one  (Denzin  and  Lincoln,  2000)  .    

  But  how  does  one  go  about  developing  this  ecological  identity?  For  the  purpose  of  this  

paper,  I  want  to  explore  three  paths  which  might  guide  this  project.  These  paths  originate  in  

ecopsychology,  in  an  ecologically-­‐informed  symbolic  interactionism,  and  in  deep  ecology.  

Although  these  three  paths  proceed  in  different  directions,  they  also  intersect  as  they  all  (a)  

emphasize  the  primordial  importance  of  the  natural  environment  which  exists  outside,  beyond,  

and  in  spite  of  our  social  constructions,  (b)  challenge  the  repression,  exclusion,  and  

misconstructions  of  the  natural  environment  in  our  theories  and  experiences  of  identity,  (c)  

advance  that  an  ecological  identity  is  an  important  step  in  developing  an  ecological  perspective  

and,  hence,  ways  of  thinking  which  can  more  effectively  evolve  beyond  anthropocentrism,  and  

(d)  suggest  that  an  ecological  identity  is  often  informed  by  epiphanic,  emotional,  and  even  

spiritual  experiences  and  insights  with/in  the  nonhuman  environment.  These  non-­‐rational  

experiences  are  of  course  enormously  problematic  since  they  cannot  be  logically  demonstrated  

14

or  even  adequately  communicated  to  others  who  have  not  shared  them  and  who  would  deny  

their  validity  by  relying  on  modern-­‐scientific  criteria  or  post-­‐structuralist  ones.    

The  Wild  and  Visceral  Path:  Ecopsychology’s  Ecological  Unconscious  

The   Ecological   Self   is   an   identity   practice   that   involves   the   deconstruction   of   the  externality  of  Nature.  It  leads  to  an  expansive  identification  of  larger  interconnected  Self...The   Wild   Within   our   selves   is   repressed   into   the   subconscious   by   reigning  discourses   of   modernist   technocratic   society;   to   release   the   Wild   Within   is  considered  a  subversive,  counter-­‐discursive  activity  (Ingalsbee  1996,  p.  269).    An   ecologically   harmonious   sense   of   self   and  world   is   not   the   outcome  of   rational  choices.  It  is  the  inherent  possession  of  everyone;  it  is  latent  in  the  organism,  in  the  interaction  of  the  genome  and  early  experience...Beneath  the  veneer  of  civilization,  in   the   trite   phrase   of   humanism,   lies   not   the   barbarian   and   the   animal,   but   the  human  in  us  who  knows  what  is  right  and  necessary  for  becoming  fully  human...We  have   not   lost,   and   cannot   lose   the   genuine   impulse.   It   awaits   only   an   authentic  expression  (Shepard  1995,    pp.  39-­‐40).    

   

  Originating  in  Freudian  and  neo-­‐Freudian  discourses,  ecopsychology  approaches  the  

development  of  an  ecological  identity  accordingly.  Roszak  -­‐-­‐a  leading  figure  in  this  approach—

suggests  that  we  are  all  the  bearers  of  an  ecological  “voice”  which  is  a  source  of  visceral  and  

intuitive  wisdom  about  self  and  environment.  Calling  this  voice  the  “ecological  unconscious”,  

Roszak  locates  it  at  the  most  fundamental  level  of  human  sensory  experiences,  preceding  even  

Freud’s  id.  Unfortunately,  he  argues,  this  ecological  unconscious  has  been  repressed  and  

atrophied  through  a  socialization  process  which  has  effectively  resulted  in  “the  permissible  

repression  of  cosmic  empathy,  a  psychic  numbing  we  have  labeled  ‘normal’”  (Roszak  1995,  p.  

11).  For  psycho-­‐historian  Shepard  also,  the  history  of  the  human  species  is  the  history  of  a  

gradual  and  violent  severing—physical,  social  and  psychological—of  a  fundamental  connection  

between  humans  and  nature,  between  psyche  and  ecology.  In  his  view,  children  growing  up  in  

15

contemporary  Western  society  reproduces  this  tragic  severing  in  their  own  biographies  and  

cannot,  as  a  result,  develop  as  healthy  individuals.      

  In  the  ecopsychological  perspective,  the  ecological  unconscious  is  such  an  important  aspect  

of  identity  because  it  allows  us  to  directly  access  and  experience  the  link  between  psyche  and  

ecology—a  link  which  is  cosmic,  evolutional,  physico-­‐chemical,  cellular  and  thus  often  hard  to  

define.  As  Roszak  explains  (1992,  p.  320),  

(1)  The  core  of  the  mind  is  the  ecological  unconscious.  For  ecopsychology,  repression  of   the  ecological  unconscious   is   the  deepest   root  of   collusive  madness   in   industrial  society;   open   access   to   the   ecological   unconscious   is   the   path   to   sanity.   (2)   The  content   of   the   ecological   unconscious   represent,   in   some  degree,   at   some   level   of  mentality,   the   living   record   of   cosmic   evolution,   tracing   back   to   distant   initial  conditions  in  the  history  of  time...(3)  Just  as  it  has  been  the  goal  of  previous  therapies  to  recover  the  repressed  contents  of  the  unconscious,  so  the  goal  of  ecopsychology  is  to   awaken   the   inherent   sense   of   environmental   reciprocity   that   lies   within   the  ecological  unconscious.  Other  therapies  seek  to  heal  the  alienation  between  person  and  person,  person  and  family,  person  and  society.  Ecopsychology  seeks  to  heal  the  more  fundamental  alienation  between  the  person  and  the  natural  environment.    

As  he  hopes,  once  this  powerful  link  is  re-­‐experienced  and  assimilated,  we  will  re-­‐assess  the  

self-­‐environment  relationship  and  reorganize  our  identity  according  to  a  logic  which  is  much  

more  attuned  to  and  informed  by  the  natural  environment.  Anticipating  the  disbelief  and  

criticism  such  ideas  were  likely  to  ignite,  he  remarks  that  (1995,  p.  7):  

In   our   culture,   listening   for   the   voices   of   the   Earth   as   if   the   nonhuman  world   felt,  heard,  spoke  would  seem  the  essence  of  madness  to  most  people.  Is  it  possible  that  by  asserting  that  very  conception  of  madness,  psychotherapy  itself  may  be  defending  the  deepest  of  all  our  repressions,  the  form  of  psychic  mutilation  that  is  most  crucial  to   the   advance   of   industrial   civilization,   namely,   the   assumption   that   the   land   is   a  dead  and  servile  thing  that  has  no  feeling,  no  memory,  no  intention  of  its  own?    

Additionally,  ecopsychologists  criticize  traditional  psychology  and  psychiatry  for  their  failure  to  

recognize  that,  ultimately,  our  relation  to  self  and  others  cannot  reasonably  be  healthy  as  long  

as  we  remain  so  alienated  from,  and  destructive  of,  our  environmental  roots  —an  assumption  

16

which  “stone-­‐age  psychiatrists”  have  always  held  as  self-­‐evident  (Roszak  1995).  As  Roszak  also  

put  it,  developing  theories  about  human  behavior  by  observing  individuals  interacting  in  urban  

environments  is  not  unlike  developing  theories  about    wild  tigers’  sociation  patterns  by  

watching  them  nervously  pace  in  their  zoo  cages.    

  For  ecopsychologists  and  deep  ecologists,  the  provincialist  reduction  of  nature  to  mere  

matter  or  symbolic  construction  already  articulates  and  reproduces  a  fundamental  alienation  

from  nature.  3  Although  ecotheorists  do  not  generally  agree  on  the  original  cause  of  this  

alienation  (physical  dislocation,  patriarchy,  gerontocracy,  monotheism,  the  advent  of  

horticultural  society,  the  agricultural  revolution)  they  have  drawn    interesting  parallels  between  

our  treatments  of  nonhuman  external  nature  and  of  human  internal  nature  —one’s  mind,  

experiences,  consciousness,  the  (always  embodied)  mechanisms  of  identity,  subjectivity,  and  

meaning-­‐construction.  As  ecopsychologists  propose,  the  pathological  behaviors  we  

(un)consciously  and  routinely  visit  upon  the  natural  environment,  those  we  collectively  

manifest  as  a  society,  those  we  privately  experience  as  individuals  (White  1998),  and  those  we  

commit  in  our  epistemologies  (Bateson  1975)  all  articulate  each  other.    

  In  this  view  (already  advanced  by  Marcuse  in  1972),  the  technological  colonization  of  

nature  out  there  complements  the  logical-­‐rational  domination  of  nature  in  here.  The  extinction  

of  species  out  there  is  matched  by  the  elimination  of  psychological  possibilities  and  insights  in  

here,  the  brutal  mutilation  of  nature  out  there  is  coordinated  with  the  violent  distortion  of  

nature  in  here.  The  list  could  go  on  indefinitely,  but  this  idea  has  been  perhaps  best  articulated  

in  Native-­‐American  leader  Black  Elk’s  few  words:  “What  man  does  to  the  earth,  he  does  to  

17

himself  and  to  others.”  As  ecopsychologists  insist,  the  awakening  of  the  ecological  unconscious  

or  “voice”  is  a  vital  means  and  ends  of  digging  out  the  psychosocial  roots  of  our  assault  on  the  

environment  and  of  healing  ourselves  (see  also  Berry  1988).  While  Roszak  insists  that  “ecology  

needs  psychology  and  psychology  needs  ecology,”  (1995,  p.  5)  it  seems  clear  that  the  same  can  

be  advanced  with  regard  to  sociology,  to  social  psychology,  and  Western  epistemology  

generally  (see  Bateson  1975).    

  Having  located  the  source  of  an  ecological  identity  in  the  pre-­‐id  ecological  unconscious,  

ecopsychologists  promote  various  strategies  to  awaken  this  “voice”  and  to  listen—with  one’s  

body,  senses,  emotions—to  the  insights  it  communicates.  One  particularly  interesting  form  of  

intervention  they  propose  in  order  to  awaken  the  ecological  unconscious  is  the  “wilderness”  

experience  4  -­‐-­‐  a  literal  “walk  on  the  wild  side”.  In  line  with  their  attempts  to  transcend  

inner/outer,  civilized/primitive,  and  human/nonhuman  dualisms,  ecopsychologists  maintain  

that  experiencing  the  wild  “out  there”—in  natural,  uncultured  spaces—will  awaken  the  wild  “in  

here”—in  mental  and  sensory  ones  (see  especially  Greenway  1995,  Harper  1995).  As  they  point  

out,  the  recovery,  experience  and  validation  of  the  “wild”  within  us  can  reawaken  the  “animal-­‐

instinctual”  self  (Harper  1995,  p.  196),  can  provide  much-­‐needed  insights  about  the  

biological/ecological  basis  of  identity,  and  can  make  us  more  human  in  the  full  (and  ecological)  

sense  of  the  term.    

  Although  wilderness  therapies  may,  at  first,  sound  somewhat  narcissistic  and  self-­‐

indulgent,  they  have  also  been  shown  to  promote  epiphanic  shifts  in  identity-­‐formation,  and  in  

some  cases  also,  to  translate  into  enduring  commitment  to  ecological  activism  (see  Greenway  

18

1995,  Ingalsbee  1996,  White  1998).  Underneath  the  neo-­‐Freudian  terminology,  

ecopsychologists  also  reiterate  what  the  main  founders  of  the  American  environmentalist  

movement  had  each  discovered,  described  and  realized  in  his  and  her  own  way:  David  Thoreau  

at  Walden  Pond,  John  Muir  in  the  high  Sierras,  Rachel  Carson  on  East  coast  beaches,  and  before  

them,  Jean  Jacques  Rousseau  on  his  solitary  nature  walks  (1968).  All  developed  far-­‐reaching  

intuitive  ecological  insights  and  discovered  new  dimensions  of  their  selves  (and  the  modern  

human  condition)  by  finding  themselves  in  wild  places.  5  

  At  the  same  time,  ecopsychologists  remark  that  most  people  having  experienced  this  

ecological  shift  in  identity  will  inevitably  “cross  back”  from  wild  natural  and  mental  spaces  to  

urban  ones,  and  as  a  result,  the  extent  to  which  the  voice  and  insights  of  the  ecological  

unconscious  can  survive  in  such  unnatural  spaces  remains  unclear.  Still,  although  difficult  to  

sustain  in  urban  settings,  ecopsychologists  believe  that  ecological  insights  encountered  in  wild  

places  will  permanently  alter  one’s  sense  of  identity  and  relation  to  the  natural  environment.    

  Ecopsychology’s  stepping  stones  (the  ecological  unconscious,  the  “voice  of  the  earth”,  the  

earth’s  psyche)  generate  quite  a  bit  of  resistance,  and  are  often  dismissed  as  essentialist  at  

best,  and  ridiculous  at  worst.  Yet,  while  critics  would  be  quick  to  point  that  individuals  

experience  the  psyche-­‐ecology  link  only  because  they  believe  in  it  and  discursively  construct  it,  

ecopsychologists  would  retort  that  individuals  the  world  over  and  throughout  history  believe  in  

this  link  because  they  have  experienced  it,  and  because  this  experience  occurred  prior  to/outside  

of  representation.  Thus,  while  it  is  certainly  true  that  we  can  “only  know  the  environment  

through  human  templates”  (see  for  example  Fine  1992,  p.  162),  this  does  not  necessarily  mean  

that  these  templates  are  always  or  only  limited  to  cultural-­‐linguistic  ones.  As  such,  following  the  

19

ecopsychological  path  by  relying  on  traditional  logico-­‐positivist  or  constructionist  maps  can  only  

lead  to  a  dead-­‐end  because  this  psyche-­‐ecology  link  cannot  be  demonstrated  to  be  either  “real”  

(in  the  logico-­‐positivist  sense)  or  to  exist  beyond  representation.  6    As  Roszak  also  notes,  our  

very  insisting  that  this  link  between  psyche  and  ecology  be  somehow  demonstrated  is  already  a  

tragic  symptom  of  the  depth  of  our  alienation  from  both.  In  other  words,  finding  oneself  in  

nature  necessarily  evokes  different  perspectives.  To  reduce  these  new  perspectives  and  insights  

to  mere  linguistic  constructions  reproduces  (ecologically)  problematic  assumptions  about  self,  

nature,  and  meaning.  Thus,  while  we  have  little  trouble  appreciating  Simmel’s  insights  about  

the  profound  effects  of  urban  “jungles”  on  our  mental,  emotional,  behavioral  and  social  

dispositions,  we  find  it  suprisingly  more  difficult  to  grant  wild  forests  a  similar  effectivity  

(Rolston  1998).  In  the  same  vein,  while  the  suggestion  of  a  psyche-­‐ecology  interpenetration  is  

readily  dismissed  as  absurd,  fuzzy,  irrational,  and  even  just  “Californian”  (anonymous,  personal  

communication),  the  beliefs  in  collapsing  boundaries,  of  merging  or  interpenetration  between  

psyche,  body  and  simulational  technologies  have  already  become  cliché  in  most  postmodern  

circles.  As  deep  ecologists  and  ecofeminists  of  various  camps  (Merchant  1994,  1992,  Salleh  

1995,  Zimmerman  1994)  point  out,  our  difficulty  to  acknowledge  this  “natural”  interpenetration  

between  psyche  and  ecology  is  intellectually  puzzling,  psychologically  interesting,  but  

ideologically  suspect.  

The  Rational-­‐Moral  Path:  Generalized  Environmental  Other  &  Transverse  Interaction  

Selves  are  meanings  we   realize   in  our  actions  and   in   the   responses  we  and  others,  including  nature,  makes  to  our  actions  (Weigert  1997,    p.  161).      All  landscapes  ask  the  same  question  in  the  same  whisper,  “I  am  watching  you—are  you  watching  yourself  in  me?”  (Durrell  1971,  quoted  in  Devall  1988,  p.  65).      

20

It  is  possible  for  inanimate  objects,  no  less  than  for  human  organisms,  to  form  parts  of  the  generalized...  other  for  any  given  human  individual,  in  so  far  as  he  responds  to  such   objects   socially...Any   thing—any   object   or   set   of   objects,   whether   animate   or  inanimate,   human   or   animal,   or   merely   physical   -­‐-­‐     towards   which   he   responds,  socially,  is  an  element  in  what  for  him  is  the  generalized  other;  by  taking  the  attitudes  of  which   toward   himself   he   becomes   conscious   of   himself   as   an   object...   and   thus  develops  as  self  (Mead,  1934,    p.  154,  in  Weigert  1997,    pp.  172-­‐173).    

   

In  Self,  Interaction,  and  Natural  Environment:  Refocusing  our  Eyesight,  Weigert  (1997)  traces  a  

second  path  to  the  ecological  identity  by  developing  an  ecological  turn  to  two  central  concepts  

of  Symbolic  Interaction  theory:  the  generalized  other  and  symbolic  interaction.  Following  this  

turn,  Mead’s  generalized  other  expands  to  the  Generalized  Environmental  Other,  and  his  

symbolic  interaction  becomes  subsumed  under  Transverse  Interaction.  In  this  approach,  if  our  

sense  of  identity  develops  through  increasingly  more  complex  role-­‐taking  skills  with  others  

(real,  present,  implied  and  imagined),  then  a  central  blind  spot  of  much  symbolic  interactionist  

work  on  identity  has  been  to  restrict  this  other  to  human  dimensions.  Weigert’s  fist  step,  the  

Generalized  Environmental  Other,  propels  role-­‐taking  skills  to  a  more  complex  level  that  now  

includes  the  nonhuman  realm  and  the  ecosphere  at  large.  Following  the  familiar  symbolic  

interactionist  logic,  the  “voice”  of  the  environment  becomes  incorporated  into  the  repertoire  of  

others  we  spontaneously  activate  as  we  mentally  rehearse  actions,  self-­‐reflect,  and  anticipate  

responses  from  an  environment  that  is  both  human  and  not.  As  Weigert  explains  (1997,  pp.  

164-­‐169),  

The   generalized   other   is   a   mental   construction   of   collective   reality   that   informs  personal  thinking  and  motivation.  It  is  not  a  physical  individual  we  see  or  touch...  So,  too,   each   of   us   has   a   generalized   understanding   of   the   physical   environment,   a  Generalized  Environmental  Other.  My  personal  General  Environmental  Other  frames  the  way  I  see  and  decide  about  interacting  with  the  earth...A  new  environmental  self  emerges  as   self   interacts  with  an  object   that  orients   self’s  actions   to   the  organized  responses  of  ever  more  inclusive  ecosystems  as  a  Generalized  Environmental  Other.  

21

 Jagtenberg  and  McKie  (1997,  p.  136)  also  elaborate  on  this  idea:  

Despite   Mead’s   apparent   consensus   orientation   …   there   is   no   a   priori   reason   to  exclude  ecological  considerations  from  the  field  of  self  in  post-­‐Meadian  theory.  With  a  broader  understanding  of  the  social  construction  and  identifications  of  self,  we  can  allow    ecological  others  to  be  significant  points  of  orientation  in  decision  making  and  other  routine  activities,  such  as  the  stories  we  tell  to  sustain  our  identities.    

Just  as  Mead  argued  for  the  social  necessity  of  a  Generalized  Other  -­‐-­‐the  internalization  of  

organized  community  responses  which  allow  for  the  constitution  of  a  moral  and  normal  social  

self-­‐-­‐  so  too,  Weigert  posits  the  moral  and  simply  self-­‐evident  necessity  to  think,  act  and  self-­‐

reflect  in  relation  to  a  Generalized  Environmental  Other:  “an  integrated  set  of  internalized  

expectations  of  the  systemic  reactions  of  the  natural  world  to  individual  and  collective  action.”  

Although  often  met  with  disbelief  and  reservations,  this  orientation  towards  the  natural  

environment  as  a  meaningful  co-­‐interactant  is  taken-­‐for-­‐granted  in  many  societies  we  call  

“indigenous”  (see  especially  Abram  1997).  As  Devall  (1988,  p.  47)  reminds  us,    

the  Koyukon  of  Alaska,  for  example,  live  in  a  world  that  watches.  The  surroundings  are  aware,  sensate,  personified.  They  feel.  They  can  be  offended.  And  they  must,  at  every  moment,  be  treated  with  respect.      

Research  in  a  variety  of  spiritual  traditions  ranging  from  Native  American  ecosophies  to  Eastern  

religions  (Gottlieb  1996)  also  indicate  that  such  an  approach  to  the  environment  appears  to  be  

the  rule  rather  than  the  exception.  While  such  a  broad  cross-­‐cultural  and  trans-­‐historical  

consensus  cannot  of  course  prove  the  claim  that  the  environment  is  indeed  an  autonomous  and  

self-­‐conscious  interactant,  it  should  at  least  motivate  an  honest  re-­‐assessment  of  our  own  

approach  to  the  environment  which  constructs  it  as  inert,  conscience-­‐less,  and  passive.  In  other  

words,  if  we  insist  that  the  only  environment  that  matters  is  the  socially  constructed  one,  we  

Comment [1]:

22

should  be  prepared  to  courageously  submit  our  own  anthropocentric  assumptions  to  

(ecocentric)  scrutiny  and  deconstruction.  

  In  order  to  better  develop  and  integrate  this  Generalized  Environmental  Other,  Weigert  

suggests  his  second  step:  expanding  our  focus  from  interhuman  symbolic  interaction  to  

transverse  interaction.  This  term  refers  to  human-­‐environment  interactions—those  routine  and  

daily  actions  we,  consciously  and  not,  perpetrate  on  the  natural  environment  (my  daily  

consumption  of  large  quantities  of  natural  resources  including  other  organisms,  production  of  

waste,  dangerous  chemicals,  etc.).  As  we  now  realize,  the  individual  and  combined  outcomes  of  

such  routine  actions  are  often  unknown  and  unknowable,  invisible  to  the  naked  eye,  can  only  

be  detected  by  relying  on  complex  technologies,  and  manifest  themselves  with  much  delay.  

But,  as  Weigert  insists,    

For  the  first  time  in  history,  modern  selves  are  self-­‐consciously  aware  of  the  need  to  analyze   their  actions  as   transverse   interaction  within   the  world   that   is   there   for  all    humans...Whatever   else   we   think   we   are   doing,   we   necessarily   affect   the  environment.  Such  transverse  interaction  has  routinely  been  out-­‐of-­‐focus,  forgotten  or  denied...(p.  159)    

Following  these  two  intertwined  ecological  steps  with  even  a  minimum  of  self-­‐awareness,  the  

environment  takes  on  a  very  different  presence  in  our  mind,  in  the  ongoing  process  of  self-­‐

reflection  so  central  to  the  development  of  identity,  and  hence  to  the  very  meaning  one  

attaches  to  identity.  The  incorporation  of  the  Generalized  Environmental  Other  as  a  meaningful  

voice  now  prompts  entirely  new  categories  of  questions  and  provides  a  new  perspective  from  

where  to  interpret  my  and  others’  everyday  actions.  As  I  reflect  on  my  transverse  interactions  

from  its  imagined  point  of  view,  many  behaviors  I  once  believed  to  be  inconsequential  or  simply  

failed  to  notice  I  now  interpret  as  harmful,  cruel,  disrespectful,  wasteful,  or  suicidal.  

23

Incorporating  the  Generalized  Environmental  Other  as  a  voice  in  my  internal  audience  now  also  

raises  new  questions  about  who  I  think  I  am  as  I  for  the  first  time  attend  to  my  transverse  

interactions,  ask  myself  what  sorts  of  social  psychological  disposition  these  articulate,  and  

compare  my  interactions  with  the  environment  to  my  interactions  with  humans.  The  natural  

environment,  the  nonhuman,  thus  becomes  a  new  “Other”,  a  new  mirror  in  which  I  see  my  own  

reflection.  As  I  increasingly  experience  it  as  part  of  me,  I  also  become  increasingly  aware  of  my  

interactions  with/in  it,  and  what  those  indicate  about  me:    

Redefining   the   other   is   part   of   self-­‐redefinition...Self   is   empirically   constituted   by  networks  of  others  with  whom  or  with  which  self  interacts.  Networks  of  interacting  selves   provide  what   Peter   Berger   calls   “plausibility   structures,”   that   is,   groups   of  confirming   others   who   validate   self   in   context   of   a   group’s   world   view   and   the  personal   identities   realized   within   that   world   view.   The   new   environmental   self  explicitly   includes  organic   and  physical   others  within   its   plausibility   structures.  An  integrated   view   of   the   social-­‐natural   world   includes   frames   for   experiencing   self,  perceiving  others,   seeing   the  world,  and  motivating  action  with  an  environmental  identity  (Weigert  1997,  pp.  163  &  170).    

Pushing  this  point  further,  Weigert  also  asserts  that  this  Environmental  Other  should  be  given  a  

more  compelling  voice  than  the  societal  one.  Thus,  if  one’s  societal  groups  have,  up  until  now,  

constituted  the  main  sources  of  the  social  self,  an  environmental  identity  is  also  importantly  

“based  on  the  realization  that  the  meaning  of  social  action  is  primarily  environmental  and  

universal,  and  secondarily  societal...”  (p.  161)  In  other  words,  while  the  meanings  of  my  

interactions  may  vary  greatly  across  contexts,  the  consequences  of  my  daily  transverse  

interactions  do  not  -­‐-­‐whether  I  am  aware  of  them  or  not,  whether  I  interpret  them  correctly  or  

not.  7  Although  it  is  certain  that  the  voice  of  this  Generalized  Environmental  Other  is  

polyphonous,  ecotheorists  still  maintain  that  our  shared  human  biological  and  genetic  makeup  

24

will  tend  to  communicate  certain  understandings  rather  than  others.  As  anthropological  and  

historical  evidence  suggests  (Bateson  1991,  1979,  1975;  Gottlieb  1996),  certain  fundamental  

ecological  meanings  have  almost  universal  recognition  and  currency  –  probably  because  they  

are  more  attuned  to  the  logic  organizing  the  natural  environment,  life  in  general,  and  hence  our  

own  (human)  nature.      

  While  Roszak’s  ecological  unconscious  is  already  within  us  and  awaiting  to  be  awakened,  

Weigert’s  ecological  self  requires  cognitive  incorporation,  conscious  attention  and  moral  

development.  While  Roszak  promotes  a  withdrawal  from  the  “socialized”  or  cultured  self  in  

order  to  access  the  nonrational  ecological  unconscious  and  the  instinctual  self,  Weigert  

presents  role-­‐taking  with  the  environment  as  a  quasi  social-­‐moral  duty.  As  such,  although  

enormously  useful  for  the  development  of  a  more  ecologically-­‐sensitive  identity  in  symbolic  

interaction  theory,  Weigert’s  path  still  tends  to  reproduce  a  certain  dualism  between  human  

self  and  an  ecological  Other  which  acts  back  on  us  in  ways  which  are  difficult  to  grasp,  feel,  or  

visualize.  In  addition,  his  justification  for  environmental  accountability,  role-­‐taking,  and  moral  

responsibility  still  remains  somewhat  anthropocentric  as  he  emphasizes  that  such  

developments  are  ultimately  essential  not  for  nature’s  own  sake,  but  for  the  sake  of  future  

human  generations.  But  those  weaknesses  can  also  be  seen  as  opportunities.  By  ecologizing  

familiar  symbolic  interactionist  concepts  and  models,  Weigert’s  sober  approach  can  appeal  to  

individuals  who  are  interested  in  exploring  ecological  identity  but  who  might  be  uncomfortable  

with  the  neo-­‐Freudian  physical  and  nonrational  path  characteristic  of  ecopsychology  or  with  

the  more  ecocentric  and  spiritual  one  distinctive  of  deep  ecology.    

The  Spiritual  and  Transcendental  Path:  Deep  Ecology’s  Ecological  Self  

25

How  do  we  develop  a  wider  self?  What  kind  of  process  makes  it  possible?  One  way  of  answering   these   questions:   There   is   a   process   of   ever-­‐widening   identification   and  ever-­‐narrowing  alienation  which  widens  the  self.  The  self  is  as  comprehensive  as  the  totality   of   our   identifications.   Or,   more   succinctly:   Our   Self   is   that   with   which   we  identify.   The   question   then   reads:   How   do   we   widen   our   identifications?  Identification   is   a   spontaneous,   non-­‐rational,   but   not   irrational   process   through  which  the  interest  or  interests  of  another  being  are  reacted  to  as  our  own  interest  or  interests  (Naess  1985,    p.  261).    It  is  through  this  process  of  self-­‐realization,  based  on  identification,  that  we  being  to  widen  our  sense  of   identification   from  self,   family  and   friends,  and  community  and  nation  to  include  the  natural  world  surrounding  us...(Frodeman  1995,  p.  131).    

      Deep  Ecology  is  among  the  most  radical  branches  of  the  environmental  movement,  and  

its  association  with  Earth  First!  and  other  eco-­‐activist  groups  has  contributed  to  such  a  

categorization.  Believing  that  the  ecological  crisis  is  at  basis  a  crisis  of  character  and  culture,  

deep  ecologists  stress  that  reforming  existing  practices  (decreasing  pollution,  pesticides,  etc.)  

without  changing  self  and  culture  will  not  suffice  in  the  long  run.  Ultimately,  although  urgently  

needed,  such  reforms  only  address  the  symptoms  of  ecological  devastation  not  its  roots.  For  

Sessions  -­‐-­‐an  important  figure  in  the  movement-­‐-­‐  “an  ecologically  harmonious  social  paradigm  

shift  is  going  to  require  a  total  reorientation  of  Western  culture.”  (Zimmerman  1994,  31-­‐32).    

  In  contrast  to  visceral  experiences  or  rational  role-­‐taking  with  the  environment,  deep  

ecologists  suggest  that  the  path  to  an  ecological  identity  should  include  an  expanded  

identification  with  the  natural  environment,  an  identification  which  is  cognitive,  emotional,  

ethical  and  even  spiritual  (Devall  1985,  Egri  1997,  Spretnak  1991).  As  developed  by  philosopher  

Arne  Naess,  and  strongly  influenced  by  Zen  Buddhism,  Native-­‐American  traditions,  American  

environmentalism,  and  various  currents  in  Western  philosophy,  deep  ecologists  posit  the  two  

fundamental  and  interrelated  axioms  of  Self-­‐Realization  and  Biocentric  Equality:    

(a)  Self-­‐Realization      

26

  Self  realization  refers  to  the  unfolding  of  a  self  whose  identification  capabilities  expand  

beyond  the  notion  of  the  isolated  ego  striving  primarily  for  hedonistic  gratification  or  salvation.  

While  in  many  traditions,  spiritual  growth  requires  that  we  cease  to  understand  ourselves  as  

isolated  and  competing  egos  and  begin  to  identify  with  other  humans  (from  our  family  and  

friends  to,  eventually,  our  species  -­‐-­‐Devall  &  Sessions  1985,  p.  67),  the  deep  ecological  path  

requires  that  identification  extends  beyond  humanity  to  include  the  nonhuman  world,  the  

“organic  whole”,  and  beyond  that,  what  they  call  the  great  Self,  the  Absolute,  a  sacred  spiritual  

force  or  presence  which  creates  and  permeates  all  that  exists.  Central  to  this  notion  of  self-­‐

realization  is  the  understanding  that  there  are  no  real  boundaries  between  humans  and  

nonhumans  as  everything  that  exists  is  a  manifestation  of  the  same  agency:    

Warwick  Fox,  an  Australian  philosopher,  has  succinctly  expressed  the  central  intuition  of  deep  ecology:  “It  is  the  idea  that  we  can  make  no  firm  ontological  divide  in  the  field  of  existence:  That  there  is  no  bifurcation  in  reality  between  the  human  and  the  nonhuman  realms…to   the   extent   that   we   perceive   boundaries,   we   fall   short   of   deep   ecological  consciousness”  (Devall  &  Sessions  1985,  p.  66)      

Summarizing  common  ecological  insights  found  in  various  traditions,  Spretnak  (1991,  pp.  207-­‐

208)  also  notices  that  in  many,  we  find  the  ideas  that:  

Perceptual   boundaries   between   the   ‘inner   and   the   ‘outer’   dissolve,   and   an   intense  awareness  of  the  whole  as  a  benevolent  and  powerful  common  is  present.…One  comes  to  understand  the  person  as  a  unique  but  integral  manifestation  of  the  social  whole  and  the  cosmological   whole…   [and]   since   interbeing   is   the   nature   of   existence,     measures   of  reciprocity  are  the  ‘internal  logic  of  life’.”      

As  deep  ecologists  also  suggest,  since  this  identity  is  rather  difficult  to  achieve,  it  is  best  to  

speak  of  process  of  identification  which  is  informed  and  energized  by  ecological  awareness,  

meditation,  a  variety  of  ecological  practices,  and  political  activism.  

 

27

(b)  Biocentric  Equality  

  When  we  experience  the  illusory  nature  of  the  boundaries  between  self  and  non-­‐self,  

human  and  non-­‐human,  inner  and  outer,  Biocentric  Equality  -­‐-­‐  the  second  principle  of  Deep  

Ecology—is  a  logical  corollary.  It  posits  that  “all  things  in  the  biosphere  have  an  equal  right  to  

live  and  blossom  and  to  reach  their  own  individual  forms  of  unfolding  and  self-­‐realization  within  

the  larger  Self-­‐realization.”  (Devall  &  Sessions  1985,  pp.  67-­‐69)  Following  this  identification  

which  collapses  the  self-­‐Other  and  human-­‐nonhuman  distinctions,  all  moral  exhortations  to  

protect  nature  become  irrelevant  as  “care  will  naturally  flow  from  humans  to  nature.”  

Following  the  deep  ecological  path,  humans’  self-­‐realization  is  intimately  connected  to  the  self-­‐

realization  of  all  other  species  and  the  ecosphere.  In  other  words,  one  becomes  impossible  

without  the  other,  and  one  is  the  precondition  for  the  other.  As  identification  with  nature  

extends  the  boundaries  of  the  self  to  other  species,  the  environment,  and  the  ecosphere,  the  

sense  of  identity  becomes  radically  transformed:  “Emphasizing  our  commonality  and  continuity  

with  the  natural  world  rather  than  the  differences  allows  us  to  reinterpret  our  sense  of  self-­‐

interest  in  terms  of  others,  our  community,  and  the  natural  world”  (Frodeman  1995,  p.  131).  As  

they  also  add,  since  there  are  no  boundaries  and  since  “everything  is  interrelated…if  we  harm  

the  rest  of  Nature  then  we  are  harming  ourselves.”  But  insofar  as  we  perceive  things  as  

individual  organisms  and  entities,  this  insight  draws  us  to  “respect  all  human  and  nonhuman  

individuals  in  their  own  rights  as  parts  of  the  whole  without  feeling  the  need  to  set  up  

hierarchies  of  species  with  humans  at  the  top.”  (Devall  &  Sessions  1985,  p.  68).    

  Starting  from  these  two  fundamental  axioms  of  Self-­‐Realization  and  Biocentric  Equality,  

the  deep  ecological  path  proceeds  along  these  8  following  principles:  

28

(1)    The  well-­‐being  and  flourishing  of  human  and  nonhuman  life  on  earth  have  values  in  

themselves.  These  values  are  independent  of  the  usefulness  of  the  nonhuman  world  for  

human  purposes  

(2)  Richness  and  diversity  of  life  forms  contribute  to  the  realization  of  these    values  and  

are  also  values  in  themselves.  

(3)  Humans  have  no  right  to  reduce  the  richness  and  diversity  except  to  satisfy  their  vital  

needs.  

(4)  The  flourishing  of  human  life  and  cultures  is  compatible  with  a  substantial  decrease  

of  the  human  population.  The  flourishing  of  nonhuman  life  requires  such  a  decrease.  

(5)  Present  human  interference  with  the  nonhuman  world  is  excessive  and  the  situation  

is  rapidly  worsening.  

(6)  Policies  must  therefore  be  changed.  These  policies  affect  basic  economic,  

technological,  and  ideological  structures.  The  resulting  state  of  affairs  will  be  deeply  

different  from  the  present.  

(7)  The  ideological  change  is  mainly  that  of  appreciating  life  quality  (dwelling  in  

situations  of  inherent  value)  rather  than  adhering  to  an  increasingly  higher  standard  of  

living,  There  will  be  a  profound  awareness  of  the  difference  between  big  and  great.  

(8)  Those  who  subscribe  to  the  foregoing  point  have  an  obligation  directly  or  indirectly  

to  try  to  implement  the  necessary  changes.  

  While  the  deep  ecological  belief  in  a  spiritual  Absolute  or  the  principle  of  self-­‐realization  for  

all  organisms  has  been  rejected  by  social  constructionists  and  others  as  metaphysical  and  

theoretically  untenable,  it  has  also  been  the  recipient  of  much  criticism  on  the  part  of  cultural  

29

ecofeminists  who  see  the  ecological  identity  advanced  here  as  suffering  from  “ideological  

pollution”.  More  precisely,  one  main  criticism  charges  that  it  reproduces  a  male  subjectivity  and  

psychology  by  “seeking  either  a  vampirish  absorption  or  an  infantile  fusion  with  nature”  

(Zimmerman  1994,  p.  286).  As  such,  deep  ecology  is  accused  (not  always  justifiably)  for  

promoting  a  sense  of  identity  with  nonhuman  Others  rather  than  respect  for  radical  difference,  

and  for  failing  to  recognize  that  “women’s  experience  could  provide  an  immediate  ‘living  social  

basis’  for  this  new  consciousness”  (p.  276).    

  Deep  ecologists  (Devall  1988)  as  well  as  a  few  voices  in  cultural  studies  (Bertland  and  Slack  

1994,  Whitt  and  Slack  1994)  and  ecological  postmodernism  (Cheney  1995)  also  point  at  

community,  “bioregion”  or  local  geography  as  the  optimal  space  for  enabling  an  ecological  

identity,  and  as  the  most  strategic  terrain  of  necessary  political  projects.  As  they  suggest,  the  

development  of  an  ecological  identity  must  be  grounded  in  “storied  residence…bioregional  

ways  of  dwelling  that  are  informed  by  narratives  arising  from  experiences  in  a  particular  place  

and  the  relationships  with  specific  beings”  (Zimmerman  1994,  p.  297).  Promoting  a  postmodern  

ecological  orientation,  Cheney  for  example  suggests  that:      

The   fractured   identities   of   postmodernism...can   build   health   and   well-­‐being   by  means  of  bioregional  contextualization  of  self  and  community.  The  voices  of  health  will  be  as  various  and  multiple  as  the  landscapes  which  give  rise  to  them...The  notion  of   socially   constructed   selves   gives   way   to   the   idea   of   bioregionally   constructed  selves  and  communities.  In  this  way,  bioregionalism  can  “ground”  the  construction  of  self   and   community  without   essentialization   and   totalization   typical   of   the   various  “groundings”   of   patriarchal   culture...Self   and   geography   are   bound   together   in   a  narrative  which   locates  us   in  the  moral  space  of  defining  relations...Mindscapes  are  as  multiple  as  the  landscapes  which  ground  them  (1995,  pp.  131&  138).    

Although  the  idea  of  a  bioregional  identity  certainly  constitutes  a  logical  step  or  stage  on  the  

deep  ecological  path,  I  believe  that  it  suffers  from  two  limitations.  First,  such  an  identity  is  

30

predicated  on  a  certain  permanence  in  and  attachment  to  “place”  –  a  relationship  to  space  

which  is  charged  with  historical,  cultural,  emotional  and  even  physical  investments,  and  which  

may  “naturally”  foster  care  and  a  sense  of  responsibility.  However,  the  increasing  geographical  

mobility  characterizing  the  contemporary  American  lifestyle,  and  the  accelerating  virtualization  

of  everyday  life,  interactions,  and  nature  itself  (Weigert  1997)  may  seriously  incapacitate  the  

developing  and  sustaining  of  such  a  relationship.  Second,  bioregionalism  could  also,  under  the  

“right”  conditions  and  subtle  slogans,  encourage  exclusionary  dispositions  towards  whomever  

happens  to  be  considered  an  “alien”  or  “stranger”  at  any  particular  point  in  time  and  place.  Still,  

the  merging  of  eco-­‐activism,  bioregionalism,  multicultural  spiritual  traditions,  and  the  principle  

of  biocentric  equality  provide  a  richly  multilayered  path  that  can  probably  appeal  to  a  wide  

variety  of  people,  as  well  as  offering  diverse  possibilities  for  a  much-­‐needed  dialogue  between  

sociology,  ecology,  and  spirituality.      

 

CONCLUSIONS:  THE  ECOLOGICAL  IMAGINATION    

There   is   an   ecology   of   bad   ideas,   just   as   there   is   an   ecology   of   weeds,   and   it   is  characteristic  of  the  system  that  basic  error  propagates   itself.   It  branches  out   like  a  rooted  parasite  through  the  tissues  of  life,  and  everything  gets  into  a  rather  peculiar  mess  (Bateson  1975,  p.  484)    Ecological  thinking...requires  a  kind  of  vision  across  boundaries.  The  epidermis  of  the  skin   is   ecologically   like   a   pond   surface   or   a   forest   soil,   not   a   shell   so   much   as   a  delicate   interpenetration.   It   reveals   the   self   ennobled   and   extended   rather   than  threatened     as   part   of   the   landscape   and   the   ecosystem,   because   the   beauty   and  complexity  of  nature  are  continuous  with  ourselves  (Shepard  1996,    p.  112-­‐113).    As  images  of    “human  nature”  becomes  more  problematic,  an  increasing  need  is  felt  to  pay  closer  yet  more  imaginative  attention  to  the  social  routines  and  catastrophes  which   reveal   (and   shape)   man’s   nature   in   this   time   of   civil   unrest   and   ideological  conflict  (Mills  1959,  p.  15)    

31

  In  this  paper,  I  have  briefly  explored  three  different  paths  guiding  the  development  of  an  

ecological  identity.  Although  following  different  directions,  all  three  paths  bypass  the  modernist  

conception  of  identity,  follow  the  post-­‐structural  turn  pointing  at  its  textual  construction,  but  

attempt  to  proceed  beyond  the  anthropocentrism  still  present  in  post-­‐structuralism.  They  all  

emphasize  that  there  is  something  beyond  or  outside  the  text.  That  something  is  nature,  the  

ecological,  the  environment  -­‐-­‐a  dimension  which  exists  before,  after,  around  and  inside  us,  a  

dimension  which  we  represent  in  our  discourses  but  which  also  exists  beyond  our  

representations.  In  their  own  way,  each  of  these  paths  suggests  that  nature  (both  external  and  

internal)  cannot  be  reduced  to  its  anthropocentric  modern  (see  Bateson  1975)  and  postmodern  

constructions.  They  all  stress  that  the  experience  and  practice  of  identity  must  expand  and  

involve  the  nonhuman,  and  that,  paradoxically,  such  an  expansion  can  only  enrich  our  

humanness,  deepen  it,  and  root  it  in  the  ground  that  matters  most  -­‐-­‐the  one  that  provides  the  

resources  necessary  for  the  survival  of  all  species,  the  source  of  all  that  exists.  All  three  paths  

also  maintain  that  developing  an  ecological  identity  constitutes  an  urgent  social,  psychological,  

moral  and  political  project.  Although  they  each  suffer  from  shortcomings,  I  believe  that  they  

still  point  to  new  and  necessary  directions  which  traditional  theories  of  identity  do  not  (or  

refuse  to)  acknowledge.  And  while  theoretical  spats  are  not  infrequent  among  scholars  

following  these  different  paths,  I  see  them  as  complementary  rather  than  mutually  exclusive.  In  

some  sense,  they  each  trace  different  points  of  access  or  even  consecutive  stages  8  in  the  

process  of  ecological  identification.    

32

    Since  the  concept  of  an  ecological  identity  utilized  by  many  of  the  thinkers  discussed  here  

contradicts  post-­‐structural  insights  about  the  discursive  construction,  fragmentary  nature,  and  

constant  recombination  of  multiple  identities,  one  way  of  resolving  this  contradiction  might  

consist  in  replacing  the  concept  of  identity  by  identification—a  position  which  is  especially  

pronounced  in  deep  ecology.  In  contrast  to  identity  which  connotes  a  stable  entity,  sameness,  a  

fixed  and  static  presence,  identification  evokes  the  ideas  of  process  and  movement,  but  also  of  

compassion,  empathy,  and  solidarity.  It  bespeaks  of  relationships  -­‐-­‐a  term  of  considerable  

importance  in  ecological  thought.  Following  this  logic,  ecological  identification  is  not  a  fixed  

label  we  apply  to  our  self  but  is  perhaps  best  conceived  as  an  ecologically-­‐informed  process.  It  

informs  who  we  think  we  are,  questions  the  limits  of  our  capabilities  for  role-­‐taking  and  

empathy,  guides  how  we  act,  invites  us  to  develop  a  different  epistemology,  and  encourages  us  

to  reposition  our  sense  of  self  in  relation  with  the  natural  environment  rather  than  in  social  or  

semiotic  structures.  

  Yet,  even  this  move  from  identity  to  identification  does  not  resolve  a  certain  tension  

between  ecological  and  post-­‐structural  discourses  because,  while  post-­‐structuralism  might  

suggest  that  an  ecological  identification  informs  one  among  many  subject-­‐positions,  ecological  

thinkers  will  respond  that  we  are  first  and  foremost  biological  organisms,  terrestrial  creatures,  

part  of  nature.  Although  this  position  can  be  doubtlessly  labeled  as  essentialist,  it  still  seems  

that,  in  contrast  to  every  other  basis  of  identity-­‐construction,  our  essential  ecological  origins  

(and  ultimate  destination)  cannot,  at  this  point,  be  reasonably  deconstructed.    

  As  a  last  thought,  one  particularly  interesting  idea  promoted  by  ecotheorists  posits  that  

humans  are  the  earth’s  “central  nervous  system”  (Berry  1988),  consciousness,  or  “risky  

33

experiment  in  self-­‐conscious  intelligence”  (Roszak,  in  Zimmerman  1991,  p.79),  and  there  are  

obvious  parallels  between  this  positioning  of  humans  as  the  planet’s  self-­‐awareness  and  the  

positioning  of  sociology  as  society’s  self-­‐awareness.  Following  ecotheorists’  call  for  an  

ecologically-­‐informed  revolution  in  our  ways  of  thinking,  the  concept  of  an  “ecological  

imagination”  seems  especially  fruitful.  Although  this  topic  can  only  be  briefly  introduced  in  the  

space  remaining  here,  I  believe  that  there  is  much  to  be  learnt  by  ecologizing  Mills’  canonical  

Promise  (1959).  For  example,  if  “the  sociological  imagination  enables  us  to  grasp  history  and  

biography  and  the  relations  between  the  two  within  society,”  (p.  6),  the  ecological  imagination  

will  posit  that  “neither  the  life  of  an  individual  nor  the  history  of  a  society  can  be  understood  

without  understanding”  the  complex  relations  of  both  with/in  their  natural  environment.  

Accordingly,  an  ecological  “quality  of  the  mind”  cannot  be  limited  to  self-­‐reflection  from  the  

point  of  view  of  environmental  activists,  9  recycling  plastic  bottles,  a  yearly  Earth  Day  

celebration,  or  thinking  about  the  ecology.  The  “quality  of  the  mind”  characteristic  of  the  

ecological  imagination  will  require  that  we  start  thinking  ecologically  –that  we  develop  thinking  

patterns  that  transcend  the  immediate  human  milieus  of  our  biography,  that  surmount  

anthropocentric  binarisms,  and  that  align  psychosphere  with  ecosphere,  epistemology  with  

ecology.    To  quote  Bateson  one  last  time  (1975,  502)  

Ideas,   to   survive   and   to   ensure   survival   must   develop   similar   characteristics   as  organisms  trying  to  adapt  to  their  environments.    

If  the  understanding  of  biography-­‐in-­‐society  is  a  primordial  insight  in  the  development  of  the  

sociological  imagination,  an  understanding  of  identity-­‐with/in-­‐environment  might  very  well  be  

the  critical  step  in  the  flowering  of  the  ecological  one.  

34

NOTES  

1.    For  an  insightful  critique  of  the  absence  of  the  ecological  in  Cultural  Studies  and  Symbolic  Interactionism,  see  especially  Jagtenberg  and  McKie  (1997)  and  Whitt  and  Slack  (1994).    2.  As  an  anonymous  author  once  put  it,  what  would  our  social  constructions  be  like  if  we  were  breathing  helium  or  nitrous  oxide  instead  of  air?  3.  See  Marcuse  (1972),  Merchant  (1994),  Metzner  (1995,  1992),  Roszak  (1995),    Salleh  (1995),  Sessions  (1985),  Tobias  (1985),  Zimmerman  (1994),  for  example.  4.  Although  ecopsychologists  criticize  traditional  psychotherapy  for  its  anthropocentrism  and  urban  logic,  there  are  still  interesting  parallels  between  traditional  psychoanalysis  and  this  “wilderness”  therapy.  Of  course,  this  does  not  mean  that  all  the  therapies  offered  by  ecopsychologists  will  occur  in  wild  places  and  will  aim  at  uncovering  the  ecological  unconscious.    5.  Although  not  referring  to  ecological  thought,  Neumann’s  study  (1992)  of  new  experiences  of  the  self    by  Grand  Canyon  hikers  provides  concrete  and  more  recent    illustrations  of  these  transformations.  6.  As  a  tangent  I  find  hard  to  resist,  since  grasping  the  ecological  unconscious  and  the  psyche-­‐ecology  link  is  most  successfully  accomplished  through  in  situ  personal  experience,  this  project  exhibits  interesting  parallels  with  self-­‐reflexive  ethnography:  It  requires  that  we  leave  our  etic-­‐anthropocentric-­‐textual  assumptions  behind,  that  we  get  our  feet  wet  (or  muddy),  that  we  go  to  “the  field”  (the  forest,  the  canyon,  the  mountain,  the  desert),  immerse  ourselves  in  it,  develop  empathy  with  its  members,  sensitivity  to  its  dynamics,  attentiveness  to  its  sounds  and  fluctuations,  and  notice  how  these  transform  our  own  subjectivity.  In  both  cases,  there  is  no  substitute  for  actually  doing  it.  In  both  cases  also,  there  is  always  much  more  going  on  than  we  could  ever  faithfully  transcribe,  cogently  represent  and  convincingly  communicate.  7.  An  addendum  to  this  idea  suggests  that  I  am  also  always  influenced  by  environmental  forces  (gravity,  sound,  electromagnetic  fields,  light,  micro-­‐organisms,  thermal  fluctuations,  smells,  ultraviolet  rays,  seasonal  cycles,  radiations,  air  and  water  composition,  altitude,  vegetation,  barometric  pressure,  etc)  whether  I  am  aware  of  them  or  not,  whether  I  interpret  them  correctly  or  not.  8.  There  are,  for  example,  interesting  parallels  between,  on  the  one  hand,  Freud’s  id,  ego  and  superego  and,  on  the  other,  the  visceral  ecological  unconscious  (in  ecopsychology),  the  rational-­‐moral  self-­‐reflection  and  self-­‐monitoring  (in  ecological  symbolic  interactionism),  and  the  transcendental  self-­‐realization  through  spiritual  identification  with  the  ecosphere  (in  deep  ecology),  respectively.    9.  This  solution  was  suggested  by  an  anonymous  reviewer.  

35

REFERENCES  Abram,  D.  1997.  The  Spell  of   the  Sensuous:  Language  and  Perception   in  a  More-­‐Than-­‐Human     World.  New  York:  Vintage.  Bateson,  G.  1975.  Steps  to  an  Ecology  of  the  Mind.  New  York:  Ballantine.  -­‐-­‐-­‐.  1979.  Mind  and  Nature:  A  Necessary  Unity.  New  York:  E.P.  Dutton.  -­‐-­‐-­‐.   1991.   A   Sacred  Unity:   Further   Steps   to   an   Ecology   of   the  Mind.   New   York:   HarperCollins     Publishers.  Berry,  T.  1988.  The  Dream  of  the  Earth.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.  Bertland,   J.   and   J.D.   Slack.   1994.   “Introduction:  On   Environmental  Matters.”   Cultural   Studies,     Vol.  8(1):1-­‐4.  Best,   J.   1998.   “Murray   Bookchin’s   Theory   of   Social   Ecology.”   Organization   &   Environment     Vol.  11(3):  334-­‐354.  Bookchin,  M.  1986.  The  Modern  Crisis.  Philadelphia:  New  Society  Publishers.  -­‐-­‐-­‐.  1989.  Remaking  Society.  New  York:  Black  Rose  Books.  Cahalan,   W.   1995.   “Ecological   Groundedness   in   Gestalt   Therapy.”   Pp.   216-­‐223   in     Ecopsychology:   Restoring   the   Earth,   Healing   the  Mind,   edited   by   T.   Roszak,  M.E.   Gomes     and  A.D.  Kanner.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.      Capek,  Stella.  2005.  Surface  Tensions:  Boundary  Negotiations  around  Self-­‐Society,  and  Nature  in     a  Community  Debate  over  Wildlife.  Symbolic  Interaction  29:2  (157-­‐182)  Cheney,   J.   1995.   “Postmodern   Environmental   Ethics:   Ethics   as   Bioregional   Narrative.”   Pp.     23-­‐42   in   Postmodern   Environmental   Ethics,   edited   by   Max   Oelschlaeger.   Albany:   SUNY     Press.  Cohen,   M.   1998.   “Project   Nature   Connect.”   Institute   of   Global   Education.   http://www.     Ecopsych.com.  Davis,  J.  1999.  “Healing  the  Postmodern  Self.  ”  The  Hedgehog  Review.  Vol  1:  47-­‐54.  Denzin,  N.  K.  and  Y.  Lincoln.  2000.  “The  Seventh  Moment:  Out  of  the  Past.”  Pp.  1047-­‐1064   in     Handbook   of   Qualitative   Research,   edited   by   Y.   Lincoln   and   N.   Denzin.   Thousand   Oaks:     Sage.  Devall,   B.   1988.   “The   Ecological   Self.”   Pp.   39-­‐72   in   Simple   in  Means,   Rich   in   Ends:   Practicing     Deep  Ecology,    edited  by  B.  Devall.  Salt  Lake  City:  Pregrine  Smith  Books.  Devall,  B.  and  G.  Sessions.  1985.  Deep  Ecology:  Living  as  if  Nature  Mattered.  Salt  Lake  City:       Peregrine  Smith  Books.  Egri,   C.P.   1997.   “Spiritual   Connections   with   the   Natural   Environment.”   Organization   &     Environment  Vol.  10(4):407-­‐431.  Featherstone,  M.  1999.  “The  Citizen  and  Cyberspace.”  The  Hedgehog  Review.  Vol  1:  63-­‐70.  Fine,  G.A.  1992.    “Wild  Life:  Authenticity  and  the  Human  Experience  of  ‘Natural”  Places.”  Pp.      156-­‐175  in  Investigating  Subjectivity:  Research  on  Lived  Experience,  edited  by  C.  Ellis  and     M.  G.  Flaherty  Newbury  Park:  Sage  Fox,    W.  1990.  Toward  a  Transpersonal  Ecology:  Developing  New  Foundations  for       Environmentalism.  Boston  &  London:  Shambala.  Frodeman,   R.   19945.   “Radical   Environmental   and   the   Political   Roots   of   Postmodernism:     Differences  that  Make  a  Difference.”  Pp.  121-­‐135  in  Postmodern  Environmental  Ethics,       edited  by  M.  Oelschlaeger.    Albany:  SUNY  Press.  Gergen,  K.  1991.  The  Saturated  Self.  New  York:  Basic  Books.  

36

-­‐-­‐-­‐.  1999.  “The  Self:  Death  by  Technology.”  The  Hedgehog  Review.  Vol  1:  25-­‐34  -­‐-­‐-­‐.   2000.   “The   Self:   Transfiguration   by   Technology.”   Pp.   100-­‐115   in   Pathology   and   the  Postmodern,  edited  by  D.  Fee.  London:  Sage.  

Gottlieb,  R.S.  1996.  "Spiritual  Deep  Ecology  and  the  Left:  An  Attempt  at  Reconciliation."  Pp.       516-­‐531  in  This  Sacred  Earth:  Religion,  Nature,  Environment,  edited  by  R.S.  Gottlieb.  New     York:  Routledge.  Grodin,  T.  and  R.  Lindlof.  1996.  Constructing  the  Self  in  a  Mediated  World.  London:  Sage.  Greenway,   R.   1995.   “The   Wilderness   Effect   and   Ecopsychology.”   Pp.   122-­‐   135   in     Ecopsychology:   Restoring   the   Earth,   Healing   the  Mind,edited   by   T.   Roszak,   M.E.   Gomes     and  A.D.  Kanner.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.    Hall,   S.   1996.   “Introduction:  Who  Needs   Identity?”  Pp.  1-­‐17   in  Questions  of  Cultural   Identity,     edited  by  S.  Hall  and  P.  DuGay.  London:  Sage.  Harper,  S.  1995.  “The  Way  of  Wilderness.”  Pp.  183-­‐200  in    Ecopsychology:  Restoring  the  Earth,     Healing  the  Mind,  edited  by  T.  Roszak,  M.E.  Gomes  and  A.D.  Kanner.  San  Francisco:  Sierra     Club  Books.      Inglasbee,   T.   1996.   “Earth   First!   Activism:   Ecological   Postmodern   Praxis   in   Radical     Environmentalist  Identities.”  Sociological  Perspectives  Vol.  39(2):263-­‐276.  Jagtenberg,   T.   And   D.   McKie.   1997.   Eco-­‐Impacts   and   the   Greening   of   Postmodernity:   New     Maps   for   Communication   Studies,   Cultural   Studies,   and   Sociology.   Thousand   Oaks,   CA:     Sage.  Kvale,  S.  1992.  (ed.)    Psychology  and  Postmodernism.  London:  Sage.  Macy,   J.   1994.   “Toward   a   Healing   Self   and   World.”   Pp.   292-­‐298   in   Ecology,   edited   by   C.     Merchant.  New  Jersey:  Humanities  Press.    Marcuse,  H.  1972.  Counterrevolution  and  Revolt.  Boston:  Beacon.    Metzner,   R.   1995.   “The   Psychopathology   of   the   Human-­‐Nature   Relationship.”   Pp.   55-­‐67   in     Ecopsychology:   Restoring   the   Earth,   Healing   the  Mind,   edited   by   T.   Roszak,  M.E.   Gomes     and  A.D.  Kanner.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.    Merchant,  C.  1992.  Radical  Ecology.  New  York:  Routledge.  Michael,   M.   1997.   “Critical   Social   Psychology:   Identity   and   De-­‐prioritization   of   the   Social.”     Pp.  241-­‐259  in  Critical  Social  Psychology,  edited  by  T.  Ibanez  &  L.  Iniguez.  London:  Sage.  Mills,  C.W.  1959.  The  Sociological  Imagination.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press  Morin,  E.  and  A  .B.  Kern.  1993.  Terre-­‐Patrie  [Earth-­‐Country—untranslated].  Paris:  Seuil.  Naess,  A.  1985.  “Identification  as  a  Source  of  Deep  Ecological  Attitudes.”  pp.  256-­‐270  in  Deep     Ecology,  edited  by  Michael  Tobias.  San  Diego,  CA:  Avant  Books.  Neumann,  M.  1992.  “The  Trails  trhough  Experience:  Finding  Self  in  Recollections  of  Travel.  Pp.    

  176-­‐201  in  Investigating  Subjectivity:  Research  on  Lived  Experience,  edited  by  C.  Ellis  and  M.  Flaherty.  Newbury  Park:  Sage  

Rolston,  H.  III.  1988.  Values  Deep  in  the  Woods.  American  Forests  (May/June).  Roszak,  T.  1992.  The  Voice  of  the  Earth.  New  York:  Simon  &  Schuster.  -­‐-­‐-­‐-­‐,   M..E.   Gomes   and   A.D.   Kanner   (1995)   (eds.)   Ecopsychology:   Restoring   the   Earth,     Healing  the  Mind.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.    Rousseau,  J.J.  1968.  Les  Reveries  du  Promeneur  Solitaire.  Lausanne  :  Editions  Rencontre  Rowe,  S.  J.  (1994).  “Ecocentrism:  the  Chord  that  Harmonizes    Humans  and  Earth”.  The       Trumpeter  11:2  (Spring):  106-­‐107.        

37

Salleh,   A.   1995.   “Class,   Race,   and   Gender   Discourse   in   the   Ecofeminism/Deep   Ecology     Debate.”   Pp.   79-­‐100   in   Postmodern   Environmental   Ethics,   edited   by   M.   Oelschlaeger.     Albany:  SUNY  Press.  Searles,    H.F.  1960.  The  Nonhuman  Environment.  New  York:  International  Universities       Press.  Sessions,  G.  1985.  “Ecological  Consciousness  and  Paradigm  Change.”  Pp.  28-­‐44  in  Deep  Ecology,  

edited  by  M.  Tobias.  San  Diego:  Avant  Books  Sewall,   L.   1995.   “The   Skill   of   Ecological   Perception.”   Pp.   201-­‐215   in   Ecopsychology:  Restoring  

the   Earth,   Healing   the   Mind,   edited   by   T.   Roszak,   M.E.   Gomes   and   A.D.   Kanner   San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.    

Shepard,  P.  1992.  Nature  and  Madness.  San  Francisco:  Sierra  Club  Books.    -­‐-­‐-­‐.  1996.    Traces  of  an  Omnivore.  Covelo,  Ca:  Island  Press.  

Spears,  R.    1997  “Introduction.”  Pp.1-­‐26  in  Critical  Social  Psychology,  edited  by  T.  Ibanez  &  L.  Iniguez.  London:  Sage.  

Spretnak,   C.   1991.   States   of   Grace:   The   Recovery   of   Meaning   in   the   Postmodern   Age.   San     Francisco:  Harper  Collins.  Thomashow,  M.  1995.  Ecological  Identity  .Cambridge:  MIT  Press.  Tobias,   M.   1985   “Humanity   and   Radical   Will:   Reflections   from   the   Island   of   Life.”   Pp.   2-­‐27     in    Radical  Ecology,  edited  by  M.  Tobias.  San  Diego:  Avant  Books.  Weigert,   A.   J.   1997.   Self,   Interaction,   and   Natural   Environment:   Refocusing   our   Eyesight.     Albany:  SUNY  Press.  White,  R.  1998.  “Psychiatry  and  Ecopsychology.”  Pp.  205-­‐212   in  The  Environment  and  Mental     Health:   A   Guide   to   Clinicians,   edited   by   A.   Lundberg.   Mahawah,   NJ:   Laurence   Erlbaum     Associates  Publishers.  Whitt,   L.   A.   and   J.   D.   Slack.   1994.   “Communities,   Environment   and   Cultural   Studies.”     Cultural  Studies  Vol.  18(1):5-­‐31.    Zimmerman,   M.   M.   1994.   Contesting   Earth’s   Future:   Radical   Ecology   and   Postmodernity.     Berkeley:  University  of  California  Press.