31
Report A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Highlights d Phylogenetic analysis revealed clear octopus orthologs of human SLC6A4 d SLC6A4 protein alignment revealed conservation of the MDMA binding site in octopuses d A novel assay was developed for quantification of octopus social behaviors d Behavioral analysis revealed conservation of prosocial function of MDMA in octopuses Authors Eric Edsinger, Gu ¨ l Do ¨ len Correspondence [email protected] In Brief Edsinger and Do ¨ len identify clear octopus orthologs of the human serotonin transporter gene, SLC6A4. This finding is paralleled by conservation of the SLC6A4 binding site and acute prosocial functions of MDMA in octopuses. These data provide evidence for the evolutionary conservation of serotonergic signaling in the regulation of social behaviors. Edsinger & Do ¨ len, 2018, Current Biology 28, 1–7 October 8, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Report

A Conserved Role for Sero

tonergicNeurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior inOctopus

Highlights

d Phylogenetic analysis revealed clear octopus orthologs of

human SLC6A4

d SLC6A4 protein alignment revealed conservation of the

MDMA binding site in octopuses

d A novel assay was developed for quantification of octopus

social behaviors

d Behavioral analysis revealed conservation of prosocial

function of MDMA in octopuses

Edsinger & Dolen, 2018, Current Biology 28, 1–7October 8, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

Authors

Eric Edsinger, Gul Dolen

[email protected]

In Brief

Edsinger and Dolen identify clear octopus

orthologs of the human serotonin

transporter gene, SLC6A4. This finding is

paralleled by conservation of the SLC6A4

binding site and acute prosocial functions

of MDMA in octopuses. These data

provide evidence for the evolutionary

conservation of serotonergic signaling in

the regulation of social behaviors.

Page 2: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

Current Biology

Report

AConserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmissionin Mediating Social Behavior in OctopusEric Edsinger1 and Gul Dolen2,3,*1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative Molecular Biology and Evolution, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole, MA, USA2Department of Neuroscience, Brain Science Institute, Wendy Klag Institute, Kavli Neuroscience Discovery Institute, Johns Hopkins

University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA3Lead Contact

*Correspondence: [email protected]

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

SUMMARY

Human and octopus lineages are separated byover 500 million years of evolution [1, 2] andshow divergent anatomical patterns of brain orga-nization [3, 4]. Despite these differences, growingevidence suggests that ancient neurotransmittersystems are shared across vertebrate and inverte-brate species and in many cases enable overlap-ping functions [5]. Sociality is widespread acrossthe animal kingdom, with numerous examples inboth invertebrate (e.g., bees, ants, termites, andshrimps) and vertebrate (e.g., fishes, birds, ro-dents, and primates) lineages [6]. Serotonin is anevolutionarily ancient molecule [7] that has beenimplicated in regulating both invertebrate [8] andvertebrate [9] social behaviors, raising the possibil-ity that this neurotransmitter’s prosocial functionsmay be conserved across evolution. Members ofthe order Octopoda are predominantly asocialand solitary [10]. Although at this time it isunknown whether serotonergic signaling systemsare functionally conserved in octopuses, etholog-ical studies indicate that agonistic behaviors aresuspended during mating [11–13], suggesting thatneural mechanisms subserving social behaviorsexist in octopuses but are suppressed outsidethe reproductive period. Here we provide evidencethat, as in humans, the phenethylamine (+/�)-3,4-methylendioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) en-hances acute prosocial behaviors in Octopusbimaculoides. This finding is paralleled by theevolutionary conservation of the serotonin trans-porter (SERT, encoded by the Slc6A4 gene) bindingsite of MDMA in the O. bimaculoides genome.Taken together, these data provide evidencethat the neural mechanisms subserving socialbehaviors exist in O. bimaculoides and indicatethat the role of serotonergic neurotransmissionin regulating social behaviors is evolutionarilyconserved.

RESULTS

Recently, we completed whole-genome sequencing and assem-

bly in Octopus bimaculoides [14]. Because octopuses are

thought to be the most behaviorally advanced invertebrates,

this resource enables testing of molecular homology for complex

behaviors, despite anatomical differences in brain organization

across evolutionarily distant lineages [15]. In this context, it is

interesting to note that in vertebrates (humans and rodents),

the phenethylamine (+/�)-3,4-methylendioxymethamphetamine

(MDMA) is known for its powerful prosocial properties [16–18].

Furthermore, the sixth trans-membrane domain (TM6) of the se-

rotonin transporter (SERT), encoded by the SLC6A4 gene [19],

has been identified as the principle binding site of MDMA

[20–22]. To determine whether this binding site is conserved in

O. bimaculoides, we performed a molecular phylogenetic anal-

ysis of the solute carrier (SLC) 6A subfamily of neurotransmitter

transporter proteins [23] across selected diverse taxa (Tables

S1 and S2). Blasting a reference gene set against 21 proteomes

resulted in a final set of 503 sequences. These sequences were

used to generate a maximum-likelihood phylogenetic tree

(RAxML’s ‘‘best tree’’) for SLC6A across all 21 species (Fig-

ure 1A; Figures S1, S2B, and S2C), along with 156 bootstrap

trees and a bootstrap ‘‘consensus tree’’ (Figure S2A). The

‘‘best tree’’ included the full set of human SLC6A genes and

no additional human genes from other SLC families (Fig-

ure 1B; Figures S2B and S2C; Table S3). Expected SLC6A4

family members were found for the fruit fly (Drosophila mela-

nogaster), the worm (Caenorhabditis elegans), and all vertebrate

species tested, although the ‘‘best tree’’ (Figure 1B) and the

‘‘consensus tree’’ (Figure S2A) differed slightly for theworm. Sur-

prisingly, two copies of SLC6A4 were found in mollusks,

including O. bimaculoides. One copy in octopuses, named

here Slc6a4-(1) (protein Ocbimv22009795m.p; gene model

Ocbimv22008529m.g) was part of a clade that contained only

mollusks. The second copy, named here Slc6a4-(2) (protein

Ocbimv22009795m.p; gene model Ocbimv22009795m.g), was

in a group that included diverse species, like the fly and worm,

but no mammals, vertebrates, or other deuterostomes. It is un-

clear whether this duplication is a molluscan innovation or has

more ancient origins in the Lophotrochozoa. Two and three

copies of SLC6A4 were present in the zebrafish (Danio rerio)

and zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata), respectively, but only a

single copy was present in mammals and in basal-branching

Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd. 1

Page 3: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

A B

Figure 1. Phylogenetic Trees of SLC6A and SLC6A4 Gene Families

(A and B) Maximum-likelihood trees of SLC6A transporters (A) and SLC6A4 serotonin transporters (B) in select taxa. Species are mapped to tree and protein

identifiers in Table S3. For a larger version of (A), see Figure S1.

(A) A maximum-likelihood ‘‘best tree’’ for the SLC6A gene family. The maximum-likelihood tree produced by RAxML includes 503 proteins and 21 species, with

tree building based on a MAFFT alignment of full-length sequences.

(B) The SLC6A4 gene family, a subtree of the maximum-likelihood ‘‘best tree’’ in (A).

See also Figures S1–S3 and Tables S1–S3.

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

deuterostomes. These patterns may reflect differential loss of

SLC6A4 after two rounds of vertebrate genome duplication

[24], with a single copy in the ancestral vertebrate, and losses

of three copies in mammals, two copies in fish, and one copy

in birds (Figure 1B).

In contrast to the octopus, the honeybee (Apis mellifera), leaf

cutter ant (Atta cephalotes), spider (Stegodyphus mimosarum),

and anemone (Nematostella vectensis) all lacked SLC6A4 ortho-

logs. To understand these losses, we next examined all mono-

amine transporters, including human SLC6A2 (DAT), SLC6A3

(NET), and SLC6A4 (SERT), along with their outgroup, a branch

that includes the fruit fly inebriated (Ine) gene (Figures S1 and

S2). Anemones had multiple paralogs within the Ine clade. Ze-

brafish andmore basal-branching deuterostomes were also pre-

sent, but other vertebrates, including humans, were absent. In

contrast, the anemone was absent in SLC64A and throughout

the monoamine transporter clade. Thus, monoamine trans-

porters may represent an ancient innovation that arose early in

bilaterian evolution, with various ancient and more recent dupli-

cations in different lineages. Representing ecdysozoans, the

honeybee, leaf cutter ant, fruit fly, and worm all had one or

more monoamine transporter proteins outside the SLC6A4 fam-

ily, whereas only the fruit fly and worm had proteins within the

family, suggesting that SLC6A4 has been lost in hymenopteran

insects (ants, bees, wasps, and sawflies). Interestingly, although

many of the invertebrates lacking SLC64A orthologs are euso-

cial, this adaptation is not ubiquitous across all eusocial species,

since the SLC6A4 gene is conserved in the naked mole rat (Het-

erocephalous glaber), which is a eusocial vertebrate. Taken

2 Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018

together, these studies underscore the complexity of mono-

amine transporter evolution in animals and identify clear

orthologs of human SLC6A4 in octopuses.

The binding pocket of SLC6A4 is formed by a subset of 12

transmembrane domains, including TM6 [19]. Previous studies

have revealed an especially important role for the region of

TM6 that spans amino acids 333 to 336 (indicated in green in Fig-

ure 2A), as it provides an overlapping binding pocket for MDMA

and serotonin [20]. Furthermore, residue Ser336 has been impli-

cated in the MDMA-induced conformational change not

observed with serotonin [20]. Significantly, for both octopus

orthologs within this region, there is 100% percent identity

when aligned to human SLC6A4 (Figure 2; Figure S4A). More

generally, many of the domains forming the binding pocket are

highly conserved in comparison to other domains and to the

full-length protein (Figure S4B). For instance, TM6 in Slc6a4-(1)

has 95.7% identity to human SLC6A4, compared to 53.4% for

the whole protein, and TM8 in Slc6a4-(2) has 91.0% identity to

human SLC6A4, compared to 39.0% for the whole protein (Fig-

ure S4B). This work further demonstrates the remarkable con-

servation of transmembrane domains forming the MDMA and

serotonin binding pocket in octopus SLC6A4 paralogs, including

complete conservation of amino acids implicated inMDMAbind-

ing in humans.

In order to experimentally quantify octopus social behaviors,

next we adapted the three-chambered social approach assay

routinely used in rodents [25, 26] for use in O. bimaculoides

[27, 28]. As diagramed in Figure 3A, a glass aquarium partitioned

into three equally sized chambers containing a novel object or a

Page 4: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

A

B

Figure 2. Protein Alignment of O. bimaculoides Slc6a4-(1) to Human SLC6A4

(A) Pairwise protein alignment. Human SLC6A4 transmembrane domain 1–12 annotations are based on Uniprot Topology annotation of protein UniProt: P31645

(SC6A4_HUMAN). The binding pocket region that overlaps serotonin and MDMA binding (green) is based on [20]. Alignment of annotated human SLC6A4 to

octopus Slc6a4-(1) was done by pairwise alignment in Geneious.

(B) Diagram illustrating the structure of MDMA, transmembrane domains of the SLC6A4 protein, and theMDMA binding site (red star). Surprisingly, domain 1 was

absent or largely absent in each of the two octopus paralogs (see also Figure S4); however, the domain (annotate all paralogs directly) was present in both

paralogs for the four other mollusks, suggesting that its absence in octopuses might be a sequencing or assembly artifact of the genome).

See also Figures S1–S3 and Tables S1–S3.

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

novel conspecific (male or female social object) in each of

the lateral chambers, as well as an empty center chamber,

served as the test arena. Social-object animals were restrained

by a perforated plastic container that allowed bidirectional ac-

cess to visual, tactile, and chemosensory cues, which are

thought to convey social information in octopuses [10, 12, 29].

The amount of time subject animals spent freely exploring

each chamber was recorded during 30-min test sessions. As

shown in Figure 3B, when the social object was a female, male

and female subject animals spent significantly more time in the

social chamber compared to the center chamber, whereas

when the social object was a male, subject animals spent signif-

icantly more time in the object chamber compared to the

center chamber. Comparisons between conditions revealed

that the time spent with the novel object was significantly

increased (Figures 3C and 3D) and time spent with the social ob-

ject was significantly decreased (Figures 3G and 3H) when the

social object was a male versus a female. The time spent in

the center chamber was unchanged across conditions (Figures

3E and 3F). These data provide the first quantification of social

approach behavior inO. bimaculoides and demonstrate a signif-

icant preference for interactions with female versus male social

objects.

We next sought to test the functional conservation of MDMA’s

effects in O. bimaculoides. The strongest test of functional con-

servation would be to determine whether MDMA induces social

approach to a social object that is normally aversive. Thus, we

again used the three-chambered social approach task

described above, but this timemeasured approach to amale so-

cial object before and after treatment with MDMA. As diagramed

in Figures 4A and 4B, baseline social approach behaviors were

tested in drug naive subjects in an arena containing a novel ob-

ject and a male social object for 30 min (pre-trial). 5 to 24 hr later,

subject animals were placed in a bath containing MDMA for

10 min, followed by a 20-min saline wash, and were then tested

again for 30 min (post-trial). As shown in Figure 4C, during the

pre-trial, subject animals spent significantly more time in the

object chamber compared to the center chamber, whereas in

the post-trial, subject animals spent significantly more time in

the social chamber compared to the center chamber. Compari-

sons between pre- versus post-MDMA conditions revealed that

the time spent with the social object was significantly increased

after MDMA treatment (Figures 4H and 4I), whereas the time

spent in the object and center chambers was not significantly

different across conditions (Figures 4D–4G). Quantification of

the number of transitions between chambers indicated no

Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018 3

Page 5: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

A B

D

C

F

E

H

G

Figure 3. Social Approach Behaviors to-

ward Male and Female Conspecifics in

O. bimaculoides

(A) Diagram illustrating the three-chambered social

approach assay.

(B) Quantification of time spent in each chambers

during 30-min test sessions (n = 5; two-way

repeated-measures ANOVA: p = 0.0110; post hoc

unpaired t test female social object, social versus

center p = 0.0009, object versus center p = 0.4031;

male social object, social versus center p = 0.1710,

object versus center p = 0.0224).

(C–H) Comparisons between female versus male

social object conditions for each chamber (paired

t test female versus male: novel object chamber

p = 0.0386; social object chamber p = 0.0281;

center chamber p = 0.7544).

Error bars represent the SEM. See also Table S4.

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

significant increase in locomotor activity (Figures 4J and 4K).

Finally, in addition to the quantitative increase in the amount of

time spent in the social chamber after MDMA, we also observed

qualitative changes in social interactions. Specifically, as shown

in Figure 4L, under saline conditions, even when subject animals

spent time in the chamber containing the social object, direct

contact between animals was limited, usually to one extended

arm. In contrast, as shown in Figure 4M, after MDMA treatment,

social interactions were characterized by extensive ventral sur-

face contact, which appeared to be exploratory rather than

aggressive in nature. Taken together, the present studies

demonstrate that the acute prosocial effects of MDMA are

conserved in O. bimaculoides and suggest that this pharmaco-

logical manipulation releases extant, but normally suppressed,

neural mechanisms sub-serving social behaviors.

DISCUSSION

The current studies are the first to examine deep evolution of

SLC6A, monoamine transporters, and the SLC6A4 gene family

across the animal tree of life and across diverse animal genomes

(Figure 1; Figures S1 and S2). Monoamine transporters,

including human SERT, DAT, and NET, appear to be a bilaterian

innovation, suggesting a possible ancient evolutionary role in

nervous system centralization and elaboration, both hallmarks

of the Bilateria, and the families have undergone complex pat-

terns of gene duplication and loss throughout the clade over

time. Phylogenetic analysis revealed clear orthologs of human

SLC6A4 in octopuses, as well as high levels of conservation in

the transmembrane domain and amino acid region critical to

MDMA binding [20]. Interestingly, we found that SLC6A4 is

broadly conserved in the fruit fly, the worm, andmost other bilat-

4 Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018

erian animals but is surprisingly absent in

both of the eusocial hymenopteran in-

sects, the honeybee and leaf cutter ant

(Figure 1; Figures S1 and S2). This

absence raises the possibility that in

these eusocial invertebrates, sociality

evolved convergently utilizing other neu-

rotransmitters or peptide hormones

[30, 31]. Alternatively, it could be that loss of SLC6A4 is a permis-

sive mutation for eusociality; however, the conservation of this

gene in the eusocial naked mole rat (H. glaber) argues against

this interpretation.

The current studies are also the first to experimentally quantify

social approach behaviors in O. bimaculoides (Figures 3A–3K)

and demonstrate that, consistent with previous ethological de-

scriptions [11], this species shows no preference for social

approach to a novel male conspecific (Figures 3A–3H). Never-

theless, somewhat surprisingly, both male and female subjects

did exhibit social approach to a novel female conspecific (Fig-

ures 3A–3H), a finding that may reflect an adaptation of labora-

tory raised animals or an incomplete ethological description of

the full repertoire of social behaviors in the wild [28]. Although

we cannot rule out the possibility that the female versus male so-

cial object preference effect is governed by relative size differ-

ences between subject and social objects, we think this is

unlikely since we observed aversion to a male social object

both when the subject was greater and smaller in size (Table

S4). Because the current study design allowed access to visual,

tactile, and chemosensory cues, the selective contribution of

these sensory systems to detecting male and female social

cues is unknown. Based on previous reports that have impli-

cated each of these sensory systems in the detection of social

cues in octopuses [10, 12, 29], it seems likely that under condi-

tions of selective sensory deprivation, the absence of one mo-

dality would be readily compensated by the presence of others.

Finally, the current studies provide the first functional evidence

that the prosocial effects of MDMA [32] are evolutionarily

conserved in O. bimaculoides (Figure 4). Although we did not

observe a significant increase in locomotor activity after

MDMA, this finding is consistent with the complex locomotor

Page 6: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

B C

A

E

D

G

F

K

J

I

H

L M

Figure 4. Prosocial Effects of MDMA in

O. bimaculoides

(A and B) Diagrams illustrating timeline (A) and

experimental protocol (B) for three-chambered

social approach assay.

(C) Quantification of time spent in each chamber

during 30-min test sessions (n = 4; two-way

repeated-measures ANOVA: p = 0.0157; post hoc

unpaired t test pre, social versus center p = 0.4301,

object versus center p = 0.0175; post, social

versus center p = 0.0190, object versus center

p = 0.1781).

(D–K) Comparisons between pre- versus post-

MDMA-treatment conditions (paired t test pre

versus post, social time p = 0.0274; object time

p = 0.1139; center time p = 0.7658; transitions

p = 0.3993).

(L) Photograph of social interaction under the

saline (pre) condition.

(M) Photograph of social interaction under the

MDMA (post) condition.

Error bars represent the SEM. See also Table S4.

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

profile of MDMA previously reported in rodents [18, 33]. In

addition, MDMAappeared to induce changes in the quality of so-

cial interactions, which are consistent with previously reported

‘‘entactogenic’’ properties of MDMA [34, 35]. Future quantifica-

tion of these aspects of the behavioral response to MDMA in

octopuses will be informative. Based on our ability to induce pro-

social approach behaviors by manipulating serotonergic

signaling, it is tempting to speculate that in octopuses, sociality

is a latent state outside ethologically relevant periods, such

as mating, and is manifested in response to MDMA treatment.

Additionally, the current studies establish the first drug delivery

protocols for behavioral pharmacology experiments in octo-

puses and indicate that effective doses of MDMA are in the

same range as those described for humans and rodents.

Beyond their utility for the current studies, development of this

experimental infrastructure fulfills an unmet need for the field

and will enable future mechanistic studies in octopuses. More-

over, this work demonstrates that medications currently in use

or under investigation in humans [23, 36–38] target a homolo-

gous binding site in O. bimaculoides (Figure 2; Figure S4) and

provides important proof-of-concept data supporting further

development of octopuses as model organisms for translational

research.

STAR+METHODS

Detailed methods are provided in the online version of this paper

and include the following:

d KEY RESOURCES TABLE

d CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

d EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

B Experimental Animals

B Housing and Husbandry

B Ethical Considerations

d METHOD DETAILS

B SLC6 and SLC6A4 phylogenetic analysis

B Octopus SLC6A4 comparative analysis

B Behavioral Analysis

d QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

B Experimental design

d DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY

Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018 5

Page 7: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION

Supplemental Information includes four figures and four tables and can be

found with this article online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank members of the Dolen laboratory, R. Caldwell, M. Kuba, and T. Gut-

nick for comments, as well as K. Peramba, M. Renard, K. Dever, D. Calzarette,

L. Menlow, J. Simmons, J. Marvel-Zuccola, J. Miao, M. Cordeiro, P. Newstein,

and T. Sakmar for guidance and assistance in octopus care and D.MarkWelch

and M. Sogin for guidance in the phylogenetic analysis. MDMA was a gift of

R. Doblin (Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies, MAPS). This

work was supported by grants from the Kinship Foundation, Hartwell

Foundation, and Klingenstein-Simons Foundation (G.D.) and the Vetlesen

Foundation (E.E.)

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

G.D. and E.E. designed the study, interpreted results, and wrote the paper.

G.D. and E.E. performed behavioral and pharmacological experiments. E.E.

performed phylogenetic analysis and octopus culturing. G.D. performed sta-

tistical analysis. Both authors edited the paper.

DECLARATION OF INTERESTS

The authors declare no competing interests.

Received: May 7, 2018

Revised: June 25, 2018

Accepted: July 25, 2018

Published: September 20, 2018

REFERENCES

1. Kroger, B., Vinther, J., and Fuchs, D. (2011). Cephalopod origin and evo-

lution: a congruent picture emerging from fossils, development and mole-

cules: extant cephalopods are younger than previously realised and were

under major selection to become agile, shell-less predators. BioEssays

33, 602–613.

2. Kocot, K.M., Cannon, J.T., Todt, C., Citarella, M.R., Kohn, A.B., Meyer, A.,

Santos, S.R., Schander, C., Moroz, L.L., Lieb, B., and Halanych, K.M.

(2011). Phylogenomics reveals deep molluscan relationships. Nature

477, 452–456.

3. Shomrat, T., Turchetti-Maia, A.L., Stern-Mentch, N., Basil, J.A., and

Hochner, B. (2015). The vertical lobe of cephalopods: an attractive brain

structure for understanding the evolution of advanced learning and mem-

ory systems. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol.

201, 947–956.

4. Shigeno, S., Parnaik, R., Albertin, C.B., and Ragsdale, C.W. (2015).

Evidence for a cordal, not ganglionic, pattern of cephalopod brain neuro-

genesis. Zoological Lett. 1, 26.

5. Liebeskind, B.J., Hofmann, H.A., Hillis, D.M., and Zakon, H.H. (2017).

Evolution of animal neural systems. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 48,

377–398.

6. Nowak, M.A., Tarnita, C.E., and Wilson, E.O. (2010). The evolution of eu-

sociality. Nature 466, 1057–1062.

7. Azmitia, E.C. (2001). Modern views on an ancient chemical: serotonin ef-

fects on cell proliferation, maturation, and apoptosis. Brain Res. Bull. 56,

413–424.

8. Anstey, M.L., Rogers, S.M., Ott, S.R., Burrows, M., and Simpson, S.J.

(2009). Serotonin mediates behavioral gregarization underlying swarm for-

mation in desert locusts. Science 323, 627–630.

9. Dolen, G., Darvishzadeh, A., Huang, K.W., and Malenka, R.C. (2013).

Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens

oxytocin and serotonin. Nature 501, 179–184.

6 Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018

10. Scheel, D., Godfrey-Smith, P., and Lawrence, M. (2016). Signal use by oc-

topuses in agonistic interactions. Curr. Biol. 26, 377–382.

11. Huffard, C.L. (2013). Cephalopod neurobiology: an introduction for biolo-

gists working in other model systems. Invert. Neurosci. 13, 11–18.

12. Mohanty, S., Ojanguren, A.F., and Fuiman, L.A. (2014). Aggressive male

mating behavior depends on female maturity in Octopus bimaculoides.

Mar. Biol. 161, 1521–1530.

13. Wells, M.J., and Wells, J. (1972). Sexual displays and mating of Octopus

vulgaris Cuvier and O. cyanea Gray and attempts to alter performance

by manipulating the glandular condition of the animals. Anim. Behav. 20,

293–308.

14. Albertin, C.B., Simakov, O., Mitros, T., Wang, Z.Y., Pungor, J.R., Edsinger-

Gonzales, E., Brenner, S., Ragsdale, C.W., and Rokhsar, D.S. (2015). The

octopus genome and the evolution of cephalopod neural and morpholog-

ical novelties. Nature 524, 220–224.

15. Albertin, C.B., Bonnaud, L., Brown, C.T., Crookes-Goodson, W.J., da

Fonseca, R.R., Di Cristo, C., Dilkes, B.P., Edsinger-Gonzales, E.,

Freeman, R.M., Jr., Hanlon, R.T., et al. (2012). Cephalopod genomics: a

plan of strategies and organization. Stand. Genomic Sci. 7, 175–188.

16. Shulgin, A.T. (1986). The background and chemistry of MDMA.

J. Psychoactive Drugs 18, 291–304.

17. Schmid, Y., Hysek, C.M., Simmler, L.D., Crockett, M.J., Quednow, B.B.,

and Liechti, M.E. (2014). Differential effects of MDMA and methylpheni-

date on social cognition. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxford) 28, 847–856.

18. Curry, D.W., Young,M.B., Tran, A.N., Daoud, G.E., andHowell, L.L. (2018).

Separating the agony from ecstasy: R(-)-3,4-methylenedioxymetham-

phetamine has prosocial and therapeutic-like effects without signs of

neurotoxicity in mice. Neuropharmacology 128, 196–206.

19. Torres, G.E., Gainetdinov, R.R., and Caron, M.G. (2003). Plasma mem-

brane monoamine transporters: structure, regulation and function. Nat.

Rev. Neurosci. 4, 13–25.

20. Field, J.R., Henry, L.K., and Blakely, R.D. (2010). Transmembrane domain

6 of the human serotonin transporter contributes to an aqueously acces-

sible binding pocket for serotonin and the psychostimulant 3,4-methylene

dioxymethamphetamine. J. Biol. Chem. 285, 11270–11280.

21. Hagino, Y., Takamatsu, Y., Yamamoto, H., Iwamura, T., Murphy, D.L., Uhl,

G.R., Sora, I., and Ikeda, K. (2011). Effects ofMDMAon extracellular dopa-

mine and serotonin levels in mice lacking dopamine and/or serotonin

transporters. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 9, 91–95.

22. Rudnick, G., andWall, S.C. (1992). Themolecularmechanism of ‘‘ecstasy’’

[3,4-methylenedioxy-methamphetamine (MDMA)]: serotonin transporters

are targets for MDMA-induced serotonin release. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.

USA 89, 1817–1821.

23. Kristensen, A.S., Andersen, J., Jørgensen, T.N., Sørensen, L., Eriksen, J.,

Loland, C.J., Strømgaard, K., and Gether, U. (2011). SLC6 neurotrans-

mitter transporters: structure, function, and regulation. Pharmacol. Rev.

63, 585–640.

24. Dehal, P., and Boore, J.L. (2005). Two rounds of whole genome duplica-

tion in the ancestral vertebrate. PLoS Biol. 3, e314.

25. Williams, J.R., Carter, C.S., and Insel, T. (1992). Partner preference devel-

opment in female prairie voles is facilitated by mating or the central infu-

sion of oxytocin. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 652, 487–489.

26. Moy, S.S., Nadler, J.J., Perez, A., Barbaro, R.P., Johns, J.M., Magnuson,

T.R., Piven, J., and Crawley, J.N. (2004). Sociability and preference for so-

cial novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like

behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav. 3, 287–302.

27. Hanlon, R.T., and Forsythe, J.W. (1985). Advances in the laboratory culture

of octopuses for biomedical research. Lab. Anim. Sci. 35, 33–40.

28. Cigliano, J.A. (1993). Dominance and den use in Octopus bimaculoides.

Anim. Behav. 46, 677–684.

29. Walderon, M.D., Nolt, K.J., Haas, R.E., Prosser, K.N., Holm, J.B., Nagle,

G.T., and Boal, J.G. (2011). Distance chemoreception and the detection

of conspecifics in Octopus bimaculoides. J. Molluscan Stud. 77, 309–311.

Page 8: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

30. Gospocic, J., Shields, E.J., Glastad, K.M., Lin, Y., Penick, C.A., Yan, H.,

Mikheyev, A.S., Linksvayer, T.A., Garcia, B.A., Berger, S.L., et al. (2017).

The neuropeptide corazonin controls social behavior and caste identity

in ants. Cell 170, 748–759.e12.

31. Stafflinger, E., Hansen, K.K., Hauser, F., Schneider, M., Cazzamali, G.,

Williamson, M., and Grimmelikhuijzen, C.J.P. (2008). Cloning and identifi-

cation of an oxytocin/vasopressin-like receptor and its ligand from insects.

Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 3262–3267.

32. Shulgin, A.A.T., Nichols, E., and Nichols, D.E. (1978). Characterization of

three new psychotomimetics. Pharmacol. Hallucinog. 2, 74–83.

33. Huang, P.K., Aarde, S.M., Angrish, D., Houseknecht, K.L., Dickerson, T.J.,

and Taffe, M.A. (2012). Contrasting effects of d-methamphetamine,

3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine, 3,4-methylenedioxypyrovaler-

one, and 4-methylmethcathinone on wheel activity in rats. Drug Alcohol

Depend. 126, 168–175.

34. Nichols, D.E. (1986). Differences between the mechanism of action of

MDMA, MBDB, and the classic hallucinogens. Identification of a new ther-

apeutic class: entactogens. J. Psychoactive Drugs 18, 305–313.

35. Dumont, G.J.H., and Verkes, R.J. (2006). A review of acute effects

of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine in healthy volunteers.

J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxford) 20, 176–187.

36. Yazar-Klosinski, B.B., and Mithoefer, M.C. (2017). Potential Psychiatric

Uses for MDMA. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 101, 194–196.

37. Perez-Caballero, L., Torres-Sanchez, S., Bravo, L., Mico, J.A., and

Berrocoso, E. (2014). Fluoxetine: a case history of its discovery and pre-

clinical development. Expert Opin. Drug Discov. 9, 567–578.

38. Pramod, A.B., Foster, J., Carvelli, L., and Henry, L.K. (2013). SLC6 trans-

porters: structure, function, regulation, disease association and therapeu-

tics. Mol. Aspects Med. 34, 197–219.

39. Alon, S., Garrett, S.C., Levanon, E.Y., Olson, S., Graveley, B.R., Rosenthal,

J.J.C., and Eisenberg, E. (2015). The majority of transcripts in the

squid nervous system are extensively recoded by A-to-I RNA editing.

eLife 4, 1–17.

40. Katoh, K., and Standley, D.M. (2013). MAFFTmultiple sequence alignment

software version 7: improvements in performance and usability. Mol. Biol.

Evol. 30, 772–780.

41. Castresana, J. (2000). Selection of conserved blocks from multiple align-

ments for their use in phylogenetic analysis. Mol. Biol. Evol. 17, 540–552.

42. Stamatakis, A. (2014). RAxML version 8: a tool for phylogenetic analysis

and post-analysis of large phylogenies. Bioinformatics 30, 1312–1313.

43. Miller, M.A., Pfeiffer, W., and Schwartz, T. (2010). Creating the CIPRES

Science Gateway for inference of large phylogenetic trees. In 2010

Gateway Computing Environments Workshop (GCE), pp. 1–8. https://

doi.org/10.1109/GCE.2010.5676129.

44. Marchler-Bauer, A., Bo, Y., Han, L., He, J., Lanczycki, C.J., Lu, S., Chitsaz,

F., Derbyshire, M.K., Geer, R.C., Gonzales, N.R., et al. (2017). CDD/

SPARCLE: functional classification of proteins via subfamily domain archi-

tectures. Nucleic Acids Res. 45 (D1), D200–D203.

45. Kalivas, P.W., Duffy, P., and White, S.R. (1998). MDMA elicits behavioral

and neurochemical sensitization in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 18,

469–479.

46. Butler-Struben, H.M., Brophy, S.M., Johnson, N.A., and Crook, R.J.

(2018). In vivo recording of neural and behavioral correlates of anesthesia

induction, reversal, and euthanasia in cephalopod molluscs. Front.

Physiol. 9, 1–18.

47. Moore, J.W., and Cole, K.S. (1960). Resting and action potentials of the

squid giant axon in vivo. J. Gen. Physiol. 43, 961–70.

48. Concheiro, M., Baumann, M.H., Scheidweiler, K.B., Rothman, R.B.,

Marrone, G.F., and Huestis, M.A. (2014). Nonlinear pharmacokinetics of

(+/-)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) and its pharmacody-

namic consequences in the rat. Drug Metab. Dispos. 42, 119–125.

49. da Silva, D.D., Silva, E., Carvalho, F., and Carmo, H. (2014). Mixtures of

3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) and its major humanme-

tabolites act additively to induce significant toxicity to liver cells when

combined at low, non-cytotoxic concentrations. J. Appl. Toxicol. 34,

618–627.

Current Biology 28, 1–7, October 8, 2018 7

Page 9: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

STAR+METHODS

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER

Chemicals, Peptides, and Recombinant Proteins

(+/�)-3,4-MDMA Organix (Woburn, MA) (+/�)-3,4-MDMA

Instant Ocean Sea Salt PetCo SKU77763

CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to andwill be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Gul Dolen

([email protected]).

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

Experimental AnimalsOctopus bimaculoides, the California Two-Spot Octopus, was used for all behavioral experiments. A wild female octopus brooding

developing embryos was commercially collected in the Los Angeles, CA area in accordance with all applicable California State and

US Fish and Wildlife regulations, and live shipped to the Marine Biological Laboratory in Woods Hole, MA. After hatching, animals

were housed together for roughly 2-3 weeks in tanks containing hundreds of other animals. Following this brief period in early devel-

opment, each animal was housed in social isolation for a period of 7 months prior to testing. 7 siblings from the cohort were used in

experiments here. Table S4 details the sex and weight for each animal, as well as which animals were used for each experiment.

Housing and HusbandryAnimals were housed in a custom-built partially recirculating aquaria system, with natural heated seawater (20-23�C) pooled in a

sump tank and shared between individual 18 and 36 L tanks. Each tank exchanged 100 L of recirculating seawater or more per

hour, while the system exchanged 500 L or more of fresh seawater per day. Tank sides were covered 3/4 their length with translucent

plasticboard to allow animals to self-isolate visually from neighboring tanks. Early hatchling stages were group cultured and select

sibling juveniles moved to individual tanks after a month. Each animal was housed individually (but with shared seawater) for the

remainder of the project, outside the experiments. The room housing the animals maintained seasonal light cycles (12:12 or 14:10

light:dark cycles). Tank environments were enriched with clay pot dens and sand or rock substrate. Post-hatching stages were

fed a daily diet of live crabs and snails. Sexually mature animals at around 9 months post-hatching were shipped to Johns Hopkins

University for behavioral pharmacology experiments. Animals were maintained at Johns Hopkins University for several days in

buckets with artificial seawater (Instant Ocean), air, dens, live food, and with water changes made twice daily. For the current studies

we did not continuously monitor organic waste products because previous pilot studies–conducted in animals that were in the same

weight range, living in buckets identical to those used in the current study, and had been fed 4 hr before–have shown that pH, nitrites,

and nitrates did not change appreciably over a 12 hr period (which is the longest interval the animals in the current studies went

without a water change). In one animal, ammonia levels did change from 0 to 2 ppm (on a scale of 0-8), but this amount was deemed

acceptable for short term culturing. Monitoring was conducted with aMarine Saltwater Master Test Kit (API Marine). Room lighting at

Johns Hopkins meant that animals had around 9 hr of dark each night and room temperature was 22-23�C. Animals were inspected

hourly ormore for evidence of wounds or disease, and their behavior generally monitored for signs of stress and agitation. Respiration

rates and signs of excess agitation or decline were closely monitored during and after drug treatments. Aggressive interactions were

also closely monitored for when more than one animal shared the experimental tank.

Ethical ConsiderationsCare of invertebrates, like O. bimaculoides, does not fall under United States Animal Welfare Act regulation, and is omitted from the

PHS-NIH ‘‘Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.’’ Thus, an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, a Committee

on Ethics for Animal Experiments, or other granting authority does not formally review and approve experimental procedures on and

care of invertebrate species, like O. bimaculoides, at the Marine Biological Laboratory. However, in accordance with Marine Biolog-

ical Laboratory Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines for invertebrates, our care and use ofO. bimaculoides at the

Marine Biological Laboratory and at Johns Hopkins University generally followed tenets prescribed by the Animal Welfare Act,

including the three ‘Rs’ (refining, replacing, and reducing unnecessary animal research).

e1 Current Biology 28, 1–7.e1–e4, October 8, 2018

Page 10: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

METHOD DETAILS

SLC6 and SLC6A4 phylogenetic analysisTo identify orthologs of SLC6A4 in octopus, and to broadly characterize monoamine transporter evolution in animals, a SLC6 refer-

ence gene set was made using nineteen SLC6 proteins from human [23], in addition to six from fly and three from worm based on all

fly and worm orthologs identified for the human genes in Ensembl (version 91). A more recent phylogenetic analysis of SLC6A in hu-

man lists 21 SLC6A genes [38]. However, the study also indicates SLC6A21 is a pseudogene. Furthermore, the study includes

SLC6A10 but Ensembl lists SLC6A10 is a pseudogene, and neither appear to be part of the current proteome for human. Thus,

we focus on the original 19 genes indicated by Kristensen 2011 [23]. All reference sequences were downloaded fromUniProt (version

2018_01; Table S1). Twenty-one species representingmolluscan diversity or that aremodels for studying sociality and that have pub-

lished genomes were identified and their proteomes downloaded from Ensembl (version 91) or National Center for Biotechnology

Center (NCBI) Genome (February 2018) (Table S2). Isoforms were collapsed to the longest protein per gene using a custom Python

script, and local Blast databases were built per species proteome usingmakeblastdb in the BLAST+ package (version 2.7.1). In addi-

tion, five published transcriptomes of different brain regions in squid [39], which still lacks a publicly available genome, were down-

loaded from the Living in an Ivory Basement blog (http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html), combined into a

single transcriptome, and translated to protein sequence with removal of shorter isoforms per default settings of EvidentialGene

(http://arthropods.eugenes.org/EvidentialGene/). To determine BLAST+ blastp settings that best capture the diversity of SLC6A4

genes, whileminimizing the introduction of excess genes from outside the SLC6 family, a series of blasts jobswere run under different

settings for ‘-maxhits’ and ‘-e-val’ (5, e10-5; 10, e10-5; 25, e10-5; 25, e-25; 100, e10-5; 100, e-25). The output was analyzed to see if

the complete human SLC6A gene set was returned and howmany unique hits were found across all proteomes, assuming that both a

full human SLC6A gene set with no additional human genes and a minimal number of total hits was ideal. For optimized blastp set-

tings (-maxhits 25 and -eval e10-5), inspection of human sequences revealed 21 proteins, two more than expected for the human

SLC6A gene subfamily. However, two proteins (ENSP000004795971 and ENSP000004800791) placedwith other humanSLC6Apro-

teins (SLC6A3 and SLC6A8, respectively) in initial phylogenetic trees, and while the genes were similarly annotated as SLC6A3 and

SLC6A8 by Ensembl, they were also flagged as potential assembly artifacts (no ungapped mapping and no stable id for the genes in

GRCh37). Based on these findings, the two sequences were removed from the gene set prior to final alignment. The final set of 503

sequences identified under optimized blastp settings was aligned using Geneious (version 11.03) MAFFT (version 7.338; algorithm

auto, gap open penalty 1.53, offset value 0.123, gap matrix BLOSUM 62) [40]. Alignments were run through GBlocks (version 0.91b;

online server http://molevol.cmima.csic.es/castresana/Gblocks_server.html; default settings except allow smaller final blocks yes,

allow gap positions within final blocks yes, and allow less strict flanking positions yes) to restrict alignments to highly conserved re-

gions [41]. Models of protein evolution were determined for full-length and GBlock alignments using ModelTest-NG (version 0.1.3;

default RAxML settings) and the best model used in tree building. RAxML-HPCBlackbox (version 8.2.10; default settings except Pro-

tein Substitution Matrix LG, empirical base frequencies yes, find best tree using maximum likelihood search yes) [42] on the CIPRES

Science Gateway [43] was used to build trees for different sets of species and with or without GBlocks to explore the robustness of

different trees and datasets. A consensus tree of resulting bootstrap trees wasmade using Geneious (version 11.03) Consensus Tree

Builder (Support Threshold 0%). Resulting trees were visually assessed in Geneious (version 11.03) and FigTree (version 1.4.3). The

tree built from full-length alignment and the gene set using blastp settings of -maxseq 25 and -eval e10-5 was used for the final anal-

ysis and all figures. For domain analysis, sequence annotation and pairwise alignments of proteins and domains were made using

Geneious (version 11.03).

Octopus SLC6A4 comparative analysisDomain annotation of human SLC6A4 was based on Uniprot and NCBI Conserved Domains CDSEARCH/cdd v3.16 [44]. Pairwise

protein alignments of annotated human SLC6A4 were made to each of the octopus SCL6A4 genes using Geneious. Each domain

alignment was then extracted and the percent identify per protein and per domain determined.

Behavioral AnalysisThe social testing apparatus, illustrated in Figure 3A, was a rectangular, glass aquarium (76 X 30 X 30 cm, 68.4 L), separated into three

equal sized chambers using black Plexiglas dividing walls, with small circular openings (3.5 cm in diameter) allowing access into each

chamber. The outer walls of the chamber were covered with blackout window film (WindowWhirl). Artificial seawater (Instant Ocean)

was prepared per instructions from the manufacturing company, and the social testing apparatus filled to several centimeters from

the top. Seawater osmolarity was measured using a micro osmometer (Precision Systems), and adjusted to 925 milliosmoles/liter

(mOsM/L). Fresh seawater was prepared for each experiment, with the tank scrubbed in deionized water per seawater change.

A single air stone was placed to the side of the center of the tank and generated mild local currents. Otherwise, water in the tank

was stagnant.

Octopuses were tested in the social approach and MDMA experiments described below. For the social approach task (Figures 3

and 4), the subject octopus was first placed in the center chamber inside a 946 mL perforated screw top plastic cylindrical bottle

(Pinnacle Mercantile), weighted down with a 450 g C-shaped lead flask ring (VWR) secured with Wet-Surface & Underwater Setting

Epoxy (Hardman). After a two-minute habituation period, subject animals were released from the screw top container, and allowed to

freely explore all three chambers for 30-min test sessions. The 30-min interaction time was determined empirically in pilot studies,

Current Biology 28, 1–7.e1–e4, October 8, 2018 e2

Page 11: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

which revealed that octopuses, like mice, exhibit maximal exploratory behavior in the first 15-20 min, followed by increasing levels of

quiescence (as measured by a decline in the number of chamber transitions). An unfamiliar octopus was placed in one of the side

chambers (social object). The social object was enclosed in a weighted orchid pot or screw top plastic bottle, which allowed contact,

but prevented fighting. Novel objects consisted of multiple configurations of 4 objects: 1) plastic orchid pot with red weight, 2) plastic

bottle with green weight, 3) Galactic Heroes ‘Stormtrooper’ figurine, and 4) Galactic Heroes ‘Chewbacca’ figurine. In all cases the

novel object included either the orchid pot or the bottle in order to keep the overall size of objects constant across experimental con-

ditions. These containers contained one of the two figurines, which were selected to be maximally different in terms of color (dark

brown versus white), but were the same size (50mm height, 25mmwidth) andmaterial (plastic, containing no shiny or metallic parts).

No obvious novel object preference bias was observed. The amount of time spent in each chamber and the number of entries into

each chamber were manually scored by a single observer sitting at a central position, within one meter of the tank. The observer was

visible to the subject animal only from the top of the tank, since all other walls were covered with a light-blocking sheet. The test arena

was located in the corner of a larger laboratory space, so care was taken to restrict access to the immediate area (5-10 m) surround-

ing the test arena during behavioral testing. Entry was defined as both eyes and the mantle in one chamber.

Pharmacology

For subject animals 1,4, and 7 the interval between the pre- and post-test was 24 hr. For the fourth subject (animal number 5), the

interval between pre- and post-test was 5 hr. This difference was due to the fact that animal number 5 was used as the social object

for other experiments, andwas not tested under ‘pre’ conditions until all other non-drug experiments were concluded. Subject animal

5’s behavior was not appreciably different from animals 1, 4, and 7 with respect to either pre (saline) or post (MDMA) response, and

formal quantification using the Shapiro-Wilk test revealed no statistical justification for excluding this animal (data normally distrib-

uted, no outliers).

The phenethylamine, (+/�)-3,4-methylendioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) was obtained from Organix Inc (Woburn, MA, Gift of

Rick Doblin, Multidisciplinary Association For Psychedelic studies, MAPS). Previous reports indicate that MDMA’s effects are maxi-

mally manifest 30-60min following drug application [18, 45], thereforeMDMAwas delivered for 10min, followed by a 20min washout

period before beginning the 30 min test session. Subject animals weighed between 130 to 200 g (Table S4), and were administered

MDMA, by placing the animals in a beaker containing MDMA dissolved in 600 mL artificial seawater. Drug delivery by submersion is

an established protocol for marine species including octopuses [46], whereby the drug enters the bloodstream directly through the

gills [47]. Since the gills in amarine animal are analogous to the lungs in a land animal, a 10-minMDMA submersion for octopus would

theoretically be the equivalent of continuously inhaled MDMA for 10 min in humans. In ongoing human clinical trials, MDMA is deliv-

ered orally (per os, p.o.) in the range of 0.67-2 mg/Kg (assuming an average adult human weighs 60 Kg). Since drug absorption

following the p.o. route of administration is less efficacious than the inhaled route, for the current studies octopuses were given

MDMA submersion doses between 0.5-0.005 mg/Kg, which corresponds to the low end of the oral dose range effective in humans.

In addition, pilot studies in 3 animals indicated that higher submersion doses of MDMA (ranging from 10-400 mg/Kg) induced severe

behavioral changes (e.g., hyper or depressed ventilation, traveling color waves across the skin or blanching, as well as catatonia or

hyper-arousal/vigilance) and these animals were excluded from further analysis. In rodents, previous studies have demonstrated that

the pharmacokinetics of MDMA are nonlinear, such that its metabolites interfere with the body’s ability to metabolize MDMA, result-

ing in additive effects that can lead to hepatotoxicity even with low-concentration repetitive dosing [48, 49]. Thus, here we only exam-

ined the acute effects ofMDMA in drug naive animals, and each subject only received a single dose ofMDMA during the course of the

experiments. Animals were returned to the aquarium system at theMarine Biological Laboratory following the experiments described

here and were used in other unrelated research projects.

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Global comparisons between experimental manipulations were made using 2x3 2-factor ANOVA with repeated-measures on both

factors. The 2-factor repeated-measures ANOVA was chosen because it is robust to violations of normality, and we are unaware

of any existing non-parametric test that can be used with a 2-factor study design. Furthermore, in order to determine the appropri-

ateness of using parametric statistics for this dataset, we have formally calculated whether our data meet statistical criteria for

normal distribution using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Out of 14 experimental groups, the data is normally distributed for all groups, and

contained no outliers. For post hoc comparisons, two-tailed, Student’s t test (paired or unpaired, as appropriate) were used. For

all statistical tests, a p value < 0.05 was considered significant. Statistical parameters (n values, SEM, p values) are reported in

the Figure legends.

Experimental designIn order tomaximize data robustness, behavioral quantification was restricted to objectivemeasures including time spent in chamber

and number of transitions and strict scoring criteria were applied (e.g., chamber transition defined as both eyes and mantle

passing through the opening). Because the scoring was carried out by an unblinded observer, care was taken to extensively

cross-validate the scorer’s inter-rater reliability against automated measures (e.g., infrared beam breaks, Med Associates) in

mice. Since mice exhibit greater locomotor activity compared to octopuses, and are therefore more difficult to score, we are

confident in the observer’s ability to accurately score the time spent in each chamber for octopus. An animal was excluded from

analysis if it failed to enter all three chambers at least once during the defined test period. In order to control for any side preference

e3 Current Biology 28, 1–7.e1–e4, October 8, 2018

Page 12: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Please cite this article in press as: Edsinger and Dolen, A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus,Current Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.061

bias, the social object and novel object positions were held constant within an experiment, and only counterbalanced between ex-

periments. No obvious side preference bias was observed. For qualitative descriptions of social behaviors, photographic examples

were provided, and clinical criteria were applied [34, 35]. Sample sizes were estimated based on previously published results using

this assay in mice.

DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY

Sequences, alignments, and tree text files and the data used for statistical analyses of behavior are all available in Mendeley Data at

https://doi.org/10.17632/z9t3x4p5kk.1.

Current Biology 28, 1–7.e1–e4, October 8, 2018 e4

Page 13: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Current Biology, Volume 28

Supplemental Information

A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission

in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus

Eric Edsinger and Gül Dölen

Page 14: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000097054

D.re

rio-E

NSDA

RP00

0000

8131

53

A.ca

lifor

nica

-XM

_013

0888

221

R.no

rveg

icus-

ENSR

NOP0

0000

0047

174

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000719295

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000504191D.rerio-ENSDARP000001222691

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22022818mp

H.g

labe

r-EN

SHG

LP00

0000

0835

91

S.pu

rpur

atus

-SPU

_022

382-

tr

H.sapiens-ENSP000003462984

S.mimosarum-KFM77545

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001194111

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000001892

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

0509

0709

2

C.el

egan

s-T2

3G55

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000197351

S.purpuratus-SPU_008617-tr

L.giga

ntea-

Lotgi

P118

867

D.pealeii-SGid17568tr154924

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

0761

58

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000009021

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000190681

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000720801

C.jacchus-E

NSCJAP000000207212

D.mela

noga

ster-F

Bpp0

3099

74

C.vir

gini

ca-X

P_02

2336

5531

S.mim

osarum-KFM

57632

C.virginica-XP_0223459921

A.mellifera-G

B49396-PA

C.ja

cchu

s-EN

SCJA

P000

0002

0611

2

C.elegans-C49C31

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000070881

H.sapiens-ENSP000003386273

M.m

ulatta-ENSMM

UP000000555921

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP81

772

S.mimosarum-KFM73192

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000030711

N.vecte

nsis-EDO39158

A.m

ellife

ra-G

B409

47-P

A

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000701611

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid73161tr8626

L.gigantea-LotgiP52211

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000081201

S.mimosarum-KFM69891

N.vectensis-EDO46589

C.vir

gini

ca-X

P_02

2342

0621

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000155511

S.purpuratus-SPU_011457-tr

C.virginica-XP_0223325901

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000091421

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22014670mp

D.pealeii-OLid53371tr53158

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000086271

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000068921

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000053001

O.bimac

uloide

s-Ocb

imv2

2009

795m

p

A.californica-XM_0130899231N.vectensis-EDO29650

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001172022

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000081726

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001493061

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP1

4132

2

M.m

usculus-ENSMUSP000000324545

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001301061

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid124336tr31090

S.purpuratus-SPU_003437-tr

A.ca

liforn

ica-N

M__00

1204

5731

D.peale

ii-Buc

calG

angli

aid83

691tr

2638

60

A.californica-XM_0050928682

S.purpuratus-SPU_027995-tr

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22014627mp

A.californica-XM_0051087342

L.gi

gant

ea-L

otgi

P103

896

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001316811

C.virginica-XP_0223325891

S.mim

osaru

m-KFM

6988

9

D.melanogaster-FBpp0079130

A.planci

-XP_022

1019

451

L.gigantea-LotgiP126022

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000052818

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_013

0812

951

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

1308

0607

1

N.vectensis-EDO29649

A.califo

rnica

-XM_013

0900

641

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000329964

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000078141

S.mimosarum-KFM61722

M.m

usculus-ENSMUSP000000255207

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

3030

37

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22007590mp

S.purpuratus-SPU_006561-tr

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

0870

80

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000223461

A.planci-XP_0220853321

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

2900

86

L.giga

ntea-

Lotgi

P134

594

C.virginica-XP_0223233651

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

6923

4

D.melanogaster-FBpp0293407

A.mellifera-GB54507-PA

C.virginica-XP_0223253901

C.elegans-M01G55

H.sapiens-ENSP000002666825

A.ca

lifor

nica

-XM

_013

0844

731

C.vir

ginica

-XP_

0223

3963

41

C.virgin

ica-XP_0

2234

5279

1

H.s

apie

ns-E

NSP

0000

0270

3499

D.melanogaster-FBpp0291681

A.californica-XM_0130856071

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

7739

7

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

7284

2

A.planci-XP_0221009831

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

0707

63

A.californica-XM_0050958382

A.planci-XP_0221117191

D.m

elano

gaste

r-FBp

p007

9102

S.mimosarum-KFM58358

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000045642

N.vecte

nsis-EDO40151

C.elegans-Y46G5A30

S.purpuratus-SPU_001698-tr

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000118793

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP1

1826

4

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000032091

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000044141

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000007651

A.mell

ifera-

GB4630

0-PA

N.vectensis-EDO29647

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000070942

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000083593

D.pe

alei

i-OLi

d295

71tr3

3372

4

D.melanogaster-FBpp0088910

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000126672

A.planci-XP_0220989201

D.pealeii-SGid119642tr155193

N.vectensis-EDO28939

C.virgi

nica-X

P_02

2333

7991

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001422421

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000194093

C.elegans-T13B51

A.cephalotes-X

P_0120570371

L.gi

gant

ea-L

otgi

P123

645

N.vectensis-EDO26364

S.purpuratus-SPU_016011-tr

H.sapiens-ENSP000004282001

L.gigantea-LotgiP153071

S.mim

osarum-KFM

67772

A.cephalotes-XP_0120626091

C.elegans-F56F43

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000234921

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001168461

L.gigantea-LotgiP124265

L.gi

gant

ea-L

otgi

P235

858

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000167383

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000061582

D.p

eale

ii-Bu

ccal

Gan

glia

id10

6188

tr164

318

D.p

eale

ii-VL

id57

769t

r280

467

M.m

ulatta-ENSMM

UP000000460991

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

0802

47

A.califo

rnica

-XM_0

0510

5671

2

S.purpuratus-SPU_014876-tr

C.elegans-W03G91

S.mimosarum-KFM74324

D.rerio

-ENSDARP0000

0120

9181

H.sapiens-ENSP000003527024

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP20

5669

D.pe

alei

i-SG

id40

281t

r116

36

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000304991

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001525251C

.virginica-XP_0222861761

S.mim

osaru

m-KFM

7319

4

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

6133

0

O.bim

aculo

ides-

Ocbim

v220

0852

9mp

A.californica-XM_0130905511

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000337527

C.virgin

ica-XP_0

2232

7327

1

N.vectensis-EDO36814

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000036612

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000741281

A.pla

nci-X

P_02

2104

1331

H.sapiens-ENSP000002531225

A.m

ellife

ra-G

B408

67-P

A

H.gla

ber-E

NSHG

LP00

0000

2346

11

N.vectensis-EDO28460

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000903814

A.californica-XM_0130811061

C.virginica-XP_0223436061

D.pe

aleii-O

Lid99

98tr1

5537

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001116362

D.melanogaster-FBpp0289933

N.vectensis-EDO29648

D.melanogaster-FBpp0291680

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000283723

C.vir

gini

ca-X

P_02

2336

5521

H.sapiens-ENSP000003392604

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000143462

O.bimac

uloide

s-Ocb

imv2

2006

761m

p

A.planci-XP_0220910471

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000002271

N.vectensis-EDO43180

S.purpuratus-SPU_001564-tr

A.californica-XM_0130850071

A.planci-XP_0220873691

A.califo

rnica

-XM_013

0792

301

S.purpuratus-SPU_003832-tr

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000188212

C.elegans-Y32F6A2

D.mela

noga

ster-F

Bpp00

7713

6

A.californica-XM_0050935372

L.gigantea-LotgiP120149

C.ja

cchu

s-EN

SCJA

P000

0002

5318

3

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000586995

C.virginica-XP_0223253931

A.mellifera-GB47438-PA

D.melanogaster-FBpp0112979

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2007

737m

p

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000880275

S.purpuratus-SPU_007290-tr

A.pl

anci-

XP_0

2210

8780

1

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000012381

A.planci-XP_0221001801

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001366761

C.vir

gini

ca-X

P_02

2336

5541

A.planci-XP_0221025351

S.purpuratus-SPU_007205b-tr

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000422196

L.gigantea-LotgiP104854

A.californica-XM_0050961432

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

7739

8

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000099157

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000436654

S.purpuratus-SPU_016010-tr

D.pealeii-SGid116315tr85633

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001196581

S.mim

osarum-KFM

67775

D.rerio-EN

SDAR

P000001244521

L.gigantea-LotgiP111250

A.californica-XM_0130863291

M.m

ulatta-ENSMM

UP000000077852

A.ce

phal

otes

-XP_

0120

5854

71

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000079951

C.elegans-T03F71

D.melanogaster-FBpp0079128

A.cephalotes-XP_0120562261

A.planci-XP_0220906201

S.mimosarum-KFM74212

L.giga

ntea-

Lotgi

P104

270

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_013

0804

531

C.elegans-Y43D4A1

T.gut

tata

-ENS

TGUP

0000

0007

7371

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2036

889m

p

T.guttata-ENSTG

UP000000075261

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000651791

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000077793

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000137823

A.planci-XP_0221009841

A.m

ellife

ra-G

B518

33-P

A

N.vectensis-EDO46588

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22000956mp

S.purpuratus-SPU_017499-tr

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

0509

5023

2

N.vecte

nsis-EDO39157

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2014

455m

p

A.califo

rnica

-XM_013

0860

061

A.planci-XP_0220911601

A.pla

nci-X

P_02

2104

1321

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22006176mp

R.norvegicu

s-ENSRNOP000000289172

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22009317mp

S.mim

osaru

m-KFM

7421

1

N.vectensis-EDO36813

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22016401mp

S.mimosarum-KFM69890

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001293791

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000821265

A.cephalotes-XP_0120560001

S.purpu

ratus-

SPU_017

227-t

r

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP11

5105

C.virginica-XP_0222861771

A.planci-XP_0220795311

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000153791

A.cephalotes-XP_0120636361

S.pu

rpur

atus

-SPU

_022

506-

tr

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000360942

L.gigantea-LotgiP209410

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000821325

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid64284tr149070

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001478901

D.re

rio-E

NSD

ARP0

0000

1415

121

A.pla

nci-X

P_02

2096

2691

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000079675

H.sapiens-EN

SP000002198338

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000262738

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_005

0999

672

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000086161A.

ceph

alot

es-X

P_01

2058

5451

D.melanogaster-FBpp0290385

M.m

usculus-ENSM

USP000001298691

C.virginica-XP_0223253921

N.vectensis-EDO49847

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP21

9163

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid80860tr56522utrorf

D.pe

alei

i-OLi

d295

70tr7

0599

H.sapiens-ENSP000004708011

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001047171

D.melanogaster-FBpp0086433

L.gigantea-LotgiP238321

A.mellifera-GB50262-PA

S.mimosarum-KFM75447

A.pl

anci-

XP_0

2209

5732

1

M.musculus-E

NSMUSP000000220485

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000312772

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000164401

A.cephalotes-XP_0120602251

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000003771

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000494227

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000019717

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22000954mp

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000392052

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000083423

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP2

1940

3

H.sapiens-E

NSP0000030530210

C.virginica-XP_0223459931

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22018385mp

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000207382

C.vir

ginica

-XP_

0223

3571

41

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000009601

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2034

899m

p

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

7069

2

S.mimosarum-KFM60691

H.glaber-EN

SHG

LP000000085961

L.gigantea-LotgiP113263

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000043391

S.purpuratus-SPU_019761-tr

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000476906

S.pu

rpur

atus

-SPU

_000

971-

tr

A.ce

phal

otes

-XP_

0120

5855

71

D.pealeii-SGid34283tr372909

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid102553tr74089

S.purpuratus-SPU_009356-tr

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000304342

N.vectensis-EDO49319

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000045731

A.planci-XP_0221001811

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001321841

C.virgi

nica-X

P_022

3247

551

D.pealeii-GFLid82511tr144225

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2034

805m

p

D.pealeii-GFLid66890tr5

2496

C.virginica-XP_0223271391

N.vectensis-EDO49846

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000098202

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22017051mp

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000127488

L.gigantea-LotgiP164761

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000745361

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22024903mp

A.planci-XP_0221001791

D.peale

ii-Buc

calG

angli

aid54

429tr

2228

7

H.sapiens-ENSP000003301993

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000425951

O.bim

aculoides-Ocbim

v22018350mp

S.purpuratus-SPU_003711-tr

D.melanogaster-FBpp0074995

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000003351

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22029337mp

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000887554

C.virginica-XP_0223441391

H.sapiens-ENSP000004343641

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000300094

A.mellifera-GB44121-PA

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000086021

N.vectensis-EDO49218

A.californica-NM__0012047301

S.purpuratus-SPU_010825-tr

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000143423

S.purpuratus-SPU_003713-tr

C.jacchus-EN

SCJAP000000236322

H.sapiens-ENSP000002544882

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

8249

2

C.virginica-XP_0223391851

N.ve

ctens

is-ED

O402

78

A.californica-XM_0130871451

M.m

ulat

ta-E

NSM

MUP

0000

0004

6793

H.sa

pien

s-EN

SP00

0003

8582

22

S.purpuratus-SPU_005942-tr

R.norvegicus-EN

SRN

OP000000222854

M.m

usculus-ENSMUSP000001272891

O.bimac

uloide

s-Ocb

imv2

2001

491m

p

T.gut

tata

-ENS

TGUP

0000

0006

7111

D.pe

aleii-B

ucca

lGan

gliaid

1237

32tr2

0951

3utro

rf

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

1308

0623

1

N.vectensis-EDO32356

T.gut

tata

-ENS

TGUP

0000

0016

3791

D.melanogaster-F

Bpp0311463

A.mellifera-GB54918-PA

M.m

uscu

lus-

ENSM

USP

0000

0022

1006

L.giga

ntea-L

otgiP22

3675

O.b

imac

uloi

des-

Ocb

imv2

2026

818m

p

A.planci-XP_0220848561

H.sapiens-ENSP000004816251

D.pealeii-OLid29402tr10680

A.mellifera-GB51190-PA

C.elegans-F55H121

S.mimosarum-KFM70690

L.gigantea-LotgiP104703

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

0509

6807

2

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000481285

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000324516

M.m

ulatta-ENSM

MU

P000000266443

D.peale

ii-Buc

calG

angli

aid64

283tr

3284

6

N.vectensis-EDO39707

A.ce

phal

otes

-XP_

0120

6212

71

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000865645

D.rerio

-ENSDARP0000

0119

8641

D.pe

alei

i-SG

id40

283t

r217

02

S.mimosarum-KFM67774

C.virgin

ica-XP_0

2232

6233

1

C.virginica-XP_0223390591

A.cephalotes-XP_0120566001D.pe

aleii-O

Lid68

132tr

4594

3

D.m

elan

ogas

ter-F

Bpp0

3078

67

H.sapiens-ENSP000002877664

S.purpuratus-SPU_010145-tr

L.giga

ntea-

Lotgi

P200

147

A.planci-XP_0220911591

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000130221

D.peale

ii-GFL

id516

87tr5

0575

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001444821

C.elegans-ZK10109

A.cephalotes-XP_0120619681

A.planci-XP_0221010251

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000334147

N.vectensis-EDO49707A.ce

phalo

tes-X

P_012

0577

931

A.mellifera-GB44295-PA

S.purpuratus-SPU_002417-tr

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_005

0950

202

S.purpuratus-SPU_008339-tr

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000737045A.californica-XM

_0051019692

S.mimosarum-KFM74769

A.m

ellif

era-

GB5

1844

-PA

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000322009

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000945322

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000149041

A.m

ellife

ra-G

B518

34-P

A

A.m

ellif

era-

GB5

1845

-PA

S.purpuratus-SPU_000076-tr

A.californica-XM_0130834301

O.bim

aculoides-Ocbim

v22022648mp

A.ca

liforn

ica-X

M_0

0509

3222

2

C.elegans-T25B67

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000160761

L.gigantea-LotgiP113527

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000321857

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000752173

N.vecte

nsis-EDO41046

A.ce

phal

otes

-XP_

0120

5986

71

H.sapiens-ENSP000003537912

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22020167mp

A.cephalotes-XP_0120593961

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000070331

N.vectensis-EDO49320

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000021711

M.m

uscu

lus-E

NSM

USP0

0000

1040

392

R.n

orve

gicu

s-EN

SRN

OP0

0000

0707

281

A.planci-XP_0221093761

L.giga

ntea-

Lotgi

P141

305

A.planci-XP_0220989171

N.vectensis-EDO36815

D.melanogaster-FBpp0079124

C.eleg

ans-Y

54E1

0BR7

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000510891H.sapiens-ENSP000003235493

A.californica-XM_0130902021

S.m

imos

arum

-KFM

6101

4

A.mellifera-GB51198-PA

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001230631

S.purpuratus-SPU_017621-tr

C.virginica-XP_0222960631

S.purpuratus-SPU_014977-tr L.gi

gant

ea-L

otgi

P104

165

S.purpuratus-SPU_014948-tr

A.californica-XM_0130860521

D.pe

alei

i-SG

id40

282t

r120

314

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000667797

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000130301

L.gigantea-LotgiP141413

D.re

rio-E

NSDA

RP00

0000

8133

64

N.vectensis-EDO39708

A.cephalotes-XP_0120557421

N.vectensis-ED

O31545

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22014672mp

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000274393

L.gigantea-LotgiP218886

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000716381

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000053111

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000179523

M.m

ulat

ta-E

NSM

MU

P000

0000

6879

3

L.gigantea-LotgiP92276

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22018387mp

SLC6A4SLC6A3

SLC6A2

SLC6A19

SLC6A18

SLC6A20

SLC6A17

SLC6A15SLC6A16

SLC6A14

SLC6A15

SLC6A9SLC6A7

SLC6A1

SLC6A8

SLC6A6

SLC6A11

SLC6A12

SLC6A13

Human

Vertebrata

Deuterostomia

Hymenoptera

Ecdysozoa

Octopus

Mollusca

Cnidaria

Taxa

SLC6A4 family

Monoamine transporters

OutgroupMonoamine transporters SLC6A deep branches lacking vertebrates

Clades

Page 15: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Figure S1. A larger version of Figure 1A with major animal groups highlighted by color. Related to Figure 1. Monoamine transporters, including SLC6A4, have undergone complex patterns of evolution in diverse animal groups. Two copies are found in octopus and all mollusc genomes examined, except the sea slug (Aplysia californica), which lacked Slc6a4-(2), an absence that may reflect incompleteness of its current genome assembly. Spider proteins were often on long-branches and placed to the outside of clades, or were embedded within non-arthropod clades, throughout the SLC6A tree (Figure 1A, S1, and S2), and their highly divergent sequences remain difficult to interpret phylogenetically. Still, there are likely monoamine transporters in spider, even if their exact affinity or origins remains unresolved. Non-SLC6A4 monoamine transporters appear to have duplicated in the ancestral bilaterian. One copy was lost in vertebrates (Clade A) but the second copy (Clade B) was duplicated in vertebrates (Clades C (SLC6A2) and D (SLC6A3)) perhaps through gene retention following the first round of vertebrate genome duplication. Consistent with previous reports [S1] zebra finch lacked DAT orthologs. A number of deep branches lacking vertebrate orthologs clustered at the base of the SLC6A tree and may include non-SLC6A proteins but this requires further analysis of SLC evolution.

Page 16: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

A. planchi

H. sapiens

A. californica

C. virginica (1)

M. mulatta

R. norvegicus

C. virginica (2)

D. melanogaster

D. pealeii (1)

L. gigantea (2)

T. guttata (1)T. guttata (2)

L. gigantea (1)

O. bimaculoides (1)

O. bimaculoides (2)

T. guttata (3)D. rerio (1)

H. glaber

D. pealeii (2)

C. elegans

M. musculus

S. purpuratus

D. rerio (2)

C. jacchus

46.15

100

100

72.44

97.44

99.36

57.69100

23.72

49.36

25.64

97.44

99.3692.31

73.08

37.18

58.33

100

100

69.87

100

100

33.97

HumanVertebrataDeuterostomia

HymenopteraEcdysozoa

O.bimaculoidesMollusca

Page 17: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Figure S2. SLC6A monoamine transporters subtree to highlight species composition per clade and branch lengths. Related to Figure 1. (A) Maximum-likelihood tree. The depicted subtree contains monoamine transporters plus their outgroup as found in the maximum likelihood tree of Figure 1A. The tree was re-formatted to highlight species composition per clade in FigTree. (B) Maximum-likelihood tree. The monoamine transporter subtree of Figure S3A is re-formatted to highlight branch lengths. Turquoise arrows indicate branch tips for proteins in the SLC6A4 gene family and include sea urchin and spider. Blue arrows indicate branch tips for proteins in the outgroup to monoamine transporters and include squid and spider. (C)

Page 18: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001244521

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid106188tr164318

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000047174

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000222854

S.purpuratus-SPU_017227-tr

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid54429tr22287

D.melanogaster-FBpp0309974

L.gigantea-LotgiP134594

A.planci-XP_0221087801

C.virginica-XP_0223396341

A.planci-XP_0221019451

S.mimosarum-KFM73194

A.californica-NM__0012045731

A.mellifera-GB46300-PA

C.virginica-XP_0223420621

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000077371

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001040392

C.elegans-Y54E10BR7

N.vectensis-EDO40151

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000266443

A.cephalotes-XP_0120577931

A.mellifera-GB40867-PA

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22008529mp

A.mellifera-GB40947-PA

N.vectensis-EDO39158

S.mimosarum-KFM74211

H.sapiens-ENSP000003858222

H.sapiens-ENSP000002703499

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000085961

S.purpuratus-SPU_022506-tr

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22001491mp

S.mimosarum-KFM61330

D.pealeii-OLid29570tr70599

D.pealeii-OLid9998tr15537

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000707281

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000234611

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813153

A.planci-XP_0220957321

C.virginica-XP_0223357141

S.purpuratus-SPU_022382-tr

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000067111

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000221006

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid83691tr263860

L.gigantea-LotgiP115105

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000163791

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000075261

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001198641

S.mimosarum-KFM61014

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813364

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22034805mp

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22026818mp

A.californica-XM_0130900641

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000206112

L.gigantea-LotgiP104270

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid123732tr209513utrorf

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001415121

N.vectensis-EDO39157

A.californica-XM_0130844731

C.elegans-T23G55

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000083591

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000046793

S.mimosarum-KFM69234

A.planci-XP_0220962691

A.cephalotes-XP_0120598671

C.virginica-XP_0223452791

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001209181

H.sapiens-ENSP000002198338

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22009795mp

S.purpuratus-SPU_000971-trD.pealeii-OLid29571tr333724

D.melanogaster-FBpp0303037

D.melanogaster-FBpp0077136

S.mimosarum-KFM69889

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000068793

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001298691

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000253183

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000236322

L.gigantea-LotgiP123645

L.gigantea-LotgiP103896

A

HumanVertebrataDeuterostomia

HymenopteraEcdysozoa

OctopusMollusca

Cnidaria

Phylogenetic Taxa

Phylogenetic Gene Family Clades

SLC6A Monoamine Transporters and Outgroup

A

B

C

D

B SLC6A Monoamine Transporters and Outgroup: Branch lengths

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid54429tr22287

A.cephalotes-XP_0120577931

A.planci-XP_0220957321

N.vectensis-EDO40151

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001198641

S.mimosarum-KFM74211

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000206112

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001040392

N.vectensis-EDO39158

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22008529mp

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000068793H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000083591

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid83691tr263860

A.californica-XM_0130844731

S.purpuratus-SPU_022506-tr

C.virginica-XP_0223357141

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001244521

A.californica-XM_0130900641

A.mellifera-GB40947-PA

D.pealeii-OLid9998tr15537

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid123732tr209513utrorf

D.melanogaster-FBpp0077136

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22009795mp

L.gigantea-LotgiP115105

A.planci-XP_0221087801

C.elegans-T23G55

D.pealeii-OLid29571tr333724

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000707281

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22001491mp

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000001298691

N.vectensis-EDO39157

S.mimosarum-KFM73194

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000085961

D.melanogaster-FBpp0309974

S.mimosarum-KFM69234

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813364

A.californica-NM__0012045731

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22034805mp

D.melanogaster-FBpp0303037

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000046793

A.mellifera-GB46300-PA

A.cephalotes-XP_0120598671

S.mimosarum-KFM69889

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000222854

H.sapiens-ENSP000003858222

D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813153

D.pealeii-OLid29570tr70599

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001209181

S.purpuratus-SPU_000971-tr

C.virginica-XP_0223420621

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000077371

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000253183

C.virginica-XP_0223396341

C.virginica-XP_0223452791

L.gigantea-LotgiP104270

S.mimosarum-KFM61014

A.planci-XP_0220962691

C.elegans-Y54E10BR7

M.musculus-ENSMUSP000000221006

L.gigantea-LotgiP134594

R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000047174

S.mimosarum-KFM61330

H.sapiens-ENSP000002703499

H.sapiens-ENSP000002198338

S.purpuratus-SPU_022382-tr

H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000234611

L.gigantea-LotgiP123645

C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000236322

L.gigantea-LotgiP103896

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22026818mp

A.planci-XP_0221019451

M.mulatta-ENSMMUP000000266443

A.mellifera-GB40867-PA

D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid106188tr164318

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000067111

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000075261

D.rerio-ENSDARP000001415121

T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000163791

S.purpuratus-SPU_017227-tr

SLC6A4 Family

Monoamine Transporters

Outgroup MonoamineTransporters

Page 19: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Figure S3. Consensus bootstrap tree for the SLC6A4 gene family. Related to Figure 1. (A) Maximum likelihood bootstrap “consensus tree”. The “consensus tree” was made based on 156 bootstrap trees generated by RAxML for the maximum likelihood “best tree” of the SLC6A gene family (Figure 1A).

Page 20: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

0.0%

60.0

57.7

36.8

60.0

69.6

65.0

91.0

40.0

31.8

25.0

47.6

0.0%

75.0

57.7

42.1

85.0

95.7

65.0

77.3

40.0

45.5

35.0

52.4

Domain Domain

Human SLC6A4 Octopus SLC6A4-AAlignment Human SLC6A4

Octopus SLC6A4-BAlignment

.PGPGLVFVV YPEAVSMLTG STFWAIIFFL MLITLGLDST FGGLESLMTG FTDEVSLI.R RHREAFVAIL ISALFLLSLP TTTYGGQYVV QLLDSHAGSF

SLLFICFAEL IAVQWCYGVK RFSNDVEKMI GHQPSYYWKF CWTVVCPCII LGMFCLSIYS YTGITLGDYK FPKWAEIVGW AVSLSSMIGI PVYIIYKAIA

TPGTFKERII KSITPETPTE IPCGD..... .....IRLNA .........TAVTHEMTFW ELIGRGQDFS LTEGENARKK SASSKVELKS IYNGQQWPHH SRTYPELPS

V.........

NVWRFPYICY QNGGGAFLLP YIASYYNTIMYTIMAIFGGI PLFYMELALG QYHRNGCISI WRKICPIFKG IGYAICIIAF AWALYYLISS FTDQLPWTSCoSlc6a4-(2)

hSERT

oSlc6a4-(2)hSERT PSPGAGDDTRMETTPLNSQK QLSACEDGED CQENGVLQKV VPTPGDKVES GQISNGYSAV HSIPATTTTL VAELHQGERE TWGKKVDFLL SVIGYAVDLG

.......... .......... .......... .......... .......... .......... .......... .......... .......... ..........

KNSWNTGNCT EFYTRHVLQI HRSKGLQDLG GISWQLALCI MLIFTVIYFS IWKGVKTSGK VVWVTATFPY IILSVLLVRGoSlc6a4-(2)

hSERT

ATLPGAWRGV LFYLKPNWQK LLETGVWIDA AAQIFFSLGP GFGVLLAFAS YNKFNNNCYQ DALVTSVVNC MTSFVSGFVI FTVLGYMAEM RNEDVSEVAKoSlc6a4-(2)

hSERT

oSlc6a4-(2)hSERT DAGPSLLFIT YAEAIANMPA STFFAIIFFL MLITLGLDST FAGLEGVITA VLDEFPHVWA KRRERFVLAV VITCFFGSLV TLTFGGAYVV KLLEEYATGP

AVLTVALIEA VAVSWFYGIT QFCRDVKEML GFSPGWFWRI CWVAISPLFLoSlc6a4-(2)

hSERT LFIICSFLMS PPQLRLFQYN YPYWSIILGY CIGTSSFICI PTYIAYRLII

oSlc6a4-(2)hSERT

1000

20095

299195

399295

499393

599493

552

.....MFFSV SFSAGAFLVP YFLMLLFLGL PMFYMELCLG QFHRCGPFKI WRKVCPIFYG IGYAIGVTSF VVGLYYNTII AWAVFYLFSS FTADLPWSSC

SLAGDLNNTI FTNTSVSSAD EYYNRKVLES HLSNGVEDIG PIKGPIVGCL AATFIVVYFS LWKGIKSSGK AVWITATVPY IVIVILLIRGNNTWNTENCINYFSE.DNIT WTLHSTSPAE

CTLPGSSDGI LYYLRPKWHK LKNLQVWVDA ASQICFSLGT GFGTLIALSS YNKFNNNSYL DAMATSVINC FTSFLAGFVV FSVLGYMSHV AKIKIDDIAV

II III

I

IV V

VI

VIII

XI

VII

IX X

XII

B

A

Page 21: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Figure S4. Details of protein and domain alignments for O. bimaculoides Slc6a4-(1) and Slc6a4-(2) to human SLC6A4. Related to Figure 2. (A) Protein and domain pairwise alignments. Human SLC6A4 transmembrane domain 1-12 annotations are based on Uniprot Topology annotation of protein UniProt: P31645 (SC6A4_HUMAN). Annotated Human SLC6A4 was aligned to octopus Slc4a4-(1) and Slc6a4-(2) using Geneious. Aligned regions were then extracted per domain. (B) Pairwise protein alignment. Human SLC6A4 transmembrane domain 1-12 annotations are based on Uniprot Topology annotation of protein UniProt: P31645 (SC6A4_HUMAN). The binding pocket region that overlaps serotonin and MDMA binding (green) is based on [S2]. Alignment of annotated human SLC6A4 to octopus Slc6a4-(2) was done by pairwise alignment in Geneious. Identical amino acids are indicated in red, while positive (physiochemically similar) amino acids are indicated in orange.

Page 22: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Species Uniprot Identifier Gene Name

Homo sapiens UniProt: A0A024R2G0 SLC6A1

Homo sapiens UniProt: P23975 SLC6A2

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q01959 SLC6A3

Homo sapiens UniProt: P31645 SLC6A4

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9Y345 SLC6A5

Homo sapiens UniProt: A0A087WY96 SLC6A6

Homo sapiens UniProt: E5RJL1 SLC6A7

Homo sapiens UniProt: P48029 SLC6A8

Homo sapiens UniProt: P48067 SLC6A9

Homo sapiens UniProt: P48066 SLC6A11

Homo sapiens UniProt: P48065 SLC6A12

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9NSD5 SLC6A13

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9UN76 SLC6A14

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9H2J7 SLC6A15

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9GZN6 SLC6A16

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9H1V8 SLC6A17

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q96N87 SLC6A18

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q695T7 SLC6A19

Homo sapiens UniProt: Q9NP91 SLC6A20

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: A0A0B4LHJ6 SerT (SLC6A4)

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: Q6NND1 GAT (SLC6A1)

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: Q1RKX2 CG5549 (SLC6A5)

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: B5RIX7 ntl (SLC6A7)

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: A0A0B4K7V4 CG43066 (SLC6A19)

Drosophila melanogaster UniProt: B7Z122 CG10804 (SLC6A19)

Caenorhabditis elegans UniProt: Q963F3 mod-5 (SLC6A4)

Caenorhabditis elegans UniProt: G5EF43 snf-11 (SLC6A1)

Caenorhabditis elegans UniProt: G5EBN9 snf-3 (SLC6A)

Page 23: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Table S1. SLC6A reference gene set for phylogenetic analysis. Related to Figure 1. A mapping of species to Uniprot identifier and gene symbol for the reference gene set is provided.

Page 24: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Name Genus species NCBI Identifier Proteome File Name / Proteome version

Proteome URL

western honey bee

Apis mellifera NCBI Taxonomy: 7460

Arthropoda_Apis_mellifera.Amel_4.5.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Apis_mellifera/Info/Index

leafcutter ant Atta cephalotes NCBI Taxonomy: 12957

Arthropoda_Atta_cephalotes.Attacep1.0.pep.all.fa

http://metazoa.ensembl.org/Atta_cephalotes/Info/Index

fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster

NCBI Taxonomy: 7227

Arthropoda_Drosophila_melanogaster.BDGP6.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Drosophila_melanogaster/Info/Index

African social velvet spider

Stegodyphus mimosarum

NCBI Taxonomy: 407821

Arthropoda_Stegodyphus_mimosarum.Stegodyphus_mimosarum_v1.pep.all.fa

http://metazoa.ensembl.org/Stegodyphus_mimosarum/Info/Index

white-tufted ear marmoset

Callithrix jacchus NCBI Taxonomy: 9483

Chordata_Callithrix_jacchus.C_jacchus3.2.1.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Callithrix_jacchus/Info/Index

zebrafish Danio rerio NCBI Taxonomy: 7955

Chordata_Danio_rerio.GRCz10.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Danio_rerio/Info/Index

naked mole rat (female)

Heterocephalus glaber

NCBI Taxonomy: 10181

Chordata_Heterocephalus_glaber_female.HetGla_female_1.0.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Heterocephalus_glaber_female/Info/Index

human Homo sapiens NCBI Taxonomy: 9606

Chordata_Homo_sapiens.GRCh38.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Homo_sapiens/Info/Index

Rhesus monkey Macaca mulatta NCBI Taxonomy: 9544

Chordata_Macaca_mulatta.Mmul_8.0.1.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Macaca_mulatta/Info/Index

mouse Mus musculus NCBI Taxonomy: 10090

Chordata_Mus_musculus.GRCm38.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Mus_musculus/Info/Index

rat Rattus norvegicus NCBI Taxonomy: 10116

Chordata_Rattus_norvegicus.Rnor_6.0.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Rattus_norvegicus/Info/Index

zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata

NCBI Taxonomy: 59729

Chordata_Taeniopygia_guttata.taeGut3.2.4.pep.all.fa

https://useast.ensembl.org/Taeniopygia_guttata/Info/Index

anemone Nematostella vectensis

NCBI Taxonomy: 45351

Cnidaria_Nematostella_vectensis.ASM20922v1.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Nematostella_vectensis/Info/Index

crown of thorns seastar

Acanthaster planci NCBI Taxonomy: 133434

Echinodermata_Acanthaster_planci.GCF_001949145.

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/7870?genome_

Page 25: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

1_OKI-Apl_1.0_protein.faa

assembly_id=301786

sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus

NCBI Taxonomy: 7668

Echinodermata_Strongylocentrotus_purpuratus.Spur_3.1.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Strongylocentrotus_purpuratus/Info/Index

oyster Crassostrea virginica

NCBI Taxonomy: 6565

Mollusca_Crassostrea_virginica.GCF_002022765.2_C_virginica-3.0_protein.faa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Crassostrea_gigas/Info/Index

snail Lottia gigantea NCBI Taxonomy: 225164

Mollusca_Lottia_gigantea.Lotgi1.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Lottia_gigantea/Info/Index

octopus Octopus bimaculoides

NCBI Taxonomy: 37653

Mollusca_Octopus_bimaculoides.PRJNA270931.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Octopus_bimaculoides/Info/Index

worm Caenorhabditis elegans

NCBI Taxonomy: 6239

Nematoda_Caenorhabditis_elegans.WBcel235.pep.all.fa

https://metazoa.ensembl.org/Caenorhabditis_elegans/Info/Index

squid Doryteuthis pealeii NCBI Taxonomy: 1051067

LPealei.Buccalganglion.Annotated.transcriptome.v1.0.fasta

http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html

squid Doryteuthis pealeii NCBI Taxonomy: 1051067

LPealei.GFL.Annotated.transcriptome.v1.0.fasta

http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html

squid Doryteuthis pealeii NCBI Taxonomy: 1051067

LPealei.OL.Annotated.transcriptome.v1.0.fasta

http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html

squid Doryteuthis pealeii NCBI Taxonomy: 1051067

LPealei.SG.Annotated.transcriptome.v1.0.fasta

http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html

squid Doryteuthis pealeii NCBI Taxonomy: 1051067

LPealei.VerticalLobe.Annotated.Transcriptome.v1.0.fasta

http://ivory.idyll.org/blog/2014-loligo-transcriptome-data.html

sea slug Aplysia californica NCBI Taxonomy: 6500

GCF_000002075.1_AplCal3.0_protein.faa

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/genome/?term=txid6500[orgn]

Page 26: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Table S2. Species and proteome sources for phylogenetic analysis. Related to Figure 1. A mapping of species names and their NCBI identifiers to proteome sources is provided.

Page 27: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Genus species SLC6A4 Tree Identifier

SLC6 Tree Identifier Genome Protein Identifier

Homo sapiens H.sapiens H.sapiens-ENSP000003858222 ENSP000003858222

Macaca mulatta M.mulatta M.mulatta-

ENSMMUP000000046793 ENSMMUP000000046793

Callithrix jacchus

C.jacchus C.jacchus-ENSCJAP000000253183

ENSCJAP000000253183

Rattus norvegicus

R.norvegicus R.norvegicus-ENSRNOP000000047174

ENSRNOP000000047174

Mus musculus M.musculus M.musculus-

ENSMUSP000001040392 ENSMUSP000001040392

Heterocephalus glaber

H.glaber H.glaber-ENSHGLP000000234611

ENSHGLP000000234611

Taeniopygia guttata

T.guttata (1) T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000163791

ENSTGUP000000163791

Taeniopygia guttata

T.guttata (2) T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000067111

ENSTGUP000000067111

Taeniopygia guttata

T.guttata (3) T.guttata-ENSTGUP000000077371

ENSTGUP000000077371

Danio rerio D.rerio (1) D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813364 ENSDARP000000813364

Danio rerio D.rerio (2) D.rerio-ENSDARP000000813153 ENSDARP000000813153

Acanthaster planci

A.planci A.planci-XP_0220962691 XP_0220962691

Strongylocentrotus purpuratus

S.purpuratus S.purpuratus-SPU_022382-tr SPU_022382-tr

Crassostrea virginica

C.virginica (1) C.virginica-XP_0223396341 XP_0223396341

Crassostrea virginica

C.virginica (2) C.virginica-XP_0223357141 XP_0223357141

Aplysia californica

A.californica A.californica-NM__0012045731 NM__0012045731

Lottia gigantea L.gigantea (1) L.gigantea-LotgiP134594 LotgiP134594

Lottia gigantea L.gigantea (2) L.gigantea-LotgiP104270 LotgiP104270

Doryteuthis pealeii

D.pealeii (1) D.pealeii-BuccalGangliaid123732tr209513utrorf

BuccalGangliaid123732tr209513utrorf

Doryteuthis pealeii

D.pealeii (2) D.pealeii-OLid9998tr15537 OLid9998tr15537

Octopus bimaculoides

O.bimaculoides (1)

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22008529mp

Ocbimv22008529mp

Octopus bimaculoides

O.bimaculoides (2)

O.bimaculoides-Ocbimv22009795mp

Ocbimv22009795mp

Drosophila melanogaster

D.melanogaster

D.melanogaster-FBpp0309974 FBpp0309974

Caenorhabditis elegans

C.elegans C.elegans-Y54E10BR7 Y54E10BR7

Page 28: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Table S3. Mapping of species to their SLC6A4 gene and tree identifiers. Related to Figure 1. A mapping is provided to link each species depicted in the trees of Figure 1 to their protein and tree identifiers.

Page 29: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Animal Sex Weight (Kg) Exp 1-female social object Exp 1-male social object Exp 2-Pre Exp 2-Post

Feb 10, 2018 AM Feb 10, 2018 PM Feb 10, 2018 PM * Feb 13, 2018 6PM

Feb 11, 2018 PM * Feb 13, 2018 11PM

1 M 0.132 Social object = 2 (female) Novel object = Chewbacca-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) Novel object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

2 F 0.156 N/A N/A N/A N/A

3 M 0.081 Social object = 2 (female) Novel object = Chewbacca-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) Novel object = Stormtrooper-Pot N/A N/A

4 F 0.176 Social object = 2 (female) Novel object = Chewbacca-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) Novel object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

5 M 0.118 N/A N/A * Social object = 7 (male) object = Stormtrooper-bottle

* Social object = 7 (male) object = Stormtrooper-bottle

6 M 0.145 Social object = 2 (female) Novel object = Chewbacca-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) Novel object = Stormtrooper-Pot N/A N/A

7 M 0.152 Social object = 2 (female) Novel object = Chewbacca-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) Novel object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Social object = 5 (male) object = Stormtrooper-Pot

Page 30: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Table S4. Experimental animals used for behavioral analysis. Related to Figures 3 and 4. Weight, sex, age and experimental date information is provided for each animal.

Page 31: A Conserved Role for Serotonergic Neurotransmission in ... · in Mediating Social Behavior in Octopus Eric Edsinger1 and Gu¨lDo¨len2,3,* 1Josephine Bay Paul Center for Comparative

Supplemental References: [S1]. Lovell, P. V., Kasimi, B., Carleton, J., Velho, T.A., and Mello, C. V. (2015). Living without DAT: Loss and compensation of the dopamine transporter gene in sauropsids (birds and reptiles). Sci. Rep. 5, 1–12. [S2]. Field, J., Henry, L., and Blakely, R. (2010). Transmembrane domain 6 of the human serotonin transporter contributes to an aqueously accessible binding pocket for serotonin and the psychostimulant 3,4-methylene dioxymethamphetamine. J. Biol. Chem. 285, 11270–80.