155
Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Historical Dissertations and eses Graduate School 1991 A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive Writing in College Introductory Algebra Classes. Naomi Barbara r Allen Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: hps://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Historical Dissertations and eses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Allen, Naomi Barbara r, "A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive Writing in College Introductory Algebra Classes." (1991). LSU Historical Dissertations and eses. 5220. hps://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/5220

A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Louisiana State UniversityLSU Digital Commons

LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses Graduate School

1991

A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced byExpressive Writing in College Introductory AlgebraClasses.Naomi Barbara r AllenLouisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College

Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion inLSU Historical Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please [email protected].

Recommended CitationAllen, Naomi Barbara r, "A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive Writing in College Introductory Algebra Classes."(1991). LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses. 5220.https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/5220

Page 2: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

INFORMATION TO USERS

This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book.

Photograpns included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information C o m p a n y

3 0 0 North Z e e b R oad , Ann Arbor. Ml 4 8 1 0 6 - 1 3 4 6 USA 3 1 3 / 7 6 1 - 4 7 0 0 8 0 0 / 5 2 1 - 0 6 0 0

Page 3: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive
Page 4: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Order Num ber 9219519

A study o f m etacognitive skill as influenced by expressive w ritin g in college introductory algebra classes

Allen, Naomi Barbara R., Ph.D.

The Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical Col., 1991

UMI300 N. Zeeb Rd.Ann Arbor, MI 48106

Page 5: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive
Page 6: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

A STUDY OF METACOGNITIVE SKILL AS INFLUENCED BY EXPRESSIVE WRITING

IN COLLEGE INTRODUCTORY ALGEBRA CLASSES

A DissertationSubmitted to the Graduate Faculty of the

Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College

in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophyin

The Department of Curriculum and Instruction

byNaomi Barbara R. Allen

B.S., Southeastern Louisiana University, 1968 M.Ed., Southeastern Louisiana University, 1980

December 1991

Page 7: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS As I reflect on those who contributed to my efforts

(and the complete list would be extensive), I must first acknowledge the gift that went beyond willingness, time, or expertise— the genuine caring that allowed me to grow personally as well as professionally.

I want to thank all of the members of my committee for their contributions: Dr. David England, Dr. David Kirshner, Dr. L. Diane Miller, Dr. Robert Perlis, Dr. Rosalind Charlesworth, and Dr. William Doll. I especially thank Dr. Miller for sharing with me her interest in writing and Dr. England for supporting that interest unfailingly throughout my research efforts. I consider them as both mentors and friends.

I am thankful to my colleagues who worked directly with me in this research, Dave Busekist, Helen Carona, and, especially, Gwen Autin. Also, as barriers seemed to emerge, I am grateful to the persons who were always there to help remove them: Cathy Moten, Dr. Leah Sadden, Dr. Anna Wagoner, Dr. Charles Teddlie, Dr. Connie Smith, Dr. Hunter McAllister, Dr. Debbie Longman, and Mr. Ravi Kallianpur (whose help was ad infinitum).

Throughout, two very special colleagues and friends stand out— Beth Gray who in her own doctoral pursuit

ii

Page 8: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

continuously forged the way, and Carol Robertson, who has read, edited, and critiqued my every effort for over a decade. My thanks for the continuous support of associates, especially Dr. Patsy Causey, who often placed my commitment to this research before their own needs. I am thankful to Dr. Florence Sanchez, whose initial support was the impetus to embark upon this journey and to Mrs. Elizabeth Murphy, who continually provided support along the way.

I am particularly thankful to my family; some member was always there for me. It would be impossible to enumerate the support they provided, but it was their commitment which maintained my own.

While family and friends provided emotional support, the power of God gave me the needed spiritual support, and, on the lighter side, Egg Roll King, the needed physical sustenance. Finally, there is a past doctoral student, somewhere, whose name is long forgotten but his words are not. He acknowledged his wife for having him take out the garbage when he could do nothing else, and this vicarious support gave me permission to do the alien household tasks that became so appealing when nothing else would come.

iii

Page 9: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

TABLE OF CONTENTSPage

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS...................................... iiLIST OF T A B L E S ................................... . .viLIST OF FIGURES....................................... viiABSTRACT .............................................viiiCHAPTER

1. INTRODUCTION TO THE PROBLEM...................... 1Theoretical Issues ............................. 2Purpose of the S t u d y ............................ 11Questions to be Investigated................... 13Organization of the Study........................14

2. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE....................... 15Developmental Education.......................... 16Role of Metacognition in Learning...............21Metacognition and Mathematics................... 36Writing to L e a r n .................................45Writing in Mathematics ........................ 52Using Writing to Promote

Metacognition................................... 603. METHODOLOGY.......................................63

M e t h o d ............................................63Data Selection................................... 66Treatments....................................... 68

iv

Page 10: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

D e s i g n ............................................71Procedure......................................... 73Evaluation of D a t a .............................. 74Data Analysis 7 6

4. RESULTS 7 8Group Equivalency Testing........................78Effect of the Pretest............................ 79Analyses and R e s u l t s 7 9

5. DISCUSSIONS, CONCLUSIONS,LIMITATIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS................. 91

Discussion........................ 91Conclusions. ...................................95Limitations of the Present Study ............. 96Implications ................................... 98

REFERENCES............................................. 102APPENDICES............................................. 121

Appendix A ....................................... 122Appendix B ....................................... 123Appendix C ....................................... 131Appendix D ....................................... 138Appendix E ....................................... 139Appendix F ....................................... 140

V I T A .................................................. 141v

Page 11: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

LIST OF TABLES Table Page1. Point Assignment for Codings...............752. Student Perception of Correct Answers . .813. Definition of Ranges for

Achievement and Awareness ............... 834. Cell Means for Awareness................... 875. Tukey's Pairwise Comparisons...............88

vi

Page 12: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page1. Data Collected from Each G r o u p ............... 722. Frequencies of Codings

per Student on Q u i z z e s ........................ 823. Comparison of Achievement Scores with

Awareness Scores for Each Q u i z ............... 844. Graph of Interaction of Cell M e a n s ........... 87

vii

Page 13: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

ABSTRACTThe quality of mathematics education has become a major

concern to mathematics educators. As a result, increased attention is being given to identifying the abilities that underlie competent performance. An outcome of this effort is an increasing belief that the development of metacognitive skills is an essential component of proficient mathematics performance. Writing, because it promotes reflective thinking, is believed to be the vehicle for this development.

Writing in the mathematics classroom has previously received anecdotal support for its benefits to the learner and to the instructor, and limited quantitative benefits in problem-solving ability and attitude toward mathematics.This study examined the effect of expressive writing on self-awareness and would suggest quantitative support that writing is beneficial in promoting student ability to assess the correctness of work. If metacognitive skills are a necessary condition for successful mathematics performance, the use of writing may provide the process for attaining these essential skills. Further research in the benefits of writing is warranted by this study.

viii

Page 14: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

The present quality of mathematics education will not support the realization of personal expectations for career goals and quality of life for many students. This belief is reflected in publications titled, A Nation at Risk: The Imperative for Educational Reform (National Commission on Excellence in Education, 1983), The Mathematics Report Card: Are We Measuring Up? (Dossey, Mullis, Lindquist, & Chambers, 1988) and Everybody Counts; A Report on the Future of Mathematics Education (National Research Council, 1989). These publications express a need for a commitment to life-long learning which will enable workers to adapt to changing job descriptions mandated by our rapidly changing technology.

Life-long learning asserts the need of greater student responsibility for and participation in the learning process. This idea is not new as indicated by the following quote from Jerome Bruner (1972): "To instruct someone in these disciplines is not a matter of getting him to commit the results to mind; rather, it is to teach him to participate in the

1

Page 15: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

process that makes possible the establishment of knowledge” (p. 72) .

This calls for a transference of power from the instructor to the pupil. Rather than in the traditional direct instruction, interest is towards a more dynamic learning environment characterized by an interactive process (Brown, 1987). This interest is confirmed in the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards for Mathematics Education, a publication by the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989) which reflects a consensus of university mathematicians and mathematics educators (classroom teachers, supervisors, educational researchers and teacher educators). As a result of these concerns, how students learn is receiving increasing attention with an emphasis on identifying the abilities that underlie competent performance.

Theoretical IssuesCognitive scientists have drawn distinctions

between novice and expert learners and have begun to investigate the differences in the way these individuals process information. The novice is

Page 16: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

believed to process new information as individual, unrelated statements rather than embedding this information into appropriate and relevant contexts. Generally, these students are not confident in controlling their performance and tend to be relatively passive in their learning (Campione, Brown & Connell, 1989). An expert, on the other hand, displays the ability to reflect upon and assess a situation, and to regulate the strategies employed (Brown & DeLoache, 1978; Long, 1986; Schoenfeld,1987). However, these activities become more covert and less observable as the learner develops expertise An attempt to identify and classify this ability has led to the adoption of the term "metacognition" which serves as an umbrella for all activities concerning either knowledge of cognition or control of cognition

MetacognitionFlavell (1976), a pioneer in the field of

metacognition, views metacognition as the monitoring of cognitive enterprises which subsumes four classes of phenomena: (a) metacognitive knowledge, (b)metacognitive experiences, (c) goals or tasks, and

Page 17: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4

(d) actions or strategies. An example of (a) would be a person's belief about his or her ability to do mathematics, and an example of (b) would be a sudden feeling that something that had just been said was not understood. A goal refers to the objective of a cognitive enterprise such as knowing a chapter of a text for a test; the strategy would be the behavior implemented to achieve that goal (outlining or rereading the chapter).

Long (1986) views metacognition as a conscious aspect of thinking and learning, referring to "purposeful actions that monitor progress in a task, and regulate the procedures used to perform it" (p.8). Developing awareness increases monitoring which, in turn, increases the ability to make productive decisions at the right time and place. These abilities are believed to be essential components of proficient mathematical performance (Campione et al., 1989; Garofalo & Lester, 1985; Long, 1986; Narode, 1985; and Schoenfeld, 1987).

Flavell (1979) sees investigations pointing to an important role for metacognition in reading comprehension, language acquisition, memory, problem

Page 18: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

solving, social cognition, attention, various types of self-control and instruction, and oral persuasion, comprehension and communication of information.Current literature affirms the view of metacognitive skills as an essential component of learning (Brown, 1987; Campione, 1987; Henderson, 1986; Long, 1986; Pramling, 1988; Schoenfeld, 1985b; Slife, Weiss, & Bell, 1985).

Available research studies on teaching strategies which address reflection about one's thinking and self-regulation span various content areas as well as diverse ability groups. Examples include studies of a metacognitive model for solving the problem of skill generalization in learning-disabled students (Borkowski, Estrada, Milstead, & Hale, 1989), the effects of self-management strategies on journal writing (Hull, 1981), and the effects of programming with Logo on metacognitive skills (Clements, 1986).

In the area of mathematics, the focus on metacognition is predominately in the area of problem­solving. Included are studies which have investigated self-regulation as a problem-solving strategy (Schoenfeld, 1987; Narode, 1985). Schoenfeld (1987)

Page 19: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

describes the main point of self-regulation in the following: "It's not only what you know, but how you use it (if at all) that matters" (p. 192).

Schoenfeld actively pursues the development of metacognitive skills through using videos of problem­solving sessions to make students aware of metacognitive issues and of their own behavior, or he models metacognitive behavior through what he terms "problem resolution," which demonstrates the planning, exploration, and evaluation that occurs during true problem solving. He also employs "whole class" discussions, in which the instructor records and moderates, to focus on control decisions. Group work is also incorporated with the instructor serving only as a consultant to direct a student's thinking toward questions that ask what (is being done), why (it is being done), and how (it will help) when searching for a solution.

Using a variation of group work, Narode (1985) applies paired problem solving in which one student solves the problem while the second student listens carefully or asks questions until both clearly follow the solution. Believing that metacognition is

Page 20: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

"facilitated through oral and written communication of think as-it-happens (p. 14)," he requires students to write about their thoughts, to read them, and to reflect upon them. While Narode feels this method to be useful, he also believes more research into the benefits of writing is needed and opens the door for that possibility.

Writing to learnWriting in the classroom has traditionally been

used to demonstrate learning rather than to promote it. During the last decade, the focus on the use of writing has shifted to its use for promoting cognitive and affective benefits for the student. Rose (1989a) provides a review of the literature that cites two broad categories: transactional and expressive. Britton, Burgess, Martin, McLeod, and Rosen (1975) describe transactional writing as public writing, intended for an audience. They contrast transactional writing with expressive writing, personal writing for the purpose of exploring and recording thoughts.While not supported by hard research, the benefits of using such writing in the classroom have been

Page 21: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

documented by many researchers (Applebee, 1984; Rose, 1989a).

Geeslin (1977) views this writing as a "diagnostic tool" for the instructor and a "learning tool" for the student. The use of writing as a strategy for active learning encourages students to explore, conjecture, and reason logically as concepts and procedures are developed. The emphasis is shifted to the process by which students learn rather than the product of their learning.

Writing to learn is based on the belief that there are strong connections between writing and learning (Applebee, 1984; Britton et al., 1975; Emig, 1977; Rose, 1989a). Emig's theoretical work on writing, thought to parallel powerful learning strategies, has been the underpinning of much of the current literature on writing to learn. She identifies four learning strategies: multi-representational and integral, connective, personally engaged, and those which generate self-provided feedback. All can be specifically achieved by writing. Her provision for self-provided feedback is particularly important to this inquiry.

Page 22: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

9

Instructional ImplicationsThe use of expressive writing allows an

approximation of the learner's thoughts to be recorded, making them available for both visible reflection by the student and for inspection by the instructor (Emig, 1977). Consequently, writing has the potential for making learners aware of their cognition as realized by Narode (1985), Goodkin (1982) and Rose (1989a). Narode states the importance of communication, either by discussion or writing, in developing self-awareness:

The communication of ideas is a sure attempt at presenting a problem or concept to another but more importantly it is the representation of an idea to the thinker him/herself. No sooner than we begin to describe our ideas do we evaluate them, alter or elaborate them and effectively monitor ourselves with a new awareness; the self-consciousness which asks questions like: How can I say what I mean? Will this particular method of solution help me? Is this problem clear? and many other such questions which cause us to reflect and to analyze what we think, (p.5)

Page 23: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0

Hence, the relationship between writing to learn and metacognition appears to be naturally convergent.

Background to the Research QuestionsIn this researcher's use of writing in a college-

level remedial mathematics classroom (Allen, 1989), students wrote that they thought they had understood a concept until they began to write about it. Students also expressed an inability to evaluate their understanding of a concept before they encountered it on a quiz or a test. When asked if their efforts were matched by results, their responses disclosed that students can unknowingly work an entire assignment incorrectly. Students indicated they viewed writing as an effective means of self-evaluation.

Insights into the role of the instructor as the perceived source of knowledge were also revealed through the writings. In summation, these informal findings from student writings support this researcher's belief that the use of expressive writing has the potential to promote the metacognitive skills displayed by an expert learner.

Page 24: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

11

Purpose of the Study Blais (1988) states that education must provide

the process which will transform a novice into an expert. As recent studies have suggested, experts seem to possess self-awareness and employ certain managerial skills which are believed to promote improved performance. Researchers believe these skills can be taught (Long, 1986; Narode, 1985; Schoenfeld, 1987), allowing the novice to make the desired transition. Campione et al. (1989) suggest alternatives to traditional teaching which encourage active student participation. An example is reciprocal teaching, a form of cooperative learning, which features guided practice in applying four concrete strategies: questioning, clarifying, summarizing, and predicting. These authors believe these strategies provide students with concrete methods for monitoring their understanding.

This study focused on the aspect of metacognition described as the "students' conscious and statable knowledge about cognition, about themselves as learners..." (Campione et al., 1989, p. 94). The aim of this study will be twofold: (a) to examine the

Page 25: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

12

self-awareness possessed by remedial introductory algebra students and (b) the effect of in-class expressive writing on their ability to further develop this awareness.

Self-awareness was students to indicate their assessment of both the accuracy of the method chosen to answer a mathematical question and the accuracy of the implementation of the chosen method. Comparing the accuracy of the indications to the accuracy of the work will yield information on the student's awareness.

The process of writing, because it promotes reflection, is sought as a vehicle for enhancing self- awareness. In-class writings, consisting of asking students to respond daily for five minutes to a given prompt, focus on questions which promote reflection and self-awareness, and which motivate monitoring of study habits. Three-part paper, which provides two carbonless copies of the student's writing, was used to allow the student, the instructor, and the researcher to each have a copy of these writings for later examination.

Page 26: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

13

The findings include implications for identifying: (a) the metacognitive skill of self-awareness these students possess, (b) the effect of reflective guestioning on this metacognitive skill, and (c) the possible role of the writing process in enhancing self-awareness or achievement.

Questions to be Investigated The following research questions will be

addressed:1. When asked to do so, can students in a college-

level elementary algebra class consistently assess the accuracy of the method chosen and of its implementation when determining an answer?

2. Does the ability to accurately assess the correctness of the method and its implementation increase over time and with experience?

3. Does the intervention of daily in-class expressive writing or lecture containing reflective questioning affect the ability of students to assess the accuracy of the method chosen and its implementation when determining an answer?

4. Does the intervention of daily in-class expressive writing or lecture containing reflective questioning affect student achievement?

Page 27: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

14

Organization of the Study Following this introduction, the second chapter

will present a review of the literature. In the area of metacognition, the reviexv will address the meanings given to this phenomenon by theorists, the current research related to mathematics education, and the status of current instructional practices relative to metacognition. To address the concept of writing to learn, the review will discuss the purported role of the writing process in learning, current uses of writing in the mathematics classroom, and current research on writing-to-learn in the mathematics classroom.

Chapter 3 will describe the research procedures, and Chapter 4 will present the findings of the study. Chapter 5 will summarize the study through drawing conclusions and suggesting implications for further investigation.

Page 28: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Researchers today stress the importance of metacognitive skills in learning and successful mathematics performance and urge mathematics educators to help their students develop a metacognitive posture when performing mathematics (Campione et al., 1989; Flavell, 1979; Garofalo & Lester, 1985; Long, 1986; Schoenfeld, 1987,1989b; Weinert, 1987). Studies reveal that the novice does not display the necessary metacognitive skills for successful learning but further research supports the belief these skills can be taught (Brown, 1987; Campione et al., 1989). Developmental mathematics students, who are clearly novices, provide a population appropriate for this study. Writing, because it promotes reflection (Emig, 1977), can provide an instructional activity for promoting the necessary metacognitive skills (Klein & Vukovich, 1991). This chapter presents evidence which supports this conclusion by summarizing a review of current literature and research in the following areas: (a) the developmental education, (b) metacognition and learning, (c) metacognition and mathematics, (d) writing to learn, (e) writing in

15

Page 29: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 6

mathematics, and (f) writing and metacognition in mathematics.

Developmental Education Nation-wide large numbers of students are

entering college (public and private) without the skills or knowledge needed to perform college-level work (Abraham, 1988), resulting in a widespread presence of developmental education programs in post­secondary institutions (National Center for Educational Statistics, 1991) . In the Southern Regional Education Board (SREB) states, about one- third of the first-time freshmen required at least one remedial course. These numbers are indicative of national averages (32%) and only slightly lower than other regional averages (Abraham, 1991).

Brief Historical NotesThe advent of programs to meet the needs of

underprepared students is not new. Abraham (1991) cites the presence of remedial courses at Yale University as early as 1828. In 1849, the University of Wisconsin (Abraham, 1991) established a Department

Page 30: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

of Preparatory Studies followed by Iowa State Collge in 1862 (Mickler & Chapel, 1989). By the 1900s, 84% of the colleges and universities in the United States had similar preparatory schools (Abraham, 1991) including Harvard, Yale, Princeton, and Columbia Universities (Maxwell, 1979). In the 1970s, this number increased to 90% (Boylan, 1986). Although the goal of the SREB for the year 2 000 is to reduce the number of underprepared students to one in five, the present need for developmental programs is not declining (Abraham, 1991).

Developmental StudentsGenerally, developmental students can be

classified as those who (a) made adverse academic decisions, (b) have been out of academia for an extended time, (c) have learning or physical deficits which may or may not have been identified in high school, (d) have acquired pre-collge education in foreign countries, and (e) lack clear-cut or well- formed academic goals (Hardin, 1988). Because these students also have diverse backgrounds, many are culturally deprived and may have learned helplessness

Page 31: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

18

(Spears, Atkinson & Longman, 1986). As a result of having difficulty locating and using available support, developmental students often set high goals but have only a limited understanding of how to go about achieving them.

For these students, varied instructional styles and methods are required to meet their needs. In the 1970s, programs began to include courses designed by content specialists and taught by faculty who were specially trained for remedial education (Mickler & Chapel, 1989). Rounds and Anderson (1985) cited instructional innovations such as individualized, self-paced, mastery, and programmed instruction.Spears et al. (1986) suggest instruction that is traditional or taught in large groups will not be effective for the developmental student. They also suggest these students do not work well independently often relying on external motivation such as instructor expectations. They cite the importance of having students feel the instructor cares about their success in a course.

Page 32: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 9

Developmental MathematicsDevelopmental programs typically offer additional

preparation in mathematics, writing, and reading as well as academic skills. Studies (Abraham, 1991) of the percentage of SREB students in the three academic disciplines revealed the need for mathematics (38.5%) to be substantially higher than writing (27.5%) and reading (26.7%). An examination of the population represented in this study revealed, in the fall of 1990, 56% of the entering freshman were required to enroll in developmental mathematics. These needs are reflected in A Nation at Risk (National Commission on Excellence in Education, 1983), which reported one fourth of all mathematics courses offered in 1980 at four-year institutions were remedial.

Developmental mathematics students often see the course as the "greatest academic hurdle" that must be crossed (Darken, 1990, ix). Because these students have not been successful in mathematics, they often develop fears which are manifested in learned helplessness (Spears et al., 1986) and anxiety (Nolte, 1991). Overcoming these barriers represents the first instructional step.

Page 33: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

2 0

In the developmental mathematics classroom, the need for emotional support and a well-organized program constitutes critical components for successful mathematics remediation (Darken, 1990). To meet these needs, McDonald (1988) stresses the importance of employing caring faculty and maintaining small class sizes. Darken (1990) suggests that developmental mathematics students respond best when passivity is avoided by actively involving the students in the instructional process. She sees small groups and instructor-to-student conferences as generating excitement and interest and providing a vehicle for individualization of instruction. She further suggests using a variety of methods for providing students opportunities to learn at their own pace and style. Feedback should be given individually, both in verbal and written forms (Spears et al., 1986) and on a daily basis (Long, 1986).

Powell (1986) uses three pedagogical devices—

journals, creative writing and research problems, and explorations and problem-generating activities— with his underprepared mathematics students. He views one of the purposes of mathematics education as assisting

Page 34: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

2 1

"learners in becoming aware of the powers they possess and how they can construct meaning and procedural know-how" (p. 183).

Garofalo (1986) suggests that developmental mathematics students are particularly deficient in aspects of mathematical performance which include not only procedural and declarative content knowledge but also knowledge that is linguistic and factual, heuristic and strategic, and metacognitive. In this article, Garofalo focuses on some instructional practices that would develop metacognitive skills, and these will be discussed later in this chapter in the section on teaching for metacognition.

Role of Metacognition in Learning At the turn of this century, educational

psychologists such as Dewey used the terms "active monitoring," "critical evaluation," and "seeking after meanings and relationships" to depict reflective reading activities now subsumed under the rubric "metacognition" (Brown, 1987). In the 1970s, terms included "reflective intelligence," which referred to consciously observing one's own thoughts, and

Page 35: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

22

"reflexive abstraction," a mechanism for extracting, reorganizing, and consolidating knowledge. Obviously, the concept underlying the adoption of the term metacognition is not new and is recognized in many areas.

Garofalo & Lester (1985) see metacognition as originally stemming from an article criticizing the lack of research on memory which particularly noted no one was considering the fact that people have knowledge and beliefs about their memory processes. Flavell began to study children's "metamemory" and went on to become a pioneer in the field of metacognition.

Defining MetacoqnitionFlavell (1976) generally defines metacognition as

"one's knowledge concerning one's own cognitive processes and....the active monitoring and consequent regulation and orchestration of these processes" (p. 232) .

Although Flavell's definition is often used,Brown (1987) focuses on metacognition as being "stable and stateable." A review of current literature

Page 36: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

23

immediately reveals that the definition of metacognition is often neither. Campione et al.(1989) aptly describes the current state of affairs in the following:

One of the most salient features about metacognition is that the term means different things to different people, with the result that there is considerable confusion in the literature about what is and what is not metacognitive.This confusion leads to apparently contradictory viewpoints, ranging from claims that the concept is too ill-defined or fuzzy to be the object of scientific inquiry to assertions that things metacognitive are the driving force of learning, and therefore the major aspects of learning we should be studying, (p. 93)Brown (1987) believes the confusion evolves from

both the historical development of the term and the difficulty in distinguishing between cognition and metacognition. An historical consideration reveals roots originating in four separate strands of inquiry which Brown discusses at length under the headings:(a) verbal reports as data restated as a reflective

Page 37: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

access of one's own thinking, (b) executive control or performing intelligent evaluation of one's own operating, planning, and monitoring, (c) self- regulation which can be described as decision-making actions influenced interpersonally, and (d) other- reaulation which results from actions fostered by external influences or activities, that is, intrapersonal. She points out that initial theorizing about metacognition was goal oriented. She asserts the need to now develop workable theories and procedures for the separate areas that emerge from a concentration on metacognition.

Garofalo and Lester (1985) attempt to clarify the confusion by generalizing cognition as endeavors involved in doing whereas metacognition is activities involved in planning and monitoring what is being done. In Pramling's (1988) research, she merely describes metacognition as a child's awareness of his learning and proceeds with her study, while authors such as Brown (1987), Campione et al. (1989), Garofalo and Lester (1985) devote pages to the term's development and current use.

Page 38: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

2 5

Metacognition is sometimes stated very informally as "thinking about one's thinking." Schoenfeld (1987) uses this explanation but he further classifies metacognition into three separate but related areas of intellectual behavior. The first area identifies how accurately one's own thinking can be described (self- awareness) , the second area centers on monitoring one's thinking (control or self-regulation), and the third examines the experiences (beliefs and intuitions) that shape the way one thinks.Schoenfeld's research focuses on the second area, self-regulation, which he sees as the ability to manage time and effort when working on complex tasks. This management task encompasses understanding, planning, monitoring, and allocating of resources. Simply stated, Schoenfeld states the main point is not only what you know but how you use it, if at all.

Collectively, metacognition is generally viewed by many as falling under the two non-distinct headings of knowledge of cognition and control of cognition. Examples given for knowledge of cognition include beliefs about oneself as a learner (whether factual or not); knowledge about the scope, requirements, and

Page 39: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

2 6

difficulties of tasks; and knowledge of general and specific cognitive strategies and their potential usefulness for certain tasks.

Control or regulation of cognition is concerned with a variety of decisions and strategic activities which have been influenced by the knowledge of one's cognition as described above. These activities include, but are not limited to, predicting, checking, planning, selecting, revising, etc.

Flavell (1976) perceives metacognition as the monitoring of cognitive enterprises which subsumes four classes of phenomena: (a) metacognitive knowledge, (b) metacognitive experiences, (c) goals or tasks, and (d) actions or strategies. An example of (a) would be beliefs about one's personal ability to do mathematics while an example of (b) would be perhaps the feeling of not understanding something that had been said. A goal would refer to the objective of a cognitive enterprise such as knowing a chapter of a text for a test while the strategy would be the behavior implemented to achieve that goal such as outlining or rereading the chapter.

Page 40: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

2 7

Learning and MetacognitionMetacognitive knowledge and metacognitive

experiences are seen to interact where knowledge can influence the control of metacognitive experiences and, likewise, these experiences can shape the acquisition of metacognitive enterprises. This dynamic interplay is illustrated by the following example from Flavell (1979).

Let us begin at the point where some self-imposed or externally imposed task and goal are established. Your existing metacognitive knowledge concerning this class of goals leads to the conscious metacognitive experience that this goal will be difficult to achieve. That metacognitive experience, combined with additional metacognitive knowledge, causes you to select and use the cognitive strategy of asking questions of knowledgeable other people. Their answers to your questions trigger additional metacognitive experiences about how the endeavor is faring. These experiences, again informed and guided by pertinent metacognitive knowledge, instigate the metacognitive strategies of

Page 41: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

28

surveying all that you have learned to see if it fits together into a coherent whole, if it seems plausible and consistent with your prior knowledge and expectation, and if it provides an avenue to the goal. (p. 909)Difficulties on one or more of these points will

consequently activate some metacognitive knowledge or experiences and the cycle will continue until the task is ended. The implications of this model for education pose questions concerning the value of cognitive monitoring in learning experiences.Campione (1987) supports the belief that low achievers are deficient in both knowledge and the ability to control that knowledge. If student learning is to be achieved, Campione asserts the need to inculcate metacognitive skills in addition to providing information.

Corno (1986) sees metacognitive components as a necessary but not sufficient condition for self­regulated learning in which a student actively acquires and transforms instructional materials.Corno characterized self-regulated learning (SRL) as a student's effort to deepen and manipulate an associate

Page 42: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

network in the content areas and to monitor and improve upon that effort. These efforts are hypothesized to direct and control concentration during school learning tasks. This metacognitive control, from an early theory of volitional control, is labeled by six strategies: attention control,encoding control, information processing control, motivation control, emotion control, and environmental control. All of these are observed by Corno in a study on fifth grade students' verbalized thoughts. Corno suggests, in conclusion, that students who access and use these controls in school tasks can be expected to be more efficient, and perhaps more effective, learners.

Experiments have been conducted to test the effects of self-management on journal writing by college freshmen (Hull, 1981). The first experiment showed that goal-setting and self-monitoring would increase the number of lines and entries in weekly journals written for traditional freshmen composition classes. The second experiment replicated the first study using basic (remedial) writers and produced the same outcomes. In addition, the second study also

Page 43: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

3 0

showed that extending the baseline rate did not increase writing output and the removal of the treatment resulted in the student reverting to their previous writing output. The study implicated that, in order for their writing behavior to change, students must actively monitor that behavior.

Teaching for MetacognitionTeaching for metacognition has resulted in

changes in the methods traditionally used to teach.The blind instruction approach is abandoned and replaced with interactive teaching where the goal is to promote student understanding of the meaning of what is being taught and to help the student become more active in and responsible for the learning that occurs. The following studies illustrate the incorporation of metacognitive and contextual factors in learning.

Biggs (1986) seeks to enhance learning skills through strategic learning which he outlines as a metacognitively-oriented theory to relate personal relevance, context, task, and student resources.Biggs focuses on five motive-strategy packages called

Page 44: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

3 1

approaches to learning: surface— minimal requirements through rote learning; deep— intrinsic interest through understanding; achieving— grade oriented through efficient comprehensive study; surface- achieving— achievement oriented but through extensive surface learning; and deep-achieving— intrinsic and grade oriented through meaning-based study. A regular study skills text was used as a background resource in addition to activities which included encouraging students to (a) reflect on their work (especially in subjects they found difficult), (b) keep diaries, (c)utilize self-testing or self-questioning, and (d) pair off with another student to monitor and discuss each other's progress. Two studies, one with college-level subjects and another with grade eleven students, resulted in a shift from surface-achieving to deep- achieving approaches and grades were significantly enhanced.

Reciprocal teaching, which promotes students to use methods for monitoring their understanding, was incorporated to teach comprehension skills to academically weak grade school children (Campione et al., 1989). Using a cooperative group format, this

Page 45: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

form of reciprocal teaching featured guided practice in applying four concrete strategies: questioning, clarifying, summarizing, and predicting. Teachers initially modeled expert performance then transferred the work to the students. The reciprocal nature of the group procedures ensured student engagement. Positive results occurred when third grade minority students were encouraged to use this method to acquire coherent knowledge about biological themes concerning animal adaptation. On independent measures of comprehension, reciprocal teaching groups increased their performance by 32 percentage points. Twelve months later the effect of the instruction could be reproduced with retention test scores of 82% as compared to posttest scores of 85%.

Palincsar and Brown (1987) have shown success in improving reading comprehension by teaching summarizing (self-review), questioning, clarifying, and predicting. Their approach also uses modeling and interrogation as major components in teaching these self-instructional procedures.

Page 46: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

33

Research on MetacognitionPramling (1988) used qualitative methodology to

study children's awareness of their own learning from their point of view. Conceptions of what was learned fell into two categories: learning of activities and learning as comprehension. Similarly, how was categorized as learning to know by either external influence or personal experience. Three teachers (three groups) worked for two to three weeks with the same content called "the shop." Two of the groups used the same structure which taught the content alternately from the customer's perspective and from the shop's perspective. One of these two also used metacognitive dialogue as an integral methodology. Interviews were conducted prior to the teaching segment, during the instruction, and again in six months. The teachers were not previously informed of the later interviews. The interview questions were variations of "what have you learned?" and "how did you learn it?" followed by "anything else?" Analysis of the first interview showed no significant differences in the three groups. Analysis of later interviews revealed the students having metacognitive

Page 47: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

dialogue developed a greater awareness of their own learning and their difference from the other two groups increased during the six months before the final interview. It was not clear if the dialogue, or the dialogue combined with structure, accounts for the differences. The author suggests that her research be seen as exploratory, motivating a larger and more controlled future study where teaching is performed metacognitively, which is different from teaching strategies or facts.

The Higher Order Thinking Skills (HOTS) program, initiated to help students who have not learned to organize their thoughts or to generalize them, represents an example of teaching metacognitively (Pogrow, 1988). Computer software is employed to create the needed learning environment but the critical aspect is the intensive series of teacher- student interactions which follow. This Socratic environment is based on three basic techniques: (a)basic questioning, (b) improvisation techniques to change unanticipated answers to advantage, and (c) coaching techniques to promote independent and active learning. An example of the first is eliminating

Page 48: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

hints from questions or asking students "why?" when simplistic answers are given. The second may be to ask students to explain the reasoning process behind unexpected answers, and the third might involve telling students you think they are smart enough to answer their own questions before walking away. Although HOTS teaches no subject area content, the results show dramatic success in improving problem­solving skills and stimulating achievement gains in basic skills.

In summation, the concept of metacognition generally focuses on two main, but non-distinct, areas: knowledge of cognition and control of cognition. Much of the current research on metacognition falls into the categories of memory development, reading, and special education. The study of metacognition argues for a change from the traditional, teacher-centered instruction to instruction which addresses cognition metacognitively. Examples of this new educational trend include reciprocal teaching, strategic learning, and using self-regulation in journal writing. In the studies cited, the role of reflective questioning was often

Page 49: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

3 6

integral to the development of metacognitive skills. Researchers also posited the importance of metacognition in mathematics instruction which includes not only problem solving but all mathematical performance (Campione et al., 1989; Flavell, 1979; Garofalo & Lester, 1985; Long, 1986; Schoenfeld, 1987, 1989b; Weinert, 1987).

Metacognition and Mathematics Lester & Garofalo (1982) found that elementary

students do not routinely analyze problem information, monitor progress, or evaluate results. Schoenfeld (1985) asserts that even most college students have not developed many metacognitive skills.

Slife et al. (1985) established that certain aspects of metacognitive awareness can be distinguished both from general ability and from mathematical attainment. In this study, elementary students who were disabled in mathematics were matched with regular mathematics students based on IQ scores and performance on the same set of ten math problems. When compared on knowledge of their problem-solving skills (predicting their likely success rate on a

Page 50: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

37

given set of computations) and on ability to monitor their problem-solving performance (identifying their correct and incorrect solutions), the learning disabled students were less skilled in each form of metacognition. The results seem to suggest that knowing how to solve a problem is different from knowing that one knows how to solve it.

In an effort to analyze metacognitive aspects of mathematics performance, Garofalo and Lester (1985) present a cognitive-metacognitive framework that is directly relevant to performance on a wide range of mathematical tasks. Comprised of four categories of activities— orientation, organization, execution, and verification— the framework specifies key points where metacognitive decisions are likely to influence cognitive actions. Their intent was to provide a guide for selecting appropriate research tasks that would provide information for delineating the role of metacognition in the learning of mathematics and implementing metacognitive aspects into mathematics instruction.

Page 51: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

3 8

Teaching for Metacognition in MathematicsCurrently, many recommendations for mathematics

teaching are addressing the development of metacognitive skills. Long (1986) supports the need for mathematics teachers to promote learning through metacognition by suggesting that teaching concentrate on a meaningful approach, making the connections between familiar knowledge and more advanced ideas explicit. She believes this will allow the student to transfer previously gained metacognitive knowledge to a new learning situation. Time should be allowed for reflection, whole class discussions, and for answering all questions carefully. Students should be encouraged to check their work often, perform inverse operations when possible, and identify strategies used repeatedly. Finally, teachers should provide short daily formative feedback to allow students the opportunity to clarify redeemable mistakes.

Birken (1986) suggests that students be taught to study mathematics within the context and content of the course. Two of her suggested strategies directly confront metacognitive issues: (a) students should identify their own problem-solving strategies and

Page 52: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

39

listen to others, and (b) students should identify where their mathematics study skills break down and then develop their own coping strategies. Both suggestions seek to make the student more aware of capabilities as well as deficits.

Campione et al. (1989) have extended reciprocal teaching to mathematics instruction in a project which involves beginning algebra word problems and uses students who have the ability to perform algorithms correctly but are unable to give evidence of the conceptual understanding underlying their use. Using small cooperative groups, the teacher embodied expert modeling, scaffolding, and coaching in the methods utilized with a focus on externalizing strategies, monitoring progress, and imposing meaning. Successive chalkboards for (a) planning, (b) representing, and (c) doing were used by a person designated as the learning leader. This leader was to help the other students proceed systematically through producing an external record of the group's work that could be monitored, evaluated, and reflected upon. After 2 0 days of instruction on single-variable linear equations, two-variable linear equations, and monomial

Page 53: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

by binomial equations, the experimental group outperformed the control group on both the target and the transfer problems. As in other studies, this example of reciprocal teaching initially featured the teacher modeling, and the student practicing metacognitive, self-regulatory skills as a strategy for learning. Students are then progressively made more responsible for selecting and monitoring the approaches they use.

This emphasis on teaching strategies that promote the development of metacognitive skills such as reflection about one's thinking and self-regulation was also cited in the use of computer programs. The design of instructional materials, no longer confined to procedural and declarative knowledge, was extended to insights into the cognitive processes that underlie the effective learning of complex topics.

Collins and Brown (1986) use the computer as a tool for focusing a student's attention directly on the thought processes being used. An example is a computer program which keeps a record of the student's attempts when solving an equation so the student can review previous work. After seeing which attempts

Page 54: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 1

were unsuccessful, the student can use the insights from these reflections to provide the direction for future attempts.

Narode (1985) uses pair problem solving with college-level remedial mathematics students as a strategy for engaging students in mathematics while monitoring their thoughts. This method, a variation of group work, calls for one student to solve the problem while the second student listens carefully or asks questions until both clearly follow the solution. The problems used are meticulously selected to provide examples that are challenging but not frustrating.The students are then asked to alternate their roles. To initiate this method, the teacher models the role of a clinical interviewer so the student can learn what is expected.

Research on Metacoanition in MathematicsSimilarly, interrogation has been used in

research by Schoenfeld (1985). Schoenfeld suggests four classroom techniques that he uses to focus on metacognition in a mathematics problem-solving class. The first is the use of videotapes of students doing

Page 55: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 2

problem solving for the purpose of promoting self- awareness through watching and discussing their behavior as well as the behavior of others. These activities produce a receptiveness to the interventionist techniques used later in the term. A second technique is the modeling of "expert performance" by the instructor. This performance illustrates the processes which yield the polished product or the solution of a problem. This modeling may include working through a problem step-by-step, including tentative explorations with evaluations throughout, and a "post-mortem" where the whole solution process is reviewed. The third technique is whole-class discussions with the teacher as a moderator— serving only as scribe and orchestrator.By asking for several proposed solutions, the class is forced to focus on control decisions. Later, the discussions are analyzed for the efficiency of the processes selected by the class. The fourth and last technique suggested is problem solving in small group settings. The teacher assumes the role of coach, moving from group to group as a problem-solving consultant. Groups understand that the "coach" may

Page 56: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

43

ask, at any time, (a) if they can describe what they are doing? (b) if they can answer why they are doing it? and (c) do they know how it will help them? As group dynamics become established, the teacher's role is minimal with students monitoring their work without the intervention of the instructor.

In an effort to research changes in monitoring behavior as these techniques were used, Schoenfeld charted the time spent by his students on six stages of problem solving. The six stages identified for this study were: read, analyze, explore, plan, implement, and verify. At the end of the semester, these charts were compared to the problem-solving behavior of an expert and revealed the students no longer spent all of their time on their first guess but evaluated their progress and attempted alternate solutions. Although Schoenfeld does not believe the acquisition of metacognitive skills will guarantee success, he purports these skills will at least give access to it.

Bell (1991) is conducting a two-year research program on the metacognitive aspects of mathematical learning and teaching. The aims of the project are to

Page 57: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 4

(a) identify and describe the levels of awareness on the part of pupils of their mathematical knowledge, and of their learning processes, (b) investigate relations between these levels of awareness and characteristics of the classroom environment and teaching style, (c) develop tactics by which awareness may be enhanced, and (d) evaluate the effects of such enhancement on the pupils' mathematical learning (p.1). At present, researchers in this study are developing two kinds of materials. The first is evaluative tests and procedures for describing and quantifying pupils' awareness, and the second is "tactics" for classroom use to enhance pupils' awareness. Pilot tests include having students estimate their chance of answering a question successfully or having students indicate their level of confidence in their answers. Analysis of this data is presently underway (Alan Bell, personal interview, April 16, 1991).

A study of initial interviews, however, suggest that students do distinguish between being "able to perform" and "understanding." The meaning of such statements are now being probed by the research team

Page 58: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 5

(Bell, 1991). Future work will address topics such as pupil-constructed tests, metaphors for learning, and concept maps as this study of pupils' awareness of learning continues.

In conclusion, current research in the area of metacognition and mathematics education supports the belief that metacognitive skills are a needed, but not necessarily sufficient, condition for learning mathematics. The studies cited focus on promoting reflective thinking and rely heavily on reflective questioning to achieve this goal. Narode (1985) carries the interrogation a step further and suggests that expressive writing could be a vehicle for promoting this reflection.

Writing to Learn Writing to learn is based on the theory that

writing is powerful not only in presenting knowledge but also in producing knowledge (Connolly, 1989). Writing about a concept being taught is viewed as a process by which students can learn a content area. This process encourages the student to organize his thoughts, commit them to writing and, finally, to

Page 59: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

46analyze what has been said. In writing to learn, the emphasis shifts away from formal correctness toward context, meaning, and the process of writing.

Theoretical Underpinnings of WritingDiscussions of writing to learn commonly cite the

theoretical works of Vygotsky (1962), Bruner (1960, 1972) and Britton et al. (1975) which focus on the role of language in the forming of meaning. Britton has examined language based on its functional use and, consequently, distinguishes between written language that is directed toward an audience, which he terms transactional writing, and language written for personal use, which he terms expressive. Britton sees expressive writing as being preliminary and exploratory writing that functions to express the writer's feelings and thoughts. This generation of exploratory writing, which is believed to be closest to the self, has value as a basis for writing to learn.

In her classic article, "Writing as a Mode of Learning," Emig (1977) initiates a case for writing as having a unique value for learning. She views writing

Page 60: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 7

as the processing of information on Bruner's three levels of actuality: the enactive, the iconic, and the symbolic. Bell and Bell (1985) restates Emig's observation of writing by the following description of the three named levels: the motor level, the hand moving across the paper; the sensory level, the eye reading what has been written; and the analysis level, the mind processing what has been written.

Emig argues that writing parallels four powerful learning strategies: multi-representational and integral, connective, personally engaged, and those which generate self-provided feedback. All can be specifically achieved by writing. The multi- representational strategy has been addressed above. Writing is connective because it forces organization, grouping, synthesis, and analysis. Writing actively engages the student at a personal rhythm; that is, it is self-paced. Finally, the provision for self­provided feedback, which is particularly important to this inquiry, enables the learner to immediately review and evaluate the product of one's own thought. Emig's article very often provides the underpinnings

Page 61: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

4 8

for the many articles and research on the use of writing in learning that have appeared in the last decade.

Writing in Content AreasAs early as the 1950s, support for writing to

learn was emerging in post-war programs for returning GI's (Russell, 1987). Examples of these were the Functional Writing Program at Colgate (1949-1961) and the Prose Improvement Committee at the University of California at Berkeley (1950-1965). Both of these programs recognized the capacity of writing to improve learning but, nonetheless, these early beginnings of writing across the curriculum still died out after more than a decade of successful operation.

Since the 1950s, a considerable number of articles have appeared with anecdotal support for the use of writing to aid learning. Specifically, these uses include improvement of basic skills, reading-writing interaction, the study of introductory psychology, and the learning of mathematics as well as its traditional use in English classes. An article by Dittmer (1986) presents general guidelines for writing

Page 62: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

assignments in content areas and specific information for the areas of mathematics, physics, accounting, and biology. Two extensive qualitative dissertation studies in the 1980s have also focused on writing to learn. Books are appearing in the science areas; Connolly and Vilardi (1989) edited Writing to Learn in Mathematics and Science, and Sterret (1990) edited Using Writing to Teach Mathematics. As evidenced by the number of publications devoted to the subject, writing in the mathematics classroom has growing support among educators.

Goodkin (1982), in her qualitative study of the "intellectual consequences of writing," provides a review of the theoretical support for the relationship of the writing process to the thinking/learning process, the established uses of writing, and the innovative writing strategies currently being implemented in the classroom. Her empirical study utilized ethnographic strategies to investigate case studies in four separate content areas: psychology, clinical nursing and dental assisting, entomology, and calculus and basic mathematics.

Page 63: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

From analysis of the case studies, Goodkin classified student uses of writing as personal, curricular, and universal. The personal uses of writing include examples ranging from lists and notes to journal entries and functions in supporting intrapersonal relationships, summarizing, and exploring and controlling feelings. Curricular uses included writing assignments which helped the students establish or improve intellectual skills such as to summarize, focus, think through, clarify, infer and abstract. Finally, universal uses, which included flash-cards, brief in-class writings, reports and research papers, provided writing exercises for reflecting, organizing, comparing and contrasting, self-testing, and thinking analytically.

In general, personal, informal writing promoted introspection and self-discovery whereas the other writings supported intellectual accommodation.Goodkin felt the study established that writing fostered thinking and concentration and represented a personal search for meaning.

However, although many support writing to learn, others still question the relationship between writing

Page 64: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

5 1

and learning. For example, Applebee (1984) questions what he calls the unexamined assumption of a relationship between writing and learning. Although the research evidence available is consistent with the ideas that writing promotes rational thought and a better understanding of the subject, Applebee feels it does not compel acceptance. He argues that "the research that does exist suggests a broad agenda for future work" (1984, p. 590).

The belief that writing promotes learning has theoretical and anecdotal support that dates back to the early 1950s. Theoretical arguments view writing as instrumental in personally developing meaning (Britton et al., 1975; Bruner, 1960; Vygotsky, 1962). The anecdotal support is evidenced by the plethora of journal articles expounding the benefits of using writing to promote learning in the content areas. Goodkin (1982), in her doctoral dissertation, provides additional support for the intellectual consequences of writing in general and in several specific content areas including the study of calculus and basic mathematics. A closer consideration of the use of writing in mathematics follows in the next section.

Page 65: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

5 2

Writing in Mathematics Rose (1989a), in her dissertation study, provides

extensive information on how the use of expressive writing can benefit the student, the teacher, and student-teacher communication, and provides extensive information on the use of expressive writing for learning mathematics. Her conceptual analysis of expressive writing delineates benefits in three areas: students as writers, teachers as readers, and reader- writer interaction. The student, as writer, benefits in the affective domain by extroverting negative feelings about mathematics, in content through verbalizing experiences and impressions, in developing processes through greater awareness of the methods being used, in conceptual understanding by exploring their beliefs about mathematics personally. In general, growth is transferable to a variety of contexts through enhanced "critical thinking" and the adoption of a reflective attitude toward learning both in school and in social settings.

Teachers benefit from reading students' writings through the diagnostic information provided, course feedback, and data for instructional improvement.

Page 66: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

53

When a student writes about mathematical concepts, understanding is revealed at a level not possible in the traditional classroom setting. Responding to student writings allows the teacher to individualize instruction and provide support as needed. The information provided by student writings allows the teacher to evaluate the effectiveness of instruction, the textbook, classroom procedures or management, and testing. Feedback may also provide surprising information about student errors or misconceptions or about the teachers themselves. Finally, the writing dialogue can afford a level of communication which establishes a better rapport between the teacher and the students, and a personalized environment for learning which is thought to be motivating for both the students and the teacher.

In addition, Rose (1989a) conducted an empirical study to further understand her conceptual analysis of the benefits of expressive writing presented earlier in this chapter. In this study, the writing activities, which included autobiographical writing, unstructured and structured dialogue journal entries written outside of class, and in-class focused

Page 67: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

writings, were an integrated component of a regular mathematics course. In general, the students in this study wrote more often about their feelings about mathematics or the course and less often about course content, their ways of doing mathematics, or their conception of mathematics. As the study progressed, however, students broadened their range of topics and related numerous ways they perceived writing as beneficial to their learning of mathematics. In particular, students viewed unstructured journal writing as having the greatest potential for creating their own need and affording the opportunity to meet that need in the writers' own way and at their own pace. Restated, the majority of the students felt journal writing helped them to write specifically about what they did not understand and to generate questions that could be asked of the instructor. Further, as Rose points out, these entries can make the teacher aware of collective needs of the class and, in turn, can provide individual diagnosis and remediation.

Using qualitative methods, Rose examined the characteristics of students' journal writing and their

Page 68: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

5 5

perceived benefits of these activities and found support for her conceptual framework. In her summary, Rose states students find journals most productive when they:

write about what they do not understand; write about specific. current material; write in their own words; solve problems as far as they can go; ask questions; stay current and consistent with the writing; are honest; and, are open; writing will not work unless it is given a fair chance.(p. 290)Writing was summarized as being beneficial to the

teacher as reader, because reading "facilitated individual diagnosis and evaluation; increased sensitivity to students; personalization; provided different points of view; provided short-term benefits for the course; provided long-term benefits for teaching" (p. 322).

For the student-teacher relationship, writing provided interaction which "promoted better one-to-one communication; provided better feedback from both parties; fostered open and comfortable relationships;

Page 69: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

5 6

provided encouragement and stimulated motivation; fostered an involved and caring classroom" (p. 323).

Research on Writing and MathematicsAs cited earlier, Applebee (1984) has indicated

there is little hard research to support the effects of writing to learn. In the area of mathematics, available research yields little deviation from this observation. In a study with students in a remedial mathematics course in an inner-city college, Pallmann (1982) found students benefitted affectively from writing about math by showing a significant difference in the retention and absentee rates of the class which used writing as an integral part of the instruction format. Statistical analysis of the pre- and posttests (California Achievement Tests) showed a higher average for the experimental group but the difference was not significant. The researcher interpreted these results as favoring the treatment group since the higher retention rate suggested the weaker students remained in the experimental class.

Weiss and Walters (1980) examined if subject- related writing tasks assigned in college courses

Page 70: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

would affect amount and clarity of student learning, writing performance, and apprehension about writing. Four disciplines— research and statistics, reading, physical science, and educational psychology— were studied. Significant differences at the .01 level were found for clarify in learning with writing tasks. Amounts of learning were higher for the experimental classes but the difference was significant at the .05 level for only the statistics class. No significant differences were found for writing apprehension although decreases were shown in all four areas. Writing ability increased in all but the physical science class but no differences were significant. Weiss and Walters suggest additional research in other disciplines and at different levels are now indicated to support their findings.

Hirsh and King (1983) tested the effect of writing assignments as compared to traditional assignments on the performance of college students in an elementary algebra course. The writing assignments asked students to respond to conceptual questions whereas the traditional assignments included exercises on algebraic topics covered in class lectures. No

Page 71: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

58

significant differences in achievement were found between the experimental and control groups. However, in this study, the writing was not an integral part of the class format as it was in the study by Pallmann (1982) .

Bell and Bell (1985) studied the effect of expressive writing on problem-solving abilities in two ninth grade general math classes. In this study, students in the experimental section expressed in words whether or not they understood the material, and used paragraphs to verbally describe their numerical processes. After a four-week period, significant differences were found at the .01 level in favor of the experimental group. Again, a basic premise of this study is that the writing component must be perceived as integral to the teaching process.

Selfe, Peterson, and Nahrgang (1986) conducted a study with calculus classes and used journal assignments about mathematical concepts throughout the ten-week session. The students in this study perceived the writing as a positive addition to the class and some students reported using the journal to think and learn about mathematics. Although the

Page 72: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

5 9

writing was an integral part of the teaching process, no significant differences in students' performance on content-area tests, students' attitudes towards mathematics, and students' writing apprehension were obtained in this study.

Others have stated that writing activities can help clarify attitudes that students have developed toward mathematics (Farkas, 1981). In an article on expressive writing, Farkas theorizes that students who use writing to express their feelings learn to cope with their emotions and have more energy to put into the academics and thus improve their work. In secondary algebra classes, Miller and England (1989) employed daily impromptu writing prompts which were placed in four major categories: contextual prompts, instructional prompts, reflective prompts, and other prompts. The Lewis Atkins Revised Mathematics Attitude Scale was administered to three experimental and four control groups. One of the Algebra II experimental groups showed a statistically significant improvement in attitude at the .01 level at the end of the year.

Page 73: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

6 0

In the area of mathematics, numerous publications focus on student and teacher benefits of writing. Geeslin (1977) sees writing as a diagnostic tool for the instructor and a learning tool for the student.The amount of literature on writing and mathematics in the last ten years supports this premise; however, little hard data exists to further substantiate this claim. The research available has shown some significant results in the area of retention, attitude toward mathematics, problem solving abilities in a basic math class, and amount of learning in a statistics class. Again, as Applebee (1984) stated, much research remains to be done.

Using Writing to Promote Metacognition Believing that metacognition is "facilitated

through oral and written communication of think as-it- happens" (p.13), Narode (1985) requires students to write about their thoughts as they think them, to read them, and to reflect upon them. The length of these papers, sometimes called thought-process protocols, can range from 3 00 to 600 words and include pictures, charts, diagrams, and equations. Narode feels this

Page 74: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

6 1

method to be useful in monitoring one's own thoughts, but he also argues the need for more research into the value of writing in order to identify the particular benefits.

Klein and Vukovich (1990) employ journal writing to promote metacognitive skills in remedial algebra classes. Topics used for journal entries include "My Math Past" which asked the student to focus on past experiences in mathematics and feelings of competence, "Test Preparation" which had a student identify specific learning strategies he possessed and also made the instructor aware of the student's needs,"Goal Setting" which sought to increase a student's self-monitoring ability, and "Time to Evaluate" which forced the student to make attributions to a recent test score. These journal entries, which focused on revealing student cognition, allowed the instructor to guide the student into more self-directed and controlled thinking.

SummaryThis chapter provided support for the belief that

metacognitive skills are a necessary but not

Page 75: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

sufficient condition for learning mathematics.Research supports the belief that metacognitive skills are exhibited by the successful student or expert whereas the less successful learner does not exhibit these skills. A case has been made for the cognitive and affective benefits of using writing as a tool for learning in the mathematics classroom. Writing, because it promotes reflection and provides a record for review, is suggested by Narode (1985) and Klein and Vukovich (1991) as a viable means of promoting metacognitive skills. However, neither qualitative nor quantitative research is available to support this assertion.

Page 76: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

CHAPTER THREE METHODOLOGY

This study examined students' awareness of both the accuracy of the method chosen to answer a mathematical question and the accuracy of the implementation of the chosen method. The findings include implications for identifying: (a) the metacognitive skill of self-awareness these students possess, (b) the relationship of this metacognitive skill to effective learning as evidenced by achievement scores on five quizzes, and (c) the role of the writing process in enhancing this self- awareness .

MethodSample

The sample for this study was four sections of Developmental Algebra 092 at a four-year regional university in the Spring Semester, 1991. Students are placed in this course when their ACT math score is 17 or below and they score between 10 and 2 0 on a 2 5- item, in-house, content-based, introductory algebra test.

63

Page 77: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

6 4

The four classes had initial enrollments ofapproximately 35 students and were taught by the sameinstructor, not the researcher. Two sections were taught on Tuesday and Thursday mornings for 75 minutes each and two sections were taught on Monday,Wednesday, and Friday mornings for 50 minutes each.

Demographic Findings. An informal survey was used to collect certain demographic information.Frequencies for math ACT [American College Testing] scores revealed that two-thirds of the group fell in a score span of 14-16 which is just below the 50thpercentile nationwide. A salient finding was that 93%of the students participating in this study had completed at least an introductory algebra course during their college preparation. Approximately 48% had credit for three or more years of secondary mathematics. When asked to indicate their expectation of how well they thought they would do in this course, 17% responded "all right," 50% responded "not very well," and 32% responded "very poor." In general, these students were enrolled in a course repeating content they had been taught in high school and, yet, these students expected to do only moderately well.

Page 78: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

6 5

Course Description. Developmental Algebra 092 [DVMA 092] is a three-hour, non-credit course focusing on introductory algebra concepts. These concepts include performing basic operations (on integers, rational numbers, polynomials, rational expressions, and radical expressions), solving linear equations in one or two variables, and solving linear inequalities in one variable.

The presentation of these concepts centers on pedagogy which promotes active class participation with responsibility for learning shared by the student and the instructor. Initial learning experiences utilize directed discovery methods and cooperative group learning. Since current research dictates the necessity of structured learning for low-achieving students, teacher-centered instruction is ultimately provided before closure on a concept.

The final grade for students in DVMA 092 is determined from the results of five 20-point announced quizzes, five 10-point unannounced quizzes, and four 100-point announced tests. Each quiz contains only new content, but all tests are cumulative. The 250-point cumulative final contains 25 content-based

Page 79: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

6 6

questions. Developmental courses are taught on a pass-fail basis with a "P" assigned for satisfactory work and a "U" for unsatisfactory or failing work. To receive a passing grade, "P", the student must earn 560 of the possible 800 points with at least 150 points earned on the final. A total of 65 bonus points can be earned by utilizing the tutorial services for remediating errors on announced tests and quizzes. In essence, the format of the course places the emphasis on student ability at the end of the course with substantive support available throughout the course for those who maintain an active effort. Students must earn a satisfactory grade in this course in order to enroll in the next course.

Data SelectionDemographic Data

This study collected data reflecting certain demographic data, student perception of correct work, and achievement scores. The demographic data included each student's age, gender, ACT mathematics score, English placement, and background in college and high school mathematics coursework. Also, affective

Page 80: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

67

information was collected in questionnaire form concerning the student's expectation for the course and attitude towards mathematics (See Appendix A).

Achievement DataAchievement was defined as the points earned on

each 20-point announced quiz and the scores on the pretest and posttest which were parallel forms of the fourth test in the course (See Appendix B). Each quiz contained 10 free-response questions based only on material introduced since the last evaluation. The fourth test in this course contained 20 free-response questions based on the course content.

Metacoonitive DataData was collected on the student's awareness of

the correctness of an answer on the above evaluations using two indicators. The first was the student's assessment of the correctness of the method chosen to answer a question; the second was the assessment of the correctness of the implementation of the chosen method. The student indicated whether he or she was

Page 81: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

68very certain, fairly certain, or not certain of the correctness for each assessment (See Appendix B).

TreatmentsWriting Treatment

Procedure. The writing treatment consisted of using impromptu writing prompts (Miller & England, 1989) which were daily in-class expressive writings where the student responded for five minutes to a given question or statement. This time period was selected as a manageable period for the student, by limiting the time the student has to organize and present thoughts, and for the instructor, by limiting the amount of reading when reviewing student responses (Miller, 1991).

Instrument. The writing prompts consisted of statements or questions which directed students thinking toward a greater awareness of understanding of the mathematical concepts presented, the processes used with these concepts, and self-evaluation of the effectiveness of these processes as they reflected upon their homework and examinations (See Appendix C).

Page 82: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

69

The following represent a sampling of the prompts used:

Look at the following problem. Is the problem worked correctly? Explain your reasoning. If the problem is incorrect, discuss where the mistake was made and why you think this may have happened.

Pick a problem from your quiz that you found the most difficult. Tell me what you find hard or confusing. Try to be as specific as possible.

Describe one topic covered in this chapter that has been the most difficult. Explain why you think you had difficulty understanding this. Do you understand it now? If yes, can you describe how you came to understand this concept?

On the test you have been returned, find the question that you understood the best. Tell me why you think you understood this concept so well.Look over the items that you missed on your test. Can you tell me why you think you missed the problems that you did? Be as specific as you can||

When asked to square a binomial, many students say, 11 (x + 5) 2 equals x2 + 52". Is this correct? If not, explain how this binomial is squared.The prompts were distributed daily on three-part

paper (5" by 8") and collected at the end of the five- minute period. The instructor responded to the writings individually with comments encouraging further student reflection or providing direction for future study. For example, the instructor asked questions such as, "How will this help you?" or "Can

Page 83: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

7 0

you tell me more?" Three-part paper, which generatestwo carbonless copies, allowed a copy of the student'swritten response with the instructor's comments to bereturned to the student at the next class meeting, asecond copy to be kept by the instructor forreflection, and the third copy to be kept by theresearcher to monitor student participation.Lecture Treatment

The lecture treatment consisted of includingreflective questioning in the class lecture format.After being given a few minutes to think about theiranswers, students could respond to these questionsverbally. The questions paralleled the reflectivequestioning included in the writing prompts— questionsdirecting student thinking toward a greater awarenessof conceptual understanding of content presented, theprocesses used with these concepts, andself-evaluation of the effectiveness of theseprocesses. Examples are given in the following:

Why is x2 + 9 not factorable over the real numbers?Why did we agree to find the GCF first when factoring?Factor 8a(b +6) - (b + 6). How would you explain why your answer is correct?

Page 84: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

7 1

How do you know when an expression is completely factored? Explain your answer in detail.

DesignThe four groups used in this study were four

intact classes as described previously. All of these classes were taught by the same instructor, not the researcher. The classes were randomly assigned intact as Group A, B, C, or D.

Group D, with 34 students, was taught at 8:00 a.m. on Tuesdays and Thursdays and served as a control for the influence of the pretest. Only the demographic and achievement data outlined above was collected.

For Group C, which met at 9:30 a.m. on Tuesdays and Thursdays and contained 33 students, the information collected included the demographic and achievement data described above. In addition, on the pretest, the students were asked to indicate their perception of the correctness of their work as outlined in the above description. No other intervention occurred for Group C until the post-test when the students were again asked to indicate their perception of the correctness of their work.

Page 85: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

72

The data collected from Group B, taught at 10:00 a.m. on Mondays, Wednesdays, and Fridays, included the demographic and achievement data described above. In addition, students in this group of 34 were also asked to indicate awareness of the correctness of their answers on all five quizzes, the pretest, and the posttest as for Group C. This group received the lecture treatment described in a later section.

Group A, taught at 11:00 a.m. on Mondays, Wednesdays, and Fridays, and also containing 34 students had data collected as described for Group B. In addition, this group performed daily impromptu writing by responding to a given prompt.

The chart in Figure 1 illustrates the data considered from each of the four groups.

DATA SOURCE

GroupDemo Pretest Quizzes Writing Posttest

A X X X X XB X X X XC X X XD X X

Figure 1. Data considered from each group.

Page 86: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

73

ProcedureOn the first day of class, the students in all

four groups were asked to complete the informal survey. Groups A, B, and C also completed the single­form pre-test at this time. On the pretest, all students were asked to indicate their certainty of the correctness of the method chosen and its implementation when answering each question. Students were instructed that this test was to provide diagnostic information to the instructor concerning the background information of the students for the purpose of assisting the instructor in meeting the individual needs of the class. Students were allowed as much time as was needed; however, none of the groups exceeded thirty minutes.

For the first four of the five quizzes given during the semester, students in Groups A and B were asked to indicate their certainty of the correctness of the method chosen and its implementation. Since these groups met at consecutive class times, the same form of the quiz was used for both groups.

The same form of test four was given to all four groups at the end of the semester. It should be noted

Page 87: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

74

that tests one through three, which were not a part of this study, used different forms of the test in each class to ensure test validity for test four where the same form was used for all four groups in the study. Test four was a cumulative test of the concepts taught in this course and students were also asked to indicate their certainty of the correctness of the method chosen and its implementation.

Evaluation of DataDemographic Data

The informal survey provided demographic information about students' ACT scores, English placement, number of years of college preparatory mathematics courses, age, and gender. These data, together with the pretest, were used to establish group equivalency.

Metacoqnitive DataFor each question, the student's work was coded

for correct method/incorrect method and for correct work/incorrect work by a developmental mathematics faculty member who was not the instructor in the study

Page 88: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

7 5

nor the researcher. The method selected was considered correct if it represented a viable, but not necessarily efficient, method for reaching a correct solution. Otherwise, the method was coded as incorrect. Correctness for the implementation of the method was more easily coded, with an agreement reached to code a careless error as correct work. Explanations for coding of a sample of specific test questions are included in Appendix D.

The instructor in this study also independently coded the students1 work and these results were compared with the original coding to determine reliability coefficients (See Appendix E).

In an effort to quantify awareness of correct work, numerical values of 2, 1, or 0 were assigned for each indicator as illustrated in Table 1.Table 1Point assignment for codings

Indication

Very Certain Fairly Certain Not certain Correct 2 1 0Incorrect 0 1 2

Page 89: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

This method of scoring gave the highest value when performance matched perception— a student performing correctly and knowing the work was correct, and likewise, a student performing incorrectly and knowing the work was incorrect. The indication, "fairly certain," was ranked next. The lowest value was assigned to performance not in agreement with perception— students performing incorrectly and thinking the work was correct, and students working the problem correctly but having no confidence in their answer.

The numerical assignments were totaled for each quiz, the pretest, and the posttest to yield scores for awareness, or ability to assess correct work.

Data Analysis To test the effects of treatment over time, an

analysis of variance for repeated measures was used to determine if significant differences in self- assessment gains from quiz one to quiz four occurred between Group A and Group B.

Analysis of variance for repeated measures was used with the pre- and posttest awareness scores to

Page 90: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

77

test the influence of the in-class expressive writings upon the student's ability to assess the correctness of work on the posttest. To test the influence of writing on achievement, an analysis of variance for repeated measures was also performed with achievement on the pre- and posttests.

SummaryThis chapter described the sample, the writing

treatment, the data collected, and the methods for analyzing the data. Chapter 4 will present the findings of the study, while Chapter 5 will summarize the study and its implications for further research.

Page 91: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

CHAPTER 4 ANALYSIS OF THE DATA

The purpose of this study was to determine: (1) the ability of college introductory algebra students to assess the correctness of the method chosen and its implementation when answering questions on a quiz; (2) the effect of experience on this ability; (3) the effect of in-class expressive writing on this ability; and (4) the effect of in-class expressive writing on achievement on tests and quizzes.

Group Equivalency Testing Analysis of variance was used to determine if the

four groups were significantly different in pretest achievements scores, pretest awareness scores, math ACT subscores, English ACT subscores, and number of years of college preparatory mathematics courses. Results showed no significant differences in the four groups on any of these measures. Since no significant differences were found on the variables tested, it was determined the groups were equivalent at the beginning of this study. The results are given in Appendix F.

78

Page 92: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

79Effects of Pretesting

Group D, the group for which only a posttest was analyzed, was included to determine if the pre-test had an effect on the control group, Group C, in this study. An analysis of variance was performed on the posttest achievement scores and the posttest awareness scores. The results found for the posttest achievement scores were F(2,97) = .21, p < .80, MS = 213.63 and for the posttest awareness scores, F(2,77) = 1.04, p < .36, MS = 209.01. Since no significant differences were found on the variables tested, it was determined the pretest did not affect the control group, Group C. Group D was not used in the analysis of the data after this point.

Analyses and Results Question One Analysis

The first research question examined was: Whenasked to do so, can students in a college-level introductory algebra class consistently assess the accuracy of the method chosen and of its implementation when determining an answer?

Page 93: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 0

This question was addressed initially by analyzing the frequencies of the student assessment scores on the pretest and the posttest. As outlined in Chapter 3, the students had been asked to indicate their perception of: (1) the correctness of the method chosento answer each question and (2) the correctness of its implementation. The pre- and posttest each had 20 items resulting in a total of 40 indicators for each test with the highest score possible being 80. On the pretest, 50% of the participants made an assessment score 26 or below while on the posttest, only 8.5% were in this range. Additionally, on the posttest, 31% of the participants scored 56 or above while only 12% of the participants on the pretest were above this score.

For the writing and the lecture groups, the students were also asked to indicate their awareness of the correctness of their work on the first four 10-item quizzes given during the course. From this information, the number of correct methods chosen and correct implementatin was compared with the total number of methods chosen and methods implemented to determine a percentage for items correct. The results are given in Figure 3. Next, the percentage for the

Page 94: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

81

instances students were "very certain" of correct work is given as perceived correct in Table 2.

Table 2Student Perception of Correct Answers

QUIZ

1 2 3 4Items Correct 86.5% 88.2% 75.5% 74.7%Those Perceived Correct

69.8% 69.8% 40.5% 33.3%

Considering quizzes one and two, in general, the rate for students to correctly choose methods and/or implementations and indicate they were certain of the correctness of their work was approximately 80%. However, for quizzes three and four, these percentages dropped to approximately 54% and 45%, respectively, although the percentage of items correctly answered only dropped between 10 and 15.

A further examination of the quiz responses was made to investigate the frequencies of all of the codings on the quizzes. A correct method or implementation was coded with "C" and incorrect work

Page 95: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

with an "I." To code assessment of awareness, a "V" was used for "very certain” of correctness, an ”F" for "fairly certain,” and an "N" for "not certain." The possible codings were paired as follows: CV, CF, CN,IV, IF, and IN. When there were no indications of assessment, the second letter was left blank. For example, "CN" represents correct work where the student indicated "not certain," and "C" represents correct work with no certainty indicated. Figure 2 depicts the results of this analysis by a frequency bar graph.

14 Lpa....................................

12 -I|............................................

10 1|...........8 .................

6 1 |

4 -K|:

2 "iiq IMZL.

CV CF CN C I IV IF IN

I H Quiz 1 E 3 Quiz 2 EEEEl Quiz 3 ^ Quiz 4

i t ^

Figure 2. Frequencies of codings per student on quizzes.

Page 96: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

83

This chart suggests a decline in the number of responses but consideration of student absenteeism accounts in part for this change. Further analysis of the data shows only 11% of the indicators were blank.An analysis of response patterns reveals only 11 (4.7%) of the quizzes had no indicators at all and another 10 (4.3%) of the quizzes showed the student stopped making indications midway through the quiz. The remaining missing indicators were evenly interspersed.

Additional analysis was made by comparing each achievement score with the corresponding self-awareness score for each of the four quizzes. Using ranges of high, average, and low for both achievement and self- awareness, cross-tabs were run. The ranges are defined in Table 3 and the results of the cross-tabs are illustrated in Figure 3.

Table 3Ranges for Achievement and Awareness

Achievement Awareness

High 14 - 20 28 - 40Average 8 - 1 3 15 - 27Low 1 - 7 0 - 1 4

Page 97: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 4

QUIZ 1 ACHIEVEMENT

LOW AVG HIGHLOW 2 3AVG 1 3HIGH 2 52

N = 63QUIZ 3

ACHIEVEMENTLOW AVG HIC

LOW 2 4 13AVG 1 5 10HIGH 1 2 25

N = 63

SELFAWARENESS

QUIZ 2 ACHIEVEMENT

LOW AVG HIGHLOW 3AVG 1 9HIGH 2 49

N = 64QUIZ 4

ACHIEVEMENTLOW AVG HIGH

LOW 1 2 7AVG 1 8 9HIGH 1 1 15

N = 45Figure 3. Comparison of achievement scores with

awareness scores on each of the four quizzes.

As the semester progressed, it is evident student ability to assess the correctness of answers declined over time. However, as the course content increased in complexity, the achievement scores remained more stable than the self-awareness scores. On the first two quizzes, 97% and 95% of the sample was categorized as having high achievement. This number was 76% on quiz 3 and dropped to 69% on quiz 4. Similar percentages for

Page 98: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 5

high self-awareness on these quizzes were 86%, 80%, 44% and 38%, respectively. A comparison of awareness scores on Quiz 1 with awareness scores on Quiz 3 revealed a decrease for 69.4% of the students. Data suggest that the students in these introductory algebra classes did not consistently assess the accuracy of their work, nor did experience on quizzes over time increase this ability.

Analysis for Question TwoQuestion two asked the following: Does the ability

to accurately assess the correctness of the method and its implementation increase over time and with experience?

This question was addressed by analyzing the awareness scores on four of the quizzes given during the semester. To determine student ability to assess correct work over time on quizzes, an analysis of variance for repeated measures was performed for Groups A and B over four times: Quiz 1, Quiz 2, Quiz 3, and Quiz 4. No significant differences were found,F (3, 198) = .52, p < .66, MS = 52.7.

Page 99: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 6

Analysis for Question ThreeThe third research question asked: Does the

intervention of daily in-class expressive writing or lecture containing reflective questioning affect the ability of students to assess the accuracy of the method chosen and its implementation when determining an answer?

To determine the effect of in-class expressive writing or reflective questioning on ability to assess correct work over time on pretest and posttest, an analysis of variance for repeated measures was performed on awareness scores for the Groups A, B, and C over two times: pretest and posttest. No significant differences were found between these groups, F(2, 72) = .23, p < .79, MS = 311.5. However, significant differences were found within the groups over time,F (1, 72) = 57.52, p < .000, MS = 11356.34. A significant difference was also found between the groups by time, F(2, 72) = 5.22, p < .008, MS =1030.72. The cell means are given in Table 4.

Page 100: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 7

Table 4Six Cell Means for Awareness

Pretest PosttestWriting 22.852 50.222

Group Lecture 33.792 43.833Control 30.833 45.708

Note. Writing n=27; Lecture n=24; Control n=24.

The relationship of these means are illustrated in the graph given in Figure 4.

eo

40

30

20

Po»t

— Writing Ltoturo -t f-Control

Figure 4 . Graph of the interaction of the cell means.

Page 101: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 8

The graph indicates there is some interaction between the groups and the time of the tests. A oneway analysis of variance was performed on the differences between the pretest scores and the posttest scores. A significant difference was found between the groups,F(2, 72) =5.22, p < .008, MS = 2061.43. A TukeyB post hoc multiple comparison test was performed and the results are illustrated in Table 5.

Table 5Tukev's Pairwise ComparisonsMean Group Lecture Control Writing

10.14 Lecture14.86 Control27.37 Writing * *

These results suggest the gains in awareness scores by the writing group are significantly greater than the gains in scores for the control group and the lecture group.

Analysis of Question FourThe fourth research question asked: Does the

intervention of daily in-class expressive writing or

Page 102: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

8 9

lecture containing reflective questioning affect the achievement of students? To determine the effect of writing or reflective questioning on achievement on the posttest, an analysis of variance for repeated measures was performed on achievement scores for the Groups A,B, and C over two times: Pretest and Posttest. No significant differences were found between these groups by time, F(2, 97) = .18, p < .83, MS = 79.42.

SummaryAn analysis of the indications given for student

perception of correct work revealed that college-level introductory algebra students declined over time in ability to assess correct work on quizzes which questioned new material only. This decline is suggested by the students' unwillingness to indicate confidence in their work. However, the ability to assess correct work again increased from the pretest to the posttest which contained questions on the content of the course.

Statistical analyses at the beginning of this study suggest the groups were initially equivalent on the measures named. After interventions of expressive

Page 103: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

writing and reflective questioning, an analysis of variance for repeated measures was performed with measures of achievement and awareness on pretest and posttest. No significant differences in achievement were found for the three groups. The analysis did show a significant difference in the interaction between the groups. Post hoc comparisons were made to examine the differences in the pretest awareness scores and the posttest awareness scores. The writing group was found to be significantly different from the lecture and control groups in the ability to assess correctness of answers on tests.

Page 104: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

CHAPTER 5DISCUSSIONS, CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS

AND IMPLICATIONS The primary purpose of this study was to examine

the influence of in-class expressive writing on student awareness of correct work. Through having students indicate the certainty of the correctness of their work on quizzes and tests and comparing this indication to the actual correctness, the researcher sought to validate a means by which awareness of correct work could be measured. As a result, the ability of students to assess the correctness of their work became a consideration. Finally, the study sought to examine the influence of writing upon student achievement as evidenced by scores on quizzes and tests. This chapter includes interpretations of the results of this study, conclusions formed on the basis of these results, limitations relative to this study, and implications for classroom practice and further research.

DiscussionRecalling that incorrect work for which students

were ”not certain” of their responses earned the maximum 2-point value along with correct work for which

91

Page 105: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

students indicated "very certain" for their responses, it becomes conceivable that a pretest, which should contain much unknown content, could yield high awareness scores based on the first condition described. Likewise, the awareness scores could also be high for the posttest, which contained content that had become familiar, based on the second condition described. Predictably, the posttest achievement scores substantially increased from the pretest achievement scores. Analysis of the differences between the pre- and posttest awareness scores, however, showed considerable gain between these measurements, also.

An item analysis for the codings on the quizzes did not show the same increase in awareness scores. Quizzes, which tested only new material, remained relatively high in achievement, showing only a moderate decline for the third and fourth scores. The awareness scores, in comparison, were initially lower and then declined substantially for the third and fourth quizzes. On the first two quizzes, students were certain of correct work 80% of the time. On the remaining two quizzes, the percentage declined to 50.

Page 106: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Analysis of the responses for the quizzes also revealed a decline in the number of indications made by students. As the material increased in quantity and became more complex/ achievement scores declined on quizzes and students were less willing, less able, or just less likely to indicate the certainty of their answers. Nonetheless, this reluctance, unwillingness or inability demonstrated on the quizzes decreased on the posttest as compared to the pretest. An effort to explain these data might question the sensitivity of the instrument in this study. This explanation would be consistent with this study's theoretical framework which regards metacognition as an aid to learning new material.

However, other explanations are possible. These findings may suggest the framework for the study is inaccurate, and the development of metacognitive skills for a concept follows, rather than parallels, the development of the matching cognitive skills. This could be explained in reference to two theories. One theory posits that the rules students learn in algebra are not the rules they are taught. Kirshner (1989, 1987) argues that during the learning stage in the study of algebra, students are acquiring a system of implicit rules that underlies algebraic performance. These rules are believed to unconscious rules, rather like the rules of

Page 107: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

94

grammar which a child acquires through interaction with a language community. Curricular rules may be only indirectly related to this implicit grammar.Alternately, Bereiter (1991) believes that experts are not using rules at all but are actually acquiring a connectionist pattern— a vast network of interrelated elements— that only approximates rule-based performance.

To explain the apparent phenomemon of introspection to rational or rule-based knowledge structures, Bereiter (1991) cites Harre's theory of the social nature of rationality. Harre' believes that when attempts are made to give a retrospective report on mental processes, instead what is given is a justification of actions based on the categories of phenomena sanctioned within the community of discourse (e.g. the rules presented by the teacher in a classroom). Thus personal rationality is an internalization of rules which occurs when the social process of justification is turned inward. This would explain why students metacognition was evidenced mainly following, not concomitant with the learning process.

Statistical analysis of the gains in awareness scores for the writing, lecture, and control groups found the gains for the writing group to be significantly different from the gains for the lecture and control groups. Since writing about mathematics is a learned

Page 108: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

95behavior and 14 weeks is a limited amount of time for effecting a change in writing about mathematics, this finding is strengthened by the limited length of the treatment. The lack of quantitative research about writing in the current literature could be explained as an artifact of length of treatment time required.

In this study, the use of writing had no significant effect on the achievement scores of these students. Adiscussion of these findings occurs in the limitations section of this chapter and in the section for implications for future study.

Conclusions The following conclusions were formed after

examination of the results obtained in this study:(1) When students are not confident in their answers for

quizzes on newly learned material, they are somewhat less likely to indicate the certainty of the correctness of their answers.

(2) As content increased in quantity and complexity, correct work and accurate assessment did not remain static. Although students maintained achievement, correct assessment of correct work declined.

Page 109: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

(3) In-class expressive writing was found to have apositive effect on the metacognitive skill ofself-assessment of correctness of work.

(4) The use of writing did not have a significanteffect on the ability to answer questions on a test correctly.

Limitations of the Present Study This section discusses limitations which affect

the usefulness of this study to other academic researchers and to practitioners. The limitations are as follows:

Generalization of the FindingsThe findings of this study are appropriate for

introductory algebra students at the college level.The mean ACT mathematics score in geographic location for the sample used is lower than the national average thus, similarity in the ACT math scores would contribute significantly to the validity of any generalizations.

Page 110: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

97Instructor Preparation

The instructor in this study participated in a preliminary study by the researcher for the purpose of becoming familiar with the use of writing prompts in the mathematics classroom. Outside readings on the use of writing were also part of the instructor's training. In addition, the researcher worked closely with the instructor in coaching the writing process in the classroom. This training became an influence on the findings within this study.

Measure for AchievementPerhaps, the benefits of writing cannot be

measured by achievement tests that do not delineate between performance based on rote versus conceptual knowledge. Achievement tests, at the introductory algebra level, may measure a student's ability to manipulate algebraic symbols without conceptual underpinnings. Developmental students often admit to working the problems over and over— a possible example of rote learning.

Page 111: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Implications Implications for Classroom Practice

This study may stimulate the use of expressive writing as a technique for developing the metacognitive skill of self-awareness. Awareness during a mathematical task, in turn, supports the development of self-monitoring which implies a decision based on the awareness. This ability to manage one's learning is seen as an integral component in problem solving as well as all mathematical performance (Campione et al., 1989; Garofalo & Lester, 1985; Long, 1986; and Schoenfeld, 1987). For developmental mathematics students, this is evidenced in this researcher's finding that these students indicated they could work entire assignments incorrectly and not know this until the next class meeting (Allen, 1989).

The close monitoring provided by writing is needed to capture the growth in students' mathematical understanding not evident in the traditional data of objective achievement tests, classroom activities, and homework assignments. Miller (1991) views writing as a means of better assessing students' understanding. She asserts that giving students the opportunity to write

Page 112: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

99about concepts or algorithms provides a plethora of information about student understanding that might otherwise never be revealed. Thus, teachers should continue to explore writing in the mathematics classroom and collect case data and formal data on the effects of such writing for the purpose of gaining a better understanding of the learning process of different students.

Implications for Future StudyAlthough students were able to maintain

performance, they were not able to maintain ability to self-monitor. As new material was introduced and became more difficult, students' certainty about their answers decreased. This suggests a need to extend this study to item analyses of each cumulative test as well as quizzes on new material to more closely monitor the development of awareness as evidenced by the willingness to indicate the certainty of answers.Should reflection be introduced after some evidence of conceptual understanding has occurred? This avenue of inquiry would begin to answer questions on the best time to initiate reflective questioning and/or writing.

Page 113: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 0

As indicated earlier, attention must be given to the time frame that should be provided to allow students to develop their ability to perform reflective writing about mathematics. For writing to become the best indicator of conceptual knowledge, students need experience over time and more practice to allow for the development of the writing process. Replication of this study with high school mathematics students would allow a time frame of nine months in which to develop writing about mathematics.

Since efforts to capture the benefits of writing for increasing achievement have failed in several studies as indicated in Chapter 2, the concentration should be on the benefits of writing for promoting student understanding. At present, whether or not a student possesses the needed understanding is usually not evident until the study of intermediate or college level algebra. Blais (1988) describes the difference in learners who are novices as compared with experts by characterizing the experts as possessing the "essence" of a statement. As an example, Blais portrays "the essence of 2/7 + 3/7 as roughly two things plus three things, which are five things" (p. 624). Yet, this

Page 114: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

question from a placement test for entering freshmen college students was answered incorrectly by 43% of the respondents. Perhaps a tool which would measure the presence of this essence would enable researchers to measure the effect of writing upon the learning of mathematics.

SummaryWriting in the mathematics classroom has

previously received anecdotal support for its benefits to the learner and to the instructor. Limited quantitative benefits now exist in problem-solving ability (Bell & Bell, 1985) and in attitude toward mathematics (Miller and England, 1989). This study suggests additional quantitative support that writing is beneficial in promoting student ability to assess the correctness of work. If metacognitive skills are really a necessary condition for successful mathematics performance (Campione et al., 1989; Garofalo & Lester, 1985; Long, 1986; and Schoenfeld, 1987), the use of writing may provide the process for attaining these essential skills.

Page 115: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

REFERENCESAbel, J. P. (1987, April). Using writing to teach

mathematics. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Anaheim, CA.

Abraham, A. A. (1991). They came to college? Aremedial/developmental profile of first-time freshmen in SREB states. Issues in Higher Education. 25. Atlanta: Southern Regional Education Board.

Abraham, A. A. (1988). Remedial education in college: How widespread is it? Issues in Higher Education. 24. Atlanta: Southern Regional Education Board.

Allen, B. R. (1989, March). Writing in a Mathematics Classroom. Paper presented at the annual meeting of National Association of Developmental Educators. Cincinnati, OH.

Applebee, A. (1984). Writing and reasoning. Review of Educational Research. 54, 577-596.

Austin, J. L., & Howson, A. G. (1979). Language and mathematical education. Educational Studies in Mathematics f 10. 161-197.

Bader, L., & Pearce, D. (1983). Writing across thecurriculum, 7-12. English Education. 15, 97-106.

Bedford, C. (1984). Why are we learning this? Mathematics Teacher. 77. 258-263.

Bell, A. (1991). Metacognition in the mathematics classroom. Unpublished manuscript.

Bell, E. S., & Bell, R. N. (1985). Writing andmathematical problem solving: Arguments in favor of synthesis. School Science and Mathematics. 85, 210-221.

Bereiter, C. (1991). Implications of connectionism for thinking about rules. Educational Researcher. 20(3), 10-16.

1 0 2

Page 116: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 3

Biggs, J. B. (1986). Enhancing learning skills: The role of metacognition. In J. A. Bowden (Ed.), Student learning: Research into practice (pp. 131- 148). Parkville, Australia: University of Melbourne.

Birkin, M. (1986). Teaching students how to studymathematics: A classroom approach. Mathematics Teacher. 79. 410-413.

Blais, D. M. (1988). Constructivism— A theoreticalrevolution for algebra. Mathematics Teacher. 81. 624-631.

Boone, B. (1983). Great teaching ideas: Writing to learn. Virginia English Journal. 33. 32-36.

Borasi, R. & Rose, B. (1989). Journal writing andmathematics instruction. Educational Studies in Mathematics. 20, 347-365.

Borkowski, J. G., Estrada, M. T., Milstead, M., & Hale, C. A. (1989). General problem-solving skills: Relations between metacognition and strategic processing. Learning Disability Quarterly. 12. 57- 70.

Boylan, H. (1986). Developmental education: A century of promise. A decade of Critics. National Conference of Exemplary Programs in Developmental Education. Journal of Developmental Education.

Britton, J., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., & Rosen, H. (1975). The development of writing abilities (11-18). London: McMillan.

Brown, A. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation and other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition, motivation, and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Brown, A. L. & DeLoache, J. S. (1978). Skills, plans, and self-regulation. In R. Seigler (Ed.), Children's thinking, what develops? (pp. 255-277). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 117: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 4

Bruner, J. S. (1972). Toward a Theory of Instruction. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

Bruner, J. S. (1960). The Process of Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Buerk, D. (1985). From magic to meaning: Changing the learning of mathematics. Paper prepared for Workshop on Teaching and Learning Mathematics, Augsberg College, St. Paul, MN.

Buerk, D. (1986). Carolyn Werbel's journal: Voicing the struggle to make meaning of mathematics. Paper accepted for the Working Paper Series, Wellesley College, Center for Research on Women, Wellesley, MA.

Burns, M. (1988). Helping your students make sense out of math. Learning 88. 16(5), 31-36.

Burton, G. M. (1985). Writing as a way of knowing ina mathematics education class. Arithmetic Teacher. 33(4), 40-45.

Burton, L. (1984). Mathematical thinking: Thestruggle for meaning. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 15, 35-49.

Campione, J. C. (1987). Metacognitive components ofinstructional research with problem learners. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition. motivation, and understanding (pp. 117-139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Campione, J. C., Brown, A. L. & Connell, M. L. (1989). Metacognition: On the importance of understanding what you are doing. In R. I. Charles & E. A.Silver (Eds.), The teaching and assessing of mathematical problem solving. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Carton, K. (1990). Collaborative writing of mathematics problems. Mathematics Teacher. 83. 542.

Page 118: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 5

Chiappelli, F. (1987). Cognitive strategies forimproving mathematics education. Contemporary Education. 59, 20-23.

Clements, D. H. (1986). Effects of logo and CAIenvironments on cognition and creativity. Journal of Educational Psychology. 78, 309-318.

Cloud, G. (1981). The student journal: Improving basic skills. Clearing House. 54. 248-250.

Cobb, P. (1987). Information-processing psychology and mathematics education— A constructivist perspective. Journal of Mathematical Behavior. 6, 3-40.

Collins, A. & Brown, J. S. (1986). The computer as atool for learning through reflection. (Report No. 376). Cambridge, MA: Illinois University, Center for the Study of Reading. (ERIC Reproduction Service No. 281503)

Connolly, P. (1989). Writing and the ecology oflearning. In P. Connolly & T. Vilardi (Eds.), Writing to Learn Mathematics and Science (pp. 1- 14). New York: College Teachers Press.

Connolly, P. & Vilardi, T. (1989). Writing to Learn Mathematics and Science. New York: College Teachers Press.

Corno, L. (1986). The metacognitve control components of self-regulated learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 11. 333-346.

Countryman, J. (1985, July). Writing to learnmathematics. Paper submitted to the Woodrow Wilson Institute.

Craig, T. (1983). Self-discovery through writingpersonal journals. Language Arts. 60. 372-379.

Page 119: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 6

Crouch, W. (1990, July). Students finding their ownvoices; Journal writing in mathematics education. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, Hobart, Tasmania.

Darken, B. (1990). "Helping students meet the challenge of mathematics," a teaching essay in Essentials in Arithmetic. Essentials in Algebra and Essentials in Intermediate Algebra by Johnston, C. L.,Willis, A. T., and Lazaris, J. Belmont, CA: Wadworth.

Davidson, D., & Pearce, D. (1990). Perspectives onwriting activities in the mathematics classroom. Mathematics Education Research Journal. 2.(1)/ 15- 22.

Davidson, D., & Pearce, D. (1988). Using writingactivities to reinforce mathematics instruction. Arithmetic Teacher. 35(8), 42-45.

Dittmer, A. (1986). Guidelines for writing assignments in the content areas. English Journal. 75(4), 59-63.

Dossey, J. A., Mullis I. V. S., Lindquist, M. M., & Chambers, D. L. (1988). The mathematics report card: Are we measuring up? (NAEP Report No. 17-M- 01). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Durst, R. K. & Newell, G. E. (1989). The uses offunction: James Britton's category system and research on writing. Review of Educational Research. 59, 375-394.

Easley, A. (1989). Learning through writing. Journal of Developmental Education. 13.(1), 10-12.

Eblen, C. (1983). Writing across the curriculum: A survey of a university faculty's views and classroom practices.. Research in the Teaching of English. 17. 343-349.

Elbow, P. (1983). Teaching thinking by teaching writing. Change. 15(6), 37-40.

Page 120: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 7

Ellis, W. , Jr. (1984). Teaching is mathematics, too.The College Mathematics Journal. 15.393-94.

Emig, J. (1977). Writing as a mode of learning.College Composition and Communication. 28. 122-127.

Evans, C. S. (1984). Writing to learn in math. Language Arts. 61. 828-835.

Farkas, E. I. (1981). Expressive writing: An affective approach to writing. Reading Improvement. 18, 152- 156.

Fennell, F., & Ammon, R. (1985). Writing techniques for problem solvers. Arithmetic Teacher. 33.(1) , 24-25.

Ferguson, A. M. (1990, March). Developmental education completers and college graduation patterns and trends. Paper presented at the annual meeting of National Association of Developmental Educators. Nashville, TN.

Flavell, J. H. (1979) Metacognition and cognitivemonitoring (A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry). American Psychology. 34. 906-911.

Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The Nature of Intelligence (pp. 231-235). Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum.

Freisinger, R. R. (1980). Cross-disciplinary writing workshops: Theory and practice. College English. 42, 154-166.

Fulwiler, T. (1984). How well does writing acrossthe curriculum work? College English. 46, 113-125.

Fulwiler, T. (1980). Journals across thedisciplines. English Journal. £9(2), 14-19.

Gagne', R. (1978). Essentials of learning forinstruction. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Page 121: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 8

Gagne', R. (1985). The conditions of learning andtheory of instruction (4th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Gambrell, L. B. (1985). Dialogue journals: Reading-writing interaction. The Reading Teacher. 38, 512- 515.

Garofalo, J. (1989). Beliefs and their influence onmathematical performance. Mathematics Teacher. 82, 502-505.

Garofalo, J. (1986). Metacognitive knowledge andmetacognitive process: Important influences on mathematical performance. Research and Teaching in Developmental Education. 2.(2), 34.

Garofalo, J. & Lester, F. (1985). Metacognition,cognitive monitoring and mathematical performance. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 16. 163-175.

Geeslin, W. E. (1977). Using writing aboutmathematics as a teaching technique. Mathematics Teacher. 70. 112-115.

Gere, A. R. (1985). Introduction. In A. R. Gere (Ed.), Roots in the Sawdust (pp. 1-6). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

Giroux, H. A. (1979). Teaching content and thinking through writing. Social Education. 43. 190-193.

Goldberg, D. (1983). Integrating writing into the mathematics curriculum. Two-Year College Mathematics Journal. 14, 421-424.

Goodkin, V. H. (1982). The intellectual consequences of writing: Writing as a tool for learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Rutgers University.

Gopen, G. D., & Smith, D. A. (1990). What's anassignment like you doing in a course like this? Writing to learn mathematics. College Mathematics Journal. 11(2), 2-19.

Page 122: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 0 9

Gordon, J. T. (1988). Writing: A teaching strategy for elementary algebraic fractions. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics. 10(1), 29-36.

Haley-James, S. (1982). Helping students learn through writing. Language Arts. 59, 726-731.

Hardin, C. J. (1988). Access to higher education: Who belongs? Journal of Developmental Education. 12(1), 2-4, 6, 19.

Harris, J. (1988) . The spectator as theorist: Britton and the functions of writing. English Education. 20(1), 41-50.

Harris, R. J. (1981, June). The Use of Student Journals on Teaching Psychology. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Rocky Mountain Psychological Association, Denver, CO.

Henderson, R. W. (1986). Self-regulated learning: Implications for the design of instructional media. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 11. 405-427.

Herrington, A. J. (1981). Writing to learn: Writingacross the disciplines. College English. 43., 379- 387.

Hettich, P. (1980). The journal revisited. Teaching of Psychology. 7, 105-106.

Hirsch, L. R., & King, B. (1983, April). The relative effectiveness of writing assignments in an elementary algebra course for college students. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.

Hodgin, K. W. (1987, April). Teaching Mathematics Through a Writing Experience Approach. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Anaheim, CA.

Page 123: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 0

Hosmer, P. C. (1986). Students can write their own problems. Arithmetic Teacher. 34.(4), 10-11.

Hull, G. A. (1981). Effects of self-managementstrategies on journal writing by college freshmen. Research in the Teaching of English. 15, 135-148.

Hurwitz, M. (1990). Student-authored manuals assemester projects. Mathematics Teacher. 83., 701- 703.

Johnson, M. L. (1983). Writing in mathematics classes:A valuable tool for learning. Mathematics Teacher. 76. 117-119.

Keith, S. Z. (1988). Explorative writing andlearning mathematics. Mathematics Teacher. 81, 714-719.

Kennedy, B. (1985). Writing letters to learn math. Learning. 13.(6 ), 58-61.

King, B. (1982). Using writing in the mathematics class: Theory and practice. In C. W. Griffin (Ed.), New Directions for Teaching and Learning: Teaching writing in all Disciplines (pp. 39-44). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Kirshner, D. (1989). The visual syntax of algebra.Journal of Research in Mathematics Education. 20., 274-287.

Kirshner, D. (1987). The grammar of symbolic elementary algebra. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

Klein, R. & Vukovich, D. (1991, March) Math journals: Focusing on metacognition and self-regulation. Paper presented at the annual meeting of National Association of Developmental Educators, Nashville, TN.

Klein, R. & Vukovich, D. (1990, Fall). Math journals: Focusing on metacognition and self-regulation. National Association of Developmental Educators Newsletter. p. 13.

Page 124: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

IllKnoblauch, C. H., & Brannon, L. (1983). Writing as

learning through the curriculum. College English. 45, 465-474.

Kreeft, J. (1984). Dialogue writing-bridge from talk to essay writing. Language Arts. 61. 141-150.

Kurtz, B. E. & Borkowski, J. G. (1987). Development of strategic skills in impulsive and reflective children: a longitudinal study of metacognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 43., 129- 148.

Kurtz, B. E. & Borkowski, J. G. (1984). Children's metacognition: Exploring relations among knowledge, process, and motivational variables. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 37, 335- 354.

Langer, J. A. & Applebee, A. N. (1985). Learning toWrite: Learning to think. Educational Horizons,64. 36-38.

Lehr, F. (1980). Writing as learning in the content areas. English Journal. 69(8), 23-25.

Lester, F. K. (1985). Methodological considerations in research on mathematical problem-solving instruction. In E. A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and Learning Mathematical Problem Solving: Multiple Research Perspectives (pp. 41-69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Linn, M. M. (1987). Effects of journal writing onthinking skills of high school geometry students. University of Northern Florida: Masters of Education Project. (ERIC Document Reproduction Project No. 289692)

Lipman M. R. (1981). Mathematics term paper!Mathematics Teacher. 74, 453-454.

Lockheed, J. (1988). Toward a scientific practice of science education. Washington, DC: National Science Foundation (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 294747)

Page 125: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 2

Long, E. (1986). Knowing about knowing. Australian Mathematics Teacher. 42.(4), 8-10.

Lunetta, V., Hofstein, A., & Giddings, G. (1981).Evaluating science laboratory skills. The Science Teacher. 48(7), 22-25.

Manning, B. H. (1984). A self-communication structure for learning mathematics. School Science and Mathematics. 84. 43-51.

Martin, N. (1976). Language across the curriculum: Aparadox and its potential for change. Educational Review. 28., 206.

Mason, G. (1989). Behind the writing. The Use of English. 4J)(2 ) , 19-26.

Maxwell, M. (1979). Improving student learning skills. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Mayher, J. S., Lester, N. B., & Pradl, G. M. (1983). Learning to Write/Writing to Learn. Upper Montclair, NJ: Boynton/Cook Publishers, Inc.

McDonald, A. D. (1988). Developmental mathematics instruction: Results of a national survey.Journal of Developmental Education. .12(1), 8-11, 14-15.

McIntosh, M. E. (1991). No time for writing in your class? Mathematics Teacher. 84. 423-432.

McMillen, L. (1986, January 22). Science and mathprofessors are assigning writing drills to focus students' thinking. The Chronicle of Higher Education. 31. 19-21.

Mett, C. L. (1989). Writing in mathematics. Clearing House. 62, 293-296.

Mett, C. L. (1987). Writing as a learning device in calculus. Mathematics Teacher. 80, 534-537.

Page 126: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 3

Mickler, M. L. & Chapel, A. C. (1989). Basic skills in college: Academic solution or dilution? Journal of Developmental Education. 13.(1), 2-4, 16.

Miller, C. A., Grossman, F. J., & Smith, B. D. (1991, March). The integration of reading and writing in the teaching of mathematical concepts. Paper presented at the annual meeting of National Association of Developmental Educators. Nashville, TN.

Miller, L. D. (1991). Writing to learn mathematics. Mathematics Teacher. 84, 516-521.

Miller, L. D. (1991, July). Teacher benefits fromreading students1 writing in secondary mathematics class. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mathematics Education Group of Australasia, Perth, Western Australia.

Miller, L. D. (1990). When students write in amathematics class. Australian Mathematics Teacher. 46(2), 4-7.

Miller, L. D. (1990, July). Writing to learn algebra:A report of action research in action. A paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mathematics Education Group of Australasia, Hobart, Tasmania.

Miller, L. D. (1990). Writing to learn mathematics: Suggestions for classroom implementation. Proceedings of the Australian Association of Mathematics Teachers. 2, 404-411.

Miller, L. D. & England, D. A. (1989). Writing tolearn algebra. School Science and Mathematics. 89, 299-312.

Minor, D. A. (1986). Algebraic symbols and technical writing concepts: A pedagogy. Technical Writing Teacher. 13. 8-12.

Mitchell, C. (1973). Standard problems and somesolutions. English Journal. 62., 263-267, 277.

Page 127: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 4

Montague, H. (1973). Let your students write a book. Mathematics Teacher. 66. 548-550.

Nahrgang, C., & Peterson, B. T. (1986). Using writing to learn mathematics. Mathematics Teacher. 79, 461-465.

Narode, R. B. (1987). Metacoanition in math and science education. Unpublished Paper. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 291558)

Narode, R. B. (1985). Pair problem-solving andmetacoanition in remedial college mathematics. New York: Exxon Education Foundation. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 290626)

National Commission on Excellence in Education. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperative for educational reform. Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office.

National Center for Educational Statistics. (1991).College-level remedial education in the fall of 1989 (NCES 91-191). Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Education.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1989). Curriculum and evaluation standards for mathematics education. Reston, VA: Author.

National Research Council (1989). Everybody counts. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Newell, G. (1984). Learning from writing in two content areas: A case study/protocol analysis. Researchin the Teaching of English. 18. 265-287.

Nolte, P. (1991). Winning at math. Pompano Beach, FL: Academic Press.

Odell, L. (1980). The process of writing and the process of learning. College Composition and Communication. 3JL(1)/ 42-51.

Page 128: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 5

Olsen, C. B. (1984). Fostering critical thinking skills through writing. Educational Leadership. 42.28-39.

Paik, M. K., & Norris, E. (1984). Writing inmathematics education. International Journal of Mathematics Educational Science Technology. 15., 245-252.

Palincsar, A. S. & Brown, D. A. (1987). Enhancing instructional time through attention to metacognition. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 20(2), 66-75.

Pallmann, M. (1982). Verbal language processes insupport of learning mathematics. Mathematics in College. 49-55.

Pearce, D. (1984). Writing in content areaclassrooms. Reading World. 23. 234-241.

Pearce, D., & Davidson, D. (1988). Teacher use ofwriting in the junior high mathematics classroom. School Science and Mathematics. 88., 6-15.

Pellergrino, J. W. & Goldman, S. R. (1987).Information processing and elementary mathematics. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 20, 23-32.

Pennsylvania Resources and Information Center for Special Education. (1987). Metacognition and related teaching and learning strategies. Harrisburg, PA: Pennsylvania State Department of Education. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 297520)

Perkins, D. N. (1985). The fingertip effect: Howinformation-processing technology shapes thinking. Educational Researcher. 14.(7), 11-17.

Platt, M. D. (1975). Writing journals in courses. College English. 37, 408-411.

Pogrow, S. (1988). HOTS: A thinking skills program for at-risk students. Principal. 67(4), 19-24.

Page 129: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 6

Powell, A. B. (1986). Working with "underprepared"mathematics students. In M. Driscoll & J. Confrey (Eds.), Teaching mathematics: Strategies that work. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Pramling, I. (1988). Developing children's thinking about their own learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 58. 266-278.

Price, G. G. (1989). Research in review: Mathematicsin early childhood. Young Children. 44.(4 ), 53-58.

Psotka, J. (1985). Reflections on Computers andMetacognition. Unpublished manuscript. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 2 63206)

Quinto, A. L. & Weener, P. D. (1983, April). Assessing metacognitive skills in problem solving. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Montreal,Quebec. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 229403)

Reece, S. C. (1980, March). The journal keeps the person in the process. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Conference on College Composition and Communication, Washington, D.C. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 193 665)

Resnick, L. B. (1986). Cognition and instruction:Recent theories of human competence and how it is acquired. Pittsburgh, PA: Pittsburgh University, Learning Research and Development Center. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 275740)

Resnick, L. B. (1985). Cognition and instruction:Recent theories of human competence. In B. L. Hammonds (Ed.), Psychology and Learning (pp. 127- 186). Washington, D. C.: American Psychology Association.

Rings, S. & Sheets, R. A. (1991). Student developmentand metacognition: Foundations for tutor training. Journal of Developmental Education. .15(1), 30-32.

Page 130: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 7

Roberts, E. H. (1980, March). Self-discovery through journal writing. Paper presented at the annual conference on College Composition and Communication, Washington, D.C. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 203 348)

Rose, B. J. (1989a). Using expressive writing tosupport the learning of mathematics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Rochester.

Rose, B. J. (1989b). Writing and mathematics: Theory and practice, In P. Connolly and T. Vilardi (Eds.), Writing to Learn Mathematics and Science (pp. 15-30). New York: Teachers College Press.

Roth, R. L. (1985). Learning about gender through writing. Teaching Sociology. 12, 325-338.

Rounds, J. C. & Anderson, D. (1985). Placement in remedial college classes: Required vs. recommended. Community College Review. 13.(1), 20- 27.

Rubin, L. (1988). Professors write to learn aboutwrite-to-learn. College Teacher. 3_6(3) , 94-97.

Russell, D. R. (1987). Writing across the curriculumand the communications movement: Some lessons from the past. College Composition and Communications. 38. 184-195.

Schmidt, D. (1985). Writing in math class. In A. R.Gere (Ed.), Roots in the Sawdust (pp. 104-116). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (in press). Learning to thinkmathematically: Problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook for Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning. New York: Macmillan.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1989a). Explorations of students' mathematical beliefs and behavior. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 20. 338-355.

Page 131: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 8

Schoenfeld, A. H.(1989b)• Teaching mathematicalthinking and problem solving. In L. B. Resnick and B. L. Klopfer (Eds.) Toward the thinking curriculum: Current cognitive research (pp. 83- 103). (1989 Yearbook of the American Society forCurriculum Development). Washington, DC: ASCD.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1987). What's all the fuss about metacognition? In A. H. Schoenfeld (Ed.)Cognitive Science and Mathematics Education (pp. 189-215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985b). Metacognitive and epistemological issues in mathematical understanding. In E. A. Silver (Ed.), Teaching and Learning Mathematical Problem Solving:Multiple Research Perspectives (pp.361-379). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Selfe, C., & Arbabi, F. (1986). Writing to learn: Engineering student journals. In A. Young & T. Fulwiler (Eds), Writing Across the Disciplines (pp. 184-191). Upper Montclair, NJ: Boynton/Cook.

Selfe, C. L., Petersen, B. T., & Nahrgang, C. L.(1986). Journal writing in mathematics. In A.Young & T. Fulwiler (Eds.), Writing Across the Disciplines (pp. 192-207). Upper Montclair, NJ: Boynton/Cook.

Shaw, J. G. (1983). Mathematics students have a right to write. Arithmetic Teacher. 3.0(9), 16-18.

Simons, P. R. J. (1987). Individual differences in the self-regulation of learning, emerging from thinking aloud protocols. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 283853)

Slife, B. D., Weiss, J. & Bell, T. (1985).Separability of metacognition and cognition: Problem solving in learning disabled and regular students. Journal of Educational Psychology. 77, 437-455.

Page 132: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 1 9

Smith, C. (1991). How effective is developmentaleducation at Southeastern Louisiana University? Hammond: Southeastern Louisiana University, Office of Institutional Research.

Smith, M. J. (1977). Bruner on writing. College Composition and Communication. 28. 129-133.

Spears, R., Atkinson, R. H., & Longman, D. G. (1986). A guide for new faculty: Identifying and educating developmental students. In G. L. Howell (Ed.), Innovative Learning Strategies 1985-86 (pp. 78- 87). Plattsburgh, NY: International Reading Association— College Special Interest Group.

Staton, J. (1980). Writing and counseling: Using a dialogue journal. Language Arts. 57, 514-518.

Stempien, M., & Borasi, R. (1985). Students’ writing in mathematics: Some ideas and experiences. For the Learning of Mathematics. 5(3), 14-17.

Sterret, A. (1990). Using writing to teach mathematics. Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.

Tchudi, S. (1986). The hidden agendas in writing across the curriculum. English Journal. 7.5(7) , 22-25.

Turner, J. A. (1989, February 15). Math professors turn to writing to help students master concepts of calculus and combinations. The Chronicle of Higher Education. Al, A14.

Van Nostrand, A. D. (1979). Writing and the generation of knowledge. Social Education. 43. 178-180.

Vukovich, D. (1985, Fall). Ideas in practice:Integrating math and writing through the math journal. Journal of Developmental Education, 9(1), 19-20.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and Language.Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.

Page 133: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 2 0

Walvoord, B. F., & Smith, H. L. (1982). Coaching the process of writing. In C. W. Griffin (Ed.), New Directions for Teaching and Learning; Teaching Writing in All Disciplines (pp. 3-14). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Watson, M. (1980). Writing has a place in a mathematics class. Mathematics Teacher. 73. 518-520.

Waywood, A. (1990, July). Mathematics and language:Reflections on students using math journals. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, Hobart, Tasmania.

Waywood, A. (1991, July). Language and learning mathematics: Research inadequacies. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia, Perth, Western Australia.

Weaver, J. F. (1987). What research says: The learning of mathematics. School Science and Mathematics.87, 66-70.

Weinert, F. E. (1987). Introduction and overview:Metacognition and motivation as determinants of effective learning and understanding. In F. E. Weinert & R. H. Kluwe (Eds.), Metacognition. motivation, and understanding (pp. 65-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Weiss, R., & Walters, S. (1980, April). Writing tolearn. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Boston, MA.

Yinger, R. J., & Clark, C. M. (1981). Reflective Journal Writing: Theory and Practice. (ERICDocument Reproduction Service No. ED 2 08 411)

Yinger, R. (1985). Journal writing as a learning tool. Volta Review. 87, 21-33.

Page 134: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

APPENDICES

1 2 1

Page 135: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Appendix AInformal Survey Name____________Spring 1991 DVMA 092Please circle the choice(s) that apply:Indicate how well you expect you will do in this course:1. Very well2. Good3. All right4. Not very well5. Very poorIndicate how well you like math on the scale below

(1 indicates least and 5 indicates greatest)1 2 3 4 5

Circle the course(s) you completed in HIGH SCHOOL:1. General Math2. Algebra I3. Geometry4. Algebra II5. Advanced Math6. Trigonometry7. Other________________Circle the course(s) taken in COLLEGE:1. DVMA 902. DVMA 0913. DVMA 0924. OtherDescribe (HONESTLY) how you feel about math:

Page 136: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Appendix B1 2 3

D VM A 92 P re-test Name______________________________ Sec_____________

Show your work fo r each exercise in the space provided to the right of each exercise .

W rite your answer on the blank provided to th e le f t of each question.

How certain are you th a t you have chosen the right approach for th is problem ?

Mark |VCl fo r very certain. iFCl fo r fa irly certain , or 1NCI for not certa in .For the approach you have used, how certain are you th a t the problem is worked co rrec tly ? Mark |VC| fo r v e ry certain. iFCl fo r fa irly certain, or iNCl fo r not ce rta in .

Simplify expressions (2 through H] completely.

[VClfFClfNC] ________________ 0 - 2 1

tVCllFCllNCl

IVCI IFCl INCl ________________ [2] J - | f - ( simplest radical form )

lYCllFCltNCl

IVCI iFC l INCl __________________ (D (3x~2y 3)2 ( leave no negative exponents )

f v c ] [Fcl In c ]

[V C ][F C ]lS c ] __________________ HI 3x3 + 5x2 - (5x2 4- 4x3) - (4x2 - 3x3)f v c ] Ff c I fhJOl

(VC][FC]|NC] ________________ g) (4x - 1 yfvcl [FCl [NCl

[VClfFClfNC] ________________ ® (2x - l)(5x + 2)

fVCl [FCl [NCl

fVCl [FCl [NCl 0 - - L + O L.___ , 4x 4x[VCl [FCl [NCl

[VCl [FCl [NCl ® - f K -h - J ^ - 5r— .. 15xy‘ 45xy[VC] EC] EC]

fv c l [f c ] Inc ] fvc] [Fcl E c ]

ID - - gx 56~

Page 137: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

Solve equations (lO] through fl~3].

fV C lfFC lfN C l

fV C lfFC lfN C l

1 2 4

113 4[1 - 3(x + 2)J -f 2x = 10

fV C llFC llN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

ED -f x - 1 = -i-

[V C lfFC lfN C l

fV C llFC l [NCl

x + 5x — 36 = 0

[V C lfFC lfN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

6 — 4x s —2x — 10

[V C lfFC lfN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

fl4l Find the coordinate solution of 2x — 4y = 12 when x = —4

[V C lfFC lfN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

Solve fo r y : 12x — 6y = 36

Factor exercises ED through 1181 completely.

[V C lfFC lfN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

3x - 48

fv c l IF c l IncI

[V C lfFC lfN C l

E ll 2x2 - 11 x + 15

fV C lfFC lfN C l

[V C lfFC lfN C l

x2 — 15x — 54

Express the following situation as an algebraic equation and solve.

fV C lfFC lfN C l

[W ][F C ][N C ]

] Three times a given number is equal to the sum of tha t number and 40. Find the number.

Evaluate the following expression for the given values.

Iv c ][Fc ] [ n c ]

iVCllFCl jNCi|20l x2 -f y - (zx) if x — —6, y ^ 2 4 , z —

Page 138: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 2 5

DVMA 92 Post-test Name. Sec_

Show your work for each exercise in the space provided to the right of each exercise. Write your answer on the blank provided to the left of each question.

How certain are you tha t you have chosen the right approach for this problem ? Mark IVCI for very certain. IFCl for fairly certain, or INCl for not certain .For the approach you have used, how certain are you th a t the problem is worked correctly ? Mark IVCI for very certain. IFCl for fairly certain, a t INCl for not certain.

Simplify expressions Q] through H] completely.

__________ a —3*[VCllFC] [NCl fVClfFClfNCl

[VCl [Fcl [NCl[VClfFClfNCl

[VClfFClfNCl [VCl [Fcl [NCl

[V ClfFClfNCl

fv c l [F c l [NCl

[VCl [F c l [NCl

fV ClfFClfNCl

fV ClfFClfNCl

[VCl [F c l [NCl

[VCl [F c l [N C l'

[V ClfFClfNCl

fvcl [Fcl [NCl fvcl [Fcl [NCl

[VClfFClfNCl

fv c l [F c l fNCl

a ( simplest radical form )

[U (2x 3y2)3 ( leave no negative exponents )

0 4x3 - 5x2 - (2x3+ 3x3) - (6x2 - 2x3)

[U (3x - 2)2

ID (3x - l)(4x + 2)

0 8 , 11 3 3x33x

1 - - ^ r8xy*9b

3 2 x V

0 5y - 45y 5y

Page 139: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 2 6

Solve eauations Eq] through f ill.

[V C JIF C ]® ________________ go] 314 - 3(x + 2)] + 2x = 4[VClfFClfNCl

[ v c M E c ] _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ (ED -f X + 1 = -g-fVClfFCllNCl

[VClfFClfNCl ________________ EU x2 + 5x - 24 = 0fVClfFClfNCl

[VC][FC][NC] ________________ [[3] 5 - 3x ^ - x - 13[VClfFClfNCl

fVCl IFCl |NC| ------------------------- [R] Find the coordinate solution of 2x — 3y = 12|VC](FC][NC] When x - - 3

|VC| (FC| INCl ________________ [Is] Solve for y : 6x — 3y = 15fVClfFClfNCl

Factor exercises [16] through [T8] completely.

[VClfFClfNCl ________________ [16] 5x2 - 20fVClfFClfNCl

[V C lfF C lfN C l_________________ [n] 3x2 - 13x + 12fVClfFClfNCl

[VC][FC][NC] _______________ m x2 + 15x - 54[VClfFClfNCl

Express the following situation as an algebraic equation and solve.

[VCl IFCl INCl _______________ [19] Four times a given number is equal to thefVClfFClfNCl sum of that number and 30. Find the number.

Evaluate the following expression for the given values.

[VC][FC][NC] ________________ [20] x + y2 - (zy) if x = 15. y = - 3 , z = - 1[VClfFClfNCl

Page 140: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

DVMA 92 Quiz 1 1.2 - 2.2 Name__________________________ .Sec______________________

Show your work fo r each ex e rc ise In the space provided to the right o f each exerc ise . W rite your answ er on the blank provided to the le f t of_ each Question.

How certain are you th a t you have chosen the right approach fo r th is problem ? Mark IVCI for very certain . iFCl fo r fairly certain, or INCl fo r not certain .For the approach you have used, how certain are you th a t the problem is worked correctly ? Mark IVCI for v e ry certain. IFCl for fairly certain, or iNCl for not ce rta in .

Simplify expressions (3 th rough (9] completely. ( 2 points each )

IW)IFC] [NCj ______________ 0 - |-IVCl[FCllNCl

[ v c ) I f c ] |H _______________ g] — I2[VCl [Fcl [NCl

[TC)(FC]|NC] ________________ ID - 6 + 1 2 -5 -3 + 1fvcl [Fcl [NCl

[VC](Fc][NC] ________________ 0 516 - 2(3 — 5)1 + 1[VCl [Fcl [NCl

IVCllFCllNCI ________________ H] At daybreak the tem perature was —4’. At noonIVCI IFCl INCl it was 22‘. What was the rise in tem perature ?

W rite each phrase as an expression. ( 2 points each )

E c]G E ][N C ] _ g] w less than 12IVClfFCllNCl

IVCIIFCl INCl 0 12 divided by the sum or x and y[VCl [FCl [NCl

IVCIIFCl INCl ________________ ID the difference between 5 times a number and 4rv c irF c ifN ci

Evaluate each expression fo r a = 2, b = —3, and c — 2 ( 2 pointB cBch )

[VC)[FC][NC] ________________ g) (a - b)2[VCl [FCl [NCl

[vc] [FcJ [n c ] ________________ ED - i L ^ - b Lfv c lfF c l In c I

1 2 7

Page 141: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

DVMA 92 Quiz 2 3.1 - 3.4 N«im .S e c _____________________

Show your work in fha epa ce provided below each e x e r c ite . W rite your gnawer on the blank provided to the l e f t o f each question.

How cerli«n are you th a t you have chosen the right approach fo r th le problem ? Mark IVCI for very sure. IFCl for fa irly aure. or INCl fo r not su re .For the approach you have uaed. how certain are you th a t the problem is worked

correctly ? Mark IVCI for v e ry certain. IFCl for fairly certain, or INCl fo r not certain .

Determine if th e number given in th e bracea ia a solution for the given equation.[YES or NO answer expected) ( 2 points )

[VCl He] |NC| 3.1 ________________ ID 2y + 8 = 3 + y ; ( - 2 )IVCIIFCl iNCl

Solve each of th e following equations fo r x. ( 2 points each )

(v c l IFcl Inc] 3.1 IVCl IFCl INCl

[VC] [Fcl [NC] 3-3

IVCl IFcl INCl

Ivcl [FCl (NCl 3.2Ivc] IFcl Inc]

Iv c l IFcl INCl 3.3 fv c l IFcl INCl

Ivcl IFcl Inc] 3.2

Ivcl IFcl [NCl

Ivcl IFcl INCl 3.3 Ivcl IFcl INCl

fvcl IFcl Inc! 3.3 Ivcl IFcl [NCl

Solve each litera l equation Tor the

Ivcl IFcl INCl 3 .4 ________________

[vcl [Fcl [NCl

Ivcl IFcl INCl 3.4 Ivcl IFcl INCl

. B) x — 7 — 3

g) 3x + 5 - 23

0 -S- - 12

g) _ _ 20

0 2(x - 4) = 7x + 2

0 4x + 1 - (3 - x) - 13

1 -±-x + 1 - - i - x _ 2

v ariab le ind icated . ( 2 poin ts each )

@ I = gr + t ; so lve fo r g

[To] A »** P(1 4- r) ; solve fo r P

128

Page 142: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

DVMA 92 Quiz 3 4.1 - 4.4Sac NftM.

1 2 9Show vour work In the sp a c e provided below each e x e rc ise . W rite vour answ er on the blank provided to the l e f t o f each question .

How certain are you th a t you have choBen th e r ig h t approach for th is problem ? Mark IVCl for very sure. IFCl for fairly Bure, or INCl fo r not su re .For the approach jfou have used, how certain are you th a t the problem iB worked correctly ? Mark IVCI for very sure. IFCl fo r fa irly su re , or INCl for not su re .

Factor each of the following completely: ( 2points each )

tvcl 033 Ec)4.1 __________________ ID 15x2 - lOy + 30

(vcl IFcl INCl

Ivcl IFcl [NC|4.1 ______________________ ID 18a - 27b[VCl IFCl [NCl

fvcl IFcl INC14.1 _________________ n 6x3 - 9x2fvcl IFcl INCl

Supply the missing fac to r: ( 2 points each )

Ivcl IFcl INC14.1________________________ 0 y (x + 2 ) - 3(x+2) - (x+2X )

Iv c l IFCl INCl

Factor the following trinomials completely: ( 2 points each )

IVCl IFCl INCI4.2 0 16x2 - 49fvc] [Fc] Inc]

fvcl IFcl INC14.3 0 a2 - 9e + 18

Ivcl IFcl [NCl

fvcl IFcl INC14.3 0 2x2 + Sx - 12Ivcl IFCl INCl

Ivcl [Fcl INC14.4 ® Sx2 + 20x + 20

[VC] (EC] EC]

[vcl IFcl [NCl4.3 ® x2 - Sx - 6

fvcl (Fcl [NCl

fvcl IFcl [NC14.3 _____________________ Eo] 6x2 - 23x - 4Ivcl IFCl INCl

Page 143: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

DVMA 92 Quiz 4 Chapters 5 A 6 Nu m .Sac______________________

Show vour w ork In the coace provided below each exerciee. W rite vour ancw er on

the blank provided to the lef t o f each question.

How certain are you th a t you have chosen the riaht approach fo r th is problem ? Mark IVCI fo r very sure. iFCl fo r fa irly sure, or INCl for not su re .

For the approach you have used, how certain are you th a t th e problem is worked correctly t Mark IVCI fo r v e ry sure . IFCl for fairly sure, or I NCl fo r not su re .

Determine th e domain of the given expression: ( 2 points )

IVC] IFcl fNClS.l[VCl [FCl [NCl

Simplify completely: ( 2 points each )

[VCl fFcl [NC15.2 [VCl [FCl [NCl

[VCl IFCl [NCl 5.3

Iv c l [Fcl [NCl

gj 8x‘ + 4x2 - 12x4x

[v c l [Fcl [NCl 5.2 [VCl [FCl [NCl

[vcl [Fcl [NCl6.i fvcl [Fcl [NCl

12x3x2 - x - 2

fv c l [Fcl [N cle.i [v c l [Fcl [n c !

x2 + 4x -t- 4 x2 - 9

[VCl [Fcl [NC16.2 [VCl IFCl [NCl

fvcl [Fcl (NC16.3 fvcl [Fcl [NCl

1

Solve the following fractional equation: ( 2 points )

[VCl IFCl [NCl 6.4 [VCl [Fcl [NCl

Express as a ratio in lowest term s: ( 2 points )

5.4

[VCl [Fcl [NCl Iv c l [FCl [NCl

Ho] On a recent exam, 15 students from a class of 35 failed. W hat is the ratio of the studen ts passing to

those who took the exam ?

Page 144: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 1

Appendix C The prompts were structured to focus on

metacognitive activities. And, although students were asked to review their work and go through processes that are cognitive, they were also asked to reflect upon and analyze these cognitive processes as they repeat them. The goal was to increase students' awareness of their cognition; therefore, although the differences between cognitive and metacognitve is a gray area, it may not be necessary to differentiate between them for the purposes of this study.Prompt Development:

The structure of the prompts were consistent with current literature on the use of writing-to-learn, the coaching of writing, and the gathering of qualitative data. An example of the former would be to coax more detailed information by having students write to someone who is not knowledgeable about the subject, such as a seventh grader or a young neighbor. For the latter, the avoidance of dichotomous questions enhances the quantity and quality of the student's response. "How" and "why" are used to promote student reflection and "what" has been avoided when declarative answers

Page 145: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 2

would not be desired. Content-oriented prompts may have asked students to think through the cognitive process first, but then the students were asked to describe their view of their cognition. The prompts focused on having students reflect on their understanding of a concept or a problem, ascertain the correctness of their work, or evaluate/monitor their study methods. The prompts have been field-tested and have produced satisfactory expressive writing responses. When necessary, the prompts were adjusted to avoid ambiguities or other difficulties.

The placement of the prompts paralleled the class schedule for topic presentation and for testing.Prompts that were content-oriented were usually presented the day after the concept was taught or, in two instances, on the day the concept was introduced. The placement of the remaining prompts was orchestrated by testing dates. Quizzes and tests became more time consuming as topics and/or concepts became more involved. Since students in previous studies had reacted negatively to writing before a test because of the time restraints, writing was not included before tests 2-4 and quizzes 4-5.

Page 146: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 3

Writing Prompts (Spring 91);Prompt 1 (January 14)How do you feel about mathematics? Be as honest as you can.Prompt 2 (January 16)Describe a predominantly good or bad experience that you still remember clearly from a mathematics class.Prompt 3 (January 18)When you work a math problem, how do you determine if your answer is correct? When you think an answer is incorrect. what do you do?Prompt 4 (January 21)Look at problem #18 from your assignment on page 53.Do you think you worked this problem correctly?Explain in detail why you answered yes or no.Prompt 5 (January 23)Today you will take your first quiz in this course.How did you prepare for this quiz? Please give a detailed answer.Prompt 6 (January 25)Now that you have had your quiz returned, how useful was the way you prepared for the quiz? What could you do differently that would be helpful?Prompt 7 (January 28)Pick out a homework problem from page 85 that you thought looked difficult but you think you worked correctly. Explain why you think your work is correct. Pretend that you are writing for someone who does not understand algebra.Prompt 8 (January 30)What topic has been the most confusing so far in this course? Do you understand the concept now and, if so, how did you learn it?

Page 147: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 4

Prompt 9 (February 1)You are going to take your first test today. How was your preparation for this test different from how you prepared for your quiz? Why did you study the way that you did?Prompt 10 (February 4)What was the most difficult question on the test yesterday? Do you know why the question was so difficult? Please explain.Prompt 11 (February 6)What type of equation do you find hardest to solve?(Use page 143 to give an example.) Explain what you think makes this problem difficult for you.Prompt 12 (February 8)How do you know when you have solved an equation correctly?Prompt 13 (February 15)Did you study differently for the second quiz and, if so, how?If you did not change how you prepared for the quiz, explain why.Prompt 14 (February 18)Students often express concern that they cannot work with verbal statements in mathematics. Do you have this concern? How do you handle verbal problems? Explain your answer in detail.Prompt 15 (February 2 0)How do you compare solving inequalities with equations? How do you remember the differences in solving the two different types of problems?Prompt 16 (February 22)Tell me what you understand best about per cents? Tell me why you think you understand this so well.No Prompt (February 25) Test 2

Page 148: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 5

Prompt 17 (February 27)What was the hardest question on the test? Do you know how to find the answer now; how did you come to know it?Prompt 18 (March 1)How do you know if an expression has a common factor? Describe your decision-making in detail.Prompt 19 (March 4)Two methods have been taught for factoring trinomials. Which method works best for you? Describe your reasoning.Prompt 20 (March 6)What type of factoring was the most difficult for you? Explain why you think you think you have trouble with this problem.Prompt 21 (March 8)Give an example of the sum of two squares and the difference of two squares. Explain how you would determine if either or both of these can be factored?Prompt 22 (March 11)What preparation did you make for quiz 3 that you found to be the most beneficial? Be specific about how you think this preparation helped.Prompt 2 3 (March 13)Half of the semester has passed and you will take test 3 at the next class meeting. How do you feel writing about mathematics has helped you? Please, answer as honestly as you can without considering what I might want you to say.

No Prompt (March 15)Test 3Prompt 24 (March 18)On the test that has been returned, find the questionthat you think you understood best. Tell me why youfeel you understand this problem so well.

Page 149: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 6

Prompt 25 (March 20)We learned division by zero is not defined in mathematics. What does this statement mean to you?How well do you understand this concept?Prompt 26 (March 22)How do you determine if a rational expression can be simplified? If an expression can be simplified, how would you do so?Prompt 27 (April 1)How do you know when two ratios are equal? Explain your answer as fully as possible.Prompt 28 (April 3)Have you had any difficulty with today's lesson? Can you identify the concept (or the problem) that is difficult for you?Prompt 29 (April 5)What has been the most confusing so far with algebraic fractions (Chapter 6)? Why do you think this has been a problem for you?Prompt 30 (April 8)What concept do you understand best about algebraic fractions (Chapter 6)? Why do you think this is easy for you?Prompt 31 (April 10)How would you go about showing that simplifying 1/2 X - 2 + 1/3 X is the same as or different from solving 1/2 X - 2 = 1/3 X ?Why are these answers alike or different?No Prompt (April 12)Quiz 4Prompt 32 (April 15)Look over the items that you missed on the quiz. Can you tell me why you think you missed the problems that you did? Be as specific as you can.Prompt 33 (April 17)Discuss your understanding of solutions for a linear equation in two variables.

Page 150: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 7

Prompt 34 (April 19)You will take your last quiz at the next class meeting. How will you prepare for this quiz? How has your style of studying changed, if at all, to better prepare you for tests or quizzes?No Prompt (April 22)Quiz 5Prompt 35 (April 24)You have often been asked this semester to write about your perception of the correctness of your answers on a quiz or a test. How or what have you learned from these exercises? Please be as specific as you can.Prompt 36 (April 26)Have you had any difficulty with today's lesson? Can you identify the concept (or the problem) that is difficult for you?Prompt 37 (April 29)What is the most difficult concept in Chapter 9? Pick a problem from this chapter that you do not understand and work the problem as far as you can. On the side of your work, explain your work.Prompt 38 (May 1)Do you feel that writing in mathematics has helped you this semester? Explain your ansv*er (and please be brutally honest). Should the use of writing be changed in any way?

Page 151: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 8

Appendix DIn the semester prior to the study, the instructor

of the experimental groups and the independent instructor did not code the students1 work the same. Following each example is the agreement reached by these two persons and the researcher.

1. Evaluate (a - b)2 for a = 2, b = -3, and c = 2.Students often knew how to replace the variables

with the indicated value but could not implement the order of operations correctly. For example, the student would square the replacement for a and then square the replacement for b. An agreement was reached to code the problem as correct method and incorrect implementation.

2. Evaluate (a - b)2 for a = 2 and b = -3(a - b)

For this problem, the substitution in the numerator varied. Students sometimes wrote - -32 then - -9. It was decided to code the following examples as indicated below.Step 1: 3 - (-4)2 3 - -42 3 - -42Step 2: 3 + 42 3 - 16 3 + 16Codings: CM, IW CM, CW CM, IW

(CM for correct method, CW for correct work, IW for incorrect work.)

3. Solve -4x = 2 05

Students sometimes began solving this equation by multiplying both sides of this equation by the number 5 rather than the more efficient method of multiplying by the reciprocal. This was considered a viable method and coded as correct method.

4. Factor 2x2 + 5x - 12The correct answer is (2x - 3)(x + 4) but the

student may have answered (2x + 3)(x - 4). It wasdecided that the student was aware of the correct method but the method was not implemented correctly.

Page 152: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 3 9

Appendix EThe following table represents the reliability

coefficients for the coding of the students' work on quizzes one, two, and three. These were determined by dividing the number of times the independent codings were not in agreement with the total number of codings made for that set of evaluations.

Instrument Coding ReliabilityQuiz 1 1199/1220 = .983Quiz 2 1238/1240 = .998Quiz 3 1159/1160 = .999Quiz 4 840/840 = 1.000

Page 153: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

1 4 0

Appendix F Group Equivalency Tests

A oneway analysis of variance was performed on the following measures: achievement pretest, awareness pretest, English ACT subscore, math ACT subscore, and number of years of high school preparatory mathematics. No significant differences between the four initial groups was found on any of these measures. The results of these tests follows.Achievement Pretest: F (2,97) = .132, p < .88Awareness Pretest: F (2,89) II to CX

I o to A 07English ACT Subscore: F (3,124) = .386, p < .76Math ACT Subscore: F (3,124) = .573, p < .63High School Preparation: Z (3,124) = .986, p < .40

Page 154: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

VITA

Naomi Barbara Richardson Allen was born in New Orleans, Louisiana, on October 31, 1946, the daughter of Lloyd and Anne Richardson. Barbara lived and attended elementary and secondary school in Ponchatoula, Louisiana. After graduating as an honor student from Ponchatoula High, she attended Southeastern Louisiana University in Hammond, Louisiana where she earned a B.S. degree in Mathematics Education in 1968 and an M. Ed. in Mathematics Education in 1980.

In 1967, she married Melvin Darryl Allen and they are the proud parents of Paige Elizabeth, Melvin Darryl, Kathe Elizabeth, and Russell Gray. After fourteen years of teaching in the secondary schools and six years at the college level, she pursued the Doctor of Philosophy degree in Curriculum and Instruction with the emphasis on Mathematics Education which was awarded in December of 1991 at Louisiana State University.

1 4 1

Page 155: A Study of Metacognitive Skill as Influenced by Expressive

DOCTORAL EXAMINATION AND DISSERTATION REPORT

C a n d id a t e : NAOMI (BARBARA) R. ALLEN

M a jo r F i e l d : EDUCATION

T i t l e o f D i s s e r t a t i o n : A STUDY OF METACOGNITIVE SKILL AS INFLUENCED BYEXPRESSIVE WRITING IN COLLEGE INTRODUCTORY ALGEBRA CLASSES.

A p p r o v e d :

-___________M a jo r P r o f e s s o r a n d C h a irm a n

* D ean o f t h e G r a d u a te S c h o o l

EXAMINING COMMITTEE:

— ,—

D a t e o f E x a m in a t io n :

OCTOBER 30, 1991