15
https://www.e-csd.org 953 This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licens- es/by-nc/4.0) which permits unrestricted noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Copyright © 2019 Korean Academy of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic Bassil M. Mashaqba a , Khalid G. Al-Shdifat b , Anas I. Al Huneety a , Mais Abu Alhala a a Department of English Language and Literature, The Hashemite University, Zarqa, Jordan b Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid, Jordan Correspondence: Khalid G. Al-Shdifat, PhD Jordan University of Science and Technology, P. O. Box 3030, Irbid 22110, Jordan Tel: +962779098548 Fax: +96227095123 E-mail: [email protected] Received: October 5, 2019 Revised: November 9, 2019 Accepted: November 19, 2019 Objectives: The aim of this research was to investigate the early word syllable structure and notable phonological processes in the speech of Jordanian Arabic (JA)-speaking chil- dren. Methods: Elicited and spontaneous speech productions of 20 children were tran- scribed and analyzed by syllable structure. The ages of the participants ranged from 1 to 3 years, divided into four age groups: 1;0-1;6, 1;7-2;0, 2;1-2;6, and 2;7-3;0. Then, the data was analyzed according to ten phonological processes influencing word syllable structure and number. Results: CVC was found to be the most prevalent syllable structure used across all age groups. In terms of syllable number, disyllabic words were the most frequently used. Onomatopoeia, weak syllable deletion, and closed syllable CVVC reduction to CVV were noticed in the productions of children in the first age group. Cluster reduction and onset/ coda deletion occurred the most with the age group 2;1-2;6, while syllable deletion was evident until age 3. Conclusion: It appears that JA-speaking children use CVC syllable struc- ture heavily in bisyllabic and polysyllabic words in their early speech; they avoid monosyl- labic CV or CVC syllables to preserve the minimal bimoraic weight of Arabic phonological words. Onomatopoeia is noticed beyond the age of 1 with syllable deletion in the speech of children up to 3. The results are discussed within the context of the current literature. Keywords: Syllable structure, Acquisition, Phonological processes, Onomatopoeia, Jorda- nian Arabic (JA) Consistency in language production is a fundamental principle for understanding, building and shaping the linguistic relations and connections that assist the production of the target units (in- cluding syllables). The configuration of prosodic words including segmental, melodic, syllabic and prosodic levels; in correlation with age-stages, plays a crucial role in determining the accuracy of the childs production. Bearing this in mind, the progressive in- crease of syllable perception and acquisition can be linked to im- provement in the accuracy and stability of correct articulation of words comprising different syllable shapes (cf. Bohland & Guen- ther, 2006). Acquisition of phonological aspects is of great interest to those concerned both with language acquisition in general and commu- nication disorders in children in particular. Different studies have been conducted on the phonological processes young children use during their acquisition of the syllable structure in different lan- guages, such as Spanish-English (Keffala, Barlow, & Rose, 2018), Dutch (Levelt, Schiller & Levelt, 2000; Rose, 1997), Polish ( Łukas- zewicz, 2007), and Indonesian (Raja, 2008). The phonological pro- cesses include assimilation, cluster reduction, syllable deletion, cluster gemination, cluster deletion, metathesis, epenthesis, sylla- ble duplication (for details, refer to Fee & Ingram, 1982; Kehoe, 1994, 1995; Pater, 1997; Dyson & Amayreh, 2000; Kehoe & Stoel- Gammon, 2001; Raja, 2008; Young, 1991, among others). These are termed normal phonological processes and take place in typi- cally developing children s speech to repair and compensate for the loss of the syllables they are not yet ready to produce when try- ing to imitate adult word production. Many studies have examined varieties of Arabic acquisition, e.g., Cairene Arabic (Ammar & Morsi, 2006; Ragheb & Davis, 2010; https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 Original Article Commun Sci Disord 2019;24(4):953-967 ISSN 2288-0917 (Online)

Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://www.e-csd.org 953This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licens-es/by-nc/4.0) which permits unrestricted noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright © 2019 Korean Academy of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology

Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian ArabicBassil M. Mashaqbaa, Khalid G. Al-Shdifatb, Anas I. Al Huneetya, Mais Abu Alhalaa

aDepartment of English Language and Literature, The Hashemite University, Zarqa, JordanbDepartment of Rehabilitation Sciences, Jordan University of Science and Technology, Irbid, Jordan

Correspondence: Khalid G. Al-Shdifat, PhDJordan University of Science and Technology, P. O. Box 3030, Irbid 22110, JordanTel: +962779098548 Fax: +96227095123 E-mail: [email protected]

Received: October 5, 2019Revised: November 9, 2019Accepted: November 19, 2019

Objectives: The aim of this research was to investigate the early word syllable structure and notable phonological processes in the speech of Jordanian Arabic (JA)-speaking chil-dren. Methods: Elicited and spontaneous speech productions of 20 children were tran-scribed and analyzed by syllable structure. The ages of the participants ranged from 1 to 3 years, divided into four age groups: 1;0-1;6, 1;7-2;0, 2;1-2;6, and 2;7-3;0. Then, the data was analyzed according to ten phonological processes influencing word syllable structure and number. Results: CVC was found to be the most prevalent syllable structure used across all age groups. In terms of syllable number, disyllabic words were the most frequently used. Onomatopoeia, weak syllable deletion, and closed syllable CVVC reduction to CVV were noticed in the productions of children in the first age group. Cluster reduction and onset/coda deletion occurred the most with the age group 2;1-2;6, while syllable deletion was evident until age 3. Conclusion: It appears that JA-speaking children use CVC syllable struc-ture heavily in bisyllabic and polysyllabic words in their early speech; they avoid monosyl-labic CV or CVC syllables to preserve the minimal bimoraic weight of Arabic phonological words. Onomatopoeia is noticed beyond the age of 1 with syllable deletion in the speech of children up to 3. The results are discussed within the context of the current literature.

Keywords: Syllable structure, Acquisition, Phonological processes, Onomatopoeia, Jorda-nian Arabic (JA)

Consistency in language production is a fundamental principle

for understanding, building and shaping the linguistic relations

and connections that assist the production of the target units (in-

cluding syllables). The configuration of prosodic words including

segmental, melodic, syllabic and prosodic levels; in correlation

with age-stages, plays a crucial role in determining the accuracy of

the child’s production. Bearing this in mind, the progressive in-

crease of syllable perception and acquisition can be linked to im-

provement in the accuracy and stability of correct articulation of

words comprising different syllable shapes (cf. Bohland & Guen-

ther, 2006).

Acquisition of phonological aspects is of great interest to those

concerned both with language acquisition in general and commu-

nication disorders in children in particular. Different studies have

been conducted on the phonological processes young children use

during their acquisition of the syllable structure in different lan-

guages, such as Spanish-English (Keffala, Barlow, & Rose, 2018),

Dutch (Levelt, Schiller & Levelt, 2000; Rose, 1997), Polish (Łukas-

zewicz, 2007), and Indonesian (Raja, 2008). The phonological pro-

cesses include assimilation, cluster reduction, syllable deletion,

cluster gemination, cluster deletion, metathesis, epenthesis, sylla-

ble duplication (for details, refer to Fee & Ingram, 1982; Kehoe,

1994, 1995; Pater, 1997; Dyson & Amayreh, 2000; Kehoe & Stoel-

Gammon, 2001; Raja, 2008; Young, 1991, among others). These

are termed normal phonological processes and take place in typi-

cally developing children’s speech to repair and compensate for

the loss of the syllables they are not yet ready to produce when try-

ing to imitate adult word production.

Many studies have examined varieties of Arabic acquisition, e.g.,

Cairene Arabic (Ammar & Morsi, 2006; Ragheb & Davis, 2010;

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657

Original ArticleCommun Sci Disord 2019;24(4):953-967ISSN 2288-0917 (Online)

Page 2: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657954 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

Saleh, Shoeib, Hegazi, & Ali, 2007), Kuwaiti Arabic (Ayyad, 2011;

Ayyad & Bernhardt, 2009), and Jordanian Arabic (Al-Btoush, 2005;

Al-Tamimi, Owais, Khabour, & Khamaiseh, 2011; Daana, 2009;

Dyson & Amayreh, 2000, Mashaqba, Al-Khawaldeh, AlGweirien,

& Al-Edwan, in press). These fall short of providing a full account

of the acquisition of syllable structure in Arabic, especially Jorda-

nian Arabic (JA). Syllable acquisition based on the number of oc-

currences was observed and types of syllables used has also been

considered (Ayyad, 2011; Jakobson, 1968; Dyson, 1988; Ragheb &

Davis, 2010; Stoel-Gammon, 1987; Young, 1991). Nonetheless, the

findings of those studies are variable, which might be attributed to

the different methodologies used in data collection. Moreover, no

quantitative work has been carried out on their syllable structure,

specifically that of JA-speakers. The present study thus aims to fill

a gap in the literature by examining the acquisition of syllabic struc-

ture by JA-speaking children. The findings of this work highlight

the importance of this study, in that it focuses on the syllable struc-

ture acquisition and word syllable structure and number in the

speech of 1- to 3-year-old JA-speaking children.

A syllable consists of an obligatory vowel (nucleus), which is pre-

ceded and followed by optional consonants (onset and coda) (Gur-

ský, 2005; Laver, 1994). JA exhibits fixed syllable structures; it has

open, closed and doubly closed syllables. The syllable types that may

occur are CVV/CV as in /ma:.ma/ ‘mum’, CCV as in /nħa.raʔ/ ‘burnt’,

CVC such as /mal.ʕab/ ‘playground’, CCVC as in /btil-ʕab/ ‘play’, CVVC

as in /ki:s/ ‘bag’, CCVVC, /ṣħu:n/ ‘plates’, CCVCC, /drobs/ ‘candy’,

and CVCC such as /kalb/ ‘dog’. The most frequent word syllable

numbers are disyllables and trisyllables, although there are mono-

syllables and a very few usages of words that are quadrisyllabic or

more (Al-Wer, 2007; Huneety, 2015; Mashaqba, 2015).

A number of phonological processes are employed by children

to repair the syllable structure: cluster reduction, deletion (onset

deletion, weak syllable deletion and coda deletion/CVVC>CVV),

syllable epenthesis, assimilation, gemination, metathesis and ono-

matopoeia. Clusters are consonants that follow each other in the

syllable structure. The process of cluster reduction is common in

children’s speech and has an impact on their syllable structure. It

is one of the persistent phonological processes during speech ac-

quisition in children (McLeod, Van Doorn, & Reed, 2001; Rvachew

& Andrews, 2002), and is still seen in the speech production of

5-year-old (Haelsig & Madison, 1986) with incidences in the speech

of children aged 8–9 (Roberts, Burchinal, & Footo, 1990; Smit,

1993; Smit, Hand, Freilinger, Bernthal & Bird, 1990; Tempun, 1957).

Nevertheless, some children produce certain clusters at the age of

2 (e.g., French, 1989; Lleó & Prinz, 1996; Preisser, Hodson, & Paden,

1988; Stoel-Gammon, 1987; Watson & Scukanec, 1997). Children

whose phonological systems are developing tend to delete at least

one of the consonants in a multi-consonant cluster (e.g., Bernhardt

& Stemberger, 1998; Ingram, 1974, 1989; Kiparsky & Menn, 1977;

Olmsted, 1971; Smith, 1973; Vihman, 1980).

Deletion of phonological elements is used widely by children at-

tempting to produce multisyllabic words in the early stages (Dys-

on & Amayreh, 2000; Kehoe, 1994, 1995, 2001; Pater, 1997; Young,

1991). The target of deletion is either the onset, the coda or the weak

unstressed syllable in multisyllabic words (Dyson & Amayreh,

2000). Intriguingly, investigating syllable structure acquisition

cross-linguistically proves the universality of weak/strong syllable

rules indicating weak syllable deletion with preservation of the

strong (stressed) syllables (Al-Btoush, 2005; Al-Tamimi et al., 2011;

Gerken, 1994; Kehoe & Steol-Gammon, 1997, 2001; Levey & Sch-

wartz, 2002; Pater, 1997; Raja, 2008). By way of contrast, epenthe-

sis refers to the addition/insertion of a segment (vowel/consonant)

in order to meet the language syllable structure (Trask, 1996). Some

linguists restrict the term to word-medial intrusion, and others to

consonant intrusion. This process is very frequent in adult speech

in Arabic—e.g. Wadi Mousa Arabic (Huneety, 2015) and Wadi

Ramm Arabic (Mashaqba, 2015). Intriguingly, insertion of bigger

prosodic elements has been registered in children’s speech, where

syllable epenthesis refers to the insertion of a syllable in the phono-

logical word (Kehoe, 1994, 1995).

Assimilation refers to the process whereby a sound overrides or

takes features of a neighboring sound (progressively or regressive-

ly), so that the sounds become more alike or identical (Ladefoged,

2006; Roach, 2009). Chomsky and Halle (1968) refer to assimila-

tion as one segment altering its features so as to become more sim-

ilar to a nearby segment. Goldsmith (1976, 1979) defines it as spread-

ing rules. Simply, assimilation is a kind of ease of articulation be-

cause of the tendency to articulate efficiently by copying or spread-

ing a phonetic property from one segment to the other (Fromkin,

Rodman, & Hyams, 2013, p. 301).

Page 3: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 955

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

Gemination typically refers to segment duplication (Al-Mashag-

bah, 2010; Raja, 2008). The behavior of geminates in linguistic sys-

tems is a problem for researchers, and various definitions have at-

tempted to identify geminate production: (1) prolongation/conso-

nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani,

1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where the pro-

duction of geminates involves two phases: the consonant acquires

a coda position, and then it acquires an onset position (e.g., Abdo,

1980; Delartte, 1968); and (3) greater energy/tension and a stronger

articulation of a sound segment (Jessen, 1998; Ridouane, 2007).

Metathesis (Ɂiqlāb in Arabic terminology) refers to the process of

transposition in the usual sequence of elements, normally of two

consonants in a word or syllable. It is very frequent in children’s

speech (Alqattan, 2015). The process is also found in performance

errors (also called tongue-slips) or in Spoonerisms (cf. Crystal, 2008).

Onomatopoeia refers to words that imitate real sounds, whether

human or non-human. In linguistics, the term refers to the direct

motivated, non-arbitrary association between the phonetic form

and its meaning (Inose, 2007; Trask, 1996).

METHODS

Participants

Samples were collected from 20 participants divided into the

four age groups (1;0-1;6, 1;7-2;0, 2;1-2;6, and 2;7-3;0 each with three

males and two females). A participant datasheet was prepared which

included all relevant information about the children, including

code names, ages, their medical history, and languages spoken at

home. Participants were screened by one of the co-authors, a spe-

cialized pathologist, for any possible hearing or developmental dis-

orders; all passed the pure-tone audiometry hearing test, with the

exception of two of the participants originally selected, because of

hearing impairment. None of the participants had any history of

speech language therapy. Approval to conduct the study was obtained

from the departmental research committee at the first author’s in-

stitution, The Hashemite University, Jordan (IRB no. 2520182019).

The participants were recruited from different nurseries in Am-

man. Consent forms were sent to the parents through the nurseries

and were signed by the parents and returned to the first author to

indicate their approval for their children to participate in the study.

Stimuli and Procedure

A trained research assistant (RA) conducted the data collection.

A picture-naming task and objects were used to elicit speech pro-

ductions from the children within a play context. The task involved

50 colorful prompts which depicted familiar objects, e.g., animals,

parts of the body, daily activities, furniture, and toys. All images

were taken from a children’s picture dictionary and included words

of different weights and syllable shapes. The task entertained the

children and ensured their continuity throughout the research

project. All JA phonemes were considered. Participants were asked

to name the object in each picture; they were given clues if neces-

sary, and if still unable to name the object, delayed imitation was

used. The data was further supplemented by spontaneous speech

samples from the children. This involved asking questions on top-

ics related to the selected pictures, and asking them to narrate rou-

tine activities. All of the children’s productions were recorded, trans-

lated and transcribed for further analysis and for inter-rater reli-

ability testing.

Analysis

The picture-naming task and spontaneous recording samples

yielded 284 words that varied in shape and syllable weight. The

syllable structure for each word was carefully analyzed to reveal

the phonological processes applied, and the results were compared

to adults’ productions in JA. The data was entered into Excel sheets,

and the frequency analysis was calculated by counting the percent-

age of each syllable structure occurring in relation to the syllable

number. The analysis considered syllable structure, syllable shapes

across age groups, word syllable structure, syllable numbers across

age groups, and notable phonological processes affecting syllable

structure. An expert who had no knowledge of the purpose and

design of the study randomly selected 25% of the data and rescored

it; inter-rater reliability was 96% and disagreement was resolved

through discussion.

RESULTS

This section summarizes the major findings concerning the

syllable shapes and numbers across age groups. A further sub-sec-

tion is devoted to explaining the phonological repair processes

Page 4: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657956 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

(prosodic and melodic) involved in the production and develop-

ment of syllable structures produced by typical JA children.

Syllable Shape across Age Groups

This section summarizes the frequency of the syllable shapes

produced by the four age stages, as shown in Table 1.

In the first three age stages, children produced open and closed

syllables. They produced a certain set of syllable shapes which in-

cluded CV, CVV, CVC, and CVVC. The most frequent syllable

shape used was CVC and the least were CVV and CVVC (see Ta-

ble 1). Consonant clusters were observed in the speech of the older

age group, but limited to monosyllabic words as in /burʒ/>[burd]

‘bridge’. In this oldest group, children could also use more diverse

open and closed syllable shapes compared to the younger age groups.

Children in the youngest age group, on the other hand, used CV,

CVC and CVV syllable shapes in the majority of their productions.

As can be seen from Table 1, the most frequent syllable shape used

was CVC (38% of the total usage of different syllable shapes) among

children in the four age groups, while the least used shapes were

those containing consonant clusters (0.2%) like CCVCC, CCV

and CCVV. This result supports the assumption that acquisition

of consonant clusters significantly emerges in the oldest group.

Word Syllable Structure

Table 2 summarizes the syllable shapes in monosyllabic, bisyl-

labic and trisyllabic words.

As seen in Table 2, children in the youngest age group tended to

produce monosyllabic words with all of its ‘bimoraic’ shapes at a

relatively high percentage (50%). The most-used monosyllabic struc-

ture was CVG, which had the highest percentage (36%); the least-

used was CV (4%), which was attested once in a bisyllabic word.

The first group also produced bisyllabic words with all of the syl-

lable structures (46%). The most-used structure was CVV/CV with

26% of occurrence, while the least-used were CVV/CVV and CV/

CVC. Children rarely produced trisyllabic words at this early age,

with usage of 3% observed in three occurrences only; only two

shapes were acquired, namely CVC/CVC/CV and CVC/CVV/CV.

No words of more than three syllables were produced at this age.

The children’s productions of monosyllabic words in the second

age group were low (17%), which is much lower than the first group;

only CVG and CVVC were noticed at this age. Bisyllabic words

were produced the most frequently with all shapes totalling 82%.

The most-used structure was CVC/CVC, accounting (28%), and

the least CVVC/CV. An increased use of trisyllabic words over the

previous age group was noticed: 11% overall, with the most fre-

quent shape being CVC/CV/CV and the least frequent CV/CV/

CVVC and CVC/CV/CVVC. Quadrisyllabic words were not pro-

duced at this age.

The production of monosyllabic words in the age of 2;1-2;6 was

further reduced, with an average of 25%, limited to CVG (11%) and

CVVC (14%). Bisyllabic words were produced most frequently,

61% overall. The most-used structure was again CVC/CVC (33%),

and the least-used CV/CVC. These children produced a higher

proportion of trisyllabic words than the previous groups, indicat-

ing the complexity and variability of word production at this age.

Three shapes accounted for 14%, of which the most frequent one

was CVC/CVV/CV (80%). Words of four or more syllables were

still not produced.

The percentage of monosyllabic words produced by the oldest

group was 29%, which is higher than the next two younger age

groups. This discrepancy might seem illogical, but consonant clus-

ters were acquired at this age and this started with the production

of clusters in monosyllabic words. Different shapes were produced,

the most frequent being CVCC and the least frequent CCVCC,

which occurred only once. Bisyllabic words were produced fre-

Table 1. Syllable shapes across age groups

Syllable shape

Age group (yr;mo)Total Frequency

(%)1;0-1;6 1;7-2;0 2;1-2;6 2;7-3;0

CV 43 32 33 45 153 30.0CVV 25 9 15 12 61 12.0CVC 40 49 60 41 190 38.0CVVC 16 23 24 11 74 14.7CCV 0 0 0 1 1 0.2CCVV 0 0 0 0 1 0.2CCVVC 0 0 0 6 6 1.2CCVC 0 0 0 3 3 0.6CCCVV 0 0 0 0 0 0.0CCVCC 0 0 0 1 1 0.2CVCC 0 0 0 11 11 2.2CCVC 0 0 0 3 3 0.6CVVCC 0 0 0 0 0 0.0

C= consonant; V= vowel.

Page 5: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 957

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

Table 2. Word syllable structure

Age (yr;mo) Word syllable shapes Number of occurrences Frequency (%) Overall frequency (%)

1;0-1;6 Monosyllabic words CVV 12 20 50CVG 22 36CVVC 13 21

Bisyllabic words CVC/CVC 7 18 46CVC/CVVC 2 5CV/CV 8 21CVC/CV 9 24CVV/CVV 1 3CVV/CV 10 26CV/CVC 1 3

Trisyllabic words CVC/CVC/CV 1 33 3CVC/CVV/CV 2 67

1;7-2;0 Monosyllabic words CVG 3 23 17CVVC 7 77

Bisyllabic words CVC/CVC 11 28 82CVC/CVVC 8 20CVC/CV 10 25CVVC/CV 2 5CVV/CV 9 22

Trisyllabic words CV/CV/CVVC 1 14 11CVC/CV/CV 3 43CVC/CV/CVVC 1 14CVC/CVV/CV 2 29

2;1-2;6 Monosyllabic words CVG 7 11 25CVVC 11 14

Bisyllabic words CV/CVVC 2 5 61CV/CVC 1 2CVV/CV 9 21CVV/CVC 2 5CVC/CV 7 16CVC/CVVC 8 19CVC/CVC 14 33

Trisyllables words CV/CVV/CV 1 10 14CVC/CVV/CV 8 80CVC/CVC/CV 1 10

2;7-3;0 Monosyllabic words CCVVC 6 32 29CCVCC 1 5CVVC 2 11CVCC 10 53

Bisyllabic words CCV/CVC 1 4 39CCVC/CVC 3 12CV/CVVC 2 8CV/CVC 7 27CVV/CV 3 12CVV/CVC 1 4CVVC/CVC 1 4CVC/CV 2 8CVC/CVV 1 4CVC/CVVC 3 12CVC/CVC 2 8

(Continued to the next page)

Page 6: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657958 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

quently with all shapes, the highest being 39%. The most-used

structure was also CV/CVC (27%). This group also produced the

highest percentage of trisyllabic words (32%), the most frequent

being CVC/CV/CV (29%). Although quadrisyllabic words were

still not produced, future research might investigate their appear-

ance in children aged 3 and above.

Table 3 shows the syllable number used by each age group. Mono-

syllables were readily produced in all groups; bisyllabic words were

also widely used and were the most frequent word shape overall.

Trisyllabic word occurrence increased with age; however, quadri-

syllabic words where not found in the current data.

While the first age group preferred monosyllabic and bisyllabic

words, across all the other groups, bisyllabic words were the most-

preferred word structures.

Notable Phonological Processes Involving Syllable

Structure

This section summarizes the key findings for the phonological

processes in the JA children’s speech corpus. Table 4 shows the syl-

lable structure processes seen in the speech samples.

Cluster reduction

Cluster reduction was consistently and frequently used by chil-

dren below the age of 3, although it declined after this as children

started to acquire clusters in syllable initial and final positions.

The percentage of this process in the first two age groups is lower

than in the third age group (23.5%, 16%) because of the tendency

to delete syllables in all positions and to use open syllables instead

of closed ones. The occurrence of this process was the highest in

the third age group (50%) because children started to suppress syl-

lable deletion processes leading to more complex phonological

systems. Examples include: /kalb.na/ ‘our dog’>[kab.na], /ʔird.

e:n/‘two monkeys’>[ʔid.e:n].

Deletion

Based on this data, deletion was divided into three groups accord-

ing to the process that affected the syllable: onset deletion, weak

syllable deletion, and coda deletion (CVVC > CVV):

Onset deletion

Onset deletion was rarely used by different age groups. It appeared

once in the speech of children in the two youngest groups, twice in

the speech of children in the third youngest group, but was not

seen in the speech of the oldest group (see Table 4, and the exam-

ples below).

(1a) /ʔa.lo:/>[ʔo:]‘hello’(1b) /ʔa.lam/>[ʔa:m]‘pen’(1c) /tə.ʕa:l/>[ta:l]‘come’In the first and third instances (1a & 1c), truncation was per-

Table 3. Syllable numbers across age groups

Syllable number

Age (yr;mo)Total

1;0-1;6 1;7-2;0 2;1-2;6 2;7-3;0

Monosyllables 49 10 18 19 96Bisyllables 38 40 43 26 147Trisyllables 3 7 10 21 41Total 90 57 71 66 284

Age (yr;mo) Word syllable shapes Number of occurrences Frequency (%) Overall frequency (%)

Trisyllabic words CV/CV/CV 1 5 32CV/CVV/CV 1 5CV/CVV/CVC 1 5CV/CVC/CV 3 14CVC/CV/CVC 1 5CVC/CV/CV 6 29CVC/CVV/CV 5 24CVC/CVVC/CVVC 1 5CVC/CVC/CV 1 5CVC/CVC/CVVC 1 5

C= consonant; V= vowel; G= geminate.

Table 2. Continued

Page 7: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 959

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

example (1b), truncation and compensatory vowel lengthening

were operating. Although the frequency of occurrence of onset

deletion is very low in the younger three age groups, their suppres-

sion in 3-year old children marks their increased use of multisyl-

labic word production.

Weak syllable deletion

Weak syllable (otherwise called unstressed syllable) deletion

was the most frequent in the youngest age group with 55.7% of oc-

currences, followed by 21.4%, 21.4%, and 1.5% for the remaining

groups. The data here points to the suppression of this phonologi-

cal process by age 3. Weak syllable deletion appears to last longer

than the onset and coda deletions. The following are examples of

weak syllable deletion:

(2a) /laj.mu:n/>[mu:n]‘lemon’ (age of 1;0-1;6)

(2b) /duk.ka:.nəh/>[na:.neh] ‘shop’ (age of 1;7-2;0)

(2c) /til.fiz.jo:n/>[ʔiz.jo:n] ‘television’ (age of 2;1-2;6)

(2d) /maʕ.kə.ro:.nəh/>[mək.ko:.nəh]‘pasta’ (age of 2;7-3;0)

As reported in previous studies (Al-Btoush, 2005; Al-Tamimi et

al., 2011; Gerken, 1994; Kehoe & Steol-Gammon, 1997, 2001; Levey

& Schwartz, 2002; Pater, 1997; Raja, 2008), children tended to de-

lete non-final and unstressed syllables.

Coda deletion (CVVC>CVV)

This process affects CVVC in monosyllabic words and in word-

final positions, changing them from closed to open (CVVC>CVV).

Examples of this process are:

(3a) /mi:n/>[mi:]‘who’(3b) /nu:r/>[nu:]‘proper f. name’(3c) /go:l/>[go:]‘goal’(3d) /tu:t/>[tu:]‘blueberry’(3e) /ṣa.la:ḥ/>[sa.la:] ‘proper m. name’As can be seen from Table 4, only children in the youngest age

group used this process in producing monosyllabic and bisyllabic

words.

Vowel lengthening and shortening

Processes of vowel lengthening (7%) and shortening (3%) were

only seen among children in the youngest age group in this study.

Vowel lengthening occurred as in /ʃa.ʕar />[sa:l] ‘hair’, where length-

Table 4. Syllable structure processes

ProcessAge (yr;mo)

1;0-1;6 1;7-2;0 2;1-2;6 2;7-3;0

Cluster reduction Number of occurrences 9 6 19 4 Frequency across age groups (%) 23.5 16 50 10.5 Frequency within age group (%) 9 10 42 33Deletion of onset Number of occurrences 1 1 2 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 25 25 50 0 Frequency within age group (%) 1 2 4 0Weak syllable deletion Number of occurrences 39 15 15 1 Frequency across age groups (%) 55.7 21.4 21.4 1.5 Frequency within age group (%) 9.5 6.25 8.25 2Closed syllable to open syllable (CVVC to CVV) Number of occurrences 7 0 0 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 100 0 0 0 Frequency within age group (%) 7 0 0 0Vowel lengthening Number of occurrences 7 0 0 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 100 0 0 0 Frequency within age group (%) 7 0 0 0Vowel shortening Number of occurrences 3 0 0 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 100 0 0 0 Frequency within age group (%) 3 0 0 0Syllable epenthesis Number of occurrences 0 0 0 3 Frequency across age groups (%) 0 0 0 100 Frequency within age group (%) 0 0 0 33Assimilation Number of occurrences 4 2 0 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 67 33 0 0 Frequency within age group (%) 4 3 0 0Gemination Number of occurrences 14 31 7 2 Frequency across age groups (%) 26 57 13 4 Frequency within age group (%) 14 51 16 17Metathesis Number of occurrences 1 6 2 1 Frequency across age groups (%) 10 60 20 10 Frequency within age group (%) 1 10 4 8Onomatopoeia Number of occurrences 20 0 0 0 Frequency across age groups (%) 100 0 0 0 Frequency within age group (%) 19 0 0 0

formed, i.e., deletion of the onset of the second syllable while the

long vowel from the second syllable was preserved. In the second

Page 8: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657960 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

ening of /a/ took place to compensate for the deletion of the onset

in the ultimate syllable, thus resyllabifying the CV.CVC into CVVC.

Vowel shortening occurrence was 3 as in /ha:.da/>[ʔa.da] ’this’

changing the first syllable from CVV into CV.

Syllable epenthesis

Syllable insertion occurred in the oldest age group, but only three

times; it is rarely used in the speech of JA-speaking children simi-

lar to English (Kehoe, 1995). Use of syllable epenthesis included

insertion of a word-medial syllable as in /tiʃ.rab/>[ti.si.la:b] ‘drink’

CVC.CVC>CV.CV.CVVC and insertion of a word-initial syllable

to break CC onset clusters, as in /mna:ħ/>[ʔin.na:ħ] ‘good’ CCVVC

>CVC.CVVC.

Assimilation

Assimilation is observed in the speech of the youngest two groups,

for example:

(4a) /Ɂaʕ.ṭi:.ni/>[ni:.ni]‘give me’ (age of 1;0-1;6)

(4b) /ḥṣa:n/>[na:n]‘horse’ (age of 1;0-1;6)

(4c) /bas.ko:.ta/>[to:.tǝh ] ‘biscuit’ (age of 1;7-2;0)

In example (4a), after the deletion of the initial syllable, the stop

/ṭ/ in the penultimate /ṭi:/ is assimilated with the initial nasal /n/ in

the ultimate syllable /ni/, producing [ni:.ni] ‘give me’. In example

(4b), one child deleted the initial /ḥ/, and then assimilated the em-

phatic fricative /ṣ/ with the final nasal /n/ in the same syllable, pro-

ducing the monosyllabic word [na:n] ‘horse’. In example (4c), the

child produced /bas.ko:ta/ ‘biscuit’ as [to:.tǝh]. Three stages were

involved: /bas.ko:ta/>[ko:.tǝh]>[to:.tǝh]. After deleting the initial

syllable, the velar plosive /k/ was totally assimilated to the alveolar

plosive /t/.

Gemination

Given that gemination is a typical phonological process in Ara-

bic, it is frequently preserved by children of different ages (see Ta-

ble 4). Examples of gemination produced by different age groups

are given below.

(5a) /ʕaṣ.fu:r/>[Ɂaf.fu:l]‘bird’ (age of 1;0-1;6)

(5b) /Ɂal.bas/>[Ɂab.bas]‘get dressed’ (age of 1;7-2;0)

(5c) /mas.ba.ħah/>[sab.ba.ħah] ‘rosary’ (age of 2;7-3;0)

In (5a), /ʕaṣ.fu:r/ ‘bird’ was produced as [Ɂaf.fu:l] in which the /ṣ/

was deleted and compensated by geminating the fricative /f/. In

(5b), /Ɂal.bas/ ‘get dressed’ was produced as [Ɂab-bas]. After delet-

ing the coda consonant /l/, the C slot needs to be filled to compen-

sate for the weight of the syllable, thus gemination of the onset of

the right-hand syllable takes place. In example (5c), gemination

takes place when metathesis was applied to the first syllable. Gem-

ination might be one of the first steps in the development of more

complex word and syllable structure in children’s early speech pro-

duction in Arabic. As can be seen from Table 4, the highest frequen-

cy of this process is seen in the second age group, i.e., 1;7-2;0 years,

after which a marked but gradual decline is evident in the two old-

er age groups. Moreover, it seems that as the children’s phonologi-

cal systems are developing, they tend to resort to the use of gemi-

nation in their attempts to produce longer and more complex words

although the word underwent other phonological processes.

Metathesis

Some interesting examples of metathesis were collected:

(6a) /ta.la.fo:n/>[ta.fa.lo:n]‘phone’ (age of 1;0-1;6)

(6b) /ʕab.lah/>[ʕal.bah]‘proper name’ (age of 1;7-2;0)

(6c) /nas.ka.fe:h/>[san.ka.fe:h]‘Nescafe’ (age of 2;1-2;6)

No rule has been applied to metathesis according to syllable or

even other factors such as sonority; for similar results, see Raja

(2008). The reordering of segments does not affect the template of

the word; for example, the words /ʕab.lah/ and [ʕal.bah] have the

same template but with different segmental ordering, i.e. both have

CVC.CVC.

Onomatopoeia

Onomatopoeia was only used by children in the youngest age

group (19%). The following are examples:

(7a) /səj.ja:.rəh/>[Ɂənn] ‘car’

(7b) /gi:.ṭa:r/>[tu:t] ‘train’(7c) /kalb/>[haww/ʔaww] ‘dog’(7d) /tuf.fa:.ħah/>[ʔam/nan.nah] ‘apple’

DISCUSSION & CONCLUSION

The JA children adhered to the universally structured syllable

that started as CV and at a later stage became CVC, as opposed to

Page 9: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 961

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

what they are exposed to in their input CVCC structure (Daana,

2009). They produced a variety of syllable types to build their own

words. The older age group produced the least-used syllable shape

CV. Intriguingly, monosyllabic words take CVV, CVVC, or CVG

forms across all age bands (the oldest age group also has CCVVC,

and CCVCC). This confirms that JA children develop the ability

to maintain the bimoraic weight parameter, which rises across age

bands. This dictates that the moraic weight of a prosodic word in

JA must minimally comprise two moras (for details on syllable

weight in JA, refer to Huneety & Mashaqba, 2016; Mashaqba &

Huneety, 2018).

In terms of syllable shape across age groups, the oldest children

were able to produce a greater variety of open and closed syllable

shapes than the younger groups. Children in the youngest age group,

on the other hand, used CV, CVC and CVV syllable shapes in the

majority of their productions, similar to the pattern of English-speak-

ing children, as these syllable shapes are usually found in children’s

late babbling stage and early word production (Locke, 1983).

According to the corpus of the present study, the first attempt to

produce a final coda cluster (other than geminate consonants) in a

monosyllabic word was registered by one child in the oldest age

group (Table 1). This observation indicates that children are able

to produce coda clusters at an early age. However, our results do

not agree with those of French (1989), who reported that his son

produced the first consonant cluster word at age 1;10 and five other

words by 2;2. The emergence of the production of clusters in mono-

syllabic words in the oldest age group might mark a shift from clus-

ter reduction to production and might be setting the stage for the

production of syllable clusters in multisyllabic words. This would

explain why the least-used syllable shapes (0.2%) were those con-

taining consonant clusters like CCVCC, CCV and CCVV. This

result supports the assumption that consonant clusters emerge in

older children. This important result suggests that children can

significantly increase the production of consonant clusters after

the age of 2;7, with maturity of their speech mechanisms and con-

tinuous development of the vocal tract. Future studies on older

Arabic-speaking children might consider looking at the trajectory

of consonant cluster production in children beyond the age of 3.

For example, Ammani Arabic children acquire consonant clusters

in both positions at 3 (Daana, 2009).

In adult-speech of JA, clusters are usually produced frequently,

but the children of this age in the current study could not produce

them correctly. Consequently, they often deleted a sound to sim-

plify the cluster. For example, word-initial CCV and word-final

CVCC syllables were simplified to CV and CVC respectively. Al-

though Dyson and Amayreh (2000) did not provide evidence of

cluster reduction in their study, this might be explained by limita-

tions in their elicitation protocol; the picture-description task which

elicited single words did not contain consonant clusters. Nonethe-

less, they did find consonant sequence reduction. Al-Tamimi et al.

(2011) found no evidence for cluster reduction in their sample of

children with cleft lip and palate; this may be because the data

scoring protocol they used in their study relied on the occurrence

of at least 20% of a phonological process for consideration in the

data analysis. Another explanation of this discrepancy might be

the severe limitations in the speech of their children with cleft lip

and palate. However, our data is in agreement with the findings of

Grunwell (1987), who found that cluster reduction is noticeably

reduced after the age of 3. It should be noted that children tend to

produce the second consonant in the cluster when it occurs in the

coda, regardless of sonority; for example /kalb.na/ ‘our dog’ to

[kab.na] and /ʔird.na/ ‘our monkey’ to [ʔid.na]. Similar findings

were reported in Cairene Arabic (Ragheb & Davis, 2010). Howev-

er, these findings disagree with those of Abdoh (2011), which can

be attributed to the limited data in the speech of her participants

(i.e., only one example of this process was found in her sample).

As for the phonological processes employed by children, the ac-

quisition of clusters is one of the longest-lasting aspects of speech

acquisition by normally developing children (Mcleod et al., 2001,

p. 99). Children as young as 2 produce some consonant clusters

correctly (e.g., Preisser et al., 1988; Stoel-Gammon, 1987; Watson

& Scukanec, 1997). Naturally, children tend to delete a consonant

in consonant clusters; such a conclusion is also attested to in previ-

ous investigations (e.g., Bernhardt & Stemberger, 1998; Ingram,

1974, 1989; Kiparsky & Menn, 1977; Olmsted, 1971; Smith, 1973;

Vihman, 1980). The acquisition of consonant clusters has been in-

vestigated in different positions of syllables; word-initial clusters

are acquired at 2, before clusters that occur word-finally. The latter

are acquired at 2;6 (Kehoe & Steol-Gammon, 2001). Given that

syllable weight is maximally bimoraic in JA where a syllable can-

Page 10: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657962 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

not include more than two moras (Huneety & Mashaqba, 2016;

Mashaqba & Huneety, 2018), and given that final codas and initial

onsets are moraically weightless (extrametrical), then children are

expected to be very sensitive to the prosodic weight at an early stage

in their language development; they reduce the syllable structure

via deleting the peripheral weightless elements and keeping the

core elements which count to the prosodic weight. Recall that V

has one mora, VV has two moras, geminates have one mora, and

onset singleton consonants and word-final consonants are weight-

less (Hayes, 1995).

The results here corroborate the findings of Ragheb & Davis

(2010), who provided several examples in support of final cluster

reduction regardless of sonority. In the onset clusters, the order of

the consonants has no influence on cluster reduction as children

produced obstruents and deleted sonorants; for example /bri:ʔ/

‘jug’ was produced as [bi:ʔ], /mħam.mad/ ‘Mohammad’ as [ħam.

mad], and /mba:.riħ/ ‘yesterday’ as [ba:.liħ]. The children also pro-

duced voiceless and deleted voiced obstruents when they occurred

in the onset, as in /btiʒ.li/ ‘she is washing’ which was produced as

[tiz.li]. Recall that the voiceless /t/ is acquired before the voiced /b/,

and /ħ/ and /b/ are acquired before /m/ in the cluster. These results

seem to be similar to those of Watson & Scukanec (1997). For more

detail on patterns of word-initial cluster reduction see Locke (1983,

pp. 68-71).

In terms of deletion (onset/coda/syllable), although the frequen-

cy of the occurrence of onset deletion is very low in the younger

three age groups, their suppression in 3-year-old children seems to

mark an increase in their multisyllabic word production. As for

weak syllable deletion, it appears to last longer than the onset/coda

deletion, confirming the work of Grunwell (1987) and Ingram

(1989). These results indicate that this process is used by children

more in the early stages because they cannot produce superheavy

syllables and multisyllabic words easily like adults. The CVVC>

CVV (coda deletion) process was seen in the speech of children in

the youngest age group, perhaps because of their limited ability to

produce a more complex syllable structure; recall that final C is

extrametrical. In the older groups, children were able to produce

multisyllabic words, which led them to use other phonological pro-

cesses like cluster reduction.

Syllable epenthesis rarely occurred in the oldest age group. This

result is in line with Kehoe (1995). Studies on Arabic-speaking chil-

dren considered epenthesis from a segmental perspective as they

documented it in cluster word-final position (Ayyad, 2009; Dyson

& Amayreh, 2000), but they provided no evidence of the syllable

epenthesis that was found here. The current study found syllable

epenthesis in word-medial position, which is in agreement with

the finding for English-speaking children (Kehoe, 1995); however,

contrary to English which has epenthesis in word-final position,

Arabic also showed epenthesis in word-initial position. Even though

use of epenthesis was rare in our data, its use was observed in the

older group, meaning that some children might be using it as a

trochaic template to compensate for the demands on their phono-

logical systems to produce multisyllabic words (cf., Kehoe, 1994).

Basically, the purpose of simple syllable insertion is to facilitate

meaningful acquisition of words, where children (whether delib-

erately or otherwise) develop their own ability to articulate multi-

syllabic words without resorting to complex structures (cf., Lleó,

1990; Moskowitz, 1973). Syllable insertion can also be regarded as

a successful strategy in repairing the prosodic word structure if

the child is unable to appropriately produce the other syllables of

the word (cf., Ingram, 1974, p. 54). Future research on children with

speech-sound disorders in Arabic might consider investigating the

use of syllable epenthesis, as it might provide prognostic signs in

those trying to compensate for their disordered phonological sys-

tem in an attempt to approximate the adult system.

Assimilation observed in the speech of the youngest two groups

is in agreement with that reported for English-speaking children.

It is believed that these assimilatory processes are part of the nor-

mal phonological development. Where they persist beyond the age

of 3, they might be a sign of a speech-sound disorder (Grunwell,

1987). This warrants more caution and testing of Arabic-speaking

children exhibiting this process on the part of speech language pa-

thologists. However, this process is not seen in all typically devel-

oping children (Grunwell, 1987) as is the case here for the Arabic-

speaking children participating in the study.

In agreement with Dyson & Amayreh’s (2000) findings, gemi-

nation is preserved even when segments have undergone substitu-

tion processes. Gemination was also maintained even though the

word underwent a syllable structure deletion process. Gemination

was preserved even when segments were simplified (cf., Dyson &

Page 11: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 963

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

Amayreh, 2000), but with the doubling of the consonant sequence.

Intrigingly, JA children resort to gemination (and assimilation) to

rescue the bimoriac minimality forced by the prosodic parameters

of Arabic phonology. Adding a new segment grants extra moraic

weight to the phonological word. If the child produces bis alone

(instead of biss ‘cat’), the word structure loses its bimoraic proper-

ty, which is a basic principle of adults’ language. Given that chil-

dren’s articulatory system is not yet fully developed, this is a sim-

ple way to use extra elements that belong to the first produced seg-

ments.

Use of onomatopoeia by the youngest group seems to be a sign

of their limited phonological systems, which are still unable to pro-

duce certain set of consonants, complex word or syllable structures;

this is obvious from their frequent use of monosyllabic (CVG and

CVVC) words. One explanation of the use of onomatopoeic words

might be their salient features, i.e., clear prosodic features; when

produced by mothers, they might help children learn them as fre-

quent early productions (Laing, 2014). However, this process had

completely disappeared in the three older groups, which also means

that the children’s abilities to produce complex word and syllable

structures were developing. Consequently, the persistence of ono-

matopoeia may provide clinicians and researchers who study child

language development in Arabic with an early indicator to war-

rant in-depth testing of children’s phonological systems, in order

to rule out the presence of speech sound disorders as most of the

target words which underwent change to onomatopoeia also un-

derwent syllable structure processes (i.e., reduction to one-syllable

words).

In conclusion, this research examined development of the sylla-

ble, word syllable structure and number, and notable phonological

processes among JA-speaking children aged 1 to 3. Children ac-

quire syllables by imitating adults’ production of different words.

They tend to apply phonological processes to repair syllables to

adult-like shapes and to compensate for the loss of a syllable or its

parts. Ten processes were investigated: assimilation, cluster reduc-

tion, syllable deletion, changing from closed to open syllables (CVVC

>CVV), gemination, metathesis, onomatopoeia, vowel lengthen-

ing, vowel shortening, and syllable epenthesis. Analysis of the data

shows that the most frequent syllable shape used was CVC and the

least were those with consonant clusters. In line with Rvachew,

Nowak, & Cloutier (2004, p. 251), it was shown that the progressive

increases of phoneme perception are mainly linked to improve-

ments in the accuracy and stability of syllable and word articula-

tion, as the children adapt their speech production to approach the

phonetic and prosodic characteristics of native language speech-

sound features. The corpus of the present work proves that chil-

dren try to develop the metrification system in Arabic at an early

stage. They are aware of the inherit weight assigned to segments in

the core and peripheral positions and of the coda non-final posi-

tions assigned by the Weight by Position rule. The children resort-

ed to multiple repair processes to preserve that mora instead of de-

leting it when encountering an articulatory problem such as gemi-

nation, assimilation, and syllable insertion.

Word syllable shapes were investigated in this study, the most

frequently used shape being bisyllabic words. Trisyllabic word us-

age increased as children grew older, but quadrisyllabic words were

not produced by any of the children in the four age groups recruit-

ed in this study. Children’s first words usually target adult words

of simple prosodic structure (minimally bimoraic), and one- or

two-syllable word forms with open syllables, no clusters and core

consonants (stops, nasals, glottals and glides). Despite the docu-

mented similarities across children in terms of the sound segment

and syllabic types produced, there is often a wide range of differ-

ences in the target words that are attempted. This discrepancy re-

lates to patterns of preference for, and avoidance of, particular sounds

or sound classes or certain syllabic shapes (Ferguson & Farwell,

1975; Vihman, 2013). The study can promote future research on

JA that might use larger samples of children.

Finally, this study might provide researchers and clinicians (speech

language pathologists) with valuable information on the develop-

ment of the early syllable and word structure in Arabic-speaking

children, which might aid in the early identification of at-risk chil-

dren and in the best choice of therapy targets in terms of develop-

mental syllable structure.

REFERENCES

Abdo, D. (1980). Studies in Arabic phonology. Kuwait: Assabah.

Abdoh, E. M. A. (2011). A study of the phonological structure and representa-

tion of first words in Arabic (Doctoral dissertation). University of Leicester,

Page 12: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657964 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

UK.‏

Al-Ani, S. H. (1970). Arabic phonology: an acoustical and physiological inves-

tigation. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

Al-Btoush, M. A. A. (2005). The acquisition of stress in Jordanian Arabic and

English among children (MA Thesis). University of Jordan, Jordan.‏

Al-Mashagbah, B. (2010). An acoustic analysis of geminated consonants in the

Arabic of native speakers of English. (MA. Thesis). Yarmouk University,

Jordan.

Al-Tamimi, F. Y., Owais, A. I., Khabour, O. F., & Khamaiseh, Z. A. (2011).

Phonological processes in the speech of Jordanian Arabic children with

cleft lip and/or palate. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 32(4), 247-255.

Al-Wer, E. (2007). Jordanian Arabic (Amman). In K. Versteegh et al. (Eds.),

Encyclopedia of Arabic language and linguistics volume 2 (pp. 505-517).

Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill.

Alqattan, S. (2015). Early phonological acquisition by Kuwaiti Arabic children

(Doctoral dissertation). Newcastle University, UK.

Ammar, W., & Morsi, R. (2006). Phonological development and disorders:

colloquial Egyptian Arabic. In Z. Hua & B. Dodd (Eds.), Phonological de-

velopment and disorders in children (pp. 204-232). Clevedon, UK: Multi-

lingual Matters.

Ayyad, H. (2011). Phonological development of typically developing Kuwaiti

Arabic-speaking preschoolers (Doctoral dissertation). The University of

British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada.

Ayyad, H., & Bernhardt, B. M. (2009). Phonological development of Kuwaiti

Arabic: preliminary data. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 23(11), 794-807.

Bernhardt, B. H., & Sternberger, J. P. (1998). Handbook of phonological devel-

opment: from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. New

York, NY: Academic Press.

Blanc, H. (1953). Studies in North Palestinian Arabic: linguistic inquiries among

the Druzes of Western Galilee and Mt. Carmel. Jerusalem: Israel Oriental

Society.

Bohland, J. W., & Guenther, F. H. (2006). An fMRI investigation of syllable

sequence production. Neuroimage, 32(2), 821-841.

Cantineau, J. (1960). Studies of Arabic Linguistics [Etudes de linguistique Arabe].

Paris: Klincksieck.

Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York,

NY: Harper and Row.

Crystal, D. (2008). A dictionary of linguistics and phonetics. Malden, MA:

Blackwell.

Daana, H. A. (2009). The development of consonant clusters, stress and plural

nouns in Jordanian Arabic child language (Doctoral dissertation). The Uni-

versity of Essex, UK.

Delattre, P. (1968). The general phonetic characteristics of languages. Santa

Barbara, CA: University of California.

Dyson, A. T. (1988). Phonetic inventories of 2-and 3-year-old children. Jour-

nal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53(1), 89-93.

Dyson, A. T., & Amayreh, M. M. (2000). Phonological errors and sound chang-

es in Arabic-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 14(2), 79-

109.

Fee, J., & Ingram, D. (1982). Reduplication as a strategy of phonological de-

velopment. Journal of Child Language, 9(1), 41-54.

Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language

acquisition. Language, 51(2), 419-439.

French, A. (1989). The systematic acquisition of word forms by a child dur-

ing the first-fifty-word stage. Journal of Child Language, 16(1), 69-90.

Fromkin, V., Rodman, R., & Hyams, N. (2013). An introduction to language

(10th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.

Gerken, L. (1994). A metrical template account of children’s weak syllable

omissions from multisyllabic words. Journal of Child Language, 21(3), 565-

584.

Goldsmith, J. (1976). Autosegmental phonology (Doctoral dissertation). Mas-

sachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA.

Goldsmith, J. (1979). The aims of autosegmental phonology. In D. A. Dinnsen

& S. R. Anderson (Eds.), Current approaches to phonological theory (pp.

202-222). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Grunwell, P. (1987). Clinical phonology (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams

& Wilkins.

Gurský, P. (2005). Comparison of parallel and random approach to a candi-

date list in the multifeature querying. Proceedings of 2005 Workshop on

Databases, Texts, Specifications and Objects (DATESO), Desna, Czech Re-

public.

Haelsig, P. C., & Madison, C. L. (1986). A study of phonological processes

exhibited by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Language, Speech, and Hear-

ing Services in Schools, 17(2), 107-114.

Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. Chicago.

University of Chicago Press.

Huneety, A. (2015). The phonology and morphology of Wadi Mousa Arabic

(Doctoral dissertation). University of Salford, UK.

Huneety, A., & Mashaqba, B. (2016). Stress rules in loan words in Bedouin

Jordanian Arabic in the north of Jordan: a metrical account. SKASE Jour-

Page 13: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 965

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

nal of Theoretical Linguistics, 13(3), 2-13.

Ingram, D. (1974). Phonological rules in young children. Journal of Child

Language, 1(1), 49-64.

Ingram, D. (1989). Phonological disability in children (2nd ed.). London: Cole

and Whurr.

Inose, H. (2007). Translating Japanese onomatopoeia and mimetic words.

Translation Research Projects, 1, 97-116.

Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. Ber-

lin: Walter de Gruyter.

Jessen, M. (1998). Phonetics and phonology of tense and lax obstruents in Ger-

man. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing.

Keffala, B., Barlow, J. A., & Rose, S. (2018). Interaction in Spanish–English

bilinguals’ acquisition of syllable structure. International Journal of Bilin-

gualism, 22(1), 16-37.

Kehoe, M. (1994). Beyond the trochaic constraint: an examination of rhyth-

mic processes in English children’s productions of multisyllabic words.

Child Phonology Conference, Sun Valley, ID.

Kehoe, M. (1995). An investigation of rhythmic processes in English-speaking

children’s word productions (Doctoral dissertation). University of Washing-

ton, Seattle, WA.

Kehoe, M. (2001). Prosodic patterns in children’s multisyllabic word produc-

tions. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(4), 284-294.

Kehoe, M., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (1997). Truncation patterns in English-speak-

ing children’s word productions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing

Research, 40(3), 526-541.

Kehoe, M., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Development of syllable structure

in English-speaking children with particular reference to rhymes. Journal

of Child Language, 28(2), 393-432.

Kiparsky, P., & Menn, L. (1977). On the acquisition of phonology. In J. Mac-

Namara (Ed.), Language learning and thought (pp. 47-78). New York, NY:

Academic Press.

Ladefoged, P. (2006). A course in phonetics. Boston, MA: Thomson Wadsworth.

Laing, C. E. (2014). Tracing the role of onomatopoeia in early word produc-

tion. First Language, 34, 387-405.

Laver, J. (1994). Principles of phonetics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univer-

sity Press.

Levelt, C. C., Schiller, N. O., & Levelt, W. J. (2000). The acquisition of syllable

types. Language Acquisition, 8(3), 237-264.

Levey, S., & Schwartz, R. G. (2002). Syllable omission by two-year-old chil-

dren. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 23(4), 168-176.

Lleó, C. (1990). Homonymy and reduplication: on the extended availability

of two strategies in phonological acquisition. Journal of Child Language,

17(2), 267-278.

Lleó, C., & Prinz, M. (1996). Consonant clusters in child phonology and the

directionality of syllable structure assignment. Journal of Child Language,

23(1), 31-56.

Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological acquisition and change. New York, NY: Aca-

demic Press.

Łukaszewicz, B. (2007). Reduction in syllable onsets in the acquisition of Pol-

ish: deletion, coalescence, metathesis and gemination. Journal of Child

Language, 34(1), 53-82.

Mashaqba, B. (2015). The phonology and morphology of Wādi Ramm Arabic

(Doctoral dissertation). University of Salford, UK.

Mashaqba, B., & Huneety, A. (2018). Emergence of iambs in Eastern Arabic:

Metrical iambicity dominating optimal nonfinality. SKASE Journal of The-

oretical Linguistics, 15(3), 15-37.

Mashaqba, B., Al-Khawaldeh, N., AlGweirien, H. & Al-Edwan, Y. (in press).

Acquisition of broken plural patterns by Jordanian children. Linguistics,

58(5).

McLeod, S., Van Doorn, J., & Reed, V. A. (2001). Normal acquisition of con-

sonant clusters. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(2), 99-

110.

Moskowitz, A. I. (1973). The acquisition of phonology and syntax: a prelimi-

nary study. In K. J. J. Hintikka et al. (Eds.), Approaches to natural language

(pp. 48-84). Dordrecht: Springer.

Olmsted, D. (1971). Out of the mouth of babes. Berlin: De Gruyter.

Pater, J. (1997). Minimal violation and phonological development. Language

Acquisition, 6(3), 201-253.

Preisser, D. A., Hodson, B. W., & Paden, E. P. (1988). Developmental phonol-

ogy: 18-29 months. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53(2), 125-

130.

Ragheb, M., & Davis, S. (2010, April). The acquisition of word-final clusters

in Cairense Arabic: an OT analysis. Arabic Linguistics Symposium, Austin,

TX.

Raja, P. (2008). Phonological processes of three Indonesian children aged be-

tween 1;8 and 2;6. k@ta lama, 9(2), 126-140.

Ridouane, R. (2007). Gemination in Tashlhiyt Berber: an acoustic and articu-

latory study. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 37(2), 119-

142.

Roach, P. (2009). English phonetics and phonology: a practical course (4th ed.).

Page 14: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657966 https://www.e-csd.org

Bassil M. Mashaqba, et al. • Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Roberts, J. E., Burchinal, M., & Footo, M. M. (1990). Phonological process

decline from 212 to 8 years. Journal of Communication Disorders, 23(3),

205-217.

Rose, Y. (1997). Sonority and the acquisition of Dutch syllable structure. To-

ronto Working Papers in Linguistics, 16(1), 133-143.

Rvachew, S., & Andrews, E. (2002). The influence of syllable position on chil-

dren’s production of consonants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16(3),

183-198.

Rvachew, S., Nowak, M., & Cloutier, G. (2004). Effect of phonemic percep-

tion training on the speech production and phonological awareness skills

of children with expressive phonological delay. American Journal of Speech-

Language Pathology, 13(3), 250-263.

Saleh, M., Shoeib, R., Hegazi, M., & Ali, P. (2007). Early phonological devel-

opment in Arabic Egyptian children: 12-30 months. Folia Phoniatrica et

Logopaedica, 59(5), 234-240.

Smit, A. B. (1993). Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska Ar-

ticulation Norms Project: word-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Speech,

Language, and Hearing Research, 36(5), 931-947.

Smit, A. B., Hand, L., Freilinger, J. J., Bernthal, J. E., & Bird, A. (1990). The

Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of

Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55(4), 779-798.

Smith, N. V. (1973). The acquisition of phonology: a case study. Cambridge,

UK: Cambridge University Press.

Stoel-Gammon, C. (1987). Phonological skills of 2-year-olds. Language, Speech,

and Hearing Services in Schools, 18(4), 323-329.

Tempun, M. (1957). Certain language skills in children. Minneapolis, MN:

University of Minnesota Press.

Trask, R. L. (1996). A dictionary of phonetics and phonology. London: Rout-

ledge.

Vihman, M. M. (1980). Sound change and child language. In E. C. Traugott

et al. (Eds.), Papers from the 4th International Conference on Historical Lin-

guistics (pp. 303-320). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

Vihman, M. M. (2013). Phonological development: the first two years. Hobo-

ken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Watson, M. M., & Scukanec, G. P. (1997). Profiling the phonological abilities

of 2-year-olds: a longitudinal investigation. Child Language Teaching and

Therapy, 13(1), 3-14.

Young, E. C. (1991). An analysis of young children’s ability to produce multi-

syllabic English nouns. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 5(4), 297-316.

Page 15: Acquisition of Syllable Structure in Jordanian Arabic · nant lengthening involving one phase of production (e.g., Al-Ani, 1970; Blanc, 1953; Cantineau, 1960); (2) duplication where

https://doi.org/10.12963/csd.19657 https://www.e-csd.org 967

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득 • Bassil M. Mashaqba 외

국문초록

요르단 아랍어에서의 음절 습득

Bassil M. Mashaqba1·Khalid G. Al-Shdifat2·Anas I. Al Huneety1·Mais Abu Alhala1

1Hashemite University, 2Jordan University of Science and Technology

배경 및 목적: 본 연구의 목적은 요르단 아랍어를 산출하는 아동의 초기 단어 음절 구조와 주요한 음운 과정을 살펴보는 것이다. 방법:

총 20명 아동의 이름대기 발화와 자발화를 전사하고 음절 구조를 분석하였다. 참여자의 연령대는 1-3세였으며 1;0-1:6, 1:7-2;0, 2:1-2:6,

2:7-3;0세 등 총 네 연령대 집단으로 구분되었다. 이후 발화자료는 단어 음절 구조와 음절수에 영향을 주는 10가지 음운 과정에 따라 분

석되었다. 결과: CVC가 모든 연령대에서 가장 빈번히 관찰된 음절 구조였다. 음절수에 있어서는 2음절 단어가 가장 빈번히 사용되었다.

의성어, 약음절 생략, CVVC 폐쇄음절의 CVV음절로의 생략 등이 첫번째 연령대에서 관찰되었다. 자음군 생략과 초성/종성생략이 2:1-

2:6개월 연령집단에서 가장 빈번히 관찰되었다. 음절 생략은 3세까지 관찰되었다. 논의 및 결론: 요르단 아랍어를 사용하는 아동은 초

기 발화의 이음절과 다음절 단어에서 CVC 음절 구조를 주로 사용하는 것으로 나타났다. 아동은 아랍어 음운 단어의 최소 양모라 값

을 유지하기 위하여 CV 혹은 CVC 일음절을 회피하였다. 의성어는 1세 이후부터 3세까지 나타났는데 음절 축약이 함께 나타났다. 이러

한 결과가 최근 문헌과 관련 지어 논의되었다.

핵심어: 음절 구조, 습득, 음운 과정, 의성어, 요르단 아랍어

ORCID

Bassil M. Mashaqba (https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0835-0075); Khalid G. Al-Shdifat (교신저자, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1025-7892);Anas I. Al Huneety (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1123-6102); Mais Abu Alhala (https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7393-8989)