11
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology I9VR, Vol. 66, No. 2, 259-269 Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-006X/98/$3.00 Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal Christopher I. Eckhardt Southern Methodist University Krista A. Barbour and Gerald C. Davison University of Southern California The cognitive correlates of anger arousal were investigated in community-based samples of maritally violent (MV), maritally distressed-nonviolent (DNV), and maritally satisfied-nonviolent (SNV) husbands. Participants performed the Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations (ATSS) paradigm while listening to anger-arousing audiotapes. Trained raters coded for irrational beliefs, cognitive biases, hostile attributiona] biases, and anger control statements. Results indicated that MV men articulated significantly more irrational thoughts and cognitive biases than DNV and SNV men. MV men articulated more hostile attributional biases than DNV and SNV men across all ATSS scenarios. SNV men. however, articulated more anger control statements during ATSS anger arousal than MV or DNV participants. Discriminant function analyses indicated that specific thoughts discriminated between the groups and differentiated mildly from severely violent participants. ATSS cognitive distortions (a) were not correlated with questionnaire measures of cognitive distortion, and (b) were superior to questionnaire measures in discriminating between the groups. The findings are interpreted in light of recent advances in understanding the relationship between information processing, anger, and marital aggression. Researchers have suggested that anger and hostility are dis- criminating characteristics of maritally violent (MV) men using both self-report and marital interaction research designs (for a review, see Eckhardt, Barbour, & Stuart, 1997). Despite these data, theories of marital violence have not adequately described how anger is aroused and how it influences the enactment of aggression in marriage. Cognitive models of emotion and psy- chopathology, such as those proposed by Ellis (1962) and Beck (1976), have gained clinical acceptance but have only recently been applied to marital violence (e.g., Holtzworth-Munroe, 1992; O'Leary & Vivian, 1990). To assess the degree to which various cognitive distortions and deficiencies characterize MV husbands during laboratory anger arousal, we employed a think- aloud cognitive assessment task, the Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations paradigm (ATSS; Davison, Robins, & Christopher I. Eckhardt, Department of Psychology, Southern Method- ist University; Krista A. Barbour and Gerald C. Davison, Department of Psychology, University of Southern California. This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH55028-01. Portions of this research were presented at the 1996 convention of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, New York, New York. We acknowledge the assistance of numerous individuals in the com- pletion of mis research, including the psychology department of the University of North Carolina—Wilmington, Howard Kassinove, John Wilson, Heather Smith, Matthew Kandies, Lara McGill, Melanie Dye, Stacey Reed, and Tapestry Theater Company. We also appreciate the helpful comments of Ellen Dennehy. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christopher I. Eckhardt, Department of Psychology, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas 75275. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. Johnson, 1983). with samples of MV, maritally distressed-non- violent (DNV), and maritally satisfied-nonviolent (SNV) men. Although ample data indicate that anger differentiates MV from nonviolent men, Margolin et al. (1988) and Burman et al. (1993) have noted that the cognitive processes associated with anger hyperarousal have not received sufficient research atten- tion, which is somewhat surprising for three following reasons. First, it is now commonplace for clinical researchers and com- munity intervention programs to espouse cognitively oriented anger management therapeutic interventions for maritally ag- gressive individuals (Arias & O'Leary, 1988; Holtzworth- Munroe & Stuart, 1994a). If one assumes that cognitive treat- ments are best targeted to cognitive phenomena, it is important to demonstrate that MV men exhibit cognitive distortions and deficiencies under appropriate conditions. Second, question- naire-based research investigating the relationship between anger and cognition in clinical and nonclinical samples has indi- cated that endorsement of Ellis's (1962) list of irrational beliefs is associated with higher scores on various anger scales (e.g., Hogg & Deffenbacher, 1986; Kassinove & Eckhardt, 1994). One would therefore expect MV men, who also show high levels of trait anger, to demonstrate similar anger-related cognitive dis- tortions. Third, a wealth of research has indicated that a variety of attitudinal and cognitive distortions are associated with mari- tal dissatisfaction (e.g., Bradbury, Beach, Fincham, & Nelson, 1996). Given that MV men also experience higher levels of marital distress than nonviolent husbands (Holtzworth-Munroe etal., 1992; Rosenbaum & O'Leary, 1981), it should follow that MV men should also show distortions in cognitive processing. Inspection of the marital violence research literature, however, suggests that many of the preceding assumptions have yet to be confirmed. Researchers examining the cognitive characteristics of MV men have typically investigated global attitudes and attributions. One consistent finding is that MV men are more 259

Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

Journal of Consulting and Clinical PsychologyI9VR, Vol. 66, No. 2, 259-269

Copyright 1998 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.0022-006X/98/$3.00

Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men DuringAnger Arousal

Christopher I. EckhardtSouthern Methodist University

Krista A. Barbour and Gerald C. DavisonUniversity of Southern California

The cognitive correlates of anger arousal were investigated in community-based samples of maritally

violent (MV), maritally distressed-nonviolent (DNV), and maritally satisfied-nonviolent (SNV)

husbands. Participants performed the Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations (ATSS) paradigm

while listening to anger-arousing audiotapes. Trained raters coded for irrational beliefs, cognitive

biases, hostile attributiona] biases, and anger control statements. Results indicated that MV men

articulated significantly more irrational thoughts and cognitive biases than DNV and SNV men. MV

men articulated more hostile attributional biases than DNV and SNV men across all ATSS scenarios.

SNV men. however, articulated more anger control statements during ATSS anger arousal than MV

or DNV participants. Discriminant function analyses indicated that specific thoughts discriminated

between the groups and differentiated mildly from severely violent participants. ATSS cognitive

distortions (a) were not correlated with questionnaire measures of cognitive distortion, and (b) were

superior to questionnaire measures in discriminating between the groups. The findings are interpreted

in light of recent advances in understanding the relationship between information processing, anger,

and marital aggression.

Researchers have suggested that anger and hostility are dis-

criminating characteristics of maritally violent (MV) men using

both self-report and marital interaction research designs (for a

review, see Eckhardt, Barbour, & Stuart, 1997). Despite these

data, theories of marital violence have not adequately described

how anger is aroused and how it influences the enactment of

aggression in marriage. Cognitive models of emotion and psy-

chopathology, such as those proposed by Ellis (1962) and Beck

(1976), have gained clinical acceptance but have only recently

been applied to marital violence (e.g., Holtzworth-Munroe,

1992; O'Leary & Vivian, 1990). To assess the degree to which

various cognitive distortions and deficiencies characterize MV

husbands during laboratory anger arousal, we employed a think-

aloud cognitive assessment task, the Articulated Thoughts in

Simulated Situations paradigm (ATSS; Davison, Robins, &

Christopher I. Eckhardt, Department of Psychology, Southern Method-

ist University; Krista A. Barbour and Gerald C. Davison, Department of

Psychology, University of Southern California.

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health

Grant MH55028-01. Portions of this research were presented at the 1996

convention of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy,

New York, New York.

We acknowledge the assistance of numerous individuals in the com-

pletion of mis research, including the psychology department of the

University of North Carolina—Wilmington, Howard Kassinove, John

Wilson, Heather Smith, Matthew Kandies, Lara McGill, Melanie Dye,

Stacey Reed, and Tapestry Theater Company. We also appreciate the

helpful comments of Ellen Dennehy.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to

Christopher I. Eckhardt, Department of Psychology, Southern Methodist

University, Dallas, Texas 75275. Electronic mail may be sent to

[email protected].

Johnson, 1983). with samples of MV, maritally distressed-non-

violent (DNV), and maritally satisfied-nonviolent (SNV) men.

Although ample data indicate that anger differentiates MV

from nonviolent men, Margolin et al. (1988) and Burman et al.

(1993) have noted that the cognitive processes associated with

anger hyperarousal have not received sufficient research atten-

tion, which is somewhat surprising for three following reasons.

First, it is now commonplace for clinical researchers and com-

munity intervention programs to espouse cognitively oriented

anger management therapeutic interventions for maritally ag-

gressive individuals (Arias & O'Leary, 1988; Holtzworth-

Munroe & Stuart, 1994a). If one assumes that cognitive treat-

ments are best targeted to cognitive phenomena, it is important

to demonstrate that MV men exhibit cognitive distortions and

deficiencies under appropriate conditions. Second, question-

naire-based research investigating the relationship between

anger and cognition in clinical and nonclinical samples has indi-

cated that endorsement of Ellis's (1962) list of irrational beliefs

is associated with higher scores on various anger scales (e.g.,

Hogg & Deffenbacher, 1986; Kassinove & Eckhardt, 1994).

One would therefore expect MV men, who also show high levels

of trait anger, to demonstrate similar anger-related cognitive dis-

tortions. Third, a wealth of research has indicated that a variety

of attitudinal and cognitive distortions are associated with mari-

tal dissatisfaction (e.g., Bradbury, Beach, Fincham, & Nelson,

1996). Given that MV men also experience higher levels of

marital distress than nonviolent husbands (Holtzworth-Munroe

etal., 1992; Rosenbaum & O'Leary, 1981), it should follow that

MV men should also show distortions in cognitive processing.

Inspection of the marital violence research literature, however,

suggests that many of the preceding assumptions have yet to be

confirmed. Researchers examining the cognitive characteristics

of MV men have typically investigated global attitudes and

attributions. One consistent finding is that MV men are more

259

Page 2: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

260 ECKHARDT. BARBOUR, AND DAVISON

likely to espouse favorable attitudes toward the use of aggres-

sion in marriage (e.g., Saunders, Lynch, Grayson, & Linz, 1987;

Stith & Farley, 1993). Some data suggest that men who believe

in conservative, traditional sex role stereotypes have higher mar-

ital aggression levels (e.g., Grossman, Stith, & Bender, 1990),

but other investigators have not found differential attitudes to-

ward women among MV and nonviolent men (e.g., Neidig,

Friedman, & Collins, 1986). Unfortunately, it is difficult to draw

firm conclusions about the predictive validity of these attitudinal

constructs because it is not clear whether MV men held these

attitudes before the onset of marital aggression, or if they

emerged as post hoc explanations for aggressive acts (Holtz-

worth-Munroe et al., 1997).

Tb guide our assessment of anger-related cognitive variables,

we adopted the cognitive taxonomy presented by Kendall and

colleagues (Ingram, Kendall, Smith, Donnell, & Ronan, 1987;

Kendall, 1992; Kendall & Dobson, 1993). In this framework, the

construct o f ' 'cognition'' is partitioned into cognitive processes,

cognitive structures, cognitive content, and cognitive products.

Cognitive processes refer to active thinking processes involved

in attending, storing, retrieving, and interpreting information.

These processes can be further differentiated into cognitive dis-

tortions, which represent illogical, faulty, or misguided pro-

cessing, and cognitive deficiencies, which represent "an insuf-

ficient amount of cognitive activity in situations wherein more

forethought would be beneficial" (Kendall, 1992, p. 5). Cogni-

tive structures refer to schematic templates that guide cognitive

processing, such as the depressive schema hypothesized by Beck

(1967) to contribute to depression. Cognitive content refers to

' 'information that is stored and organized in memory" (Kendall,

1992, p. 2), such as representations of specific life events. Cog-

nitive products refer to the end-stage cognitions that result from

prior structures, processes, and content, such as causal attribu-

tions. The present research focuses on cognitive processes (both

cognitive distortions and cognitive deficiencies) and cognitive

products (hostile attributional bias), although all four cognitive

operations are presumed to be interactive (Kendall, 1992).

Recently, Holtzworth-Munroe (1992) presented a cognitive

model of social skill deficits in MV men outlining the sequence

of information processing stages that can result in marital ag-

gression. During the first stage, decoding, the individual attends

to, perceives, and interprets social stimuli. However, "various

cognitive deficits, including unrealistic expectations, faulty attri-

butions, and irrational beliefs could result in the misconsrrual

of social stimuli" (Holtzworth-Munroe, 1992, p. 607). Thus,

automatic, overlearned information processing distortions may

alter the significance of incoming stimuli or result in other cogni-

tive products, such as hostile attributions, that may otherwise

disrupt accurate, goal-congruent processing of social stimuli.

During the second decision-making stage, the individual is con-

fronted with the task of strategically constructing a number

of potential responses to manage the demands of the specific

situation. After selecting the most appropriate response, the indi-

vidual decides if he has sufficient skill to enact that response and

judges the response's likely positive and negative consequences.

Finally, the third stage describes response enactment, during

which the individual puts the selected response into action and

monitors its impact. Affective variables such as anger and hostil-

ity serve as "transitory factors" that can disrupt the social

information processing sequence.

Using the social information processing heuristic, Holtz-

worth-Munroe and Hutchinson (1993) assessed the attributions

occurring during the decoding stage in MV, DNV, and SNV

men. Nine brief conflict vignettes were presented to participants,

who completed attribution questionnaires following the presen-

tation of each vignette. Results indicated that MV men were

more likely than comparison men to attribute negative intent on

the part of the wife as the primary causal agent of the conflict.

To assess the processes occurring during the decision-making

stage, Holtzworth-Munroe and Anglin (1991) had MV, DNV,

and SNV men read 22 marital conflict vignettes. When asked

what they would say or do in each situation, MV men offered

the fewest competent responses during scenes in which the wife

rejected, teased, or taunted the husband, or wanted something

from the husband. Similar findings were obtained by Dutton

and Browning (1988), who found that MV men were more

likely to choose coping responses that involved little construc-

tive reasoning, more verbal aggression, and more physical ag-

gression after imagining themselves the subject of a series of

videotapes depicting marital conflict.

Although the preceding research added valuable information

regarding the faulty attributions and deficient decision-making

skills of MV men, additional improvements are needed. With

the exception of the Holtzworth-Munroe and Anglin (1991)

study, most research in this area has utilized paper-and-pencil

cognitive assessment procedures. These measures assess partici-

pants' post hoc generalizations about how they believe they

usually think and do not provide ' 'on-line'' accessibility to more

automatic cognitive activity arising without conscious delibera-

tion or strategic filtering (Davison, Feldman, & Osborn, 1984).

Given the automaticity of emotion-relevant cognitions, it follows

that cognitions will be more accessible and reportable if they

are assessed concurrently with mood state activation (Persons &

Miranda, 1992).

Davison and associates (Davison et al., 1984; Davison et

al., 1983) developed a methodology to assess naturalistic and

representative on-line cognitive activity occurring during emo-

tion activation. The ATSS paradigm provides open-ended verbal

reporting of thoughts as they are experienced during an emo-

tional reaction. In the ATSS method, participants listen to an

audiotaped scenario and imagine that they are an active part of

the interaction. After a brief segment of the tape is played, they

are asked to articulate their thoughts for 30 seconds. The tape

then continues, followed by another pause for articulation, and

so on. Articulations are transcribed and subsequently rated along

relevant dimensions by trained coders. Research by Davison and

colleagues using nonclinical and clinical samples has supported

the validity of the ATSS procedure as an alternative to question-

naire-based cognitive assessment (for reviews of the ATSS pro-

cedure see Davison, Navarre, & Vogel, 1995, and Davison,

Vogel, & Coftman, 1997).

The present study assessed anger-related cognitive distortions,

cognitive deficiencies, and cognitive products hypothesized to

occur during the decoding and decision-making stages of Holtz-

worth-Munroe's (1992) social information processing model in

samples of MV, DNV, and SNV men. All participants initially

completed self-report measures of irrational beliefs (Ellis, 1994)

Page 3: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

ARTICULATED THOUGHTS AND MARITAL VIOLENCE 261

and cognitive biases (Beck, 1976). Participants then articulated

their thoughts during an anger-arousing ATSS procedure, and

these responses were later coded for irrational thinking, cogni-

tive biases, hostile attributional biases, and anger-controlling

statements. A number of predictions guided our data analyses.

First, we hypothesized that MV men would articulate more total

irrational beliefs, total cognitive biases, and hostile attributional

biases during ATSS anger arousal relative to both nonviolent

comparison groups. Second, we predicted that both nonviolent

comparison groups would articulate more anger-controlling

statements than MV men (suggesting a cognitive deficiency in

MV men). Third, we predicted that specific ATSS cognitive

distortions (the irrational beliefs demandingness, self/other rat-

ing, and low frustration tolerance, and the cognitive biases arbi-

trary inference, magnification, and dichotomous thinking)

would be stronger predictors of group membership than ques-

tionnaire measures of cognitive distortion. Fourth, we did not

expect a relationship to emerge between the questionnaire-as-

sessed cognitions and those articulated by MV men in the labo-

ratory due to the heightened accessibility of the latter cognitions

during anger arousal. Finally, we expected that MV men who

had engaged in severe forms of marital violence in the previous

year would be characterized by higher levels of cognitive distor-

tions and hostile attributions than MV men without episodes of

severe violence.

Method

Participants

The participants were 88 married men who responded to local newspa-

per and radio advertisements.1 The men were contacted by phone by

trained interviewers who administered a variety of screening measures

to determine group membership. All participants were paid $40.

Husbands' reports were used to determine group assignment, as recent

research has indicated that husbands' reports of wife-directed violence

are not strikingly dissimilar from the reports of their wives (see Jacobson

et al., 1994, p. 983) .2 Participants in the MV group consisted of 31 men

who reported at least four incidents of physical aggression against their

wives within the past 12 months on the Modified Conflict Tactics Scale

(MCTS; Pan, Neidig, & O'Leary, 1994a), which assesses the frequency

of psychological aggression (12 items; e.g., threatened to leave relation-

ship), mild physical aggression (5 items; e.g., pushed, grabbed, or

shoved spouse), and severe aggression (6 items; e.g., beat up spouse)

used during marital conflicts.^ The validity of the qualitative distinction

between mild and severe MCTS marital aggression is supported by data

presented by Pan, Neidig, and O'Leary (1994b). Inclusionary criteria

for the 23 DNV men were (a) a score of 95 or below on the Short

Marital Adjustment Test (SMAT; Locke & Wallace, 1959),4 a 15-item

screening measure that assesses both general marital happiness as well

as the degree to which couples agree on a variety of more specific

marital issues (e.g., finances, sex, recreation), and (b) no reported

incidents of physical aggression used against their wives at any point

in the marriage on the MCTS. Thirty-four men classified as SNV were

required to (a) score 115 or above on the SMAT, and (b) report no

incidents of physical aggression used against their wives at any point

in the marriage on the MCTS. Finally, potential participants answered

various demographic questions.

All participants were required to be between the ages of 19 and 49

and married for at least 1 year. There were no significant differences

between the groups on age, race-ethnicity, socioeconomic status, yearly

household income, length of marriage, number of children, years of

education, and frequency of weekly alcohol and drug usage. The average

participant was White, age 35, and was married for 8 years; had 14.5

years of education, two children, and a household income of $46,500;

and reported drinking 4.5 alcoholic beverages per week and using other

drugs less than once per week. We also asked participants if they were

involved in individual or marital counseling. Only I MV man and 2

DNV men indicated that they had been involved in marital counseling.

On the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976), MV and DNV

groups were well matched, and both groups' mean DAS scores were

significantly lower than SNV husbands' scores (MV: M = 86.9, SD =

20.2; DNV: M = 85.7, SD = 14.9; SNV: M = 122.0, SD = 12.2).

Materials

State Anger Scale. To provide a measure of participants' anger levels

at the time of the experiment, we administered the State Anger Scale

from the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI; Spielberger,

1988). This 10-item scale assesses the intensity of angry feelings at the

time of assessment. An alpha of .90 was reported in a large sample of

nonclinical men (Spielberger, 1988) and accumulated data point to the

scale's excellent validity (Spielberger, 1988; Spielberger, Reheiser, &

Sydeman, 1995).

Survey of Personal Beliefs. The 50-item Survey of Personal Beliefs

(SPB; Demaria, Kassinove, & Dill, 1989) was used to assess the en-

dorsement of irrational ideation. Items on the SPB assess the four hy-

pothesized core irrational beliefs of rational emotive behavior therapy

(Ellis, 1994; Ellis & Dryden, 1987) along a 6-point Likert scale. The

SPB yields subscale scores for awfulizing (e.g., "Some situations in

life are truly terrible"), low frustration tolerance (e.g., "I can't stand

some of the things that have been done by members of my family"),

demandingness (e.g., "My family should be reasonable in what they

ask of me"), self/other rating (e.g., "A person who sins or harms

others is a 'bad person' "), and a total rationality score. Higher scores

indicate greater rationality. Previous research has supported the reliabil-

ity of the SPB (Demaria et al., 1989; Kassinove, 1986). The items

make no mention of emotion, which addresses criticisms made by Smith

(1982) regarding the lack of discriminant validity used in the construc-

tion of other measures of irrational ideation that use affect-laden word-

ing. The SPB has been shown to correlate significantly with various

1 Recruitment began by placing advertisements in local newspapers

and on radio stations for married men willing to participate in a research

project about their opinions of their marriage and their spouse. Partici-

pants left their first names on a voice mail system and were later con-

tacted by trained interviewers who administered the screening measures

over the telephone.2 Nevertheless, other researchers have indicated that MV men tend to

minimize their own violence frequency. The issue at hand is the degree

of confidence that our samples are actually representative of the popula-

tion from which they are purported to be drawn. This is less of an issue

for the MV husbands, who had to indicate four incidents of violence to

qualify. The DNV group (and to a lesser extent the SNV group), how-

ever, may include men who did not report on their own marital aggres-

sion. However, if this were true, group differences between violent and

nonviolent husbands would he harder to discover.3 Analysis of the MV group's MCTS-assessed marital aggression indi-

cated that 67.7% (n = 21) engaged in only "mild" acts of physical

aggression and 32.3% (n = 10) reported at least one act of severe

violence.

* Although the widely used SMAT is adequate as a gross screening

measure of marital satisfaction (e.g., Smith, Vivian, & O'Leary, 1990),

the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976) was also adminis-

tered upon participants' arrival to the laboratory to provide a more

comprehensive assessment of marital quality and cohesion.

Page 4: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

262 ECKHARDT, HARBOUR, AND DAVTSON

measures of negative emotion and psychopathology in nonclinical and

clinical samples from the United States and abroad (Kassinove & Eck-

hardt, 1994; Muran, Kassinove, Ross, & Muran, 1989).

Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. The 40-item Dysfunctional Attitudes

Scale (DYS; Weissman & Beck, 1978) was originally designed to mea-

sure specific depressogenic cognitive biases. More recent findings have

indicated that the DYS assesses cognitive biases that are not necessarily

specific to depressive syndromes; nondepressed clinical samples also

scored higher than relevant comparison samples (Hollon, Kendall, &

Lumry, 1986). As such, the DYS represents the only available measure

of general cognitive bias. Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale. A

total cognitive bias score is computed by summing across the 40 items

with higher scores reflective of greater cognitive bias.

Procedure

ATSS. Upon arrival at the laboratory, all men signed a consent form

acknowledging that the study was investigating negative emotions in

marriage and that the actual experience of negative emotions during the

experiment was likely. Following completion of an assessment packet,

participants were introduced to the ATSS procedure via tape-recorded

instructions, which stated that they would be asked to listen to several

taped scenarios, imagine that they were actually involved in each, and

when prompted by a tone, to talk out loud about their thoughts and

feelings into a microphone connected to a hidden tape recorder. The

anonymity of participants' responses was emphasized to encourage open

and frank thought articulation. When participants indicated that they

understood the procedure following a nonarousing practice scenario, the

digital-quality ATSS scenarios were played by means of a Sony Digital

Audio Tape (DAT) player and Pioneer large cup headphones. The experi-

menter left the laboratory room until the end of the ATSS procedure.

The ATSS procedure included three stimulus scenarios (control, over-

heard conversation, and jealousy), each divided into eight 30-s segments.

The taped characters were portrayed by paid professional actors. On the

basis of pilot data by Eckhardt and Kassinove (in press), we used three

stimulus scenarios, two of which were anger-inducing.s In the control

scenario, the participant was asked to imagine that he and his wife were

invited to another couple's house, where they all played a game. Al-

though the couple was somewhat competitive and annoying, the source

of this annoyance had nothing to do with the participant's wife. Tn the

anger-arousing overheard conversation scenario, the participant imagined

that he was overhearing a conversation between his wife and her friend,

who negatively evaluated the participant and expressed uncertainty about

remaining in the marriage. This scenario was scripted around the themes

of wife abandonment and wife-to-husband ridicule, which have been

previously shown to be associated with spousal violence (e.g., Daly &

Wilson, 1988;Holtzworth-Munroe&Hutchinson, 1993).In the jealousy

scenario, the participant imagined that he had arrived home early to find

his wife talking and laughing with a male acquaintance. The participant

decided to listen to their conversation, which consisted of subtly flirta-

tious dialogue. The purpose was to elicit anger by suggesting that the

wife may have been or might become amorous with another man, capi-

talizing on the theme of male sexual jealousy (Daly, Wilson, & Weghorst,

1983).

The three stimulus tapes were presented to participants successively

on a single tape. Because it was expected that participants might initially

be uncomfortable with the procedure, the control tape was always pre-

sented first. The presentation order of the two anger-arousing tapes was

counterbalanced. Following the ATSS procedure, the State Anger Scale

was again administered.

Debriefing. Following the ATSS procedure, participants were fully

debriefed for a period of 15 to 30 min, The "process" debriefing was

designed to fully explain the rationale of the study and explore the nature

of the men's affective reactions to the procedure. All participants were

given the opportunity to engage in relaxation training, and all MV partici-

pants were given the phone numbers to local spouse abuser support

groups and mental health centers. No participant was allowed to leave

the laboratory in emotional distress, and follow-up telephone calls were

made to MV participants to assess postexperimental reactions.fi

ATSS Coding Procedures

Each participant's tape containing his articulated thoughts for the

three stimulus situations was transcribed by undergraduate research as-

sistants. Two raters (advanced undergraduates) who were not aware of

the experimental conditions received 30 hr of training according to an

ATSS coding manual7 and on basic issues regarding cognitive theories

of emotion. Following training to criterion, the two raters read transcrip-

tions of participants' articulated thoughts while listening to their articu-

lated thoughts tape and independently rated each segment on a special-

ized coding sheet. Pearson product-moment coefficients were computed

to assess inter-rater agreement.

Ratings were made in four dependent variable categories: irrational

beliefs, cognitive biases, hostile attributional biases, and anger-control

strategies. Irrational belief and cognitive bias verbalizations were rated

on a 5-point scale (0-4) that assessed the degree to which each variable

was present in each segment, with 0 indicating not present at all and 4

indicating extremely present. Scores were calculated by averaging the

two coders' ratings on each variable for each segment, and summary

scores were calculated by totaling the averaged ratings across the eight

segments of each scenario. We assessed the hostile attributional bias

and anger-control statements variables by tallying the frequencies of each

verbalization across each scenario.

Irrational beliefs. The total irrational beliefs score, a composite of

the four specific irrational beliefs, had very high intercoder reliability

(r = .92). As outlined by Ellis and Dryden (1987), four core irrational

beliefs are presumed to contribute to a general style of irrational think-

ing: (a) awfulizing (an exaggerated rating of the "badness1" of an

aversive event evidenced by words such as "awful," "horrible," "terri-

ble," or "catastrophic"); (b) low frustration tolerance (asevere intoler-

ance for discomfort evidenced by phrases such as "can't stand," "can't

take," "can't tolerate"); (c) demandingness (rigid, absolutist beliefs

that events or people must be a certain way and that conditions such as

3 Transcripts of the ATSS stimulus tapes can be obtained from Christo-

pher I. Eckhardt.6 Here is an additional note about debriefing. Because anger induction

is neither pleasant to administer nor receive, we were concerned about

participants (especially MV men) leaving our study angrier than when

they arrived. However, it should be kept in mind mat the entire duration

of anger arousal was about 14 min (half of which consisted of articula-

tion pauses). The process debriefing was structured for all participants to

assess (a) thoughts about the experiment, (b) any emotions experienced

during the ATSS procedure, (c) the intensity and subjective experience

of those emotions, (d) whether the men actually thought of their own

relationships during the imagined scenarios, and (e) whether they felt

more negative emotions at the current time than when they began the

experiment on a scale ranging from 1 (perfectly calm) to 10 (intensely

aroused). By the end of the debriefing session, 25 (81 %) of the 31 MV

participants indicated that they did not feel more negative emotions than

when they arrived. The 6 men who indicated that they did feel upset

indicated an emotionality level of 5 or below when queried. All MV

and DNV men were encouraged to seek counseling and appropriate

referrals to competent professionals were given. Finally, the follow-up

phone calls indicated that no participant felt any lingering unpleasantness

as a result of their participation.1 A copy of the ATSS coding manual can be obtained from Christopher

I. Eckhardt.

Page 5: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

ARTICULATED THOUGHTS AND MARITAL VIOLENCE 263

success and approval are absolute necessities); and (d) self/other rating

(evaluations of the total value or worth of a human being on the basis

of a specific behavior or attribute in the basic form of "He/She/They/I is/am/are a "). Interrater reliabilities ranged from .67 (awfuliz-ing) to .94 (low frustration tolerance), with a mean of .85.

Cognitive biases. The total cognitive bias score, a composite of thefollowing six automatic thoughts, had good intercoder agreement (r =

.82). The following six individual cognitive biases outlined by Beck(Beck, 1976; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) were assessed: (a)

arbitrary inference (making assumptions or drawing conclusions in theabsence of supporting evidence); (b) selective abstraction (understand-

ing an experience on the basis of a detail taken out of context, while

ignoring more salient aspects of the situation); (c) overgeneralization(constructing a general rule from one or a few isolated incidents and

applying this rule universally); (d) magnification (overestimating thesignificance of events and reacting incongruously to the presenting situa-tion); (e) personalization (the tendency to engage in self-referent think-

ing even when presented with situations having little to do with theself); and (f) dicholomous thinking (categorizing an event in one of

two extremes and thinking in all-or-none terms). Interrater agreement

ranged from .11 (selective abstraction) to .88 (magnification), with anaverage of .72. Omitting the very low reliability for selective abstractionresults in r — .84.

Hostile attributional biases. This variable was coded by tallying thenumber of statements wherein participants blamed the cause of an event

on the malicious and hostile intentions of another character. Hostileattributional bias was considered a special case of the arbitrary inference

distortion in that the individual arrives at a conclusion in the absence ofconfirming evidence (an incorrect conclusion regarding the protagonist'sintentionality). However, the hostile attributional bias involves not only

the misperception of causal intent but also the assumption of hostilemotivation (Epps & Kendall, 1995). For example, the statement "It'sall her fault!" would be classified as arbitrary inference because it is

not clear if this intentionality conclusion is valid. However, the statement

"She meant for this happen just to get back at me!" is classified ashostile attributional bias because there is a question of both intent and

hostile motivation. Interrater agreement was very high (r = .92).Anger-control statements. Finally, articulated usage of strategies for

anger control were rated by summing their frequency of occurrence.These statements included expressing the desire to walk away from the

character or escape the situation in order to calm down, attempting toactively change one's views about the situation or characters in order to

decrease negative emotionality, suggesting counseling or other externalmediation, initiating a request to calmly talk over the situation, or otheranger-control strategies. Interrater agreement was again very high(r = .94).

Results

Validity o/ATSS Anger Induction

To assess whether the three groups of men were indeed an-

gered by the ATSS procedure, we conducted three dependent

samples t tests on STAXI State Anger scores gathered before

and immediately after the ATSS procedure. Results indicated

that men in all three participant groups reported significant in-

creases in state anger after the ATSS procedure: For MV, f(30)

= 3.82, p < .001; for DNV, t(22) = 3.55, p < .002; and for

SNV, f(32) = 3.23, p < .003. The three groups did not signifi-

cantly differ in post-ATSS state anger, F(2, 84) = 2.26, ns.

Thus, all three groups reported that the ATSS tapes were anger

inducing.

Group Differences on ATSS Cognitive Variables

Table 1 presents means and standard deviations for all ATSS

cognitive variables. As post hoc analyses revealed that there

were nonsignificant within-group differences between ATSS

jealousy scenario and overheard conversation scenario scores

for any cognitive variable, the total scores of these two scenarios

were averaged into a single anger scenario score. To examine

general group differences in articulated cognitive processes,

products, and deficiencies, we first present the results of four 3

X 2 (Group X Scenario) mixed ANOVAs on the ATSS variables

of total irrational beliefs, total cognitive biases, frequency of

hostile attributional biases, and frequency of anger-control state-

ments. Effect sizes are reported in the form of eta (77). Where

significant main effects coexisted with significant interaction

terms, only significant interactions were probed. Finally, direct

discriminant function analyses were undertaken with group

membership being predicted by the four irrational beliefs, six

cognitive biases, frequency of hostile attributional biases, and

frequency of anger-control statements.

Irrational beliefs. On the total irrational beliefs score, the

ANOVA indicated significant effects of group, F(2, 77) = 7.08,

p < .002,17 = .39; scenario, F(1, 77) = 27.57, p < .001, r\ =

.51; and the Group X Scenario interaction, F(2, 77) = 4.63, p

< .013, r) = .33. To explore this interaction, we evaluated

within-group differences between the control scenario and anger

scenario using dependent t tests. In addition, we examined group

differences within each scenario using univariate ANOVAs and

Tukey's (1977) honestly significant difference (HSD) test as

a control for Type I errors resulting from multiple pairwise

comparisons. Although MV and SNV husbands articulated a

significantly greater level of articulated irrational beliefs in the

anger tape versus the control scenario—for MV, ((27) = 4.91,

p < .001; for SNV, /(30) = 4.07, p < .001—DNV men articu-

lated a similar degree of irrationality across the two scenarios,

1(20) = .66, ns. During the control scenario, the ANOVA indi-

cated a significant effect of group, F(2, 79) = 4.23, p < .02.

The HSD test indicated that MV men were not significantly

different than DNV men during the control scenario, with both

M V and DNV men articulating greater levels of irrational beliefs

than SNV men. During the anger scenario, the univariate

ANOVA indicated a significant group effect, F(2, 77) = 7.42,

p < .01, with MV husbands scoring significantly higher than

DNV and SNV men. DNV did not differ significantly from

SNV men.

Cognitive biases. On the total cognitive biases score, the

ANOVA indicated significant effects of group, F(2, 77) =

15.32, p < .001, rj = .53; scenario, F(l, 77) = 121.92, p <

.001, rj = .78; and the Group X Scenario interaction, F(2, 77)

= 7.49, p < .001. All three groups significantly increased their

level of cognitive bias articulations from the control scenario to

the anger scenario: For MV, <(27) = 7.69, p < .001; for DNV,

((20) = 3.09, p < .006; for SNV, r(30) = 9.66, p < .001.

During the control scenario, the univariate ANOVA indicated a

significant effect of group, F(2, 78) = 9.06, p < .001. The

HSD test indicated that MV men were not significantly different

from DNV men, with both MV and DNV men articulating

greater levels of cognitive bias than SNV men. During the anger

scenario, the univariate ANOVA indicated a significant group

Page 6: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

264 ECKHARDT, BARBOUR, AND DAVISON

Table 1

Means (and Standard Deviations) for ATSS Cognitive Variables

Control scenario

MV

Variable

Irrational beliefsAwfulizingLFTDemanding

RatingTotal

Cognitive biasesArbitrary inf

Selective absOvergen

MagnificationPersonalization

Dichot thinkTotal

Hostile

Ang con

M

0.03

0.435 17

2.167.79

3.12

0.031.02

5.880.463.98

14.91

0.590.07

SD

0.18

1.294.38

2.756.14

3.18

0.181.28

4.470.97

4.289.12

1.180.18

DNV

M

0.07

0.454.64

2.727.89

2.160.001.165.020.305.25

13.89

0.36

0.18

SD

0.320.944.93

3.427.15

2.45

0.001.65

6.470.594.53

11.74

0.73

0.45

SNV

M

0.03

0.053.24

0.794.1 1

1.110.000.94

1.920.21

1.795.97

0.220.18

SD

0.12

0.272.50

1.253.31

1.68

0.001.78

2.010.44

2.015.34

0.700.35

Anger scenario

MV

M

0.19

1.6710.04

3.7415.55

6.71

0.351.67

12.223.03

10.2834.26

1.140.87

SD

0.71

3.106.15

3.7610.20

3.360.561.46

7.084.124.72

13.16

0.87

1.00

DNV

M

0.030.766062.529.23

4.62

0.072.367.250.826.72

21.73

0.36

1.06

SD

0.12

1.044.15

1.834.78

3.160.273.493.081.163.708.03

0.51

1.01

SNV

M

0.14

0.986.10

1.398.60

4.230.031.915.702.376.20

20.45

0.252.64

SD

0.281.295.041.345.75

3.660.112.155.003.023.46

9.61

0.46

2.31

Note. ATSS = Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations; MV = maritally violent (n = 31); DNV =maritally distressed-nonviolent (ti = 23); SNV = maritally satisfied-nonviolent (n = 34); LFT = lowfrustration tolerance; Demanding = demandingness; Rating = self/other rating; Arbitrary inf = arbitraryinference; Selective abs = selective abstraction; Overgen = Overgeneralization; Dichot think = dichotomousthinking; Hostile = Hostile attributional bias; Ang con = Anger-control statements.

effect, F(2,78) = 14.49,p < .001, with MV husbands articulat-

ing a significantly greater degree of cognitive biases than DNV

and SNV men. DNV men did not differ significantly from

SNV men.

Hostile attributional biases. On the hostile attributional bias

variable, the ANOVA indicated a significant effect of group,

F(2, 79) = 9.52, p < .001, r, = .44. No significant effects were

found for the scenario effect, F(1, 79) = 2.56, ns, or the Group

X Scenario interaction, F(2, 79) = 2.35, ns. Across both sce-

nario conditions, MV men articulated a significantly greater

number of hostile attributional biases than DNV men, ((79) =

3.06. p < .003, and SNV men, f(79) = 4.19, p < .001. DNV

men did not differ from SNV men, ((79) = .78, ns.

Anger-control statements. For the frequency of articulated

anger-control statements variable, the ANOVA indicated a sig-

nificant effect of group, F(2, 79) = 10.36, p < .001, 77 = .46;

a significant effect of scenario, F(l, 77) = 58.15, p < .001,

17 = .65; and a significant Group X Scenario interaction, F(2,

79) = 9.79, p < .001. »j = .45. All three groups significantly

increased their level of anger-control statements from the control

scenario to the anger scenario; ForMV, ((28) = 4.24, p < .001;

for DNV, ( (21) = 3.67, p < .001; for SNV, ((30) = 6.09, p <

.001. During the control scenario, the ANOVA indicated no

effect of group, F(2, 79) = 0.96, ns. During the anger scenario,

the univariate ANOVA indicated a significant group effect, F(2,

79) = 10.50, p < .001, with SNV husbands scoring significantly

higher than MV and DNV men. MV men did not differ signifi-

cantly from DNV men.

Predicting group status from ATSS cognitive variables. Be-

cause our prior analyses clearly indicated that MV men were

characterized by excesses on aggregate cognitive variables dur-

ing anger arousal, in the present section we examine the role of

specific cognitive variables in the differentiation of participant

groups. To control for the increase in Type I error rates that

would have resulted from numerous univariate ANOVAs for

specific cognitive variables and to more directly answer the

research question, we used a direct discriminant function analy-

sis with specific ATSS anger scenario cognitive variables to

predict group status. In accordance with recommendations by

Pedhazur (1982) and Mueller and Cozad (1993), interpretation

of the functions will be made primarily using structure coeffi-

cients (pooled within-group correlations between predictors and

functions) rather than standardized discriminant function coef-

ficients, although both types of data are presented in Table 2.

The first of the two obtained functions explained 59% of the

variance in group membership, ^;2(22) = 63.76, p < .0001,

and the remaining function explained 15% of the variance in

group membership, x2(10) = 11.65, ns. Using predictor-func-

tion correlations above .30 as the guideline for determining

significance (Pedhazur, 1982), examination of correlations be-

tween the two functions and the set of cognitive predictor vari-

ables indicated that hostile attributions, magnification, dichoto-

mous thinking, self/other rating, arbitrary inference, and de-

mandingness were significantly correlated with Function 1,

whereas anger-control statements and personalization loaded sig-

nificantly on Function 2. Anger control statements were nega-

tively correlated with Function 1. Analysis of group centroids

indicates that high scores on Function 1 were characteristic of

MV participants (Function 1 centroid = 1.32; Function 2

centroid = .17), whereas higher scores on Function 2 were

Page 7: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

ARTICULATED THOUGHTS AND MARITAL VIOLENCE 265

Table 2

Results of Discriminant Function Analysis Using ATSS Cognitive

Variables to Predict Group Membership

Correlations with discriminantfunctions

Variable Function 1 Function 2

Standardized discriminantfunction coefficients

Function 1

Function Eigenvalue Canonical R Wilks's A

Function 2

Hostile attributionsMagnificationDichotomous thinkingSelf/other ratingArbitrary inferenceDemandingnessAnger-control statementsPersonalizationAwfulizingOvergeneralizationLow frustration tolerance

.65

.51

.47J9.36.34

-.42

.14

.07-.05

.13

.38

.11

.17-.08

.26

.27

.76

.60.27

-.23

.20

.93

.18

.41

.13-.44

-.21-.61

.37-.25-.33

-.16

.06-.00

.05

.02

.21

.15

.70

.38

.12-.09

.27

<tf12

1.060.18

.72

.39.41.85

63.7611.65

2210

.001.31

Note. Significant loadings in boldface type. ATSS = Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations.

more characteristic of SNV participants (Function 1 centroid =

— 1.01; Function 2 centroid = .31). DNV participants' group

centroid was low on both Function 1 (centroid = —.26) and

Function 2 (centroid = —.68). Overall classification results

indicate that 70% of cases were correctly classified according

to the two discriminant functions.

Self-Report Measures of Cognitive Distortion

Between-group one-way ANOVAs on the total scores of the

SPB and DYS were examined. Results indicated a significant

group effect on Total SPB score, F(2, 71) = 4.04, p < .02,

with MV men endorsing greater irrationality than SNV men,

((71) = -2.68, p < .009, but not DNV men, r(71) = -.47,

ns. DNV men endorsed greater irrationality than SNV men,

f(71) = -2.03, p < .05. On the total score of the DYS, no

significant group effects were observed, F( 2, 79) = 2.72, ns.

To assess the differential ability of the questionnaire measures

of cognition to predict the group status of individual cases,

the SPB subtests of awfulizing, demandingness, low frustration

tolerance, and self/other rating, and the DYS total score were

entered into a discriminant function analysis. The analysis pro-

duced two nonsignificant functions, with Function 1 explaining

19.2% of the variance in group membership x2(10) = 14.24,

p < .16, and Function 2 explaining 2.7% of the variance in group

membership, x2(4) = 1.86, p < .76. As might be expected from

the lack of significance among the two functions, only 48.61%

of cases were accurately classified and no additional analyses

were undertaken.

Correlations Between ATSS and Questionnaire

Cognitive Variables

This lack of convergence led us to evaluate the degree of

correlation between the cognitions articulated by MV men dur-

ing the ATSS procedure and those assessed by the question-

naires. No significant correlations were obtained (range =

.01 -.26). However, because participants experienced anger dur-

ing the ATSS anger scenario but not during the completion of

the questionnaires, we also evaluated correlations between ATSS

control scenario articulations (no anger arousal) and question-

naire measures of cognitions within the MV sample. Once again,

no significant correlations were obtained. The overall pattern

of nonsignificance remained even when the three groups were

collapsed into one, although one significant correlation emerged

between ATSS anger scenario total irrational beliefs and SPB

total irrationality (r = -33, p < .01).

Differentiating Mildly Versus Severely Aggressive Men

To evaluate the ability of ATSS cognitive data to discriminate

between MV men who engaged in "mild" acts of physical

aggression and those who reported engaging in at least one act

of severe wife-directed violence, a discriminant function was

calculated using all ATSS cognitive variables as predictors. The

resulting function, although nonsignificant, xz(12) = 17.32,.

p < .14, explained 58% of the variance in the prediction of

mild versus severe aggression. Because the eigenvalue, canoni-

cal correlation, and amount of explained variance were of large

magnitude, the function was deemed interpretable. As can be

seen in Table 3, the ATSS anger scenario articulations of

demandingness, magnification, and awfulizing were signifi-

cantly correlated with the function. Frequency of anger-control

statements approached significance. Analysis of group centroids

indicated that high scores on the function were associated with

being classified a severe batterer (severely aggressive = 1.79;

mildly aggressive = —.72). Overall, the function correctly clas-

sified 92.9% of MV participants.

Using cognitive data gathered from the SPB and DYS, we

Page 8: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

266 ECKHARDT, HARBOUR, AND DAVISON

Table 3

Results of Discriminant Function Analysis Using ATSS Cognitive Variables

to Predict Severe Versus Mild Aggression

Predictor

DemandingnessMagnificationAwfulizing

Anger-control statementsSelective abstraction

OvergeneralizationSelf/other ratingArbitrary inferenceDichotomous thinkingHostile attributionsLow frustration tolerancePersonalization

Function Eigenvalue

1 1.38

Correlations withdiscriminant functions

.44

.40JO

-.29.24

-.21.14.10.04.02.01.00

Canonical R Wilks's \

.76 .42

Standard discriminantfunction coefficients

1.48.44

-.04

-.29.43

-.51-.22-.30

.94-.38

.22

.58

X2 df p

17.32 12 .14

Note. Significant loadings in boldface type. ATSS = Articulated Thoughts in Simulated Situations.

evaluated the ability of self-reported cognitive data to differenti-

ate between mildly and severely aggressive men. The single

nonsignificant function obtained from the questionnaire data

explained only 15.3% of the variance in the prediction of mild

versus severe violence, X 2 (5) = 3.25, p < 66. Because of the

small amount of variance explained by this function, no further

interpretation of these data will be made.

Discussion

The data indicated that articulated cognitive distortions, cog-

nitive deficiencies, and cognitive products differentiated MV

husbands from their nonviolent counterparts during anger induc-

tion. During anger arousal, aggregate irrational beliefs and cog-

nitive biases were articulated to a greater degree by MV men

than DNV and SNV men, as cognitive theorists (Beck, 1976;

Ellis, 1977) would predict. Additionally, discriminant function

analyses indicated that specific ATSS irrational beliefs and cog-

nitive biases differentiated (a) between MV men and their non-

violent counterparts, and (b) between severely violent and

mildly aggressive husbands. MV men also articulated more hos-

tile attributional biases, but fewer anger-controlling statements.

Although few notable differences in cognitive processing were

present between MV and DNV men in the absence of anger

arousal, MV men clearly differentiated themselves from the

other groups during anger-arousing scenarios. This points to a

substantial strength of the ATSS assessment method in that it is

designed for the measurement of cognition while respondents

are experiencing affective arousal. Because we controlled for

the effects of marital discord on aggression by including a com-

parison sample of DNV men, we are able to conclude that these

cognitive distortions are associated with the presence of violence

in the participants' relationships rather than levels of marital

discord.

How might cognitive distortions contribute to marital vio-

lence? A social information processing framework (Holtzworth-

Munroe, 1992) posits that MV men may selectively attend to,

actively misconstrue, or otherwise distort situations in ways that

increase the likelihood of marital anger and aggression. MV

participants in the present study, for example, were character-

ized by verbalizations reflective of a tendency to magnify the

importance of aversive situations, dichotomous thinking that

establishes rigid all-or-none ground rules for acceptable and

unacceptable behavior, inflammatory ratings of other individu-

als' worth, articulations characterized by absolutist demands

that people act appropriately, and arbitrary inferences estab-

lished in the absence of confirming evidence. In addition, we

found that specific cognitive distortion articulations distin-

guished between violent husbands who had engaged in more

severe forms of marital violence and those husbands who used

more mild forms of aggression. The 32% of our MV sample

who reported at least one act of severe violence were discrimi-

nated on the basis of high levels of ATSS demandingness, magni-

fication, and awfulizing, and low levels of anger-control state-

ments. If these cognitive distortions are assumed to be trait-

like, automatic processes that are applied in a wide variety of

potentially provocative situations, it follows that MV men will

experience anger arousal and perceive threat provocation at a

higher frequency than individuals who do not exhibit this pattern

of information processing.

Social information processing models also predict that defi-

ciencies in making effective and appropriate decisions can also

contribute to aggressive outcomes (Holtzworth-Munroe, 1992;

Holtzworth-Munroe & Anglin, 1991). MV men in the present

research did not spontaneously produce thought articulations

that function to de-escalate and control anger arousal. SNV

husbands articulated anger-controlling cognitions at a signifi-

cantly higher rate than MV and DNV men, which corresponds

to similar findings by Dutton and Browning (1988) and Holtz-

worth-Munroe and Anglin (1991). As reported by numerous

marital interaction researchers (e.g., Burman et al., 1993), MV

Page 9: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

ARTICULATED THOUGHTS AND MARITAL VIOLENCE 267

couples appear to be entrenched in a communication style during

marital conflicts that involves an escalating cascade of recipro-

cated anger, contempt, and belligerence. The present data sug-

gest that, in part, MV men are unable to extricate themselves

from this aversive communication pattern because of their in-

ability to spontaneously generate anger-controlling, prosocial

coping strategies. There are important clinical implications asso-

ciated with this finding. Whereas cognitively oriented therapies

are generally structured around the goal of identifying and modi-

fying distortions in information processing that affect emotions

and behavior, merely substituting rational beliefs for their irra-

tional counterparts, or objectively valid thoughts in place of

faulty automatic inferences, does not address the individual's

decision-making skill deficiencies in the area of anger coping

and conflict management. Skill-building techniques such as so-

cial skills training, problem-solving therapy, or interventions that

teach couples how to argue "better" (Gottman, 1994) may thus

be important treatment adjuncts for MV men.

Do the ATSS data support the primacy of cognition in the

enactment of marital aggression? Perhaps, but our data are cor-

relational and not without their limitations. First, it is still un-

known whether MV men possessed these cognitive processes

and products before engaging in violence against their wives,

or whether they emerged as a result of the violence. Second,

the present assessment scheme coded for the presence of hostile

attributional biases regardless of the individual they were di-

rected toward, which stands in contrast to previous studies of

MV men (e.g., Holtzworth-Munroe & Hutchinson, 1993) that

only assessed wife-directed attributional biases. In addition, al-

though the ATSS anger-arousing scenarios depicted conflict situ-

ations of direct relevance to the marital relationship, they did

not involve an actual husband-wife argument per se. Future

studies might examine the utility of including argument-based

scenarios. Finally, although the ATSS procedure provides an

optimal method for assessing affect-relevant cognitions as they

are occurring on-line, there is still an opportunity for respon-

dents to withhold or otherwise distort their responses. However,

the fact that respondents do not verbalize everything that is on

their minds during ATSS assessment does not compromise data

that are coded schematically (Kashima & Davison, 1990), as

they were in the present study. Indeed, the relatively large effect

sizes reported in the present research suggest that MV men were

minimally affected by social desirability and censoring issues.

Thus, given these limitations, the present data suggest that cogni-

tive variables may strongly influence affective arousal and play

a key role in the complex interaction of events that unfold during

relationship conflicts.

Because cognitions are so heavily connected to intense af-

fective states such as anger, it is critical that future researchers

continue to tailor cognitive assessment methods to address this

cognition-emotion association. A strength of the ATSS proce-

dure is its ability to tap into on-line, affect-relevant cognitive

processes that may not be accessible in the absence of affective

arousal. One may hypothesize that for participants in the present

study, the elicitation of anger during the ATSS procedure acti-

vated anger-relevant cognitive processes. In accordance with

a spreading activation perspective on cognition and emotion

(Anderson & Bower; 1973; Bower, 1981; Collins & Loftus,

1975), MV men are hypothesized to possess more associative

connections between cognitive operations (e.g., cognitive distor-

tions, memories, images) and angry affect that are activated and

articulated during think-aloud tasks such as the ATSS procedure.

This theoretical position, which Persons and Miranda (1992)

have recently modified into the mood-state hypothesis, may ex-

plain the lack of relationship found in the present study between

the questionnaire measures of cognitive distortion and ATSS

cognitive data. The SPB and DYS possess strong psychometric

properties but were nevertheless poor predictors of group mem-

bership and severity of marital violence in the discriminant func-

tion analyses. According to the mood-state hypothesis, one

might hypothesize that cognitions were more accessible, and

thus more reportable, during the ATSS procedure because the

individual's emotional state was manipulated at the time of as-

sessment. However, it should be kept in mind that anger induc-

tion data in the present study are limited because anger was

only assessed before and immediately after the ATSS tapes were

presented. Future studies should more precisely assess the pro-

cess of anger arousal in MV men and at what point this arousal

influences cognition.

The present data support a social information processing

model of marital aggression (Holtzworth-Munroe, 1992). MV

men were differentiated from DNV and SNV husbands on the

basis of articulated cognitive processes and products during

anger arousal, and these cognitions further distinguished mildly

aggressive from severely aggressive husbands. Future research

should address whether additional patterns of cognitive activity

distinguish between different subtypes of MV men (Holtzworth-

Munroe & Stuart, 1994b). For example, are batterers who pre-

sent with a psychopathic pattern of emotional (under) arousal

and behavior (generally aggressive/antisocial subtype) charac-

terized by a different set of distorted cognitive processes than

batterers who are aggressive while intensely angry and enraged

(dysphoric/borderline subtype)? In addition, researchers could

examine whether information processing mechanisms operating

at a preattentive level differentiate violent from nonviolent hus-

bands. Do MV men characterized by high levels of anger bias

their attention during preliminary information processing to-

ward anger-relevant cues at the expense of other contextual cues

that would otherwise not induce anger? It appears crucial to

continue to integrate findings from diverse theoretical perspec-

tives and experimental methodologies in order to arrive at a

comprehensive model of marital violence to aid in the treatment

of abusive men and the prevention of the suffering experienced

by their victims.

References

Anderson, J., & Bower, G. (1973). Human associative memory. Wash-

ington DC: Winston.

Arias, I., & O'Leary, K. D. (1988). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of

physical aggression in marriage. In N. Epstein, S. E. Schlesinger, &

W. Dryden (Eds.), Cognitive-behavioral therapy with families (pp.

118-150). New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical

aspects. New "fork: Harper & Row.

Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders.

New "Vbrk: International Universities Press.

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. E, & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive

therapy of depression. New-York: Guilford Press.

Page 10: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

268 ECKHARDT, BARBOUR, AND DAVISON

Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36,

129-148.

Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R. H., Fincham, F. D., & Nelson, G. M.

(1996). Attributions and behavior in functional and dysfunctional

marriages. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 569-

576.

Burman, B., Margolin, G., & John, R. S. (1993). America's angriest

home videos: Behavioral contingencies observed in home reen-

actments of marital conflict. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-

chology, 61, 28-39.

Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory

of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.

Grossman, R., Stith, S., & Bender, M. (1990). Sex role egalitarianism

and marital violence. Sex Roles, 22, 293-303.

Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New %rk: Aldine de Gruyter.

Daly, M., Wilson, M., & Weghorst, S. J. (1983). Male sexual jealousy.

Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 11-27.

Davison, G. C, Feldman, P. M., & Osbom, C. E. (1984). Articulated

thoughts, irrational beliefs, and fear of negative evaluation. Cognitive

Therapy and Research, 8, 349-362.

Davison, G. C., Navarre, S. G., & Vogel, R. S. (1995). The articulated

thoughts in simulated situations paradigm: A think-aloud approach to

cognitive assessment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4,

29-33.

Davison, G. C., Robins, C., & Johnson, M. K. (1983). Articulated

thoughts during simulated situations: A paradigm for studying cogni-

tion in emotion and behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7,

17-40.

Davison, G. C., Vogel, R. S., & Coffman, S. G. (1997). Think-aloud

approaches to cognitive assessment and the Articulated Thoughts in

Simulated Situations paradigm. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 65, 950-958.

Demaria, T., Kassinove, H., & Dill, C. ( 1989). Psychometric properties

of the Survey of Personal Beliefs: A rational-emotive measure of

irrational thinking. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 329-341.

Dutton, D. G., & Browning, J. J. (1988). Concern for power, fear of

intimacy and wife abuse. In G. T. Hotaling, D. Finkelhor, J. T. Kirkpat-

rick, & M. Straus (Eds.), New directions in family violence research

(pp. 163-175). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Eckhardt, C. I., Barbour, K. A., & Stuart, G. L. (1997). Anger and hostil-

ity in maritally violent men: Conceptual distinctions, measurement

issues, and literature review. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 333-

358.

Eckhardt, C. I., & Kassinove, H. (in press). Articulated cognitive distor-

tions and cognitive deficiencies in maritally violent men. Journal of

Cognitive Psychotherapy.

Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New "York:

Citadel Press.

Ellis, A. (1977). Anger: How to live with and without it. New York:

Citadel Press.

Ellis, A, (1994J. Reason and emotion in psychotherapy (2nd ed.). New

York: Citadel Press.

Ellis, A., & Dryden, W. (1987). The practice of rational-emotive ther-

apy. New York: Springer.

Epps, J., & Kendall, P. C. (1995). Hostile attributional bias in adults.

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 159-178.

Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hogg, J. A., & Deffenbacher, J. L. (1986). Irrational beliefs, depression,

and anger among college students. Journal of College Student Person-

nel, 27, 349-353.

Hollon. S. D., Kendall, P. C., & Lumry, A. (1986). Specificity of de-

pressogenic cognitions in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 95, 52-59.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A. (1992). Social skill deficits in maritally violent

men: Interpreting the data using a social information processing

model. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 605-618.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Anglin, K. (1991). The competency of re-

sponses given by maritally violent men versus nonviolent men lo

problematic situations. Violence and Victims, 6, 257-268.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Bates, L., Smutzler, N., & Sandin, E. (1997).

A brief review of the research on husband violence. Part I: Maritally

violent versus nonviolent men. Aggression and Violent Behavior: A

Review Journal, 2, 65-99.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Hutchinson, G. (1993). Attributing negative

intent to wife behavior: The attributions of maritally violent versus

nonviolent men. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 102, 206-211.

Holtzworlh-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (I994a). An introduction to

marital violence treatment. In L. Vandecreek (Ed.), Innovations in

clinical practice: A source book (Vol. 13, pp. 5-14). Sarasota, FL:

Professional Resource Press.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Stuart, G. L. (1994b). Typologies of male

batlerers: Three subtypes and the differences among them. Psychologi-

cal Bulletin, 116, 476-497.

Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Waltz, J., Jacobson, N. S., Monaco, V,, Fehren-

bach, P. A., & Gottman, J. M. (1992). Recruiting nonviolent men as

control subjects for research on marital violence: How easily can it

be done? Violence and Victims, 7, 79-88.

Ingram, R. E., Kendall, P. C., Smith, T. W, Donnell, C., & Ronan, K.

(1987). Cognitive specificity in emotional distress. Journal of Per-

sonality and Social Psychology, 53, 734-742.

Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., Waltz, J., Rushe, R., Babcock, J., &

Holtzworth-Munroe, A. (1994). Affect, verbal content, and psycho-

physiology in the arguments of couples with a violent husband. Jour-

nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 982-988.

Kashima, K., & Davison, G. C. (1990). Functional consistency in the

face of topographical change in articulated thoughts. Journal of Ratio-

nal Emotive & Cognitive Behavioral Therapy, 7, 131-139.

Kassinove, H. (1986). Self-reported affect and core irrational thinking:

A preliminary analysis. Journal of Rational Emotive Therapy, 4, 119-

130.

Kassinove, H., & Eckhardt, C.I. (1994). Irrational beliefs and self-

reported affect in Russia and America. Personality and Individual

Differences, 16. 133-142.

Kendall, P. C. (1992). Healthy thinking. Behavior Therapy, 23, 1-12.

Kendall, P. C., & Dobson, K. S. (1993). On the nature of cognition and

its role in psychopathology. In P. C. Kendall & K. S. Dobson (Eds.),

Psychopathology and cognition (pp. 3-19). San Diego: Academic

Press.

Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital adjustment and

prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family

Living, 21, 251-255.

Margolin, G., John, R. S., & Gleberman, L. (1988). Affective responses

to conflictual discussions in violent and nonviolent couples. Journal

of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 24-33.

Mueller, R. O., & Cozad, J. B. (1993). Standardized discriminant coef-

ficients: A rejoinder. Journal of Educational Statistics, 18, 108-114.

Muran, J. C., Kassinove, H., Ross, S., & Muran, E. (1989). Irrational

thinking and negative emotionality in college students and applicants

for mental health services. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 188-

193.

Neidig, P. H, Friedman, D. H., & Collins, B. S. (1986). Attitudinal

characteristics of males who have engaged in spouse abuse. Journal

of Family Violence, I, 223-233.

O'Leary, K. D., & Vivian, D. (1990). Physical aggression in marriage.

In F. D. Fincham & T. N. Bradbury (Eds.), The psychology of mar-

riage (pp. 323-348). New York: Guilford Press.

Pan, H., Neidig. P., & O'Leary, K. D. (1994a). Male-female and ag-

gressor-victim differences in the factor structure of the Modified

Page 11: Articulated Thoughts of Maritally Violent and Nonviolent Men During Anger Arousal

ARTICULATED THOUGHTS AND MARITAL VIOLENCE 269

Conflict Tactics Scale. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 366-

382.

Pan, K, Neidig, P., & O'Leary, K. D. (1994b). Predicting mild and

severe husband-to-wife physical aggression. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 62, 975-981.

Pedhazur, E. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research. New

York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Persons,!. B., & Miranda, J. (1992). Cognitive theories of vulnerability

to depression: Reconciling negative evidence. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 16, 485-502.

Rosenbaum, A., & O'Leary, K. D. (1981). Marital violence: Character-

istics of abusive couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-

ogy, 49, 63-71.

Saunders, D. G., Lynch, A. B., Grayson, M., & Linz, D. (1987). The

inventory of beliefs about wife beating: The construction and initial

validation of a measure of beliefs and attitudes. Violence and Victims,

2, 39-57.

Smith, D. A., Vivian, D., & O'Leary, K. D. (1990). Longitudinal predic-

tion of marital discord from premarital expressions of effect. Journal

of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 790-798.

Smith, T. W. (1982). Irrational beliefs in the cause and treatment of

emotional distress: A critical review of the rational-emotive model.

Clinical Psychology Review, 2, 505-522.

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for

assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Mar-

riage and the Family, 38, 15-28.

Spielberger, C. D. (1988). Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression

Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Spielberger, C. D., Reheiser, E. C., & Sydeman, S. J. (1995). Measuring

the experience, expression, and control of anger. In H. Kassinove

(Ed.), Anger disorders: Definition, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 49-

67). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.

Stith, S. M., & Farley, S. C. (1993). A predictive model of male spousal

violence. Journal of Family Violence, 8, 183-201.

Tukey, J. W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-

Wesley.

Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation

of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale. Paper presented at the annual

convention of the American Educational Research Association, To-

ronto, Ontario, Canada.

Received October 21, 1996

Revision received March 14, 1997

Accepted August 15, 1997 •

Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates

Keeping you up-to-date. All APA Fellows, Members, Associates, and Student Affiliatesreceive—as part of their annual dues—subscriptions to the American Psychologist andAPA Monitor. High School Teacher and International Affiliates receive subscriptions tothe APA Monitor, and they may subscribe to the American Psychologist at a significantlyreduced rate. In addition, all Members and Student Affiliates are eligible for savings of upto 60% (plus a journal credit) on all other APA journals, as well as significant discounts onsubscriptions from cooperating societies and publishers (e.g., the American Association forCounseling and Development, Academic Press, and Human Sciences Press).

Essential resources. APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases ofAPA books, including the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association,and on dozens of new topical books each year.

Other benefits of membership. Membership in APA also provides eligibility forcompetitive insurance plans, continuing education programs, reduced APA convention fees,

and specialty divisions.

More information. Write to American Psychological Association, Membership Services,750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242.