240
Clemson University TigerPrints All Dissertations Dissertations 5-2018 Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning by Promoting In-home Play: A Reason to Squeeze in Some Play Andrea Miller Emerson Clemson University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: hps://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_dissertations is Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations at TigerPrints. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Dissertations by an authorized administrator of TigerPrints. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Emerson, Andrea Miller, "Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning by Promoting In-home Play: A Reason to Squeeze in Some Play" (2018). All Dissertations. 2152. hps://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_dissertations/2152

Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

Clemson UniversityTigerPrints

All Dissertations Dissertations

5-2018

Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches toLearning by Promoting In-home Play: A Reason toSqueeze in Some PlayAndrea Miller EmersonClemson University, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_dissertations

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations at TigerPrints. It has been accepted for inclusion in All Dissertations byan authorized administrator of TigerPrints. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationEmerson, Andrea Miller, "Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning by Promoting In-home Play: A Reason toSqueeze in Some Play" (2018). All Dissertations. 2152.https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_dissertations/2152

Page 2: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

CULTIVATING HEAD START CHILDREN’S APPROACHES TO LEARNING BY PROMOTING IN-HOME PLAY: A REASON TO SQUEEZE IN SOME PLAY

_____________________________________________________________

A Dissertation Presented to

the Graduate School of Clemson University

_____________________________________________________________

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Philosophy Curriculum and Instruction

_____________________________________________________________

by Andrea Miller Emerson

May 2018_____________________________________________________________

Accepted by: Dr. Sandra M. Linder, Committee Chair

Dr. David Reinking Dr. Jennifer Bailey Bisson

Dr. Barbara Bradley

Page 3: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

ii

ABSTRACT

This dissertation study addresses the need for innovative home-based

interventions supporting parent-child play through feasible, culturally responsive, and

engaging interventions for families in poverty with preschool aged children. This study

utilized an embedded mixed methods design to understand the family feasibility and

influence of the Play Pack Intervention. The Play Pack intervention aimed to support the

in-home parent-child play of Head Start families in the Upstate of South Carolina and

develop assertions that further the understanding and application of the present study’s

intervention. In this embedded mixed methods study, the Project Play Pack intervention

was implemented in which delivery of six Approaches to Learning themed take-home

play bags over six weeks was intended to support parent-child in-home play and

children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors. The goal of the study of the Project Play

Pack intervention was to further the understanding of families’ experiences with take-

home bag intervention models and explore Approaches to Learning among parent-child

play. Overall, there was qualitative evidence that this intervention was feasible for

families. Their positive descriptions of the intervention experience characterize the ways

the intervention supported more and new in-home parent-child play. Moreover, this

study provides new results regarding the occurrences of Approaches to Learning

behaviors of Head Start children within parent-child play interactions. In addition, the

quantitative results provide complementary support to assertions regarding family

feasibility and children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play.

Findings from the final mixing phase of the study revealed new insights into Head Start

Page 4: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

iii

families’ experiences of parent engagement interventions and the utility of parent-child

play for preschool children’s Approaches to Learning.

Page 5: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

iv

DEDICATION

I dedicate this dissertation to Bill, Bob, & the fellowship.

Page 6: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

v

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I want to thank all of my committee members for sharing time and expertise with me.

Dr. Linder, thank you for your tireless support and guidance throughout the last four

years. Your dynamic mentorship was transformative for my identity as a scholar and

early childhood advocate. I am extremely grateful for all of your help and

encouragement.

Dr. Reinking, thank you for all of the provocations and reassurances throughout my

dissertation process. I owe a debt of gratitude to you for your contributions to my

writing, arguing, and problem solving skills.

Dr. Bradley, thank you for your time and support of my dissertation work and my Italian

education. You have been a model for my growth as a teacher educator and a scholar at

home and abroad.

Dr. Bailey Bisson, thank you for your careful consideration and consistent support. Your

steady attention and collaborative spirit have been a quiet inspiration to me.

I would also like to thank all of my research participants and the Head Start teachers and

staff who supported this work with their time, attention, and trust.

I must acknowledge the power of the support I received from my family, friends, and the

fellowship throughout this process. This achievement is a direct result of the love and

support that has been so freely given to me. I am forever grateful.

Finally, thank you to my husband. Thank you for your patience, love, and belief in me.

It is such a pleasure to realize dreams with you. We did it!

Page 7: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS Page

TITLE PAGE ....................................................................................................................... i

ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................................ ii

DEDICATION ................................................................................................................... iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................v

LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix

LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................... xi

CHAPTER

I. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................1

Dangers of Less Play .......................................................................2 Benefits Lost ....................................................................................4 Preschool Focus on Approaches to Learning...................................5 Approaches to Learning and Play ....................................................6 Compounding Threats for Head Start Children ...............................8 Research Questions ..........................................................................9 Definition of Terms........................................................................10 Theoretical Framework ..................................................................14 Conclusion .....................................................................................16

II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ....................................................................18

Parent Involvement and School Success ........................................18 Achievement and Approaches to Learning ....................................41 Approaches to Learning and Self-Regulation ................................46 Facilitating Approaches to Learning Themed Play .......................50

III. METHODOLOGY ....................................................................................56

Rational for Using Mixed Methods Research ................................56 Mixed Methods Study Design .......................................................60 Intervention ....................................................................................66 Data Collection ..............................................................................79 Data Analysis .................................................................................88 Mixing ..........................................................................................101

Page 8: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

vii

Table of Contents (continued) Page

Legitimation .................................................................................103 Conclusion ...................................................................................105

IV. RESEARCH QUESTION 1 RESULTS ..................................................106

Introduction ..................................................................................106 Research Question 1: Feasibility .................................................108 Research Question 1: Conclusion ................................................123

V. RESEARCH QUESTION 2 RESULTS ..................................................128

Introduction ..................................................................................128 Research Question 2: Influence ...................................................128 Play Frequency.............................................................................129 Parent Play-Involvement .............................................................132 Approaches to Learning ...............................................................138 Research Question 2 Conclusion .................................................144

VI. DISCUSSION ..........................................................................................149

Introduction ..................................................................................149 Feasibility Summary ....................................................................151 Influence Summary ......................................................................158 Significance..................................................................................169 Limitations and Attempts to Control For Challenges ..................171 Future Research ...........................................................................173

APPENDICES .................................................................................................................176

A: Play Pack Tip Sheet .................................................................................176 B: Feedback Sheet ........................................................................................177 C: Semi-Structured Interview Protocol ........................................................178 D: Play Bag Preference Ranking Task ..........................................................179 E: In-Home Play Frequency Form ...............................................................180 F: Parent Play-Involvement Coding Sheet ...................................................181 G: PALBO Coding Sheet ..............................................................................182 H: Sample of PALBO Phase Approaches to Learning

Behavior Categorization ......................................................................184 I: Feedback Form Emergent Theme Development .....................................190 J: Post Interview Emergent Coding Example ..............................................195 K: Post Interview Emergent Coding Scheme ...............................................196

Page 9: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

viii

REFERENCES ................................................................................................................198

Table of Contents (continued) Page

Page 10: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

ix

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

2.1 HSELOF Approaches to Learning Sub-Domains ..................................................47

3.1 Project Play Pack Objectives and Data Collection Strategies ...............................61

3.2 Head Start Center Demographics..........................................................................62

3.3 Demographic Characteristics of Head Start Children ............................................64

3.4 Demographic Characteristics of Head Start Parents .............................................65

3.5 Triadic Strategies for Implementing Project Play Pack .........................................68

3.6 Project Play Pack Themes and Connections to Head Start Standards ...................71

3.7 Project Play Pack Bag Contents .............................................................................73

3.8 Selecting Children’s Literature for Project Play Pack Bags ..................................74

3.9 Selecting Manipulatives for Project Play Pack Bags .............................................77

3.10 Initial Open Codes and Second Grouping of Approaches to Learning Behaviors 95

3.11 Operational Definitions of Preschoolers’ Approaches to

Learning in Play Labels .............................................................................98

3.12 Behavioral Indicators of the PALBO Coding Categories ......................................99

4.1 Head Start Family Participation ...........................................................................107

4.2 Weekly Feedback Forms Theme Variation .........................................................110

4.3 Occurrences of Parent Examples of In-Home Play

Involvement With Play Bags ...................................................................119

5.1 Mean Percentages of Parental Involvement in Parent-child Play

Interactions, Pre and Post .........................................................................133

Page 11: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

x

List of Tables (Continued)

Table Page

5.2 PPI Pre, Post, and Learning Center Selection Correlations .................................134

5.3 Demographics and PPI Difference Scores Correlations ......................................135

5.4 Case by Case of Parental Involvement in Parent-child Play Interactions ............136

5.5 Mean Percentages of Children’s Approaches to

Learning Behaviors, Pre and Post ............................................................141

5.6 PALBO Pre, Post, and Learning Center Selection Correlations ..........................142

5.7 Case by Case Percentages of PALBO .................................................................144

Page 12: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

xi

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

3.1 Project Play Pack Embedded Mixed Methods Design ..........................................59

3.2 Data Collection Schedule .......................................................................................80

3.3 QUAL quan Depiction of Data Sources ..............................................................102

4.1 Play Pack Ranking Task Result ...........................................................................112

4.2 Play Pack Preference Rankings ...........................................................................113

6.1 Complementary Triangulation Model: Feasibility...............................................150

6.2 Complementary Triangulation Model: Influence ...............................................150

Page 13: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

1

CHAPTER ONE

INTRODUCTION

The decline of in-home play is problematic for preschool children and their

parents, resulting in a breakdown in children’s social emotional development (Ginsburg,

2007; Singer, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2006; Singer, Singer, D'Agnostino, & DeLong,

2009). Play is defined as intrinsically motivated, emotionally meaningful, enjoyable

actions including motor play, pretend play, construction play, or game play (Trawick-

Smith, 2012). Children’s play is a primary support for the development of their social,

cognitive, and language competence in early childhood (Bergen, 2002; Fowler, 2018;

Nichols & Stich, 2000; Vygotsky, 1978).

The “play-learning” belief represents the shared notion among a number of

different researchers and national organizations focused on the wellbeing and education

of young children that many forms of play serve as a natural, developmentally

appropriate means for children to explore and learn (Fisher, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, &

Gryfe, 2008; International Play Association, 2014; National Association for the

Education of Young Children (NAEYC), 2009; Sandall, Hemmeter, Smith, & McLean,

2005; Singer et al., 2006; Singer & Singer, 2005). As children play with peers they

develop important dispositions towards learning or Approaches to Learning, including

the skills and behaviors used to engage in learning (Coolahan, Fantuzzo, Mendez, &

McDermott, 2000). Play is simultaneously enjoyable and valuable for learning new

skills, specifically learning social collaboration skills (Coolahan et al., 2000; Fantuzzo,

Coolahan, Mendez, & McDermott, Sutton-Smith, 1998; Guralnick, 1993; Vygotsky,

Page 14: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

2

1976). Skill development occurs within peer play interactions, as children are naturally

exposed to concepts such as wait time, turn taking, persistence when facing a challenge,

and creative expression. However, these Approaches to Learning behaviors are

understudied within parent-child play interactions (Fantuzzo & McWayne, 2002).

We know that parent involvement in parent-child play interactions supports the

social, cognitive, and language development of young children (Bennet, Wood, &

Rodgers, 1997; Carson & Parke, 1996; Hart & Risley, 1995; Smilansky & Shefatya,

1990; Thorp, Stahmer, & Schreibman, 1995). Parent play-involvement also benefits

parents, by promoting parental self-efficacy (Boyce et al., 2017; Hoover-Dempsey et al,

2005; Hoover-Dempsey & Sandler, 1997; Jones & Prinz, 2005). The following sections

introduces the literature regarding play as a resource for young children, play and

Approaches to Learning, specifically Approaches to Learning within parent-child play

interactions, and effective parent-child engagement initiatives targeting parent-child play.

The purpose of this study was to examine the feasibility of a take-home play bag

intervention, called Project Play Pack, for supporting families and promoting parent-child

play and to explore the potential of parent-child play for supporting young children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors through play.

Dangers of Less Play

Play has positive implications for young children’s learning and development in

home and in school settings (Ginsburg, 2007). Alternatively, the consequences of a lack

of in-home play among young children may originate from the reduction of quality open-

ended play interactions among parents and children (Hay, Payne, & Chadwick, 2004;

Page 15: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

3

Kerns & Barth, 1995; Leve & Fagot, 1997). Open-ended play is a type of play without

predetermined rules (Bekker, Hopma, & Sturm, 2010; Bekker, Groenendaal, Wesselink,

& Eggen, 2008). Less parent-child play, particularly open-ended play interactions,

represents missed opportunities to provide support for social emotional development

through play (Denham, Renwick, & Holt, 1991). Common open-ended activities for

young children from three to five years old include materials that are manipulated by

hand and involve making things such as with recycled materials, play dough, or art

materials (Nell, Drew, & Bush, 2013). When open-ended play occurs between parents

and children, it provides unique opportunities to practice behaviors such as relating,

comforting, helping, being creative, encouraging, and attempting to settle conflicts,

which offers important support to the development of social and emotional skills

(Fantuzzo et al., 1998; Coolahan et al., 2000).

Recently, researchers have connected the decline of in-home play (Chudacoff,

2007; Gray, 2011) with serious consequences for young children (Almon & Miller, 2011;

Milkie, Kendig, Nomaguchi, & Denny, 2010; Stanger & Gridina, 1999). Studies

combining research in both play and brain science yield important information about the

dangers of play deprivation (Dickey, Castle, & Pryor, 2016; Milteer, Ginsburg, &

Mulligan, 2012; Rushton, Juola-Rushton, & Larkin, 2010) for child development and for

early learning. Negative outcomes for reduced play in early childhood include: less

creative and imaginative activities among children (Cordes & Miller, 2000; Van Evra,

2004), correlations to increased anxiety and depression among children (Gray, 2011), or

increased feelings of failure in work life balance of parents (Milkie et al., 2010).

Page 16: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

4

Benefits Lost

Lost opportunities to foster children’s abilities to relate to one another and adults

from a lack of in-home play are concerning for several reasons. Early success in school

is closely tied with ability to manage social and learning situations (Duncan et al., 2007;

McClelland, Morrison, & Holmes, 2000). We know, “learning is facilitated when

children are able to meet expectations for appropriate classroom behavior such as

following directions, cooperating with peers and adults, and containing frustration in the

face of difficult tasks or unsatisfied desires” (Coolahan et al., 2000, p. 459). Difficulty

relating to others and managing emotions in early childhood impact preschoolers’ early

school adjustment (Stallard, 1993). Moreover, long-term outcomes for young children

experiencing difficulties relating to others in early childhood include later emotional

disorders, delinquent behavior, and school failure (Denham & Holt, 1993; De Rosier,

Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994; Spence, 2003).

In sum, when the benefits of play for young children are lost due to a lack of

playing, the repercussions may be serious for children (fewer initiations of creative and

imaginative play, increases in anxiety and depression) and can lead to difficulties relating

to others (emotional problems or challenging behaviors). Moreover, a lack of in-home

play can contribute to less parental self-efficacy. The resulting troubles in relating to

peers and early childhood educators are evident in research highlighting the links

between young children’s abilities to relate to family members and their inclinations to

engage in learning (Hughes & Kwok, 2006). Preschool children excel in the classroom

when they relate well to their peers and their teachers and when they possess a curiosity

Page 17: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

5

to learn (Fantuzzo, Perry, & McDermott, 2004), and open-ended play may enhance those

outcomes.

Preschool Focus on Approaches to Learning

These associations among play, social skills, and dispositions towards learning are

not new. Kagan, Moore, and Bredekamp (1995) introduced the Approaches to Learning

dimension to the National Education Goals Panel in order to highlight the importance of

curiosity, creativity, cooperativeness, and persistence in bolstering the early learning and

development of young children. Approaches to Learning refers to the inclinations,

dispositions, and skills children use in learning (Hyson, 2008; Kagan et al, 1995). The

Head Start Early Learning Outcomes Framework (HSELOF) (2015) for children birth to

five included an expanded Approaches to Learning Central Domain. This increased

focus within the HSELOF reflected research linking Approaches to Learning with school

readiness (Blair & Raver, 2015). New research in the field of early childhood linked

skills associated with Approaches to Learning (emotional and behavioral self-regulation,

executive functioning, initiative, curiosity, and creativity) and long term academic

success (McDermott, Rikoon, & Fantuzzo, 2014).

Moreover, recent literature suggests correlations among play in early childhood

and the emotional, behavioral, and self-regulatory skills that embody Approaches to

Learning (Bodrova & Leong, 2012; Diener, Wright, Brehl, & Black, 2016). Specifically,

correlations among preschool peer play interactions, Approaches to Learning, and later

school success are notable (Coolahan et al., 2000). A number of researchers continue to

explore preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning within peer play interactions (Coolahan et

Page 18: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

6

al., 2000; Fantuzzo et al., 1995). First, Fantuzzo et al. (1995) created a teacher report

scale for evaluating preschool children’s interactive play, the Penn Interactive Peer Play

Scale (PIPPS), which considers Approaches to Learning elements such as play

engagement, disruption, disengagement, and creativity in peer play. The PIPPS was a

culturally and developmentally appropriate tool to help identify children who

demonstrated positive play relationship with their peers, to help identify children who

were struggling to foster positive play relationships, and to help identify the play

strengths of resilient at-risk preschool children (Fantuzzo et al., 1995). The PIPPs work

revealed three underlying constructs within preschoolers’ free play behaviors with peers:

play interaction, play disruption, and play disconnection (Fantuzzo et al., 1995).

Children’s popularity, isolation, and rejection were related to their peer play behaviors as

identified by the PIPPS. Since then, a parent report version generated the same three-

factor solution among the same population and asserted the construct validity of the

PIPPS (Fantuzzo, Mendez, & Tighe, 1998). These works serve as a preliminary tie

among play behaviors and social outcomes in early learning settings.

Approaches to Learning and Play

Furthermore, in 2000, Coolahan et al. examined how interactive peer play relates

to learning behaviors among low-income minority students via teacher reported

preschool’s peer play and learning behaviors. This work utilized the PIPPS in concert

with the Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale (PLBS; McDermott, Green, Francis, &

Stott, 2000). They found a negative correlation between play disruption (physical or

verbal aggression, not taking turns, demanding to be in charge, or snatching things away

Page 19: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

7

from peers) and the learning behavior attitudes dimension of the PLBS (tendency to

cooperate, accept help, or act out when frustrated) (Coolahan et al., 2000). These studies

provide additional support for correlations between children’s peer play and Approaches

to Learning behaviors. However, the authors eventually concluded that these correlations

reflected preschooler’s social abilities and sense of belonging in school (Coolahan et al.,

2000).

This paper purposes that preschoolers play patterns are a reflection of their

learning behaviors rather than their sense of belonging and social fit. However,

preschoolers learn through play (Broadhead, 2006; Han, Moore, Vukelich, & Buell,

2010; Schickedanz, 1978; Schulz & Bonawitz, 2007). When play is considered in this

way, play behaviors are learning behaviors. For example, it is more likely for a

preschooler to persist in building a block tower (play action), than to persist in engaging

a reluctant peer (social action), and the former behavior has more interesting implications

for children’s later academic performance. However, these kinds of assertions are not

easily made through teacher’s reports of preschool children’s behaviors. The most

appropriate way to assess Approaches to Learning skills within play is to analyze

preschoolers’ behaviors while in a play situation. Learning is a social process, rooted

within interactions (Vygotsky, 1976). One of the primary learning interactions for young

children are parent-child play interactions. Research should consider children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions. Yet, few studies

have explored parent-child play interactions and Approaches to Learning (Fantuzzo &

McWayne, 2002). Arguments for the promotion of play in homes and in early learning

Page 20: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

8

settings could be bolstered by empirical assertions of the links among parent-child play

and Approaches to Learning behaviors.

Compounding Threats for Head Start Children

The negative consequences of a lack of in-home play and preschooler’s troubles

relating to others in early childhood are particularly concerning for Head Start children

and families facing multiple risk factors (Kestenbaum et al, 2016). Challenges relating to

poverty and increased stress in the lives of Head Start children make them an especially

vulnerable group to social and emotional difficulties (Anthony et al., 2005; Evans &

English, 2002). Work to uncover the assets within and needed supports for the

Approaches to Learning skills of children in poverty is just beginning (Fantuzzo et al,

1995; McDermott et al., 1996). However, families experiencing the stress of poverty can

be a challenging group to reach through parent engagement initiatives (Waanders,

Mendez, & Downer, 2007). Parent engagement initiatives for families at-risk face

continue to face implementation challenges including: parent work schedule conflicts,

family church activity conflicts, family illness, problems finding child care to attend

events, parent tiredness (Mendez, 2010), general work demands (training, timing,

scheduling: Webster-Stratton, Reid, & Hammond, 2001), and fatigue related to long or

unconventional work hours (Gutman & McLoyd, 2000). Interventions contributing to

demands for time and resources are less likely to be effective for these families. More

flexible, non-invasive, home-based interventions are needed to reach parents combatting

the challenges conditions of living in poverty (Webster-Stratton et al., 2001).

Page 21: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

9

The preceding section provided an introduction into the importance of in-home

play, the relevance of parent-child play interactions and children’s Approaches to

Learning, and how these specific skills, developed within play, support children’s early

learning. This section concluded by reviewing the some of the challenges of parent

engagement interventions with Head Start populations. However, knowledge gaps

remain regarding: (1) how Approaches to Learning skills are supported through parent-

child play, (2) if take-home play bag parent-child engagement initiatives can influence

the in-home play of families in poverty and (3) if in-home play interventions offer

support to preschool children’s Approaches to Learning (via play and task practice).

Moreover, there is a need for new and innovative ways to support children in poverty and

their families through dynamic approaches (Mattingly, Prislin, McKenzie, Rodriguez, &

Kayzar, 2002; Mendez, 2010). Researchers have been challenged to provide support to

in-home parent-child interactions through convenient, culturally responsive, engaging in-

home play initiatives for families facing multiple risk factors with preschool aged

children (Peterson, Luze, Eshbaugh, Jeon, & Kantz, 2007; Raikes, Green, Atwater,

Kisker, Constantine, & Chazzan-Cohen, 2006). The purpose of this study is to examine

the efficacy of a parent-child engagement initiative, take-home play bags, for Head Start

families.

Research Questions

The Project Play Pack, take-home play bag, intervention focused on supporting

Head Start children and families to cultivate their own parent-child play practices. The

intervention was designed to inspire more frequent play interactions through the delivery

Page 22: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

10

of weekly materials, take-home play bags. In addition, the bags supported Head Start

parents’ interest in helping their children acquire Approaches to Learning skills, such as

patience, creativity, persistence, behavioral regulation, sustained attention, and emotional

regulation. These six skills inspired the six take-home play bag themes. A new bag each

week for six weeks, featuring a new theme and new materials, encouraged repeated play

experiences intended to foster children’s Approaches to Learning skills through parent-

child play.

A quasi-experimental embedded mixed method design (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009)

was used to examine the following two main research questions and sub-questions:

1. What factors enhance or inhibit the feasibility of the Project Play Pack

intervention? (QUAL & quant mixed: Weekly Feedback Forms, Play Pack take-

home bag Ranking Task, and parent post interviews)

2. To what extent does the Project Play Pack intervention influence parent-child play

among Head Start families? (QUAL & quant mixed: Weekly Feedback Forms,

Play Pack take-home bag Ranking Task, Play Frequency Forms, & parent post

interviews; QUAL to quant transformation: Parent-child play observation videos

transformed)

Definition of Terms

Parent-child play

The use of the term “parent-child play” in this paper refers to play where adult

care givers and children are mutually engaged in play. This term is used to generate a

Page 23: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

11

distinction from preschoolers’ peer play, preschoolers play in early learning settings, or

preschoolers solitary play at home.

Parent

Broadly defined here as regular in-home caregivers (mothers, fathers,

grandparents, aunts, and uncles).

Parent play-involvement

Parent play-involvement refers to the degree to which parents do or do not engage

within parent-child play (Giallo, Treyvaud, Cooklin, & Wade, 2013). Watts and Barnett

(1973) defined characteristics of adult involvement in children’s activities. These

characteristics framed the parent play-involvement coding protocol by Tejagupta (1991)

that assigns designations to the level of parent engagement in play regardless of type of

play including: participation (actively engaging), facilitation (encouraging, or suggesting

things, but not involving as a player), neutral (only observing), restriction (disapproving

or preventing activities), and non-involvement (engaging in a different activity without

paying attention to child’s play

Open-ended play

Open-ended play is a particular type of play devoid of predetermined rules

(Bekker et al., 2010; Sturm et al., 2008). It is distinguished from closed-ended play or

homework assignments that have one prescribed way of correct completion. Moreover,

open-ended play encourages players to “create their own (emerging) game goals”

(Bekker et al., 2010, p 2). Open-ended materials could be constructing materials (Drew

& Rankin, 2004; Nell et al., 2013) or tools and materials that invite exploration.

Page 24: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

12

Constructing materials (making or collage materials) can be used to “make” in a variety

of ways: creating, constructing, or sorting. Exploratory materials (building, pretending,

or musiking toys) can also be used to facilitate open-ended play through exploration and

experimentation (Krafft & Berk, 1998). Board games can also become open-ended when

players are free to construct their own rules or re-imagine the goal of the game freely

(Bekker et al., 2008). When play materials and spaces are designed to allow for

creativity, children offer perceive the activity as more fun and more engaging (playing for

longer periods) (Bekker et al., 2008; Lin, Chang, Liu, & Chu, 2006; McLoyd, 1983).

Moreover, open-ended play provides necessary support for learning, experience with

materials and or themes, through exploration and discovery (White et al., 2007).

Non-Invasive

The term “non-invasive” referred to the intervention design. The intervention was

designed to influence a targeted family habit without pervading unrelated family habits.

Merriam-Webster defines “non-invasive” as tending not to spread, specifically not

tending to infiltrate and destroy healthy tissue (“non-invasive,” n.d.). This term is

introduced in this work to generate a distinction from interventions that target family

habits by imposing additional responsibilities upon parents which bring about unintended

changes in other family routines. Thus, the use of the term “non-invasive” is intended to

reflect an intention to not disrupt unrelated family practices by including design features

which: offer intervention that occurs within regular family habits, facilitate parent’s

active participation in intervention strategies with their own children, and honor the

Page 25: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

13

parent-child relationship as a primary avenue for enacting behavioral changes among

young children.

Non-invasive refers to the design features of the intervention fit within intended

families’ routines.

Approaches to Learning

Approaches to Learning is an emergent term being used to label preschoolers’

learning behaviors in early learning contexts. Approaches to Learning focuses on the

how rather than the what of learning and preparing for school, such as dispositions,

inclinations, or styles of engaging in learning experiences (Chen, Masur, & McNamee,

2011; Hyson, 2005, 2008; McWayne, Fantuzzo, & McDermott, 2004). Hyson (2008)

defines Approaches to Learning as the “motivations, attitudes, and behaviors that

children display when participating in classroom learning activities” (p 2). HSELOF for

children birth to five, Approaches to Learning Central Domain (2015) is described as

focusing on “how children learn. It refers to the skills and behaviors that children use to

engage in learning” (p 10). The HSELOF includes goals for Approaches to Learning

include: creativity in thinking and communication, imagination in play and interactions,

initiative and curiosity, persistence in challenging tasks, remembering directions or

stories, the ability to control impulses and sustain attention. Educational research also

refers to these behaviors commonly as, learning behaviors (McDermott, 1999;

McDermott, Leigh, Perry, 2002). Learning behaviors is a term used to refer to

“flexibility, reflectivity, strategic problem solving, vigilance, persistence, response to

novelty and error, as well as manifestations of effectiveness motivation and attitudes

Page 26: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

14

toward learning” (McDermott et al., 2002, p. 353). The Preschool Learning Behaviors

Scale (PLBS) has been nationally standardized and validated to assess preschool

children’s learning behaviors including: motivation (willingness for a challenge,

independence, curiosity and courage for novel activities), attention/persistence

(distractibility, impulsiveness, don’t care attitude, poor attention), and attitudes towards

learning (desire to please teacher, cooperativeness, frustration when challenged).

The definition for Approaches to Learning utilized in this work will be:

motivations, attitudes, and behaviors that children display when engaging in playful

learning. The overarching domain of Approaches to Learning includes both cognitive

(executive function) and behavioral self-regulation (socially contextualized regulatory

behaviors). The behavioral indicators related to this paper’s conception of Approaches to

Learning include: waiting, creativity, persistence, turn-taking, attention, and emotional

regulation.

Theoretical Framework

This work utilized the theoretical framework of Bronfenbrenner’s ecological

theory (1986). This building connections theory helped to honor the multiple learning

environments of young children. Parents’ in-home support of young children’s

development represents a microsystem. Microsystems are places that immediately

contain us, our closest surroundings, the home or the preschool classroom are examples.

But, microsystems do not exist in a vacuum, they are embedded within other systems and

collide among themselves (microsystem to microsystem) in what Bronfenbrenner refers

to as mesosystems (Bronfenbrenner, 1979).

Page 27: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

15

The mesosystem is the connection between home and school. Parent-teacher

partnerships are mesosystems. This particular mesosystem actively supports healthy

learning and development. Research on school and family partnerships asserts that

teachers and parents reciprocally influence each other's beliefs regarding parents' role and

involvement (Eccles & Harold, 1996). However, it is often difficult to find ways to

strengthen the home-school connection with parents living in poverty who often face

multiple time constraints (tiredness, work scheduling, child care: Mendez, 2010; limited

time and transportation resources: Brown & Talmi, 2005). More dynamic strategies,

non-invasive interventions, are needed. A Triadic (parent–child–professional; McCollum

& Yates, 1994) approach to promote parent–child and parent–professional partnerships

may pose fewer threats to parents’ limited time.

Non-invasive interventions which offer responsive materials for families to

incorporate at home (microsystem), with specific attention to the parent-child relationship

have the potential to act as a bridge within the mesosystem of the parent-school

relationship. For example, well-designed take-home play bags could reflect an early

childhood educator’s understanding of their students’ interests, their expertise in early

childhood education, and their enthusiasm for connecting with their students’ parents.

This intervention provided take-home play bags designed to support strong microsystem

connections by honoring the complexity of parents, children, and family-intervention

partnerships through intentional selection of bag materials and convenient material

delivery (outlined in Chapter 3).

Page 28: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

16

Conclusion

Work exploring the decline of in-home play reveals children’s play is threatened

by time constraints brought on by parent work/life balance (e.g. availability to play with

children: Milkie et al., 2010), digital media saturation (television supplemented not

replaced by new media, cumulatively more time spent on digital media: Stanger &

Gridina, 1999), and some parents’ limited understanding of play (see Parker, Boak,

Griffin, Ripple, & Peay, 1999). However, research asserts that parent-child play is

particularly beneficial for young children and their parents (Ginsburg, 2007). Parent-

child play supports early development and learning (Carson & Parke, 1996). Moreover,

research tells us that most parents greatly value involvement in their children’s learning

(Hill & Taylor, 2004). Recent work highlights how low income parents specifically,

value their children’s learning (Drummond & Stipek, 2004; Epstein, Williams, & Nesbitt,

2002) as well as the importance of play in early childhood (Fogle & Mendez, 2006).

However, families from low incomes face additional challenges to find the time

(Ginsburg, 2007) and the resources (Milteer et al., 2012) to play. Specifically, Head Start

families need support for maintaining in-home play in the face of multiple environmental

threats brought on by poverty (dangerous neighborhoods: Scott & Munson, 1994; limited

parent time due to challenging work schedules: Korfmacher et al., 2008). Although

parents often view play positively, the afore mentioned challenges limit opportunities for

children and parents to play and connect. Moreover, many parents struggle to know

where to start when attempting to offer play experience to their young children within

their limited free time (Drummond & Stipek, 2004; Parker et al., 1999).

Page 29: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

17

This introduction provided a rationale for the Project Play Pack intervention by

outlining the importance of play promotion in early childhood and the need for more

dynamic interventions for families in poverty. Persistent gaps in our understanding of

play, parent involvement in play, Approaches to Learning, and parent-child engagement

initiatives were reviewed. The stated purpose of this study was to examine the feasibility

of a take-home play bag intervention, Project Play Pack, for supporting families and

promoting parent-child play and to explore the potential of parent-child play for

supporting young children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors. I outlined the research

questions guiding the study of the intervention and defined the key terms. The theoretical

framework positioned the intervention goals and research questions within the context of

the lives of young children and their families.

In brief sum, the comprehensive goal of Project Play Pack intervention was to

support Head Start children and families in engaging in more frequent parent-child play

interactions and to contribute to a greater understanding of how Approaches to Learning

behaviors are expresses within parent-child play interactions. The next chapter will

further define the key elements of the paper by reviewing the contributions and gaps

within the literature.

Page 30: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

18

CHAPTER TWO

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

This chapter provides a review of literature synthesizing work relating to family

engagement initiates focused on promoting play and Approaches to Learning related

skills of young children. First, why was parent involvement selected as a focal element

of this intervention?

Parent Involvement and School Success

Defining parent involvement

Parent involvement in their children’s education has been linked with positive

outcomes throughout research (Clark, 1993; Epstein & Dauber, 1991; Jimerson, Egeland,

& Teo, 1999). Parent involvement is a key protective factor in fostering cognitive and

emotional resilience among low-income families (Garmezy, 1991; Myers & Taylor,

1998; National Research Council (NRC), 2000; Shumow, Vandell & Posner, 1999).

Specifically, parent involvement in their children’s education can buffer the negative

influence of poverty on children’s language, cognitive and social competencies

(Shonkoff, Phillips, & NRC, 2000). Moreover, a number of studies show home-based

learning is important for children from families of color experiencing poverty (Ingram,

Wolfe, & Lieberman, 2007; Ryan, Casas, Kelly-Vance, Ryalls, & Nero, 2010).

Fantuzzo, McWayne, Perry, and Childs (2004) found only parent home-based

involvement predicted Head Start children’s competencies, including higher levels of

proactive learning behaviors, greater receptive vocabulary, and lower levels of conduct

concerns when studied alongside other common predictors.

Page 31: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

19

Literature regarding parent involvement in school varies according to the age of

the student (Waanders, Mendez, & Downer, 2007). For example, parent involvement in

school in k-12 settings is commonly considered in terms of helping with homework,

attending parent-teacher meetings, etc. (Lee & Bowen, 2006). Researchers link home-

based parent involvement with older students and higher academic scores in reading and

writing (Epstein & Dauber, 1991; Griffith, 1996; Keith et al., 1998). For younger

children, avenues for parent involvement are less clear and less frequently studied

(Arnold, Zeljo, Doctoroff, & Ortiz, 2008; Benedict, Horner, & Squires, 2007; Taylor &

Machida, 1994). Regardless of the age of the child, when or where parents are actively

involved in their children’s learning, school readiness and academic achievement

increases (Arnold et al., 2008; Barton et al, 2014; Miedel & Reynolds, 2000; Parker, et

al., 1999; Reynolds, Mavrogenes, Bezruczko, & Hagemann, 1996; Strain & Timm, 2001;

Stevenson & Baker, 1987). Contemporary conceptualizations of parental involvement

for children of all ages recognize parents’ contributions as dynamic work occurring at

home and within school contexts (LeFevre & Shaw, 2012; Walker, Wilkins, Dallier,

Sandler, & Hoover-Dempsey, 2005), and as directly fostering academic and character

building skills (DeRousie & Durham, 2008).

While the body of literature supporting the positive influence of parent

involvement is promising, there are limitations within the collection. Domina’s (2005)

longitudinal analysis of kindergarten through first grader parental involvement and

academic achievement from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 revealed

contradictory findings. After controlling for family background and children’s prior

Page 32: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

20

academic performance the effects of parental involvement on academic achievement

were not significant. However, the majority of the parent involvement measures in the

National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 data set referred to school-based

involvement of elementary school children.

In addition, divergent definitions of parent involvement make it difficult to draw

specific conclusions in ways that help illuminate the connections between family

involvement and academic success (Fan & Chen, 2001). Nonetheless, Fan and Chen’s

(2001) meta-analysis of twenty-five parent-involvement studies, with a range of students

in preschool to post-secondary education, revealed 4 common categories of parental

involvement including: parent-child communication, home supervision, educational

aspiration, school-participation. They found that parental educational aspirations and

expectation for children’s educational achievement had the strongest relationship with

students’ academic achievement across their study collection (Fan & Chen, 2001).

While the results of Fan and Chen (2001) are helpful for setting the stage for an

understanding of parental involvement, a clear focus on the unique elements of parent

involvement in early childhood is needed. Fantuzzo et al.’s, (2000) parent involvement

questionnaire (Family Involvement Questionnaire: FIQ) specifically designed for parents

of children birth to age seven mirrors this synthesis of Fan and Chen (2001). Fantuzzo et

al. (2000) focused on early childhood including three defined dimensions of parent

involvement in early childhood. They found that parents engaged in school-based

involvement (volunteering in the classroom or attending parent meetings), home-based

involvement (creating spaces for learning activities at home or facilitating enrichment

Page 33: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

21

activities for the child within the community), or home-school conferencing (parent-

teacher or parent-administrator communication about child) (Epstein, 1992; Fan & Chen,

2001; Fantuzo, Tighe, & Perry, 1999). Studies utilizing the FIQ revealed relationships

among home-based involvement and a variety of outcomes including: children’s

prosocial behaviors, and peer play interactions at both home and school (Fantuzzo et al.,

2000) as well as motivation, persistence, and attitudes toward learning (Fantuzzo et al.,

2004).

Similarly, the Parent Involvement in Early Learning scale (PIEL, Manz et al.,

2014) utilized action research methods to partner with low-income parents of toddlers to

design a measure of in-home parent involvement that could be contextually responsive

and useful for evaluating child-development-focused home visits. This collaborative

work with parents highlighted the importance of parent-child communication and the

home supervision elements from Fan and Chen’s (2001) review as well as reaffirmed the

focus on the importance of play for considering parent home-based involvement in

children’s learning in the early years. The PIEL makes a distinction between and

assesses parents’ direct teaching as well as involvement in developmentally appropriate

playful interactions such as drawing and game playing through intentional wording of

items (Manz et al., 2014). The contribution of the PIEL is significant because it begins to

reflect parents’ direct involvement in young children’s play as an integral piece of parent

involvement. However, the helpfulness of the distinctive wording is limited by parents’

interpretations in self-report. For example, a parent self-report PIEL item which reads

“Teach child new words” could be enacted through literal parent teaching or playful

Page 34: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

22

parent-child interaction. It is impossible to determine the degree to which the interactions

are playful or instructional in nature. These limitations suggest a need for observational

studies to assess how parents engage in parent-child play with young children.

These parent involvement measures cast a light on the source of the potential

benefits of home-based parent involvement for young children. Parent-child interactions

are key elements of home-based early learning supports. In particular, parent-child play

is a primary means for supporting the home-based early learning of young children.

However, parents may feel unsure of how or what to do maximize their home-based

parent-child interactions (Emerson & Linder, in press; Giallo et al., 2013). Moreover,

parent self-report of behaviors within parent-child play may not clearly depict

distinctions between parent-child instructional or playful interactions. None the less,

parent-child play interactions can be complex exchanges of interpersonal behavior which

foster important emotional regulation skills for future peer and adult relationships

(Comfort & Farran, 1994). The parental investment in parent-child play for young

children’s learning and the promotion of children’s self-regulation is imperative

(Gonzalez-DeHass, 2016). In fact, the predominant psychosocial task for children from

two to six years old is emotional regulation (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004). Young

children who have opportunities to watch and imitate the ways their parents display

persistence, intellectual curiosity, and dynamic problem solving learn positive

dispositions to learning within play interactions and are more likely to internalize the

same habits (Gonzalez-DeHass, 2016; Zimmerman & Schunk, 2008).

Page 35: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

23

The previous paragraphs acknowledge the multifaceted definition of parent

involvement within educational research. This body of literature stresses the importance

of parent involvement in early learning outcomes. Despite the divergent definitions of

parental involvement within the literature, home-based involvement includes: academic

teaching at home, supporting the child’s participatory (playful) learning opportunities and

activities, and as fostering family and moral values (LeFevre & Shaw, 2012; Manz et al.,

2004; Manz et al., 2016). Project Play Pack focused on parent-child play interactions

which support children’s participatory learning. The next section highlights the

importance of parent perceptions for influencing parent practices.

Parent Perceptions

The literature regarding parent involvement in children’s education specifically

attends to the influence of parental beliefs and perceptions on parent-school involvement.

Parent beliefs about involvement and perceptions of school’s engagement efforts are

linked to parent involvement behaviors (Grolnick, Benjet, Kurowski, & Apostoleris,

1997; Pomerantz, Moorman, & Litwack, 2007; Ritblatt, Beatty, Cronan, & Ochoa, 2002).

Specifically, parents’ perceptions of their roles in children’s education and their

perceptions of their own parental self-efficacy influence their levels of parental

involvement (Bandura, 1994, Hoover-Dempsey, Bassler, & Brissie, 1992; Hover-

Dempsey & Sandler, 1997).

Moreover, Ritblatt et al. (2002) conducted a study of over 500 parents considering

parent perceptions of the home-school connection and parent involvement behaviors.

Parent perceptions of the sensitivity of school personnel to parents and mutual support of

Page 36: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

24

schools and parents for each other were significantly correlated to parental involvement

behaviors (Ritblatt, et al, 2002). Similarly, Anderson and Minke (2007) studied

relationships between parent perceptions of teacher invitations and parent involvement

among 351 parents, primarily parents of color (93% of the sample) and found parents’

beliefs about involvement influenced their involvement behaviors according to their

perceptions of being specifically invited to participate, invitations mediated the

relationship between parent beliefs and parent behaviors (Anderson & Mink, 2007).

Parent perceptions of their involvement and of involvement initiatives influence their

involvement behaviors. Parent’s positive perceptions of the Project Play Pack

intervention are expected to be linked to increases in in-home play in the midpoint and

final mixing phases.

Parent Involvement Studies

Interventions aimed at promoting home-based parent involvement are not new.

However, very few studies define specific elements of home-based parent involvement

when stressing its importance for preschoolers’ social or academic development. The

social learning theoretical models drives the rational for the majority of home-based

parent involvement interventions. Learning does not occur in a vacuum, but instead

requires joint engagement. Vygotsky situated children’s cognitive development within

the social world (1976). Social learning theory frames the majority of parent engagement

initiatives seeking to motivate parents to gain or improve the ways that the support their

children’s learning at home through encouragement and support.

Page 37: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

25

Mantzicopoulos (1997) examined maternal school involvement (Parent/Family

Involvement Index, PFQ, Cone, Delawyer, & Wolfe, 1985) and home literacy activities

(Home Literacy Activities and Maternal Educational Expectations: Stipek, Milburn,

Clements, & Daniels, 1992) and the school readiness (measured by Kaufman Assessment

Battery for Children—Achievement Battery, K-ABC Achievement, Kaufman &

Kaufman, 1983; Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for

Young Children, Harter & Pike, 1984) of 93 Head Students and their families. The

hierarchical regression model failed to reveal causal relationships among these different

elements. However, a correlational relationship was found among home literacy

activities and some measures of Head Start students’ school readiness.

Fantuzzo et al. (2004) explored home-based involvement and preschoolers

learning outcomes among 144 Head Start students in the Northeastern United States. The

researchers considered family involvement (measured by the FIQ) and children’s

Approaches to Learning (measured by the PLBS, McDermott et al., 1996) and receptive

vocabulary skills (measured by the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Third Version,

PPVT-III, Dunn & Dunn, 1997). They found home-based parent involvement (they

defined as reading at home, making a space for educational activities at home, and asking

children about school) was strongly related to preschool classroom competencies

(Approaches to Learning behaviors) and children’s receptive vocabulary skills at the end

of the year (Fantuzzo et al., 2004).

Ingram et al. (2007) conducted a descriptive study utilizing surveys to explore

Epstein’s (1987) framework for how parents get involved in children’s education

Page 38: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

26

(measured by the FIQ) within a sample of high-risk but high-achieving school

populations. They identified 6 parent involvement typologies (parenting,

communicating, volunteering, learning at home, decision making, and collaborating with

the community). Among high-risk but high-achieving school populations parenting

(conceptualized as providing for a child’s basic needs such as food, clothing, shelter,

health, and safety) and learning at home (conceptualized as learning opportunities in the

home such as developing a child’s social skills, basic skills, advanced skills through

support on homework or other school activities completed at home) were the most

commonly reported typologies of parent involvement in schools (Ingram et al., 2007).

Taken together these three studies hint toward the utility of the provision of early

learning materials and the encouragement of home-based parent involvement for

improving outcomes for young children experiencing poverty. The Project Play Pack

intervention sought to support learning spaces and activities outside of school among

parents and children, specifically space and materials for learning activities. Project Play

Pack aimed to facilitate home-based parent involvement by the provision of curated

materials and ideas to incite parent-child play. The next section will focus more intently

on home-based parent involvement interventions in early childhood.

Home visitation parent involvement interventions. The previous section

reviewed studies exploring the influence of parent involvement, specifically home-based

involvement. The relationships between parent home-based involvement and positive

outcomes for young children served as a catalyst for a number of home visitation parent

involvement interventions. Home visiting programs are noted here because they are a

Page 39: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

27

common service delivery model for home-based parent involvement interventions among

many disciplines targeting high-risk families (Paulsell, Grosso, Suplee, 2014). These

home environments initiatives typically utilize child-development-focused home visiting

programs from the field of psychology which openly recognize parents’ pivotal role in

bolstering children’s outcomes (Manz & Bracaliello, 2016; Raikes al., 2006).

Home visiting intervention programs have targeted everything from infant health

to parenting skills (see Home Visiting Evidence of Effectiveness (HomVEE)

comprehensive review, Avellar & Paulsell, 2011). This body of literature is as broad and

diverse as its foci are multifarious. HomVEE reviewed 178 empirical articles on early

childhood home visiting programs for women or families with children from birth to 5

years olds targeting at least one of eight child outcomes: child health, child

development/school readiness, family economic sufficiency, referrals to community

services, maternal health, positive parenting practices, reduction in child maltreatment,

and reduction in violence, delinquency, or crime from 1979-2010. For the purpose of this

work, it is important to note that they found home visiting programs effective in

influencing positive parenting practices and young children’s social and cognitive

outcomes (Filene, Kaminski, Valle, & Cachat, 2013).

Specifically, two notable home visiting intervention studies drew upon the

Parent–Child Home Program, PCHP, a home visiting intervention program rooted in

child development principles focused on guiding parents in formulating developmentally-

salient, playful interactions with their child with the goal of enhancing children’s

language, social–emotional, emergent literacy skills, and parent-child attachment. The

Page 40: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

28

PCHP was originally designed in 1965 to enrich the parenting skills of parents challenged

by low incomes and limited education through the utilization of a unique play-oriented,

non-didactic approach to fostering attachment between parents and children (Levenstein,

1988). A longitudinal study of PCHP participants “found that the former PCHP children,

all at risk for educational disadvantage, graduated from high school at the same rate as

middle income students (Levenstein, Levenstein, Shiminski, & Stolzberg, 1998) and at a

significantly higher rate than randomized controls” (Levenstein et al., 2002, p 333).

Not only did the PCHP yield long term positive outcomes for young children

which provides support for Project Play Pack’s focus on parent-child play interactions,

but the PCHP intervention model occurred within homes and was rooted in play. The

PCHP intervention included twice-weekly home visits including play sessions with

parents and their 2-year-olds (3-year-olds in Year 2), where a facilitator shared a

curriculum of conceptual verbal interaction and other positive parenting techniques by

showing parent how to combine conversation with play. Home visitors structured these

modeling sessions around a weekly program gift of a colorful book or toy of high quality.

First the facilitator modeled, then provided feedback to parent attempts, and finally faded

out of each session as soon as the parent began to gain confidence (Levenstein & O’Hara,

1993). Although not explicitly, this play interaction focused home visit seemed to target

the home-based parent involvement subcategory of supporting the child’s participatory

learning opportunities and activities. The bi-weekly gift and facilitator modeling

contributed to supporting the child’s participatory learning opportunities and activities

(Levenstein et al., 2002). The PCHP intervention influenced multiple outcome including:

Page 41: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

29

children’s cognitive, social, behavioral abilities, and academic achievement (Gfellner,

McLaren, & Metcalfe, 2008; Levenstein et al., 2002; Levenstein et al., 1998; Manz &

Bracaliello, 2016)

Manz et al., 2016, studied the Parent–Child Home Program’s (PCHP) specific

influence on children’s expressive language abilities. They utilized a quasi-experimental

design to compare an intervention and control group to reveal a statistically significant

difference between groups in children’s performance on the modified Expressive One-

Word Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised (EOWPVT-R; Brownell, 2000), with the

intervention group showing higher levels of performance at the end of the first program

year (Manz et al., 2016).

Gfellner et al. (2008) studied the efficacy of the PCHP of the Child and Family

Services in Western Manitoba for 185 families between 1984-2005 in a much broader

sense. They considered baseline, end of first year, and end of 2nd year/post testing of

participants with three tools for measuring: positive home environments (Home Session

Behavior Scale: Levenstein, 1988), learning behaviors (Child Behavioral Traits scale,

CBT: Levenstein, 1988), and parent-child interactions (Parent and Child Together

Inventory, PACT: Levenstein, 1988). None the less, they found significant positive

increases across all three measures which they interpreted as an indicator in-home

environments, children’s behaviors, and parent-child interactions steadily improved

throughout the course of the program.

Both of these studies provided rationale for home-based parent involvement

interventions that harness parent-child play interactions for facilitating positive

Page 42: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

30

preschooler social and cognitive growth among at risk families. However, a number of

PCHP efficacy studies reported problems of attrition among home visiting program

participants (Levenstein, Levenstein & Oliver, 2002; McCurdy & Daro, 2001). For

example, Gfellner et al. (2008) noted marked attrition among Aboriginal families when

compared to Non-Aboriginal families. Azzi-Lessing’s (2011) critical review of home

visitation programs calls for improved strategies for engaging and retaining home visit

intervention participants.

Above all home visiting programs are expensive. Cost comparisons are

challenging because services, worker education levels/pay grades, treatment times, vary

widely (Boulatoff & Jump, 2007). However, Boulatoff and Jump (2007) generated a cost

analysis of the universal home visiting program, a program designed to inform and

support parents to raise healthy and productive children. The home visiting component

included an initial visit with a public health nurse and additional home visits by a

paraprofessional family visitor. Families had options for levels of service including

family visitor: weekly (1-2 hours), monthly (2 hours), monthly (1 hour), or quarterly

(2hrs). Their work generated annual costs per family across these varied service levels

by family visitor ranging from 549 dollars to 6,888 dollars per family per year on the

2005 dollar. If I divide the weekly visiting annual cost by 52, the quotient is $132 a week

per family. The budget for Project Play Pack totaled approximately 2,000 dollars for 6

weeks of weekly service for 20 families. If I divide the 6-week service total operating

costs by 6, the quotient is 333.33 a week for 20 families. If the Project Play Pack cost per

week is divided by 20 families, the quotient is $16.66 a week per family. Project Play

Page 43: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

31

Pack capitalized on the home-visiting design element that roots the intervention efforts

within families’ natural contexts and actively places parents and children together to

enact the intervention without the cost of a personnel visit. The take-home play bags of

Project Play Pack were a cost-saving, less invasive, proxy for an interventionist.

Home visitation is not the only intervention strategy used to target home-based

parent involvement in early childhood. Interventions designed to promote home-based

parent involvement through focused efforts, such as, specialized homework (Webster-

Stratton, Reid, & Stoolmiller, 2008) the promotion of reading to young children (Bradley,

2002; Melhuish et al., 2008), playing with numbers, drawing, enjoying songs, poems,

or rhymes (Marcon, 1999; Melhuish et al., 2008) are also utilized for families of children

under the age of five.

Mattingly et al. (2002) evaluated the effectiveness of forty-one parent

involvement programs in the United States, from kindergarten to 12th grade, and found

only modest support for parent involvement programing improving students learning.

The majority of the studies evaluated focused on content focused learning outcomes of

children, such as math or reading scores. Perhaps content focused learning outcomes are

poor measures of the most profitable parts of parent involvement, especially among

young children’s learning. The profitable part of parent involvement with young children

are the benefits that emerge from parents and children interacting positively in relation to

school more frequently. The arguable root of parent involvement programs in early

childhood is to support the parent-child relationship rather than child outcomes.

However, a recent review of early childhood (birth to 5 years old) classroom-based and

Page 44: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

32

parent-focused social-emotional interventions by Barton et al. (2014) highlights the

primary format of the majority of these programs was more “work” for parents, either

parent workshops (12 studies) or homework for parents to implement with children (7

studies). The following is a brief review of interventions targeting home-based parent

involvement in early childhood that utilize alternatives to the home visiting model.

Other home-based parent interventions. Webster-Stratton, Reid, and

Hammond (2001) examined the effectiveness of a parent teacher training program in

Head Start for social emotional competence and behavioral problem reduction, including

guiding parents on parent-child interactions. They found participants in the experimental

group whose mother attend 6 or more training session showed significantly fewer

conduct problems. The positive results are overshadowed by the difficulty of this parent

training intervention design for participants, requiring substantial time commitments from

families already experiencing environmental related stress and time demands. In this

case, the parent training consisted of a 12-week maternal training program offering 6-10

parent training sessions. Participants in the experimental group attended an average of

5.73 (SD = 5.26) sessions. In terms of hours of intervention received, participants in the

experimental group received an average of 14.32 hours (SD = 13.14). Almost half of the

experimental participants (49%) were considered “noncompleters” because they attended

less than 6 parent training classes (12%) or attended no parent training classes (37%).

The authors explain that families who could not participate cited work demands,

scheduling difficulties, or employee training as a reason for absences, not a lack of

Page 45: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

33

interest. With this note, the authors called for programing that is accessible all families,

perhaps by offering flexible home-based interventions.

Webster-Stratton, Reid, and Stoolmiller (2008) designed an intervention

curriculum focused on promoting social competence and emotional self-regulation skills

through activities implemented in Head Start, kindergarten, and first grade classes whole

group/circle time, small group practice games, and home-based settings. The Child

Social and Emotional curriculum (Dina the Dinosaur Social Skills and Problem Solving

Curriculum) intervention included weekly dinosaur homework, sent home to encourage

parent involvement in the social emotional curriculum for the children. They conducted a

randomized control trail efficacy study utilizing measures of parent involvement

(Teacher-Parent Involvement Questionnaire, INVOLVE-T: researcher revised

questionnaire developed by the Oregon Social Learning Center) and a researched

developed post intervention parent involvement/satisfaction survey (parent self-report of

their feelings about the curriculum, their opinions of the value of the homework, and how

much their children used the program strategies at home). They found no intervention

effects on parent involvement (as measured by the INVOLVE-T). They found 87.3% of

parents reported the homework useful and 72.5 said their children used the Dinosaur

School strategies at home. However, there is a lack of explanation of what the homework

was and the authors do not provide information on how or if it was collected and

evaluated for completion.

Similarly, DeLoatche, Bradley-Klug, Ogg, Kromrey, and Sundman-Wheat (2015)

explored the efficacy of intervention designed to promote Head Start student’s early

Page 46: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

34

literacy through home-based parent involvement, pre-literacy homework. The

intervention included a 75-90-minute pre-intervention training for parents on how to

implement in the in-home activities. The in-home activities included 27 lessons to be

given over 9 weeks in 15-20 minute sessions by parents with children at home. These

activities were designed to help parents teach literacy at home: each session utilized a

mnemonic device to teach a letter name, reviewed letter and name pairings, and small

assessment activity for the parent to guide and the child to complete on the letter taught.

Parents submitted the ending activity, homework, to Head Start centers every three

weeks. The authors fail to provide information about the consistency or fidelity of these

submission other than parents completed 97% of the assignments. The authors evaluated

the efficacy of the intervention based Family Involvement Questionnaire (FIQ, Fantuzzo

et al. 2000) pre and posttest comparison. They concluded that the intervention promoted

parent-directed early literacy activities in participants’ homes which in turn influenced

other home-based involvement indicators on the Family Involvement Questionnaire.

Neuman (1996) evaluated the efficacy of a home-based parent involvement

intervention designed to support Head Start children’s early literacy through providing

literacy materials and opportunities for parent-child storybook reading in a 12-week book

club. Neuman utilized a book club model that made space for parent and children to

receive instruction on read a-louds from a facilitator, immediately practice read a-loud

interactions, and take a new book home after every session. The author found emergent

literacy growth among children in the book club across Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

(PPVT, Dunn & Dunn, 1997) a measure of receptive language, and children’s concepts of

Page 47: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

35

print test as indicators of print conventions knowledge. The author provides no

information regarding attrition or rate of participation in book club meetings.

Starkey, Klein, and Wakely (2004) studied the efficacy of a mathematics

intervention utilizing a pre-k mathematics curriculum for home and school among 163

pre-k students from low or middle-income families using a quasi-experimental design.

The intervention group was assessed using a pretest-posttest design of child math

understanding utilizing the Child Math Assessment (CMA, researcher developed, math

knowledge including number, arithmetic, space/geometry, measurement, patterns, and

logical reasoning). They found that children in the intervention group scored

significantly higher on the CMA. The intervention consisted of a classroom curriculum

and a home based component intended to promote parental support of children’s

mathematical development. For the home based component, parents and children

attended three parent training classes presenting math curriculum and teaching parents

how to engage in dyadic math activities with children. Parents were given materials and

guide sheets for continuing to teach the math activities at home. However, the authors do

not provide information about parent attendance, home math activity tracking, or fidelity

information.

These five studies provide examples of home-based parental involvement

intervention models utilized with children and families in early childhood: parent training

programs and home/school curriculums typically including a homework connection

component. These examples are helpful because they target a variety of outcomes (social

emotional competence, early math or early literacy skills) but suffer from many of the

Page 48: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

36

same limitations as home visiting designs. Many of these studies understate the home-

based involvement procedures and do a poor job of tracking and reporting on the fidelity

of these pieces. Some of them avoid the topic of participant attrition altogether. When

participant attrition was addressed implications for the strain that these program designs

put on families were clear. The final alternative model for home-based parental

involvement intervention in early childhood is take-home bags.

Take-home bag interventions. Take-home bag intervention designs offer an

alternative to home-based family intervention through the provision of support materials

rather than more work. Previously, interventions featuring take-home bags focused on

specific content areas with success in encouraging and empowering families to enact new

practices at home. Rather than parent training or workshops that require parents to be

away from their children (particularly challenging for at risk families: Mendez, 2010) or

additional homework tasks for parents to use to tutor their children, take-home bag

designs can be non-invasive. The design capitalizes on early childhood’s foundational

belief that parents and the home environment play critical roles in both the intellectual

and social development of all young children (Eccles & Harold, 1996; Mattingly et al.,

2002). Take home play bag interventions have been successful in engaging families

through invitations to take action, together at home, through: book bags (Barbour, 1998;

Dever, 2001; Dever & Burts, 2002; Zeece & Wallace, 2009), science bags (Kokoski &

Patton, 1997), and math bags (Linder, 2017; Linder & Emerson, in revisions). Some

book bag interventions have included toys for enrichment without focusing directly on

increasing play [for example: puppets (Abadiano & Turner, 2003; Richgels & Wold,

Page 49: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

37

1998), folder games and shaving cream, (Grande, 2004), stuffed animals and literacy

games (Brand, Marchand, Lilly, & Child, 2014), open-ended math manipulatives (Linder,

2017).

Brand et al. (2014) designed a home-literacy take-home play bag intervention

called Reading Partners for 20 three to five year olds. The take-home bag included: a

children’s book, a stuffed animal, a letter to parents, a bag-contents list, a theme-based

game and instructions on how to play, crayons, a journal for parents and children to

record their experience, and a parent satisfactions survey. Parent evaluation forms were

used to explore the outcomes of the project. Parents’ open-ended feedback highlighted

how the take-home bags provided additional opportunities for parents to engage in

literacy activities at home, helped parents generate new ideas for engaging activities to do

at home, and promoted parent understanding of the importance of reading with their

children. This article fails to provide information regarding attrition or participant

response rates of the parent evaluation.

Grande (2004) designed a home-literacy take-home play bag intervention for nine

(1st, 2nd, and 3rd grade classrooms). Students across all classrooms took the bags home at

least twice during the year. Perhaps because of the small number of treatments, attrition

was not addressed in this article. Parents completed a survey of their knowledge of

grade-level expectations and at-home literacy activities at the beginning of the school

year and immediately after completing the bags at home. Each bag contained a “user

friendly” instruction sheet, materials (bingo game of sight words, a stuffed toy, file folder

games on phonemic awareness, picture books, shaving cream, parent feedback journal (to

Page 50: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

38

solicit parent opinion and experiences), and a “parent packet” that included an

informative practitioner article, a list of multisensory teaching techniques for spelling,

and a list of book suggestions. Pre/posttest comparisons of the parent knowledge surveys

indicated gains as a result of the bags (supporting statistics were not provided by the

author). Parent feedback journals indicated parents’ plans to continue to replicate the

activities from the bags even after their turn with the take-home bag was over.

While both of these studies offer hopeful accounts of parents’ and children’s

warm reception of take-home play bag they both reflect a common issue among

publications regarding this home-based intervention design. Both either employ weak

research methodology design or struggle to provide adequate descriptions of intervention

implementation. Specifically, both of the articles failed to provide important information

about the recruitment and retention of participants or frequency measures relating to

changes in targeted home-based parental involvement behaviors.

Dever and Burts (2002) provide a few more details in their study of the efficacy

of their Family Literacy Bag (FLB) project, to engage parents in learning at home, among

2,340 participants across four school districts in the Western United States. Each take-

home bag contained 3 children’s books of varying reading level and genre, an extension

activity based on a theme, a parent guidebook, guidelines for reading and discussing

books with children, and suggestions for additional extension. Each participating family

received a bag for one week at least every three weeks for the duration of the school year.

They utilized four data sources to measure: pre/post intervention book reading at home (a

researcher developed questionnaire asked about frequency, time spent, who was reading

Page 51: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

39

with the child, the quality and variety of books, and frequency of book discussion), parent

evaluation of the project (a researcher developed questionnaire with likert-type responses

on questions of preference such as enjoying the books, enjoying the activities, or finding

them helpful), a teacher qualitative teacher survey (a researcher developed survey of their

assessment of the FLB project), and teacher anecdotes (notes from teachers of informal

parent reactions to the FLB project). The authors report increases in reading frequency

between pre (n=1712) and post (n=1010) parent reading at home questionnaires with no

mention of the attrition (41%), the mean differences were not statistically significant.

The authors suggest that this is because reading frequencies were high within pretest

responses. The authors qualitative analysis of parents’ open-ended responses to their use

of the FLB project bags was insightful as they found parents’ preference for the bags

related to 4 main themes: organization (the bags provided structure in in-home literacy

interactions), information (the bags provided new insights into how to promote learning

through in-home literacy interactions), engagement (the bags were fun), and opportunity

(the bags provided new opportunities for in-home literacy interactions).

Linder and Emerson (in revisions) studied the influence of a 5-week intervention

of math take-home play bags on nine Head Start families of three and four year olds in

the southeastern United States. The authors measured parents’ early childhood

mathematics beliefs and practices as well as parent-child play interactions before and

after a take-home play bag intervention. The math take-home play bags, including a tip

sheet guiding parents through the theme and materials, children’s book, manipulatives,

and a feedback sheet tailored to the mathematic content theme of each bag, aimed at

Page 52: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

40

promoting family mathematic play interactions. The authors found the take-home math

bag intervention influenced Head Start families’ math play. Comparisons of emergent

codes from the parent-child play interactions before and after the intervention revealed

increased parent’s math questioning, parent’s motivation towards math play, and parent

involvement styles. Linder and Emerson (in revisions) contributes original insights into

the potential for themed play bags to influence parent-child play interactions. Moreover,

the parent interviews cast new insights into Head Start parents’ challenges with work-life

balance and preference for the bags as a more flexible delivery system within their busy

schedules. Although the intervention focused on the content area of math, the “non-

intrusive intervention” design situated within home contexts and comparing parent-child

play interactions as indications of intervention influence inspired the design of the Project

Play Pack bags. This work directly informed the Project Play Pack intervention.

These designs have yet to be applied to parent involvement interventions focused

on increasing play specifically or approaches to leaning themes. Parent workshops,

family focused interventions, targeting particular aspects of social emotional

development (like reducing externalizing behaviors) have had some positive influence

among preschoolers of at risk families (Gross, Fogg, Webster-Stratton, Garvey, & Grady,

2003; Webster-Stratton, 1998; Webster-Stratton et al., 2001, reviewed previously).

However, these designs have also experienced challenges with intervention enrollment

and retention (Webster-Stratton et al, 2001). Furthermore, interventions that take parents

away from their children, through long hours of training leave them with even less time

to play which contradicts the needs of at-risk families. A meta-analysis by Kaminski et

Page 53: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

41

al. (2008) highlights how parent training programs that utilized parents

interacting/practicing new skills with their children garnered larger effect sizes than those

that did not. The need for dynamic family involvement initiatives for Head Start families

remains. A gap exists within the literature describing a strong research design for a take-

home bag intervention targeting home-based parent involvement. Now, we must turn our

attention to the literature regarding how social emotional development and Approaches to

Learning skills could be facilitated through home-based parental involvement.

Achievement and Approaches to Learning

Achievement and Social Emotional Development

The benefits of home-based parent-child play lend important support for young

children’s early social emotional development and Approaches to Learning behaviors.

Social skills and understandings make early learning experiences (in-school settings)

easier to traverse (Curby, Brown, Bassetta & Denhama, 2015). Parent-child interactions

lay the foundation for the social emotional demands of early learning settings.

Regardless of whether researchers are referring to social emotional skills, knowledge, or

competence, the assertion of social emotional foci’s relevance in early learning success is

clear (skills: Baker, 2013; Smith & Wladen, 2001; knowledge: Denham, 1998;

competence: Denham et al., 2012). Social-emotional development is most commonly

used as an umbrella theme or an antecedent to another more focused term. The

Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning, 2014, defines social

emotional learning as the process of acquiring the skills to recognize and manage

emotions, set and achieve positive goals, appreciate the perspectives of others, establish

Page 54: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

42

and maintain positive relationships, make responsible decisions, and handle interpersonal

situations effectively (Miller, 2015; Yoder, 2014). This definition gives way to a subset

of skills including Approaches to Learning behaviors (Coolahan et al., 2000; Kagan et al.,

1995).

Intervening on social emotional development. Researchers have identified

social emotional development as a precursor to school success a number of interventions

targeting preschool children’s social emotional development through parent involvement.

Barton et al. (2014) conducted a review of literature of classroom-base and family-

focused interventions targeting social-emotional development of young children (0-early

elementary). They found 9 parenting interventions ranging from individual sessions to

group session or a combination, 3 treatments to 24 treatments over the course of a year.

The majority of these interventions used various forms of parent training: sharing of

information on topics such as managing stress or supporting prosocial development,

coaching of parents and children to improve interactions, or designing of individualized

family service plans to support at risk families. For example, Child FIRST was a home-

based intervention focused on promoting positive parent-child interactions and social

emotional and cognitive development of young children (Lowell, Carter, Godoy,

Paulicin, & Briggs-Gowan, 2011). Facilitators, a clinical team, did almost bi-monthly

home visits with individual families supporting and guiding warm parent-child

interactions. Randomized control trial evaluations of the intervention found participants

to be less likely to have language or externalizing behavior problems. Barton et al.’s

(2014) review of literature and Lowell et al.’s (2011) Child FIRST intervention

Page 55: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

43

highlighted the importance of parent-child level interventions when targeting young

children’s social emotional development. These examples of effective interventions

targeting social emotional development through parents were hopeful. However, the

costs related to parent training, parent coaching, and the design of individualize family

service plans remains prohibitive of replication.

Social Emotional Development and Play

The specific role of play in facilitating young children’s social emotional

development is well established across early childhood disciplines (Coolahan, et al.,

2000; Torres, Domitrovich, & Bierman, 2015). The role of play in supporting children’s

emotional learning follows closely behind (Denham, Bassett, Zinsser, & Wyatt, 2014).

Play naturally facilitates the practice of skills such as recognizing and managing

emotions, setting and achieving personal goals, taking the perspectives of others into

consideration, and fostering positive relationships (Jarvis, Newman, & Swiniarski, 2014).

Many forms of play serve as a natural, developmentally appropriate means for children to

explore and learn in the early learning years (Fisher et al., 2008; International Play

Association, 2014; NAEYC, 2009; Singer, Golinkoff, & Hirsh-Pasek, 2006). Home-

based parent involvement in young children’s learning through active parent participation

and facilitation of play are assets for early learners (Marcon, 1999; Melhuish et al., 2008;

Parker et al., 1999).

Play and Approaches to Learning

Early school success is not only supported by home-based parental involvement

in the form of parent’s teaching at home, but also through the development of strong

Page 56: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

44

learning behaviors among young children that emanate from parent-child at-home play

experiences.

While few would argue the merit of in home parent-child play, methods for

identifying the links between parent-child play and Approaches to Learning are missing.

More research is needed to reveal the complex utility of parent-child play interactions

(MacDonald & Parke, 1984; Lindsey, Mize, & Pettit, 1997). Fantuzzo et al. (1998)

established important links among peer play, social competence, and learning behaviors

among samples of African American Head Start children. However, it did not suggest

links between children’s play behaviors and Approaches to Learning. Empirical work

highlighting the role of young children’s play in Approaches to Learning skill

development is in a nascent stage. Some literature suggests that play facilitates children’s

purposefulness and goal direction (Anderson, 2002). Other work highlights play’s utility

to support exploration and persistence (Dennis & Stockall, 2015). John Fantuzzo and

Paul McDermott contributed extensively to the body of literature regarding peer play

interactions and early learning behaviors among preschool children in early learning

settings reviewed previously in the introduction. Yet, a gap remains in exploration of the

relationship between young children’s parent-child play and their Approaches to

Learning skill development. Given this gap in the literature, it is not surprising that there

is no research describing Approach’s to Learning interventions for young children target

through parent engagement.

Coolahan et al. (2000) studied relationships among Head Start children’s peer

play behaviors, learning behaviors, and problem behaviors. This study utilized a large

Page 57: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

45

sample of diverse Head Start students across 14 Head start centers. Coolaha et al. (2000)

measured preschoolers’ interactive peer play with the PIPPS (Fantuzzo et al., 1995),

preschool children’s learning behaviors with the PLBS (McDermott et al., 1996), and

preschool children’s problem behaviors with the Conners Teacher Rating Scale (CTRS-

28, Conners, 1997). They found that children who displayed positive interactive play

behaviors (social emotional skills) were also actively engaged in classroom learning

(Approaches to Learning skills). Alternatively, children whose teachers reported as

having trouble connecting and playing with peers also had trouble attending to, staying

motivated with, and actively participating with teachers. Finally, children who were

disruptive within play with their peers also had more teacher reported conduct problems.

Play, Approaches to Learning, and parents. It is important to highlight how

parent-child bonding and children’s Approaches to Learning establish parallels here.

Sroufe (2000) describes the preschoolers’ stage of attachment as a launch pad.

Preschoolers are preparing to blast off, now more than other periods in their

development, they are looking for reassurance and praise from parents as their social

world and experiences broaden. At this age, children typically expect parents to explore

and entertain, they thrive on interactive conversations and games (Sroufe, 2000). How

parents and children are playing matters more than what they are playing. Similarly,

“Early childhood teachers recognize that children differ not only in what they know and

are learning, but also in how they approach activities” (Chen & McNamee, 2011, p.71).

This parallel is notable as it aligns these two important constructs playful parent-child

interactions and preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning with Vygotskian theory

Page 58: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

46

(Vygotsky, 1976). Socio-cultural theory also emphasizes the how of interactions and

activities and learning as more important than what a child learns (Vygotsky, 1978,

1999).

Cheng (2015) eloquently wrote, “researchers have indicated that childhood

education should capitalize on interactive and relational ways of learning, that is,

capitalize on children’s natural interests rather than adult determined agendas” (p. 3).

Frequent and rich in-home play could meet parent’s and children’s developmental needs

while simultaneously supporting children’s Approaches to Learning skill development.

But there is no research exploring links among parent-child play and preschoolers

Approaches to Learning behaviors.

Approaches to Learning and Self-Regulation

Approaches to Learning, as a term, could be considered as broad as social

emotional development. It is necessary to discuss more detailed elements of the

multifaceted idea. As described in the introduction, this term encompasses the

inclinations, dispositions, and skills children use in learning: curiosity, creativity,

cooperativeness, and persistence, emotional and behavioral self-regulation, executive

functioning, and initiative. Self-regulation, yet another broad term, plays a primary role

in Approaches to Learning. It should be noted that self-regulation encompasses: the

ability to manage emotions (Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Schultz, Izard, Ackerman, &

Youngstrom, 2001), to regulate attention (ability to process new information and to

develop learning strategies: Fantuzzo et al., 2004; McClelland et al., 2000) and regulate

behaviors (compliance and impulse control: McClelland et al., 2000; Smith-Donald,

Page 59: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

47

Raver, Hayes, Richardson, 2007). Growth in self-regulatory behavior can be observed

among young children as they face the increased demands for attention and control of

action in structured early learning environments (McClelland & Cameron, 2011).

The HSELOF: Ages birth to five (2015) describes Approaches to Learning with

five central domains, defined as the primary areas of early learning and development of

children birth to age five. The 5 domains are helpful for organizing the terms utilized in

this paper to refer to social emotional development and Approaches to Learning skills,

they are: emotional and behavioral self-regulation, cognitive self-regulation (executive

functioning), initiative and curiosity, and creativity (see Table 2.1).

Table 2.1. HSELOF Approaches to Learning Sub-Domains

Emotional & Behavioral Self-

Regulation

Cognitive Self-Regulation

(executive function)

Initiative & Curiosity

Creativity

-displays appropriate expressions of emotions

-seeks assistance amidst intense emotions

-uses coping strategies

-takes turns

-shows control over words & actions when frustrated

-controls impulses (waits, persists, attends)

-displays flexible thinking (problem solves in a variety of ways)

-tries try new things

-selects & gathers materials for an activity

-displays excitement about new activities

-expresses creative ideas or actions

-displays creative problem solving

-asks questions displaying interest and flexible thinking

-engages in pretend play

Page 60: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

48

Two Types of Self-Regulation

Self-regulation is multidimensional (Vohs & Baumeister, 2004; McClelland et al.,

2012). Inquiries regarding self-regulation within learning contexts usually focus on

cognitive dimensions of self-regulation otherwise known as executive function

(McClelland & Cameron, 2012).

However, two types of self-regulation are distinguished between two domains in

the HSELOF. They are naturally similar. However, the emotional and behavioral self-

regulation domain features an explicit focus on children’s ability to manage their

emotions with growing independence and appropriate seeking of support. Moreover, the

corresponding behavioral markers under the emotional self-regulation include:

appropriate expressions of emotions, seeking assistance when emotions are too intense,

and the utilization of coping strategies. Behavioral markers for emotional management

include: demonstrating control over words and actions when in a frustrating situation,

getting quiet even when excited when asked, waiting for turns, and refraining from

aggression.

Secondly, the cognitive self-regulation, or executive function, sub-domain focuses

on impulse control, attention, persistence, working memory, and flexible thinking.

Behavioral markers for this sub-domain include: inhibiting impulses independently or

with adult support, stopping an activity and transitioning to the next calmly, waiting for

appropriate time to share in whole group, asking for materials rather than snatching them,

interesting oneself in focused play, maintaining a focus on the same activity for long

periods of time, or completing tasks despite obvious challenges. Children’s executive

Page 61: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

49

functioning is related to proficiency in math (Blair & Razza, 2007; van der Sluis, de Jong,

& van der leij, 2004), reading abilities (van der Sluis et al., 2004), and academic

achievement (Bienderman et al., 2004).

Behavioral regulation. Behavioral regulation requires cognitively based

operations that fall under a broader umbrella of self-regulatory behaviors (Vohs &

Baumeister, 2004; Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews, & Morrison, 2009). Early learning

contexts are marked by behavioral demands of young children (McClelland & Cameron,

2011). Recent research asserts early learning programing focused on the development of

emotional competence and self-regulation are inclined to result in greater academic

outcomes than programs directed entirely on academic content (Algozzine, Wang, &

Violette, 2011; Blair & Diamond, 2008; Finlon, et al, 2015). Thus, the field of early

childhood education has pivoted towards a fresh attention to the how of young learners’

experiences. Growing national interest in school readiness has increased the focus on the

self-regulatory skills for shaping classroom learning environments (Rimm-Kaufman &

Wanless, 2012). Empirical support for relationships between children’s Approaches to

Learning and later success are emerging. Some researchers link Approaches to Learning

to language skills (Fantuzzo et al., 2004), achievement outcomes (Duncan et al., 2007;

Li-Grining et al., 2010), and reading and math standardized achievement scores (Denton

& West, 2002). Again, it should be noted that Approaches to Learning constructs have

traditionally been bound to classroom settings. However, possible causal relationships

among play and Approaches to Learning skill development need to be further explored

within play settings. The same behaviors that facilitate learning within classrooms

Page 62: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

50

facilitate learning through play. Previous research has yet to consider how Approaches

to Learning skills are expressed and fostered outside of classroom instructional contexts.

Therein, this literature review presented literature on home-based parental

involvement, a variety of home-based involvement intervention models, and emergent

literature relating to Approaches to Learning and Approaches to Learning sub-domains.

All of these summaries lead up to a discussion of an innovative take-home bag home-

based parental involvement model harnessed for supporting Head Start student and

families practice Approaches to Learning skills within in-home play interactions. The

design of the Project Play Pack Approaches to Learning themed take-home play bags was

rooted in the following existing literature.

Facilitating Approaches to Learning Themed Play

Guidance for the creation of Approaches to Learning themed play bags was non-

existent, since there are no examples of Approaches to Learning interventions through

parent-child play. The limited take-home bag interventions noted in the literature review

were mined for intervention details. Qualitative parent feedback from these take-home

bag interventions provided the most important initial insights into what parents of young

children might prefer within the Approaches to Learning themed take-home play bags of

Project Play Pack. Particularly, Dever and Burts (2002) analysis revealed parents’

preferences for take-home bags because the bags provided: opportunity, information,

engagement, and organization. In addition, the body of research that underpins common

early learning setting classroom play practices also set broad examples for what materials

and interactions might facilitate Approaches to Learning skill practice through play. The

Page 63: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

51

social interactions facilitated within play provide children with opportunities to practice

satisfying their wants and needs, controlling the behavior of others and themselves,

engaging in social exchanges, expressing feelings or ideas, utilizing fantasy, and

gathering and sharing information to and from others (Athanasiou, 2007; Craig-Unkefer

& Kaiser, 2002; Zigler & Bishop-Josef, 2004). These examples provide clear

connections between children’s skill development in play and their Approaches to

Learning skills (curiosity, creativity, cooperativeness, and persistence). For example,

symbolic and socio-dramatic play promotes problem solving skills and children's social

competencies (Aurelli & Colecchia, 1996). Structured, challenging, and goal-oriented

activities improve concentration and perseverance (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). These

commonly accepted assertions regarding the importance of specific types of play guided

the selection of Project Play Pack take-home play bag materials. The following

information regarding the selection of Project Play Pack take-home play bag materials

includes supporting literature linking specific types of play with Approaches to Learning

skills.

Children’s Literature

Richardson, Miller, Richardson, and Sacks (2008) provided a concise review of

take-home book bag designs that attempted to capitalize on the power of read-alouds at

home. Read-alouds provided exceptional opportunities for children to learn, ask and

answer questions, explore stories, and engage in related activities (Brand et al, 2014;

Dickinson & Tabors 2001; Green et al. 2002). In addition, reading with young children

provided predictable routines that established and cultivated conversation skills among

Page 64: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

52

young children and their families (Brand et al, 2014; Pentimonti & Justice, 2010;

Neuman & Roskos, 1993). Books selected for read-alouds should be well-illustrated,

quality literature, (Lennox, 2013; Moschovaki & Meadows, 2005) including strong plots

and interesting crafting (Temple, Martinez, Yokota, 2015; Zeece & Wallace, 2009).

Moreover, culturally relevant literature, or texts that support the intellectual, social,

emotional, and political empowerment through the use of appropriate cultural references

and examples, are important to include in early learning settings (Ladson-Billings, 1995;

Schmidt & Lazar, 2011).

Early childhood teachers often extend read-alouds within classrooms by offering

corresponding activities such as: dramatic play props, collage materials, block center

scenarios, etc. (Stadler & Ward, 2005; Stroud, 1995; Tsao, 2008). These book and play

couplings provide opportunities for children and teachers to explore ideas, concepts, or

themes introduced within the literature through play interactions as well as cultivate

literacy rich routines (Zeece & Wallace, 2009). Moreover, research indicates that play

related to stories may encourage critical comprehension skills and children’s love of

stories at the same time (Bellin & Singer, 2006). These strong pairings between quality

children’s literature and play experiences are less common in homes. Therefore, the

Project Play Pack take-home play bags included a children’s book to initiate each play

theme.

Manipulatives

Exploratory materials. Exploratory play involves exploring materials, making

observations, applying your own rules, initiating routines, and interacting with the

Page 65: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

53

initiations of others (Dennis & Stockall, 2015). Exploratory play has been linked to

children’s development of language, communication, and representational abilities (Fein,

1981; Hadeed & Sylva, 1999; Weinberger & Starkey, 1994). Exploratory materials

typically facilitate open-ended play. Open-ended experiences have been correlated with

problem-solving, conceptual understanding, creativity, and enhanced social skills (Bellin

& Singer, 2006; Bergen & Mauer, 2000; Fisher et al., 2008).

Pretending props. Pretend play involves a “pretense” superimposed on reality.

The players intentionally project circumstances, ideas, or assertions on top of reality

(upon objects or people) in the spirit of play (Lillar, 1993; Moore & Russ, 2006). Pretend

play peaks between the ages of three years old and five years old (Singer & Singer,

1992). It has important implications for young learner’s development as players are

pushed to use purposeful language to maintain and advance the pretend play scenario

(Giffin, 1984; Bellin & Singer, 2006). Moreover, pretend play is inherently social,

research has long since linked children who spend more time in pretend play as more

socially active (Connolly & Doyle, 1984; Nielson, 2011).

Pretend play materials should be inspiring (suggestive) as well as dynamic

(changeable). For example, themed prop boxes have been a mainstay of early childhood

classrooms for some time (Myhre, 1993). Prop boxes are collections of materials that are

added to existing dramatic play centers in early childhood classrooms to generate new

interest, invite new explorations, and inspire new dramatic story lines. These boxes often

contain an intentional mix of themed materials and raw materials. Themed materials for

a pizza party prop box might include a pizza box and a pizza slicer. Alternatively, raw

Page 66: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

54

objects should be neutral enough able to be imagined into various things (for example:

large cardboard circles rather than plastic pretend toy pizzas or Play-Doh that can be

molded into a variety of pizza ingredients such as crust or toppings). Pretend play offers

opportunities for practicing independent and collaborative thinking as well as decision

making (Stephens, 2009). Furthermore, pretend play has been correlated with children’s

self-regulation, both executive function and emotional regulation (Bergen, 2002;

Bredekamp, 2005; Bodrova, 2008; Elias & Berk, 2002; Uren & Stagnitti, 2009). While

both causal and correlational relationships between pretend play and self-regulation have

been called into question recently, Lillard et al. (2013) reviewed four promising studies

linking (both correlational and causal) pretend play with emotional regulation (Barnett,

1984; Barnett & Storm,1981; Galyer & Evans, 2001; Moore & Russ, 2008).

Games. Game play is a developmentally appropriate way to provide young

children with opportunities to practice behavioral self-regulation (Tominey &

McClelland, 2011). Rule based games such as Red Light, Green Light help children to

practice behavioral self-regulation (Tominey & McClelland, 2011). Some closed-ended

games like puzzle play and board games (requiring turn taking) facilitate Approaches to

Learning skills such as reasoning, planning, flexible problem-solving, and persistence

(Hyson, 2005; Hyson, 2008). Games that require problem solving allow young children

to examine, investigate, focus, and generalize skills in various situations (Dennis &

Stockall, 2015). Game play has been causally linked to improved behavioral regulation

among preschool children (Ford, McDougall, & Evans, 2009; Landry, Smith, Swank, &

Miller-Loncar, 2000; St. Clair-Thompson, Stevens, Hunt & Bolder, 2010). Game play

Page 67: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

55

can also be considered open-ended when children develop their own games, rules, and

goals (Bekker et al., 2010).

This chapter provided a review of literature on all of the converging elements of

this study beginning with parent involvement research. It then discussed home-based

interventions targeting parent engagement as well as improved early childhood outcomes.

After the limitations of home visiting interventions were reviewed a small number of

take-home bag interventions were noted. Another focus of this chapter was to further

define and present literature on Approaches to Learning, cognitive self-regulation and

behavioral regulation. The chapter concluded by combining multiple lines of literature to

present a rationale for the materials in Project Play Pack take-home play bags, an

Approaches to Learning themed home-based non-invasive intervention seeking to

support families, promote parent-child play, and bolster children’s Approaches to

Learning skills. The next chapter will describe the research methods used to conduct and

study the intervention.

Page 68: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

56

CHAPTER THREE

METHODOLGY

Rationale for Using Mixed Methods Research

The mixed methods design allowed for an in-depth and multifaceted study of the

influence of the intervention on the participants. Johnson, Onwuegbuzie, and Turner

(2007) defined mixed methods research as a combination of “elements of quantitative and

qualitative research approaches (e.g., use of qualitative and quantitative viewpoints, data

collection, analysis, inference techniques) for the broad purposes of breadth and depth of

understanding and corroboration” (p. 123). I preferred this definition because of the way

it featured the necessity of both a qualitative and quantitative approach with the purpose

of corroboration. Moreover, I preferred Johnson et al.’s (2007) conception of mixed

methods research as a powerful third paradigm that “often will provide the most

informative, complete, balanced, and useful research result” (p 129). This conception

lends itself perfectly to my concerns for the exclusively quantitative and exclusively

qualitative work regarding the efficacy of this particular type of home-based parental

involvement intervention. The preceding literature review depicts the division among

this literature between interventions that either focus so intently on quantitatively

measured child outcome changes that they lose sight of the parent involvement they set

out to support or focus intently on the qualitatively measured experiences that they

struggle to make assertions about change as a result of the intervention or intervention

fidelity.

Page 69: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

57

To thoroughly address this study’s research questions, I used an embedded mixed

methods design adapted from Leal et al.’s (2016) (see Figure 3.1). The figure depicts

how parallel strands of data collection (blue rectangles), quantitative and qualitative,

facilitated the comprehensive study of the influence of the take-home play bag

intervention. Specifically, the concurrent collection of both quantitative (focused on

children’s Approaches to Learning skill and family’s implementation of the intervention

within the play bag feedback surveys) and qualitative (focused on parent-child play

observations and family’s perceptions of the intervention) data generated a

comprehensive picture of the participant’s experience of the intervention. This extensive

focus on participants’ experiences rendered the time and resources to include a control

group prohibitive. Nonetheless, utilizing a mixed methods approach improved the quality

of this study’s findings compared to the studies of similar interventions which chose a

singular methodology (Onwuebuzie & Johnson, 2006). This design provided a better

understanding of the participating families’ experiences in Project Play Pack and the

relationship between experiences with Project Play Pack and children’s Approaches to

Learning behaviors.

In addition, the conversion elements of the design allowed for the conversion of

one type of data from one form to another. These points of data conversion were also

considered mixing points. For example, qualitative data was converted to quantitative

data in the case of the parent-child interaction videos in two ways: both parent play

involvement behaviors and children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors were

transformed into quantitative codes. This approach allowed for the consideration of

Page 70: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

58

emergent patterns gleaned from the qualitative lens as well as a quantitative lens for

comparing pre/post interactions within and across families. Teddlie and Tashakkori

(2009) use another term for this type of mixed methods design, a multi-strand parallel

conversion design. This dynamic design allowed for the collection of both qualitative

(QUAL) and quantitative (quan) data collection, comparison, and integration.

Page 71: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

59

Figure 3.1 Project Play Pack Embedded Mixed Methods Design

Embedded Mixed Methods Design (adapted from Leal et al., 2016)

Page 72: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

60

Mixed Method Study Design

The quasi-experimental embedded mixed method design explored the influence

and feasibility of the Project Play Pack intervention (Creswell and Plano-Clark, 2007;

Greene & Caracelli, 1997; Rallis & Rossman, 2003). Embedded is term used to denote

when one methodology is placed within the framework of another (Plano-Clark et al.,

2013). Two types of embedded designs are found within mixed methods research. First,

embedding qualitative work within randomized control trials is a trending way to capture

the hows (or experiences) of participants within experimental research designs. A less

common but published practice is to embed quantitative work within a qualitative study

(Christ & Makarini, 2009; Quinlan & Quinlan, 2010). The later, utilizes quantitative data

to examine change within quasi-experimental research designs as supporting details for

the qualitative rich comprehensive experience description.

In the case of this work, the quantitatively driven sub-research question: does the

take-home pay bag intervention influence children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors

within parent-child play interactions? was dependent upon the overarching qualitative

inquiry exploring how Head Start families’ utilized the intervention in their in-home play

practices. In other words, the quantitative data was embedded within the qualitative data.

First, the rich descriptions of parent perceptions of the take-home play bag intervention

(qualitative) told a story about families’ understanding and use of the intervention.

Second, the quantitative data (closed-ended weekly feedback form responses, play pack

take-home bag ranking task, and transformed parent-child play observation videos)

supported the detailed experience depiction by reinforcing qualitative themes and

Page 73: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

61

indicating change among children’s Approaches to Learning behavioral expression. I

used a variety of data sources and analysis to study the intervention (see Table 3.1 for the

alignment between project objective and data sources).

Table 3.1. Project Play Pack objectives and data collection strategies. Evaluation Objective Data Collection Strategy and Specific Data Source

Objective # 1: Implement Project Play Pack (Approaches to Learning themed take-home play bags) with families in Head Start

(1) Unobtrusive-quan- Participation/Retention: a. Number of bag cycles participated, b. completed feedbacks sheets returned, c. pre and post data collection meeting attendance

Objective # 2: Explore the influences of Project Play Pack on Head Start families play

(2) Questionnaire-quan- Pre & Post Play Frequency forms (3) QUEST-MM- Efficacy of Play packs: Feedback sheet in

each play pack (close ended: play frequency and open-ended: play extension)

(4) Observation-QUAL – Parent’s play involvement: 10 min videoed parent-child play observation

TRANSFORMED

Objective # 3: Explore the influences of Project Play Pack on Head Start children’s Approaches to Learning

(5) Observation-QUAL- Child’s Approaches to Learning: 10 min videoed parent-child play observation TRANSFORMED

Objective # 4: Describe Head Start families’ experience of Project Play Pack

(6) Questionnaire -MM- Parent and Child perception of play packs: Feedback sheet in each play pack (close ended: novelty/ease of use/ purposefulness/fun (y/n) and open- ended: ex: favorite part)

(7) Interview-MM- Parent and child interview: Semi-structured: preference rank of bags (quan), reflections on reasons for preference rank (QUAL), insights about in-home interactions with bags (QUAL)

*Table constructed from the example of Luo and Dappen, 2005 in Teddlie and Tashakkori, 2009

Page 74: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

62

Context

The Project Play Pack intervention took place within two Head Start centers in

South Carolina. In South Carolina, 24 % of children were living in poverty in 2015

(Annie E. Casey, 2017). Families with children represented 50% of households in the

city where Project Play Pack took place. Among those families, 30.5% of household

incomes are at or below the federal poverty threshold (ESRI Business Analyst, 2016).

I selected two Head Start center sites for convenience (primarily for the logistics

of rotating the play bags among families within centers). See Table 3.2 for Head Start

Center compositions. Four-year-old classrooms were targeted (two from the larger Head

Start center and the only four-year-old classroom from the smaller Head Start center).

Families with three, four, or five-year olds from Head Start participated in the

intervention.

Table 3.2. Head Start Center Demographics

Center A Center B

Number of HS Children 53 17

Number of HS Classrooms 3 1

Race/Ethnicity

African American

Bi-Racial

Latino/Latina/Hispanic

White

44

5

4

0

16

1

0

0

Page 75: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

63

Participants

Project Play Pack utilized a three-tiered approach to participant recruitment.

First, I contacted the SHARE Head Start administration, local Head Start Grant Fundee.

I requested access to convenient centers with which I already had a relationship. Second,

I had meetings with each respective center directors and teachers to explain the

intervention as well as center, teacher, and participating family expectations. Third, the

directors, teachers, and principal investigator worked in partnership to recruit families by

distributing an interest letter to families in selected classrooms. Interest forms were

distributed to 37 students across the two Head Start classrooms. Eighteen families

returned forms on time. Fifteen families (parent-child dyads) consented to the project

and were available to attend the two after school data collection meetings. Finally,

interested families completed consent forms. Cell phones calls and text messages

coordinated data collection scheduling. The quasi-experimental research design utilized

a convenience sample. While the convenience sampling was not ideal for drawing

conclusions, this intervention design and limited human resources necessitated the

prioritizing of implementation logistics.

This convenience sample included 15 children, 7 females and 8 males (see Table

3.3). The majority of parents identified their children as Black/African American (n =

12), the second subgroup was parents who identified their children as Hispanic (n = 2),

the smallest subgroup was more than one race (n = 1), none of the families identified as

White/Caucasian. Two of the 15 participating children’s first language was Spanish.

Parents’ level of education varied from some high school (n = 2), to High School

Page 76: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

64

graduate/GED (n = 2), to some college (n = 8), to an associate’s degree (n = 2), to a

bachelor’s degree (n = 1). Parents’ employment ranged from unemployed (n = 3), to part

time (n = 3), to full time (n = 6), to full time exceeding 40 hours (n = 1), to more than

one job (n = 2) (See Table 3.4).

Table 3.3 Demographic Characteristics of Head Start Children (N = 15) Characteristic n %

Gender

Female 7 45

Male 8 55

Age (all born in 2012)

3 1 7

4 10 67

5 4 26

Race

White/Caucasian 0 0

Black/African American 12 80

Hispanic/Latino 2 13

More than 1 response 1 7

Primary Language

English 13 84

Spanish 2 13

Head Start Teacher

Ms. Anthony 5 34

Ms. Timms 8 53

Ms. Fitzgerald 2 13

Page 77: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

65

Table 3.4. Demographic Characteristics of Head Start Parents (N = 15) Characteristic n % Gender

Female 14 93 Male 1 7

Role

Mother 12 80 Father 1 7 Grandmother 2 13

Race

White/Caucasian 0 0 Black/African American 12 80 Hispanic/Latino 2 13 More than 1 response 1 7

Primary Language

English 13 84 Spanish 2 13

Education

Some High School 2 13 High School Graduate / GED 2 13 Some College 8 53 Associates Degree 2 13 Bachelor 1 7 Masters 0 0

Employment

Unemployed 3 20 Part time (20 hours or less) 3 20 Full time (40 hours) 6 40 Full time plus (more than 40 hours) 1 7 More than 1 job (more than 40 hours) 2 13

Page 78: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

66

Intervention

The home-based intervention, a weekly take-home play bag delivery, provided

novel materials conveniently delivered to families in order to: (a) stimulate parent-child

interactions during play interactions, (b) promote parent involvement in play, and (c)

facilitate the development of Approaches to Learning skills among Head Start children.

The take-home play bags capitalized on children’s natural interest in playing as well as

play’s natural function in supporting Approaches to Learning skill development. The

weekly delivery of a new take-home play bags, every Friday for six weeks, filled with

Approaches to Learning themed culturally responsive children’s literature, manipulatives,

tip sheets, and feedback sheets, engaged families through convenience and partnership.

The distribution of take-home play bags took place at family’s respective to Head Start

classrooms. Each week a new bag appeared for children to enjoy at home with their

parents. Upon each rotation, families returned the prior take-home play bag with a

completed feedback form indicating how that particular bag was received (liked, disliked,

seen as useful, or materials extended in new ways).

This take-home bag intervention logistical design employed lessons learned from

previous iterations of a math take-home play bag intervention (Linder, 2017; Linder &

Emerson, in revisions). Specifically, the bag content outline of tip sheet, toys, children’s

book, and feedback sheet were informed by the Math Pack intervention (Linder, 2017;

Linder & Emerson, in revisions.

In addition, this this take-home bag intervention work was further informed by the

Triadic Interaction Model (McCollum & Yates, 1994), a model originally designed for an

Page 79: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

67

infant home-visiting program. The triadic interaction model maintained specific attention

to the interactions of the three partners in family engagement interventions: child, parent,

and interventionist. Partnership was paramount because this work maintained an asset

orientation towards supporting Head Start families. The efficacy of parent engagement

interventions utilizing triadic and collaborative designs are established within the

literature (Knoche, Sheridan, Edwards, & Osborn, 2010; McCollum & Yates, 1994;

Sheridan & Kratochwill, 2008). This model intentionally avoids the deficit orientations

which sometimes explicitly or implicitly emerges within interventions for families of

color in poverty as described in Miller’s (2010) reflective piece on her mistaken “if only”

perspective on parent training.

This family centered design prioritized the parent-child interaction as the focus of

the intervention. This triadic interaction model provided a unique fit for Project Play

Pack because it allowed for a flexible implementation of the intervention. Specifically,

the model provided researchers, parents, and children with diverse needs, a flexible

framework for infusing typical play interactions that fit each parent-child pair. The

intervention materials were the same for each participant, however participants were

invited to use them in their own ways after following the tip sheet once. McCollum and

Yates (1994) provide an overview of triadic strategies in their paper describing the

development of parent-infant play groups (see Table 3.5).

Page 80: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

68

Table 3.5. Triadic Strategies for Implementing Project Play Pack Triadic Strategies* Project Play Pack Approach

1. Establish a supportive context. Environment arranged to increase probability of interaction

• Play Pack, take-home bags distributed with materials to create/enrich in-home play environments

2. Acknowledge parent competence. Positive (DAP) behaviors of parent are recognized

• Interest letter and Project Play Pack intervention materials reflect asset orientation throughout. “We know you play with your child, we want to provide convenience and fresh ideas to what you are already doing”

• Tip Sheets offer new approaches with casual language and invite extension “Make it your own”

• Weekly feedback requests to families acknowledges and invites their voice throughout the intervention, and provides a week to week view of their play

3. Focus attention. Aspects of the interaction are expanded on in order to draw parents’ attention to competencies or actions

• Project Play Pack tip sheets provide Approaches to Learning themed: starting points, gentle suggestions, and thought provoking questions for parents to use in conversation and in play with their children

4. Provide information. Information about the child’s developmental agenda is given within the context of play.

• Interest letter and Project Play Pack intervention materials clearly state the agenda of the intervention.

• Parent perception of purpose is compared and contrasted with the intervention intentions through qualitative analysis of open-ended weekly feedback forms and post interviews

5. Model. Interaction roles are modeled by interventionist

• Project Play Pack tip sheets provide specific examples for Approaches to Learning themed parent-child interactions

6. Suggest. The parent is given a specific suggestion as to what to do with the child

• Project Play Pack suggests families try the play pack with the tip sheet ideas at least once before making it their own

• Project Play Pack bags provided a specific impetus for parent-child play

* McCollum and Yates (1994)

Page 81: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

69

These effective strategies for designing family-centered interventions were even

more important when working with a sometimes reluctant population such as families in

poverty. The intentionally structured take-home play bags focused on six Approaches to

Learning themes: patience, creativity, persistence, self-regulation, sustained attention,

and emotional regulation. The take-home play bags allowed for families to explore

experiences and strategies for developing the behaviors that underlie these themes by

playing with waiting, turn-taking, or expressing emotions. The themes aligned with the

2015 HSELOF Approaches to Learning domain. Head Start parent preferences for these

themes originated from interview data collected within the Math Pack project (Linder &

Emerson, in revisions). Head Start parents in the same South Carolina city expressed

desires for their children to be able: to wait and be patient, “to not get discouraged”, to

focus and pay attention, and to be able to express their feelings and needs before entering

kindergarten.

Upon closer examination, similarities emerged among parents’ initial expressions

and the sub-domains and goals of the Approaches to Learning domain of the HSELOF.

The Project Play Pack themes and their alignment with 2015 HSELOF are depicted in

Table 3.6. The themes expressed in the interventions branding are listed first, followed

by the Approaches to Learning labels, followed by explicit connections to the HSELOF

including: sub-domains, goals, and behavioral indicators. These final six themes were

included because of their clear links to either parents’ original expressions (Linder &

Emerson, in revisions) or play bag preference feedback (Emerson & Linder, in press) and

Page 82: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

70

the HSELOF. Lastly, these Approaches to Learning themes are easily tailored to in-

home play experiences as detailed below.

Page 83: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

71

Table 3.6. Project Play Pack Themes and connections to Head Start Standards

Pack# 1 2 3 4 5 6 Brandin

g Waiting Creativity Facing a

challenge Turn Taking Focus Feeling Words

A.to L. links

PATIENCE CREATIVITY PERSISTENCE BEHAVIORAL-REGULATION

SUSTAINED ATTENTION

EMOTIONAL REGULATION

2015 Head Start

Standard Connect.

Emotional & Behavioral Self-

Regulation Child manages actions, words, and behaviors with increasing independence

• Demonstrates control over actions & words amidst challenges

• Manages behaviors according to expectations

• Refrains from aggressive behavior towards others

Creativity Child uses

imagination in play and

interactions with others

• Engages in social and pretend play

• Uses imagination with materials to create

• Uses objectives or materials to represent something else during play (pretend play)

Cognitive Self-Regulation (Executive

Functioning)

Child persists in tasks

• Completes tasks that are challenging or less preferred despite frustration, either by persisting independently or seeking help

• Returns with focus to an activity or project after being away

Emotional & Behavioral Self-

Regulation Child manages

actions and behaviors with

increasing independence

• Takes turns • Manages

behaviors according to expectations

• Begins to understand the consequences of behavior, can describe the effects their behavior

Cognitive Self-Regulation (Executive

Functioning)

Child maintains focus

and sustains attention with

minimal support

• Maintains focus on activities for extended periods of time 15 min or more

• Engages in purposeful play

• Attends to adults

Emotional & Behavioral Self-

Regulation Child manages emotions with

increasing independence

• Expresses emotions appropriate to situation

• Seeks assistance when emotions are too intense

• Utilizes a range of coping strategies

Page 84: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

72

This take-home bag intervention was designed to provide families with

reoccurring encouragement and support to engage in more frequent in-home play.

Participants received six weeks of novel materials and ideas for in-home play interactions

which they were invited to adapt and extend to fit their families home environment (see

Table 3.7 for details of take-home bag contents). Selection criterion for the take-home

play bag elements are described in the next paragraphs.

Take-Home Play Bag Composition

Children’s literature. I selected children’s literature designed to facilitate

engaging parent-child read-aloud and play activities. The selection criteria for children’s

literature included: suggested practices for classroom read-alouds such as quality

children’s literature (as indicated by story or awards: Fisher et al., 2004), goodness of fit

with Approaches to Learning themes, and culturally representative literature wherever

possible. Research highlighting children’s levels of engagement when they see

themselves in their books motivated a decision to include culturally relevance children’s

literature (McNair, 2011; Myers, 2014; Roethler, 1998; Souto-Manning, 2009). See

Table 3.8 for the selection criterion used for determining the children’s literature for each

bag. The intentional pairing of related children’s literature and play activities provided a

segue for Head Start families to explore new avenues for facilitating play like other take-

home play bag designs (Grande, 2004; Linder & Emerson, in revisions).

Page 85: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

73

Table 3.7. Project Play Pack Bag Contents Pack# 1 2 3 4 5 6 Theme Waiting Creativity Challenge Turn Taking Focus Feeling vocab.

PATIENCE CREATIVITY PERSISTENCE SELF-REGULATION

SUSTAINED ATTENTION

EMOTIONAL REGULATION

TS: Tip Sheet Tip Sheet Tip Sheet Tip Sheet Tip Sheet Tip Sheet Book: Waiting

by Kevin Henkes Waiting is Not Easy by Mo Willems

Pete’s a Pizza? by William Steig

Shortcut by Donald Crews

Yo Yes! by Chris Raschka

They All Saw a Cat by Brendan Wenzel

How are you Peeling? by Freymann & Elffers I Love my hair by Natasha A. Tarpley

Manipulatives

:

-kaleidoscopes (2)

-binoculars (2)

-sand timers (2) -dry erase board -dry erase marker Exploratory (Game Play)

-Play-Doh -plastic pizza slicer -pizza box -chef hats Pretend (Exploratory)

2x24 puzzle set Game Play (Exploratory)

“ker-plunk” game -colander -pom poms & bouncy balls -bbq skewers (points cut off) Game Play (Exploratory)

-chutes & ladders board game Game Play

-sunglasses -google eyes -stress balls (2) -fidget toy (1) -paper & crayons -paper bags Exploratory (Pretend)

FS: Feedback Sheet Feedback Sheet Feedback Sheet Feedback Sheet Feedback Sheet Feedback Sheet

Page 86: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

74

Table 3.8. Selecting Children’s Literature for Project Play Pack Bags

Begin with a Theme Waiting Creativity Challenge Self-

Regulation Sustained Attention

Emotional Regulation

Waiting by Kevin Henkes Waiting is Not Easy by Mo Willems

Pete’s a Pizza? by William Steig

Shortcut by Donald Crews

Yo Yes! by Chris Raschka

They All Saw a Cat by Brendan Wenzel

How are you Peeling? by Freymann & Elffers

I Love my hair by Natasha A. Tarpley

Select a Children’s Book that promotes that theme

Depicts characters waiting

Depicts a father and son in pretend play

Depicts a group of children trying to find a solution to a problem

Depicts two friends of different races making friends with predictable text

Depicts a cat seeing the world and the world seeing the cat in predictable text and story

Depicts emotional vocabulary of varying forms

Quality (good story & engaging illustrations: : Fisher et al., 2004; Wanless & Crawford, 2016)

Caldecott Honor & Theodor Seuss Geisel Honor

Story of a father and son pretending is exceptional

Caldecott Honor

Caldecott Honor

Representation (reflected population race & context: Meyers, 2014; Souto-Manning, 2009; Wanless & Crawford, 2016)

Characters are animals

Characters are primarily of color

Different races are equally represented

Characters are animals

Characters are inanimate objects or primarily people of color

Page 87: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

75

Play pack manipulatives. I selected play materials to promote play experiences

related to Approaches to Learning themes. Particular types of play lent themselves well to

different types of Approaches to Learning theme exploration and skill practice. Different

materials and respective links to Approaches to Learning themes are reviewed below.

Four selection criterion drove play pack manipulatives choices: 1) to support Approaches

to Learning themed play interactions; 2) to have a high probability of novelty among in-

home environments, 3) to seamlessly build upon themes of the paired children’s literature

selection, and 4) to be cost effective. See Table 3.9 for the scheme utilized for selecting

manipulatives for bags. Moreover, the six bags targeted three types of play: exploratory,

pretend, or game play.

The exploratory play materials facilitated both tool based exploration and making,

including: kaleidoscopes, children’s binoculars, sand timers, dry erase boards and

markers, hand held mirrors, sunglasses, google eyes, fidget toys, paper bags, and

construction paper and crayons. Exploratory play and open-ended making materials were

harnessed to promote patience (through problem-solving, conceptual understanding, and

creativity) for the emotional and behavioral regulation Approaches to Learning skill in

the Waiting take-home play bag.

The pretending play materials facilitated pretending scenarios that corresponded

with the paired children’s book including: home-made Play-Doh, plastic pizza slicer,

pizza box, chef hats, and puppet making materials. A mix of themed (chef hat, pizza box,

pizza slicer) and raw materials (play dough) promoted pretend play for the creativity

Approaches to Learning skill in the Creativity take-home play bag. Pretend play and

Page 88: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

76

making materials promoted emotional regulation for the emotional and behavioral

regulation Approaches to Learning skill in the Feeling Words take-home play bag.

The game play materials facilitated problem solving and turn taking as well as

provide opportunities for exploratory play extension. These materials included: 2x24

puzzle, chutes and ladders board game, and a homemade ker-plunk game. Three of the

six Project Play Pack take-home play bags harnessed game play. Game play and puzzles

promoted persistence for the cognitive self-regulation Approaches to Learning skill in the

Challenge take-home play bag. Game play and a prediction game promoted behavioral

regulation for the emotional and behavioral self-regulation Approaches to Learning skill

in the Turn Taking take-home play bag. Game play and a board game promoted

sustained attention for the cognitive self-regulation Approaches to Learning skill in the

Focus take-home play bag. Game play is considered open-ended when players are

invited and encouraged to make up their own rules. This invitation was explicit among

the tip sheets designed for these bags. It should be noted that puzzles are traditionally

considered closed ended activities because there is one prescribed goal (complete the

puzzle). However, the 2x24 puzzle activity was selected because it posed the challenge

of working two different puzzles at the same time. This challenge presented an

opportunity for open-ended play as there were a variety of avenues for children to take

when sorting, choosing between on puzzle or the other, and solving the problem of two

puzzles found in one box. Nonetheless, it must be noted that the Challenge back was the

least open-ended of all of the take-home play bags in the Play Pack intervention.

Page 89: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

77

Table 3.9. Selecting Manipulatives for Project Play Pack Bags

Begin with a Theme Waiting Creativity Challenge Self-Regulation

Sustained Attention

Emotional Regulation

-kaleidoscopes (2) -binoculars (2) -sand timers (2) -dry erase board -dry erase marker

-play dough -plastic pizza slicer -pizza box -chef hats

2x24 puzzle set

“ker-plunk” game -colander -pom poms & bouncy balls -straws

-chutes & ladders board game

-mirror & sunglasses -google eyes -stress balls (2) -fidget toy (1) -paper, crayons, paper bags

Type of Play Exploratory (Game Play)

Pretend (Exploratory)

Game Play (Exploratory)

Game Play (Exploratory)

Game Play Pretend (Exploratory)

Supports Approaches to Learning play through

Focus on time Facilitate pretending

Facilitate a challenge

Promote turn taking Promote attending Facilitate expression of feelings

Novelty Binoculars were child sized and provided in pairs

Play dough was homemade and scented with oregano

The puzzles were included in the same box

This game was homemade

Mirrors, the stress balls were animals, the fidget toys were exceptional

Closely linked to literature

These materials provided tools for waiting races or eye spy games that children and families could do looking out their own windows

These materials were a spin-off of the pretend play portrayed in the book.

The puzzles presentation presented a natural challenge.

This game promoted back and forth turn taking with a quick and exciting end (where the bouncy balls and pom-poms fall out).

This game required attention to multiple players, facilitated paying attention as the players of the board game.

Materials provided opportunities for children and parents to act out emotions through facial expressions or puppets.

Page 90: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

78

tip sheets. Tip sheets in each take-home play bag provided gentle suggestions for

parent-child play interactions relating to each bag theme and materials (see Appendix A).

Interventions which provide parents with specific instructions and suggestions for

interaction with and questioning of children have been more effective in positively

influencing children’s learning than other forms of intervention (Kroesbergen & van Luit

2003; Linder & Emerson, in revisions; Niklas, Cohrssen, & Tayler, 2016). Each Project

Play Pack take-home bags included a single page tip sheet. All tip sheets followed the

same structure. The tip sheets provided ideas for parents to support play and facilitate

specific Approaches to Learning strategies. Importantly, the tip sheets featured casual

language and open invitations for parents and children to make the experience their own

and expand the activity within their home environment.

feedback sheets. Feedback sheets also accompanied each take-home bag to

collect participant feedback throughout the intervention (see Appendix B). The triadic

interaction model (McCollum & Yates, 1994) provided the guidance that values the

partnership among children, parents, and interventionists. The construction of rapport

continued throughout the intervention through this dual purposes of the feedback sheet.

The feedback sheets captured data regarding the participant’s perception of the

intervention as well as participant’s implementation of the intervention. More

importantly, the feedback sheets repeatedly conveyed the message of partnership to

families. The formatting of the sheet attempted to communicate the sentiment of shared

interests in the intervention efficacy as well as the intervention experience. The feedback

Page 91: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

79

sheets followed the same structure each week. The feedback sheets provided families

with the opportunity to respond to both survey items and open-ended prompts.

Data Collection

Data were collected during meetings with pairs of parents and their children

before and after the intervention. These meetings lasted between fifteen and twenty

minutes and took place after Head Start dismissal. The pre and post meetings included

the completion of a play frequency form (collecting information on in-home play during

the week prior to the meeting and distributing the form for recording in-home play

information in the week following the post meeting) and a parent-child play interaction

observation (10-minute recording of parent and child playing together in an empty Head

Start classroom). In addition, participants also completed a feedback sheet each week

with each play bag. Finally, a post intervention parent interview [semi-structured

interview protocol (Appendix C) and play bag preference rank activity (Appendix D)]

was included as an additional measure at the post intervention meetings. Figure 3.2

provides a linear representation of the data collection schedule.

Research Team

I utilized a team of undergraduate creative inquiry students in parts of the

intervention and data collection. Creative inquiry is a program at Clemson University

that promote undergraduate student exposure to research by supporting small research

projects. The creative inquiry program provides course credit hours to undergraduate

students and funding for research. A team of 7 creative inquiry students (2 returning and

Page 92: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

80

5 new students) attended trainings and assisted in the weekly rotation of take-home play

bags and mobile laboratory set up, recordings, and take-down during pre- and post-data

collection sessions at the Head Start centers. All creative inquiry students completed

Collaborative Institutional Training Initiative (CITI) training on responsible research

practices. The intervention IRB named the assisting students.

Data Sources

Play frequency form. To examine the frequency of in-home play interactions,

parents completed an In-Home Play Frequency Form (see Appendix E) reporting on their

in-home play prior to and following the intervention. The form was an adaptation of a

Figure 3.2. Data Collection Schedule

Intervention ASSESS

Pre Data Prep Week

Pack 1

Pack 2

Pack 3

Pack 4

Pack 5

Pack 6

ASSESS Post Data

Consent Forms Sent

-- Consent Forms

Collected ---

Pre-data collection scheduling

--- Pre-play

frequency forms

distributed

Family

Demographic

Questionnaire

-- Parent-

child Play observati

ons --

PFF

Week

1 F.Back sheets

Week

2 F.Back sheets

Week

3 F.Back sheets

Week

4 F.Back sheets

Week

5 F.Back sheets

Week

6 F.back sheets

returned --

Parent interview

s --

Parent-child Play observati

ons --

PFF distribute

d

WK1 WK2 WK3 WK4 WK5 WK6 WK7 WK8 WK9 WK10/11

Page 93: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

81

pencil and paper form from a previous study (Choi, 2013). The form requested parents to

report time spent playing with their children across seven days of the week. In addition,

the form requested parents to report their work schedule for each corresponding day. The

addition of the work schedule, a change to Choi’s (2013) design, provided a context for

many Head Start parents’ limited times to play. This information provided the

opportunity to consider play to work ratios. For each of the seven days of the week,

parents reported play time and work scheduling (if applicable). The play frequency form

quantitative data answered research question two, regarding the influence of Project Play

Pack on parent-child play.

Parent-child play interaction observation. To explore changes within parent-

child play before and after the intervention, 10-minute parent-child play sessions were

recorded in families’ corresponding Head Start center after school. This parent-child

play observation video protocol was adapted from careful consideration of the wide

variety of parent-child play observational assessments established within the fields of

clinical psychology and early childhood education. Published observational protocols

differ in length of observation (ranged from 3 to 25 minutes), play materials (prescribed

bags of toys versus a variety of open-ended items to choose from), environments

(occurred homes or laboratory settings), and play activity prescription (defined parent-

child choice or researcher prescribed tasks) (Brestan-Knight & Salamone, 2011; Noeder,

2015; Short et al., 2011).

I decided the length of the observation should be 10 minutes to align with

established, two national research projects: Love et al. (2005)’s adaptation of the Three

Page 94: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

82

Box coding protocol from the National Institute of Child Health and Human

Development and the Early Child Care Research Network (1997) evaluation study of

Early Head Start including a 10-minute session. Similarly, Lindsey et al. (1997) used a

10-minute video protocol to explore mutuality in parent-child play interactions within a

laboratory setting with 5 year olds. Some researchers utilized protocols with longer play

periods including warm up periods. However, Thornberry and Brestan-Knight (2011)

found no statistical differences between the warm-up periods and target play periods of

parent-child play observations, suggesting that warm up session extensions are

unnecessary. I did not include a formal warm-up period within the play observation

session in an effort to make the data collection visits as quick as possible to meet the

challenges of Head Start family schedules. I did however ensure that the recording

equipment was set up and the research team had left the room before starting the 10-

minute timer. I did not include session interruptions such as child bathroom breaks in the

10-minute count. At the end of the play session, the research team thanked the parents

and children and made it clear that the same adult would need to be present for the post-

data collection observation (for example, mom both times, dad both times, or grandma

both times).

I left the choice of play materials and activities to the discretion of the parent-

child dyads. I set up cameras in the dramatic play or block centers, in an effort to save

time, but the team offered to adjust for parents and children to select any area to play.

Some established protocols utilize semi-structured protocols prescribing toys, order of

play, or clean up tasks. For example, Love et al. 2005 presented parent and child (3 years

Page 95: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

83

old) with two bags of toys and instructed them to play however they wished, but in a

specified order, a semi structured play protocol. Alternatively, Noeder, Short, Li, and

Cooper’s (2011) Parent-Child Play Scale protocol included a specific selection of toys

with defined shapes (such as a car or a horse) and a choice of several open-ended objects

(ball or cups) and instructions to play as they would at home, loosely structured play

protocol. However, because play is naturally unstructured (Lewis, 2009), self-motivating

(Nell et al., 2013), and best when enjoyed, the Project Play Pack intervention study

utilized a protocol that provided the greatest possibilities for ecological validity and open-

ended play experiences by providing a variety of toys, activities, and play opportunities

(Kelly-Vance & Ryalls, 2005). I invited participants to select a center within the Head

Start classroom that looked appealing to them and to play there as they would typically

play together at home. The pleasure of play is the driving force behind parent and child

engagement (Roopnarine & Davidson, 2015).

This study utilized a mobile laboratory in three empty Head Start (4-year-old)

classrooms in the two participating centers. Multiple classrooms allowed for work with

three families simultaneously as close to dismissal time as quickly as possible. Many

families requested exact dismissal time meetings because they needed to complete our

meeting in time to travel to another school to collect another child. Each mobile lab

included two video cameras (a primary camera connected to a lapel microphone

capturing parent-child discussion audio and a secondary back up camera), a voice

recorder (as a backup audio source), and a go pro camera linked via password protected

Page 96: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

84

Wi-Fi to a MacBook Air in the hallway which allowed for the team to surveil the play

interaction recording without being present in the room.

Finally, I made the decision to have the parent-child dyads play with the Head

Start center toys rather than providing the Play Pack manipulatives to ensure the most

natural interaction. The pleasure and enjoyment of play begins with the freedom to

choose play materials and activities (Nell et al., 2013). Moreover, the Play Pack take-

home bag manipulatives were selected to be novel. In an effort to capture parent-child

play, I avoided presenting parents with novel toys and asking them to play as they usually

do (which would be difficult if they have never played with the provided toys before).

Rather than training parents to play with specific manipulatives, this project aimed to

incite and enrich established parent-child play patterns. In both cases, this protocol

allowed parents and children choice in material selection and play in ways that were

representative of their typical at-home play habits. The parent-child play interaction

observation qualitative data answered research question two regarding parent’s play

involvement before and after the intervention and children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors within parent-child play interactions before and after the intervention.

Weekly feedback forms. To collect both quantitative and qualitative data from

each family’s weekly experience with each play pack, I designed weekly feedback forms

informed by previous iterations of take-home play bag interventions (Emerson & Linder,

2017; Linder, 2017; Linder & Emerson, in revisions). The weekly feedback forms

included a mix of closed-ended yes/no responses and open-ended short answer questions

(see Appendix B). The format and questions repeated across the different bags, with only

Page 97: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

85

slight changes in wording to refer to the appropriate bag theme or task. Results from

pilot studies revealed strong family preference for turn-taking bags (self-regulation

theme) featuring game play (Emerson & Linder, in press). Moreover, the pilot study

work results highlighted the novelty of these particular themes and activities. The weekly

feedback forms answered research question one and two regarding parent perceptions of

Project Play Pack and the influence of Project Play Pack on parent-child play.

Post-Data collection. I distributed the play frequency form for families to report

daily parent-child play interactions in the forthcoming week (without the Project Play

Pack take-home play bags) and collected parent-child play observation videos at the post-

data collection meetings. In addition, I collected the following data.

Play Pack take-home bag ranking. To examine family’s preferences for the play

bag themes and materials I developed the Play Pack take-home bag ranking task. This

ranking task was the first step of the post-data collection meetings. Audio recording

began and I presented a single page with the images of the six Play Pack bags and

provided parents with post-it notes (from 1 – 6) to identify their ranked preference for the

different take-home play packs (see Appendix D). Parents labeled a favorite book and a

favorite toy if applicable. The rankings generated quantitative data. I included the

dialogue within the interview analysis (qualitative). The weekly feedback forms

answered research question one regarding parent perceptions of Project Play Pack,

research question two regarding the influence of Project Play Pack on parent-child play,

and served as a visual refresher of the take-home play bag experience to kick off the post-

data collection session.

Page 98: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

86

semi-structured interview. To explore the in-depth experience of Project Play

Pack, I developed a semi-structured interview protocol for parent’s post intervention

interviews. The protocol included questions about parent’s preference for or aversion to

different play packs and how they used the play packs in their home environment, to

ascertain parent perceptions of the play pack intervention in their own words (see

Appendix C). The protocol was researcher designed based of prior iterations of this work

(Emerson & Linder, in press; Linder & Emerson, in revisions). Mixing occurred within

the interview when I referenced key elements of the families’ weekly feedback forms for

expansion or clarification. This post intervention interview served as an additional

opportunity to share power with and listen to participating families. The semi-structured

interviews answered research question one regarding parent perceptions of Project Play

Pack as well as research question two regarding the influence of Project Play Pack on

parent-child play.

Specifically, this mixed methods design and these data sources worked to answer

the following research questions:

1. What factors enhance or inhibit the feasibility of the Project Play Pack take-home

play bag intervention? (QUAL & quant mixed: Weekly Feedback Forms, Play

Pack take-home bag Ranking Task, and parent post interviews)

A. Do parents find the bags to be novel, useful, enjoyable? (quant)

(Weekly Feedback Forms survey responses)

Page 99: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

87

B. How do parents describe the purpose of the bags for their families?

(QUAL) (Weekly Feedback Form open-ended responses & parent post

interviews)

C. How do parents describe the support of the take-home play bag

intervention? (QUAL) (Parent post-intervention interview)

2. To what extent does the Project Play Pack take-home play bag intervention

influence parent-child play among Head Start families? (QUAL & quant mixed:

Weekly Feedback Forms, Play Pack take-home bag Ranking Task, Play

Frequency Forms, & parent post interviews; QUAL to quant transformation:

Parent-child play observation videos transformed)

A. Does the take-home play bag intervention influence parent-child play?

• Does the take-home play bag intervention increase the frequency

of parent-child play interactions? (Mixed: parent post-intervention

interviews & Play Frequency Forms)

Does the take-home play bag intervention increase parent’s play

involvement in parent-child play interactions? (QUAL to Quant

transformation: Parent-child play observation videos, Parent Play

Involvement Coding Tool: Tejagupta, 1991; Watts & Barnett,

1973)

B. Does the take-home play bag intervention influence children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions?

Page 100: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

88

Which Approaches to Learning behaviors do children display

within parent-child play interactions? (QUAL: Parent-child play

observation videos emergent coding)

Do Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play

increase as a result of the intervention? (QUAL to Quant

transformation: Parent-child play observation videos, researcher

developed Play Approaches to Learning Behaviors Observation

tool (PALBO))

Data Analysis

Research Question 1

To examine participant descriptions of their experiences of the intervention, their

perceived purpose of the intervention, and whether or not the intervention provided their

family with needed support, I individually analyzed the weekly feedback forms

(quantitative and qualitative data separately), play pack take-home bag ranking task, and

parent post-intervention interview.

To examine parents’ assessments of the take-home play bags as novel, useful, or

enjoyable, I generated percentages of the dichotomous variables to compare quantitative

data among the six bag types from the weekly feedback form survey responses. The

weekly feedback forms revealed preferred features of the Project Play Pack take-home

play bags. I used this quantitative data to explore possible correlations between play-bag

feature preferences and outcome measures in the mixing phase. In addition, the take-

home bag ranking task provided information about family preference of play bag themes

Page 101: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

89

and provided exploration of similarities and differences among Head Start family

preferences. I generated comprehensive preference scores for each of the six bags by

summing the frequency of each family’s weighted ranking collected by the take-home

bag ranking task.

To explore parents’ description of their perceived purpose and support of the

bags, I conducted multiple emergent coding passes on the open-ended weekly feedback

form responses and the parent post interviews. Emergent coding was used to analyze

these open-ended feedback form prompts. First, I conducted a complete read of the

responses. I concluded that respondents gave relevant responses for each item which

made it logical to consider them individually. Second, I considered responses to each

item within each bag together for common themes (for example: all participant responses

for the understanding/misunderstanding prompt for the Focus bags were considered

together and themes were generated). Next, I generated summaries for each item within

each bag and they were combined (for example: a summary was generated for each item

on the Focus bag forms and those 5 item summaries were used to generate a

comprehensive synopsis of the experience of the Focus bag). Finally, I generated

summaries for each item across all six bags for a comprehensive depiction of perceived

purpose and support. In addition, interview data were recorded and transcribed. I

completed a full read of all interview transcripts. I used an initial emergent coding to

analyze this post-intervention interview data. My second pass noted emergent patterns

across the data (see Appendix J for an example of second pass coding). The interviews

did not produce one to one responses. In other words, every participant didn’t provide an

Page 102: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

90

explicit response to any number of specific prompts. Therefore, quantifying these results

would be inappropriate. Emergent themes and patterns within and across participants

were utilized in the mixing phase.

Research Question 2

To examine if Project Play Pack take-home play bags promoted parent-child play

interactions the play frequency forms quantitative data revealing parent-child play the

week before and the week after the intervention was analyzed and compared. Only four

parents provided play frequency forms for the week following the intervention.

Unfortunately, it was difficult for participants to submit play frequency form the week

after the end of the intervention. Due to this setback, it was difficult to consider pre/post

differences in play frequency. Therefore, exploration of the possible influences of

Project Play Pack intervention on parent-child play habits turned toward looking for

changes within play behaviors and among parent description of play changes in the post

intervention interviews. Nonetheless, pre-intervention play frequency forms provided

important contextual information regarding the lives and play patterns of the participating

Head Start families.

To further examine if Project Play Pack take-home play bags promoted parent-

child play, interactions parents were asked directly about changes in play frequency in the

post intervention interview. I asked, “Did the bags allow you to play with your child in

ways that you wouldn’t have usually? Can you give me an example? Did you and your

child play more because you had the bags? Why or why not?” I used the previously

described emergent coding phases to generate common themes among participant’s

Page 103: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

91

responses to these prompts relating to changes in their play frequency as a result of the

influence of the intervention.

To explore if Project Play Pack take-home play bags influenced changes within

parent-child play, the software program ELAN will be used to code video data. This

software allowed for direct video to code links. The software stores video files linked to

time stamped codes. This software allowed for generation and storage of time epochs,

quantitative codes, and qualitative notes for the parent play involvement and child

Approaches to Learning behaviors analysis. The codes were easily exported to Excel and

SPSS.

To explore if Project Play Pack take-home play bags influenced parent

involvement in parent-child play before and after the intervention, I coded the videos for

parent play involvement. The data were transformed into quantitative data using

Tejagupta’s (1991), Parent Play Involvement (PPI) measure. The PPI assessed parents’

involvement in forty 15 second intervals. The PPI prescribed the categorization of each

interval as participation, facilitation, neutral, restriction, or non-involvement. The PPI

prescribed determinations of parent engagement levels with detailed categorization

definitions. For example, the participation categorization was assigned when the parent

was actively engaged in play as a player (examples: making sounds and actions relevant

to play context, using play materials for complementary involvement, showing shared

interest and matching physical movements). Facilitation was determined when the parent

was indirectly encouraging play as an outsider or play leader rather than a co-player

(example: suggesting new ideas, explicitly directing or modeling play, questioning play

Page 104: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

92

or play materials, or praising). Neutral was determined when the parent shows some

interest in the play but was not actively engaged (examples: watching child play).

Restriction was determined when the parent makes negative comments or restrictive

actions toward child or play activity (examples: suggesting the line of play end, telling

the child no, refusing to participate in an offered partnered play). Noninvolvement was

determined when the parent is disengaged (examples: looking at cell phone or doing

something unrelated to child’s play). Each category was summed for number of

occurrences and divided by 40 for percentages (See Appendix 6, example of a coding

sheet).

I coded all of the videos using the Parent Play Involvement (PPI) measure. In

addition, I trained second coders from the research team on the PPI across multiple

coding meetings. First, the trainings discussed the structure and utility of the PPI at

length. Second, I used how-to memos to guide the novice coders through the process of

using the PPI. Next, we discussed the definitions of coding terms and the ways to look

for and identify behavioral markers multiple times as a group. Novice coders received

hands-on coding training by practice coding pilot study parent-child interaction videos.

We discussed conflicting codes in group settings. I encouraged the novice coders to

defend their coding choices among each other and gain a stronger understanding of why

they selected codes and how these perceptions might be biased or incorrect. I recorded

these discussions through memos and we agreed on resolutions based on the coding tool

definitions and parameters. After a number of iterations, we established reliability and I

randomly assigned the coders to the Play Pack pre and post videos. This transformation

Page 105: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

93

of qualitative data (video) into quantitative data (codes and percentages) allowed for

comparison within (post-pre changes among parent-child dyads) and across participants

(post-pre = difference).

To explore changes within among children’s Approaches to Learning behavioral

expressions within parent-child play before and after the intervention, I explored the

parent-child play observation videos for incidents of Approaches to Learning skills

displayed by children. I coded the play observation videos for children’s Approaches to

Learning behavioral expression utilizing two coding phases. First, I used open coding to

uncover emergent patterns of Approaches to Learning behaviors among participants. I

conducted multiple views, noted emergent patterns, recorded variation and consistencies

as consecutive elements of “bottom up coding” (Saldana, 2015). I used informal coding

memos to note examples of Approaches to Learning play behaviors that children

displayed within the videos.

Again, the emergent coding of children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors

within the parent-child play interactions was conducted in two steps. First, I viewed all

of the parent-child play observations as a whole group and reflected upon common

occurrences. Second, I generated coding memos to note examples of Approaches to

Learning behaviors among children within the parent-child play interactions.

Thirdly, Approaches to Learning skills among children were coded from the “top

down” utilizing a researcher developed coding tool. I developed the Play Approaches to

Learning Behaviors Observation tool (PALBO) to describe the frequency of children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions within the pilot

Page 106: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

94

study. I coded all of the videos using the PALBO. A second coder, a trained research

team member, also coded a random selection of 25% of the videos across both

participants and phases(pre/post) with the PALBO.

Page 107: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

95

Table 3.10. Initial Open Codes and Second Grouping of Approaches to Learning Behaviors

First Coding Pass Second Coding Pass

PID General Notes From Video Highlights Examples of CSR

Examples of BSR

Examples of NBR

Example of EMO

Examples of CRE

2 Block Center: - child has some trouble getting settled, - child add cars to wooden block play, child sets a goal for a “big castle” but spends most of his time playing with cars (labeling cars and placing them around the castle. - child shifts play from blocks to tape measure - child ends tape measure, mom shifts to dinosaurs - child shifts play to dinosaur castle/cage including blocks - mom shifts redirects play to dinosaur pretend play (longest period of play) - child shifts play to mini Frisbee, sets the rules of the game, smiles when mom complies, mostly flat affect but calm relaxed disposition

Child isn’t focused on a particular type of play for very long, so examples of BSR are hard to see, when play foci are short lived he isn’t likely to be

challenged, instead of being challenged he shifts focus. Rarely challenged, never

frustrated He takes turns with his mom

without prompting or correcting

Some BSR in dino pretend play because they are play

fighting & the child is playing an aggressive dino, but keeps it

within some limits, he is pretending aggression, when he pauses & looks up his face

is calm & he listens to his mom’s narrative

*doesn’t take aggression out of context, doesn’t have

trouble shifting in & out of it within the pretend play

boundaries*

Measuring tape play,

child selected toy and

goal

Pretend dino play, controlled

pretend aggression

Controlled

Frisbee throws

Getting out

wooden blocks, absent

mindedly playing

and talking

about cars

Smiles, is happy when mom

agrees to play

Frisbee

creativity in constructing

Pretending with dinos

Pretending

when including dinos in Frisbee

Page 108: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

96

Creation of the PALBO. The construction of the PALBO began with the

HSELOF: Ages birth to five (2015) standards, the Learning to Learn (LTLS; McDermott

et al., 2011) teacher report measure of preschool children’s learning behaviors, and the

Scale for Teachers' Assessment of Routines Engagement (STARE; Casey & McWilliam,

2007) observation tool. I carefully compared and contrasted these three established

Approaches to Learning measures to develop a new measure for considering

preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions.

The PALBO categories emerged from commonalities identified among these measures.

First, each of the three established measure included preschoolers’ ability to understand

and comply with developmentally appropriate rules, routines, and expectations while

simultaneously refraining from aggression. The emotional and behavioral self-regulation

sub domains of the HSELOF provided an overarching theme for these abilities.

Moreover, the STARE delineations of active engagement as well as LTLS factors of

effective motivation and interpersonal responses in learning noted these regulatory

abilities. Each of the three established measures supported the relationships between

these forms of within play compliance and preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning.

Second, preschoolers’ ability to control impulses in order to remain engaged in an

activity unites the Head Start Cognitive self-regulation (executive functioning) sub

domain, the strategic planning and sustained focus factors of the LTLS, and the opposite

of the lack of sophistication and complexity noted within the STARE. Again, the three

established measures aligned to support a shared focus on the ways that children are able

Page 109: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

97

to control impulses in order to start, stop, and complete activities as an important

approach to learning skill.

Finally, the HSELOF subdomain of Emotional and Behavioral self-regulation is

the only resource to explicitly focus on the appropriate expression of emotion, above and

beyond the ability to refrain from aggression. Although expression of emotion is not

consistently included across the three Approaches to Learning measures, enjoyment is a

key element of play (Isen & Reeve, 2005). Moreover, Hyson noted “joy, happiness, and

enjoyment” when describing enthusiasm and pleasure’s role in Approaches to Learning

(2008, p 16). It seemed acumen to retain a focus on the appropriate expression of

emotion when considering Approaches to Learning within parent-child play interactions.

When considering interactions between parents and children expressions of positive

emotions are to be expected and perhaps to facilitate the development of positive

Approaches to Learning within play interactions.

I created the PALBO as an inclusive amalgamation in a multi-step process.

First, open coding allowed for children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors in parent-

child play to be freely identified. Second, these emergent codes were grouped, compared,

and contrasted with the PALBO. Careful consideration of the three established resources

and the first open coding round of the parent-child play observation videos resulted in the

development of the PALBO in my pilot study work. I decided to include five final

Approaches to Learning categories: a) emotional/behavioral self-regulation, b) cognitive

self-regulation, c) expression of emotions, d) creativity, and e) sustained attention.

Page 110: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

98

Operational definitions for preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning in play labels are

derived from the three primary resources (see Table 3.11.).

Table 3.11. Operational definitions of preschoolers’ Approaches to Learning in play labels

Emotional/ behavioral self-

regulation Cognitive self-

regulation Expression of

Emotions Creativity Sustained Attention

Child manages actions, words, and behaviors with increasing independence. (emotion is evident within the child’s body language, expression, or speech, primarily within interactions between people)

(Executive Functioning) Child demonstrates an increasing ability to control impulses, maintain focus, and think flexibly & problem solve (less about interactions with people and more about interactions with challenges)

Child manages and expresses emotions in a developmentally appropriate way

Child expresses creativity in making, constructing, or pretending

Child maintains focus on activities and or engages in purposeful play (a goal is clear) for extended periods of times

I completed four independent cycles, or iterations, of code development by

applying the PALBO to the videos and encountering problems in my pilot study work.

After each refinement of the tool, the coding process was restarted. Within these cycles it

became clear that distinctions were needed between emotional, behavioral, and cognitive

self-regulation as they are easily confused when labeling children’s behaviors in play. In

addition to the operational definitions, indicators of the play behaviors for each label

were developed (samples presented in Table 3.12). In addition, it was noted that

sometimes children did not display any discernable examples of any type of regulation,

so a “no example of regulation” code was added. Moreover, children’s expressions of

emotions, creativity, and sustained attention occurred at the same time as many of the

Page 111: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

99

examples of regulation. It became clear that the Approaches to Learning subdomains

were not mutually exclusive, the scaling of the categories needed to vary.

Table 3.12. Behavioral indicators of the PALBO coding categories

Emotional/ behavioral

self-regulation

Cognitive self-

regulation

No Behavioral regulation Examples Expression of Emotions Creativity

Sustained Attention

-successfully matches behaviors to expectations of the play (maintains gentle tosses in an improvised gentle tossing game) -refrains from aggression when frustrated, angry, or sad -describes consequences of behavior (their own or someone elses) -waits when needed -takes turns

-calmly transitions to another activity when prompted by parent -attends to parent when they are giving directions -clearly states and works towards a play goal -returns to a play goal after being interrupted -persists in a task after failing (tower falls, begins again)

-child is engaged in play but does not display clear examples of regulation -child is not engaged in play (wandering, staring, being aggressive, or crying)

Appropriate -appropriately expresses emotions in situation (laughs at something silly; crinkles mouth and opens eyes wide when a tower might fall) -seeks assistance when emotions are too intense (cries and goes to sit in mom’s lap)

Inappropriate -screaming & crying without provocation -inconsolable crying or screaming

-independently using imagination in play -pretending in play -making something (building blocks or manipulating dough into a product)

-type of play and play goal are carried over from the last 30 second time interval

Applying the PALBO to the Project Play Pack intervention data. I

approached the Project Play Pack video data regarding children’s Approaches to

Learning behavioral expressions in the same sequence as I did with the pilot data.

First, I conducted an initial round of emergent coding. I took a fresh look at the

new data for emergent Approaches to Learning behavioral expressions among Project

Page 112: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

100

Play Pack participants. Similar to my pilot study work, the Project Play Pack parent-

child play interactions provided explicit examples of children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors within play. For example, children engaged in creative pretend play, one child

labeled their play dough work as “cookies”. Children displayed cognitive self-regulation

as they paused and resumed play throughout the videos. For example, many children’s

play was interrupted by parent’s questions or prompts (“what color is this?”) which the

child answered calmly and then resumed their original play focus. Children appropriately

expressed emotions when their block tower fell and instead of crying or screaming,

crossed their arms and sighed before beginning again. This open coding pass generated

lists of Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play (see Table 3.10).

Second, I coded all of the parent-child play observations with the PALBO. In

addition, I enlisted a second coder, a research team member, to train on and code the

videos with the PALBO. I conducted coding cycles with the second coder working

towards reliability which lead to a second refinement of the PALBO. Two cycles, or

iterations, of code refinement were conducted by applying the PALBO to the videos and

discussing convergent and divergent codes between coders. Within these cycles it

became clear that behavioral markers needed to be even more clearly defined and

distinguishable because confusion remained when individual coders were labeling

children’s behaviors in play. In addition to clear definitions, indicators of the play

behaviors for each label were made more specific (see example of the coding tool in

Appendix G and samples of behaviors coded into the PALBO coding sheet in the last

coding phase see Appendix H).

Page 113: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

101

The final version of the PALBO did not provide a numerical comprehensive total

of Approaches to Learning behaviors within play interactions. Instead it generated a

clear and organized view of the ways that children displayed Approaches to Learning

behaviors within their parent-child play interactions. Time-sampling truncated the

coding of behaviors within pre-determined epochs of time across the 10-minute parent-

child play observations recordings. Thirty-second play behavior intervals received

separate codes determined by displays of: self-regulation (emotional/behavioral,

cognitive, or no clear example), expressions of emotions (appropriate, none, or

inappropriate), creativity (yes or no), and sustained attention (linked to the previous 30

second play focus, unlinked but unchanged, or changed). The totals for each label of

children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors in play were summed. These numbers were

divided by 20 (the number of time intervals) to generate percentage scores or compared

across parent-child dyads. This transformation of qualitative data (video) into

quantitative data (scores) allow for comparison within (pre-post changes among parent-

child dyads) and across participants (post-pre = difference) using t-test difference scores.

Mixing

The mixing of quantitative and qualitative data occurred in multiple places with

this research. The first mixing occurred when quantitative weekly feedback form data

were drawn upon to inform post parent interviews. The second mixing occurred within

the transformation of the parent-child paly observations video qualitative data into

quantitative codes (Parent Play Involvement Measure and the PALBO). The third mixing

occurred when all of the data sources, quantitative results and qualitative themes, were

Page 114: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

102

mixed according to research question after the individual analysis of each data source had

occurred.

These two parallel, parallel analysis not collection, stands of data (quantitative

and qualitative) were integrated to generate a comprehensive and rich descriptive picture

of the experience of the Play Pack intervention (see a QUAL quan depiction in Figure

3.3).

Figure 3.3. QUAL quan Depiction of Data Sources

The QAUL quan embedded design intentionally prioritized the qualitative data

and utilizes the quantitative data as complementary, supporting elements. This

predetermined research method design decision determined how the analysis should

proceed. Parallel track analysis (Li, Marquart, & Zercher, 2000) served as the analytical

framework for the quantitative and qualitative elements. This analytical framework

established the mechanics for the second mixing phase where I anticipated

complementary triangulation. First, I reduced and summarized each data source

Page 115: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

103

independently. Second, I considered data comparisons and dissimilarities at the

summarized level for each research question. Finally, the comprehensive themes were

considered for each research question and a decision was made regarding if the

triangulation of findings was complementary, convergent, or divergent (Östlund, Kidd,

Wengström, & Rowa-Dewar, 2011). Parallel track analysis set the stage for triangulation

and helped me to consider different interpretations. This framework helped me to ensure

my intended weighting of findings from qualitative and quantitative methods (Datta,

2001).

Legitimation

Onwuegbuzie and Johnson (2006) set forth a legitimation framework for mixed

methods research that fits the quantitative, qualitative, and mixing approaches utilized in

my work. Onwuegbuzie and Johnson (2006) articulated 9 legitimation types, or quality

considerations including: sample integration, inside-outside, weakness minimization,

sequential, conversion, paradigmatic mixing, commensurability, multiple validities, and

political. This study’s quasi-experimental embedded mixed method design demands

particular attention to four of Onwuegbuzie and Johnson’s (2006) legitimation types.

First, inside-outside legitimation refers to the extent to which the researcher

understands the insider’s (participant’s) and observer’s (researcher’s) experiences. My

work intentionally balanced the possible divergence of inside and outside views. These

balances are represented in the research questions where I was interested in Head Start

parents’ perceptions of the purpose of the intervention. The qualitative analysis of both

the open-ended weekly feedback form responses and parents’ responses to the purpose of

Page 116: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

104

the intervention interview question carefully balanced my etic view of emergent themes

and the participants’ explicit emic descriptions. Inside-outside legitimation also appeared

in the midpoint mixing when I drew upon notable quantitative weekly feedback form data

from families to personalize and illicit parent expansion and clarification in the post

intervention interviews. Second, weakness minimization refers to the extent to which the

weaknesses of one approach are compensated for by the other approach. My intentional

research design selection and implementation worked to minimize weaknesses in either

approach. The embedded design was the best fit for generating a rich picture of the

feasibility and the influence of the intervention. The quantitative data provided important

complementary assertions to both rich qualitative pictures of the intervention experience

and participant changes. Next, conversion refers to the extent to which the

transformation of data yields quality conclusions. My work quantitizing (Onwuegbuzie

& Johnson, 2006) the qualitative data was intentionally planned to achieve high inference

quality data. Different techniques were used for different types of transformation with

careful consideration for existing literature’s prescriptions for data transformation and

analysis. For example, the coding tools used to quanititize were either previously

established or deeply rooted in existing literature. Finally, multiple validities refer to the

way in which traditional considerations of validity are considered for each respective

approach: qualitative, quantitative, and mixing. My analysis worked within each

approach and data source individually first in order to ensure order and the unique

validity considerations for each piece. Mixing and meta-inference conclusions generated

a complete picture that contributes more than the individual data sources and approaches.

Page 117: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

105

Conclusion

This chapter addressed the specific methodology and procedures used to answer

my research questions. I presented a rationale for and description of the embedded mixed

methods design as well as descriptions of data collection and data analysis procedures

used in this study. I described the Head Start centers and the research participants. I

provided a sequential depiction and specific description of each intervention element and

procedure. I collected multiple data points to capture the intervention. I described data

collection, analysis, and mixing plans for both quantitative and qualitative pieces. The

following two chapters discuss the results from each individual data source analysis as

well as the combined consideration. Chapter Four presents the results for research

question 1. Chapter Five presents the results for research question 2. Chapter Six

summarizes and frames all of the results with the discussion. Chapter Seven concludes

this dissertation by highlighting the significance and implications of this work.

Page 118: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

106

CHAPTER 4

RESEARCH QUESTION 1 RESULTS

Introduction

This chapter presents results from the individual data analysis of each source

relating to Research Question 1. First, the chapter recaps the challenges that Head Start

families encountered in their attempts to participate in the Project Play Pack intervention.

Next, the chapter describes the analysis of each data source relating to parent perceptions

of the intervention. The result of the mixing of these individual data sources to gain a

rich understanding of the feasibility of the intervention concludes the chapter.

Threats to Head Start Family Participation

Fifteen original sample families attended pre-data collection meetings, but only

thirteen families completed the 6 play pack take-home bag rotation. Two families

dropped out of the study for access reasons. One family struggled with illnesses followed

by family car trouble that resulted in sustained absences from Head Start. They became

unable to receive or return bags and withdrew to focus on family challenges.

Unfortunately, difficult life circumstances often inhibit Head Start families’ ability to

become involved or stay involved in home-school partnerships (Lamb-Parker et al.,

2001). A breakdown of Head Start family participation in the Project Play Pack

intervention is provided in Table 4.1.

The second family became unresponsive in the second week of the project,

unavailable by phone or text and not returning bags. Lamb-Parker et al. (2001) identified

two primary challenges to parent involvement for Head Start families: having (work)

Page 119: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

107

schedule conflicts with activities and having a baby or toddler at home. I confirmed with

the center director that this second family had 6 children, ranging in age from 4 months to

10 years old, the mother was working two jobs, and the mother was diagnosed with a

progressive eye disease which threatened her ability to see during the time of the

intervention. These two families were not included in the results below.

Further, thirteen families completed the take-home play intervention, but four

families had persistent scheduling conflicts that prohibited them from completing the

post-data collection meetings including the post-intervention play observation. This was

not considered attrition because they had a willingness to attend and never expressed

complaints or wishes to withdraw from the project. One was available to complete post-

intervention interviews by phone.

Table 4.1. Head Start Family Participation (n = 15)

Family Pseudonym

Pre Play Freq Form

Pre Data Collection

Number of bags received

Number of

Feedback forms

returned Post Data Collection

Post Freq Form

the Serranos x x 6 6 x the Whitakers x 6 5 the Watsons x x 6 3 phone the O’Neals x x 6 6 x the Myers x x 6 6

the Woodrows x 4 0 the Thompsons x x 6 1 x

the Evans x x 6 6 x x the Garcias x x 6 6 x x the Browns x 6 4 x the Baltzs x x 6 1

the Jimenezs x 6 4 x the Boyds x x 6 5 x x the Millers x x 6 5 x x

the Johnsons x 4 0

Page 120: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

108

Research Question 1: Feasibility

This section addresses data collected to inform the overarching first research

question: What factors enhance or inhibit the feasibility of the Project Play Pack

intervention? Data collection included both qualitative and quantitative methods.

Participant descriptions of their intervention experience, perceived purpose of the

intervention, and whether or not the intervention provided their family with needed

support were considered among the weekly feedback forms, play pack ranking tasks, and

post-intervention interviews.

The next section addresses data that were collected to inform Sub-Question A, do

parents find the bags to be novel, useful, enjoyable?

Weekly Feedback Forms

Fifty-seven feedback forms were returned from the thirteen final families

(response rate of 73%). The families returned an average of 4.54 of the 6 feedback forms

across the six-week intervention, with a range of only 1 returned to all 6 returned. Family

use and assessments of the features (novelty, usefulness, and enjoyableness) of the play

bags were explored by analyzing the weekly feedback forms. Families reported playing

with the play packs an average of 3.19 times a week.

Collectively, families rated the play packs favorably across the following

dichotomous (yes/no) survey items: appropriateness (97% yes), enjoyableness (92% yes),

clarity of instructions (94% yes), opportunity to practice target skill (98% yes),

opportunity to practice talk about target skill (95% yes), and overall novelty (35%).

Correlations among the feedback form items and difference scores of the PALBO and

Page 121: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

109

PPI were explored for possible relationships. Two statistically significant correlations

were found with the average number of times families played with the play packs

(number of times played averaged across all submitted feedback forms). Number of

times played was significantly correlated with the parent play involvement facilitation

difference score, r(8) = .68, p < .05). As the reported average number of times played

with play packs increased the parent play facilitation difference score increased. Number

of times played was significantly correlated with the parent play involvement

participation difference score, r(8) = -.87, p < .01). As the reported average number of

times played with play packs increased the parent play participation difference score

decreased.

In addition, the feedback forms provided important insights into the families’

experiences with each play pack. The averages of the dichotomous play bag ratings are

displayed by play pack theme in Table 4.2. The variation among parent reviews of the

bag themes was small, the reviews were overwhelmingly positive. However, the

Challenge, Focus, and Creativity bags garnered 100% agreement across appropriateness,

enjoyableness, clear instructions and opportunity to practice and talk about target skill.

Conversely, the Waiting, Turn Taking, and Feeling Words bags were more often rated as

novel activities (45%, 60%, and 56 % respectively). The dichotomy between perfectly

high scoring bag themes versus bags that were more often rated as novel was interesting.

The feedback form items reporting families’ assessments of whether or not the play bags

provided opportunity to practice and talk about the target themes were overwhelmingly

positive, 98 and 95 respectively. These results hint towards the support provided to

Page 122: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

110

families through the play bags. Overall, the weekly feedback forms highlight

participants’ positive experience with the different take home play bags.

Table 4.2. Weekly Feedback Forms Theme Variation Waiting N % yes

Times played 8 Appropriate 8 100 Enjoyed 9 88 Clear Instructions 9 88 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 7 86 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 7 86 Novelty 9 45

Creativity

Times played 11 Appropriate 11 100 Enjoyed 11 100 Clear Instructions 11 100 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 10 100 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 10 100 Novelty 11 27

Challenge

Times played 7 Appropriate 8 100 Enjoyed 8 100 Clear Instructions 8 100 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 7 100 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 7 100 Novelty 8 13

Turn Taking

Times played 8 Appropriate 10 100 Enjoyed 10 80

Page 123: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

111

Clear Instructions 10 80 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 8 100 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 8 88 Novelty 10 60

Focus

Times played 8 Appropriate 9 100 Enjoyed 9 100 Clear Instructions 9 100 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 8 100 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 8 100 Novelty 9 33

Feeling Words

Times played 7 Appropriate 9 88 Enjoyed 10 90 Clear Instructions 10 90 Provided opportunity to practice target skill 7 100 Provided opportunity to talk about target topic 7 100 Novelty 9 56

Play Pack Ranking Task

Nine families ranked each themed play pack at the meeting after the intervention.

The ranking provided further insights into families’ preferences for play bag themes,

books, and materials by considering their preferences across the different bags.

Variability among families was evident. Overall, the Feeling Words bag was the most

preferred bag across families, Creativity was the second preferred bag, Waiting was the

third preferred, Turn Taking was the fourth preferred, Focus was the fifth preferred,

Challenge was the least preferred bag. Interestingly, the data reveal great variability

Page 124: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

112

among families, the weighted scores revealed the Focus bag was the fifth preferred bag

but it did not receive a most preferred rank from any families (see Figure 4.1 below).

Families’ rankings reflected variation in preference, for example the Creativity bag was

ranked most liked by three families and least liked by two other families (see Figure 4.2

below for inter-bag comparisons).

This information provided insight into families’ preferences within the experience

of the intervention. Families preferred different bags for different reasons. Moreover,

the support provided by the bags for families was sometimes unique between families.

The feedback form survey item results asserted the positive reception of the bags and the

play pack ranking task results highlighted the differences among family preference for

bag theme. In other words, family preferences for bag theme were unique, however

respondents were collectively positive about the experience of the play packs. The post-

intervention interviews began by talking through the Play Pack Ranking task. More of

the nuances of preference and support were explored in the interview analysis.

Figure 4.1. Play Pack Ranking Task Result

Page 125: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

113

Figure 4.2. Play Pack Preference Rankings

Open-Ended Weekly Feedback Form Responses

Additionally, families provided a substantial amount of feedback in response to

five open-ended response items on the feedback forms, including: 1. What

understandings (or misunderstandings) did your child show when participating in the

Page 126: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

114

task? (53/57 feedback forms included responses to this item) 2. What questions or

conversations did you add to the task (any examples of ways you personalized the

activity to you and your child) as you were playing? (51/57 feedback forms included

responses to this item) 3. How did you continue this task throughout the week (perhaps

with different materials or situations)? (54/57 feedback forms included responses to this

item) 4. What was your favorite part of the play interaction? (54/57 feedback forms

included responses to this item) 5. Do you have any other comments or questions about

the activity? (17/57 feedback forms included responses to this item).

I noted common threads across prompts as well. For example, participants

sometimes referenced Approaches to Learning skills that were different from the

Approaches to Learning skill targeted within the bag. One respondent on the Focus

feedback form wrote, “I asked her what other situations required her to wait her turn.

When she didn't win at Chutes and Ladders it was a good opportunity for us to talk about

being able to lose without being upset” (Ms. Evans). See Appendix I for an example of

the feedback form emergent theme development and synthesis.

Feedback form open-ended responses also described the experience of the play

packs as positive, engaging, and enjoyable. Reports of children enjoying materials,

parents enjoying themes, and families enjoying interaction were found across all six play

bags. Families wrote about preferred features of the play bags with details ranging from

simply enjoying spending time with their child (Focus, Challenge, Creativity, Turn

Taking, and Waiting) to appreciating the novel bag topic (Feeling Words). One parent

wrote about the Feeling Words play bag, “usually our play is directed to learning words

Page 127: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

115

or numbers, never really talking about feelings so it was a good change for us to be able

to play in a different way” (Ms. Evans). Another parent wrote about their favorite part of

the Creativity play bag, simply noting they enjoyed “being silly with my son” (Ms.

O’Neal). Parents described enjoying watching their child improvise with the Waiting,

Focus, and Creativity bags. Another parent described how even the more demanding (of

children) activities were mitigated through play, “Once I started it, she was happy to

participate. She said this makes her happy, when I get off work and we play” (Ms.

Whitaker, Challenge Bag).

The next section addressed data that were collected to inform Sub-Question C,

how do parents describe the support of the take-home play bag intervention?

Open-ended responses indicating support. Analysis of the open-ended

feedback forms items revealed indicators of the intervention’s support of parent-child

play among parents’ open-ended responses. Parents’ choices to describe ways that they

extended play through questions, discussion, or play improvisation reflected an

engagement in parent-child play using the play bags. These responses provided initial

information for the question of whether or not the take-home play bag intervention

promoted parents’ play involvement. Indicators of parents’ engagement in play were

evident across bag themes.

Specifically, participants were supported with the Focus bag, parents described

examples of supported engagement by asking question to promote the task (3/9) or theme

(1/9), repeating the activity exactly (5/9), using the materials to play a different game

(5/9), expanding the game with siblings (2/9), or extending the staying-focused theme to

Page 128: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

116

other contexts such as personal care routines (1/9), or practicing other Approaches to

Learning themes again throughout that week (3/9). A number of respondents for the

Focus bag provided more than one example of engagement.

Additionally, seven of the eight respondents for the Challenge bag, which

included a 2 in 1 puzzle, wrote about extending the persistence theme through play. Three

parents extended the task by simply repeating the activity. Two other parents extended

the Challenge bag puzzle challenge by brainstorming and trying different strategies for

separating and matching the pieces. Another two families made a game out of the

difficult task of sorting the pieces, one turned the sorting task into a time trial and the

other turned it in to a heat (mom vs. child race). Finally, all eleven respondents for the

Creativity bag wrote about how they continued the activity throughout the week:

extending the theme creativity/pretend theme through improvisation in play (9/11) or

repeating the activity and added siblings (2/11). Respondents reported repeating the

Feeling Words activity exactly (4/9) or extending the feeling words theme through play

(by making faces (the Evans), drawing moods (the Jimenezs), making a puppet show (the

Watsons).

Moreover, these responses revealed ways the play packs supported parents to

challenge their children through play. For example, a third (3/9) of respondents reporting

on the Focus bag noted that their children had trouble with the task: Chutes and Ladders

board game. However, these descriptions also indicated practice and discussion

embedded within play related to the Focus theme. For example, parents wrote, “he didn’t

want to do it at first” (Ms. Evans); or “She sit and listen before we start playing while I

Page 129: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

117

read the rules” (Ms. Whitaker). These respondents provided examples of resistance from

their children that they worked through. One child attended to (focused) on parent

reading rules.

Similarly, about a third (3/8) of respondents reporting on the Challenge bag wrote

about how their children had trouble with the Challenge bag task but also noted

persistence on behalf of their children. Importantly, their responses illustrated how the

activity promoted the target Approaches to Learning task of persistence on multiple

occasions (3/8). One parent wrote, “getting started on the first puzzle was a bit difficult

but after she figured out what to do, then it was a little less challenging” (Ms. Evans).

Alternatively, some respondents reported resistance among children with the Turn

Taking bag: child didn’t want to take turns at first (K.J. Meyers); child was not

interested/refused to play the Kerplunk game but enjoyed the book (Saraih Jimenez); play

bag did not keep child’s attention (Maria Brown). Finally, some families noted how their

children didn’t enjoy waiting (on the Waiting bag feedback forms), they also reported

that the practiced helped or that they got the hang of the waiting task before the week was

out (3/9).

Despite the effortful work of the Turn Taking and the Focus bags, indicators of

parent support and enjoyment were still reported for the Focus bag such as liking the

activity (4/9), the book (5/9), enjoying spending time playing with their child (3/9), or

watching their child improvise (2/9). Likewise, respondents still explicitly noted how

they continued to use the Turn Taking bags, despite its challenging nature, by simply

repeating the activity (2/10), extending the theme of turn taking into other contexts like

Page 130: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

118

taking turns around the house (Mr. Garcia) or on the trampoline with cousins (Ms.

Evans). Finally, two respondents for the Waiting bag wrote about how they enjoyed:

watching their child improvise new waiting games (Ms. O’Neal) and watching and

listening to their child’s ideas about patience and waiting (Ms. Boyd).

Open-Ended responses indicating purpose. Finally, I considered the feedback

form synthesis for each bag together to generate a picture of participants’ perception of

the purpose of the intervention, as defined by how they chose to report their use of the

bags.

The comprehensive view of parents’ open-ended responses on the feedback forms

painted a rich picture of , , , . Despite the same prompts across different play bag

feedback forms parents’ responses reflected different perceptions and uses. The overall

assessment of the feedback form summaries shows that parents reported on different

types of involvement depending on the play bag. I conducted frequency counts of

occurrences of examples of engagement through questions, discussion, or action

(repeating activity, extending activity, improvising with materials, or playing with the

play bag topic in another way). When the weekly feedback forms prompted parents to

report ways in which they personalized their in-home experiences with the play bag

materials, across all of the returned feedback forms, their responses were skewed toward

action (see Table 4.3 below).

Page 131: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

119

Table 4.3. Occurrences of parent examples of in-home play involvement with play bags Play Pack Questions Discussion Action

Focus 6 3 13

Challenge 5 4 8

Creativity 4 0 7

Feeling Words 4 0 14

Turn Taking 2 7 5

Waiting 3 5 5

Totals 24 19 52

Parents took action (repeating the activity, expanding the original game,

extending theme into other contexts playfully throughout the week) more often with the

Focus bag and the Feeling Words bag. Interestingly, these were the bags whose overall

preference scores totaled most liked (Feeling Words) and second least liked (Focus).

Even though respondents noted the difficulty of focusing and taking turns with the Focus

bag materials (Chutes & Ladders), their descriptions of what they liked about the bag and

the actions the took reflected an understanding of the purpose of the Play Pack

Intervention as more than simple fun, but fun themed playful interactions. Despite the

challenges posed by the Focus bag, Head Start parents described the use of playful

actions to engage with their children with the difficult theme more frequently than

questioning or discussing. In this way, Head Start parents reflected an understanding and

internalization of the intervention. They used play action more frequently to traverse the

more difficult themes rather than questioning or discussion (engagement strategies)

despite the provision of all three strategies in the Play Pack tip sheets.

Page 132: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

120

In addition, Head Start parents also described the most action behaviors with the

Feeling Words bag (most preferred among participants). The Feeling Words bag

materials (stress ball, Tangles toy, crayons, googley eyes, crayons, paper bags, and

construction paper) were less challenging, but more creative and ranked as novel (57%).

However, parents chose to describe playful action most frequently in their description of

their use of this play bag. Parents wrote about extending the theme into play like making

faces related to feeling words, drawing faces of different moods, or making a puppet

show. In this case, the theme and materials were enticing for children, the activity was

less challenging. Nonetheless, Head Start parents chose to describe their use of playful

actions to engage with their children in the absence of need (meaning the task wasn’t so

difficult that the parent need to make it look like more fun by using play action to entice

children). This continued parent involvement where children’s interest was already

evident reflected an understanding and internalization of the intervention not only to

occupy children but to promote parent-child play.

These results lent support to the assertion that families and researchers shared the

same understanding of the purpose of the take-home play bag intervention. And, that the

take-home play bag intervention promoted parent’s play involvement.

Parent Post-Intervention Interview

Ten post intervention semi-structured interviews were conducted and audio

recorded in the two weeks following the intervention (10/13, response rate of 77%).

These data painted a rich picture of all three sub-questions: the experience, support, and

perceived purpose of the intervention in the voices of Head Start parents all at once.

Page 133: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

121

Some of the emergent themes aligned with the weekly feedback form items. The

expanded descriptions from parents and alignments were encouraged at the mid-

intervention mixing point where I drew upon families’ feedback form responses to illicit

additional information in the post-intervention interviews. I added these additional

questions as needed to the semi-structured interview protocol guided by three primary

prompts: 1. Tell me about a time when you were playing with the bags at home; 2. Do

you think the bags, books, or activities, had anything to do with the labels; 3. Did the

bags allow you to play/to have discussions with your child in ways that you wouldn’t

have usually? The next three paragraphs present a summary of the themes relating to

families’ experience of the intervention, perceptions of novelty, and purpose.

Overwhelmingly, interview respondents spoke about a positive intervention

experience. Parents discriminated more about the differences between their preference,

as parents, and the preferences of their children (whereas the feedback form only had one

option). Sometimes preferences diverged, for example one parent preferred the

Challenge (persistence) bag because they thought it was good practice for their daughter

and enjoyed watching her persist and overcome the obstacle of separating the two puzzles

(Ms. Evans). Other times parents and children shared the same preference. More than

one parent-child pair agreed in their preference for the Creativity bag (pizza Play Doh

pretending materials). Children enjoyed the Creativity book, homemade scented Play

Doh, and pretend pizza tools (hat, box, rolling pin, and slicer). Parents enjoyed watching

their children enjoy, joining in the pretending, or extending the activity to the whole

Page 134: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

122

family. One parent shared that this was the first time they had seen their son engage in

pretend play (Ms. Thompson).

A majority of interview respondents stated that they had never seen any of the

books before, while the remaining respondents said they had only seen one of the books

before. Parents shared a variety of ways that the intervention supported them to support

their children’s task practice. Parents shared the most about the Turn Taking and

Patience bags, but also about the Creativity and Feeling Word bags. Parents also talked

about how they had incorporated the bags into families with multiple siblings in helpful

ways, providing a common goal for a mutli-age group of siblings or enhancing task

practice for the target child. These situations included the whole family playing chutes

and ladders together (Focus bag) or having the target child practice taking turns and

sharing the pretending toys (Creativity bag) with siblings. Parents were explicit in

describing how their children enjoyed the fresh children’s literature and how they found

the books particularly helpful for launching new discussion about Approaches to

Learning topics (reframing waiting, Ms. Boyd; feeling word vocabulary, Ms. Serrano,

etc.). The novelty of the books and materials was confirmed.

Interview responses indicating purpose. Three purpose themes surfaced as

parent perceptions for the purpose of the intervention. First, a number of parents focused

on the Approaches to Learning theme without prompting. They seemed to reflect an

understanding of the project purpose as targeting Approaches to Learning tasks or they

simply decided to use the intervention to focus on Approaches to Learning with their

children. One parent shared, “I thought they (the bags) had a good point to them. You

Page 135: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

123

know for him, as far as leaning a lesson other than just playing. … I think it was an equal

balance, enough to keep him interested, but at the same time talk about the bigger points

with him as much as he could comprehend it” (Ms. Boyd). Second, a group of parents

focused their interview responses around fresh connections with their children. They

shared about enjoying seeing their children display new skills or specifically about

bonding and less about practicing specific tasks such as waiting or turn taking. These

parents still shared examples about being engaged, playing, and interacting together with

the materials and their children. However, they seemed to have perceived the purpose of

the intervention or chosen to use the intervention as a means to spend more quality time

with their children. A third group of parent interview respondents stayed close to the

semi-structured prompts. These respondents didn’t share enough information to indicate

their perceptions of the purpose of the intervention through their interview responses.

Research Question 1 Conclusion

The first overarching research question focused on the feasibility of the Project

Play Pack intervention by attending to parent and family perceptions of the intervention.

Data sources informing my understanding of parent perceptions of the intervention

illuminated the family experience of Project Play Pack and revealed enhancing and

inhibiting elements of the intervention for familes.

Qualitative Primary

The two qualitative data sources informed the understanding of the parent

perceptions of Project Play Pack. The open-ended weekly feedback form responses and

parent post-intervention interview revealed overwhelming positive experiences among

Page 136: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

124

intervention participants. The open-ended weekly feedback from prompts provided

unique insights from families about how, as a family, they understood, talked about, used,

extended, and enjoyed the take-home play bags. The semi-structured interview provided

additional details regarding the experience, support, and perceived purpose of the

intervention. I integrated the feedback form prompts with interview responses to

generate depth. Parents described an overall positive experience in the interview and the

open-ended weekly feedback form response added from-the-moment details of the

experience of the individual bags. When combined, a picture of how Head Start families

soaked up the potential of Project Play Pack and utilized these resources to connect with

their children, reflect on their play practices, and try new Approaches to Learning themed

play with their entire families began to emerge. This collection of parents approving

feedback regarding the intervention culminated in parents’ qualitative accounts of

increases in play attributed to the play packs described in the next chapter.

Quantitative Complimentary

The two quantitative data sources complimented the qualitative by providing

reinforcement of parents’ positive perceptions of the bags as appropriate and enjoyable as

well as parents’ utilization of the bags as opportunities to practice the target Approaches

to Learning skill. The results of feedback form survey items: “We enjoyed playing with

this bag” and “This provided an opportunity to practice a (bag specific) skill” asserted

families’ positive perception of each individual bag. They complimented general reports

of enjoying the intervention overall and confirmed parents understanding, agreement, and

compliance with the Approaches to Learning purposes of the intervention. The Play Pack

Page 137: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

125

ranking task also added dimension to families’ positive perception of the intervention and

provided reinforcing evidence of their manipulation of all the materials as they reported

on their favorites and least preferred elements in vivid detail. The quantitative data aligns

with the qualitative data in a positive refrain but also denotes the uniqueness of each

family’s preferences. No one bag was overwhelmingly preferred or disliked, families

enjoyed different bags for different reasons that were specific to their unique in-home

contexts. These variabilities lent support for the open-ended and flexible nature of the

bag design. One size did not fit all, but families were adept at tailoring the play bags to

fit when explicitly invited through intentional wording on tip sheets. Moreover, bag

designs did not need to be rigid in order for parents to understand or implement the

overall intention of the intervention to engage in themed playful parent-child interactions.

Mixed End

Emergent themes from both the open-ended feedback form responses and the

parent post-intervention interviews aligned with the weekly feedback form and ranking

task results. These data are triangulated (Erzberger & Kelle, 2003) as the qualitative data

described the significantly positive reception of the play packs and parent reports of

supported play, while the quantitative feedback form items and play pack ranking task

offer complementary data to support the proposition that Play Pack intervention design

features were positively received by Head Start families and supported parent-child play.

Parent perception of novelty, from the quantitative weekly feedback form

designation of overall activity novelty and the qualitative indicators from the open-ended

weekly feedback forms and post intervention interviews of book, activity, approach, and

Page 138: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

126

theme novelty, are best considered through mixing. The weekly feedback form responses

revealed half of the bags as relatively familiar, Challenge (13%), Creativity (27%), and

Focus (33%) bags, but unanimously appropriate, enjoyed, and providing of opportunities

to practice and talk about target theme. Alternatively, Waiting, Feeling Words, and Turn

Taking were rated novel more frequently and received positive (80% or above) but

somewhat mixed ratings for appropriate, enjoyed, and providing of opportunities to

practice and talk about target theme. This pattern was interesting because it hinted

toward a correlation between bag novelty and parents’ positive perception of bags.

However, the open-ended weekly feedback form responses and post intervention

interviews added dimension to parents perceptions of novelty. Specifically, parents

provided examples of how the bags infused novel materials, approaches to play, and play

themes to parent-child in-home play in positive and enjoyable ways. For example, more

than one parent shared specifically about how the binoculars were a new for their

children (7/10). In addition, parents shared about new approaches or types of play, Ms.

Evans wrote “usually our play is directed to learning words or numbers, never really

talking about feelings so it was a good change for us to be able to play in a different way”

(Feeling Words Bag) and Ms. Thompson said, “We had never played a board game or

Ker-plunk game before” (Turn Taking and Focus Bags). Importantly, post intervention

interviews revealed the unique connections between what elements were novel to

families and how the novelty helped to incite play.

The positive reception lends substantial support to the feasibility of Project Play

pack as described by Head Start parents’ perceptions of the take-home play bag

Page 139: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

127

intervention. Head Start families found the take-home play bags to be novel (in different

ways), useful, and enjoyable. Parents and researchers understood and exhibited a shared

understanding of the purpose of the bags to promote themed parent-child play

interactions. And finally, the intervention supported Head Start families through

intentional design features that made the intervention novel, useful, enjoyable, and

purposeful for participants.

Page 140: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

128

CHAPTER 5

RESEARCH QUESTION 2 RESULTS

Introduction

This chapter presents results from the individual data analysis of each source

relating to Research Question 2. The results and mixing of data for the first research

question described the feasibility of the intervention as a positive and supportive

experience for Head Start families. This chapter summarizes the influence of the Project

Play Pack intervention on Head Start families by describing the results of each data

source analysis relating to influences on parent-child play. First, the results for Research

Question 2 explore the influence of the intervention on parent-child play in two ways:

influence on parent-child play frequency (sub-question A), influence on parent play

involvement (sub-question B), and influence on children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors within parent-child play (sub-question C). Then, I describe the mixing of these

individual data sources to gain a rich understanding of the influence of the intervention.

Finally, the initial implications relating to the influence of the take-home play bag

intervention on parent-child play and children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors are

discussed.

Research Question 2: Influence

To what extent does the Project Play Pack take-home play bag intervention

influence parent-child play among Head Start families? The next section addressed data

that were collected to inform sub-question A, does the take-home play bag intervention

promote parents’ play involvement? The pre- and post- play frequency forms, the pre

Page 141: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

129

and post parent-child play interaction analyses, and the post intervention interviews

considered the influence of the intervention on parents’ play involvement, frequency and

parent involvement style.

Play Frequency

Play Frequency Forms

Ten parents provided the play frequency form for the week before the start of the

intervention. The average amount of in-home play reported the week before the

intervention was 7.3 hours (ranging from .75-14 hours a week). The average number of

working hours the week before the intervention was 26 hours (ranging from 0-56 hours

the week prior to the intervention). Half of reporting parents indicated that they worked

40 hours or more on average on the initial participant form, however the hourly work that

was reported on the Play Frequency Form in the week prior to the intervention was

variable. This was not surprising because hourly work schedules and hours vary widely.

The variability of these types of work schedules contributes to the financial and

scheduling difficulties experienced by many of the participating families. The average

number of in-home play time for parents who were unemployed or working part time was

7.1 hours a week (ranging from .75 – 25 hours). The average number of in-home play

time for parents who were working full time or more was 7.55 hours a week (ranging

from .75 to 10 hours).

Changes in in-home parent-child play frequency. Only four participants

returned the post-intervention play frequency form. The quantitative data provided by

the four pre/post pairs were not sufficient for comparisons. The play frequencies of these

Page 142: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

130

four families before and after the intervention cannot assert a change in play frequency as

a result of the intervention. However, the work to play hour ratios were interesting. Ms.

Evans worked 18 hours more the week after the intervention than the week before the

intervention; she played 22 hours less with Kalasia the week after the intervention than

the week before the intervention. Mr. Garcia worked the same number of hours the week

before and after the intervention, he played with Jesus 1.5 hours more the week after the

intervention than the week before the intervention. Ms. Boyd worked 17 hours less the

week after the intervention than the week before the intervention; she played 17.75 hours

more with Aedan the week after the intervention than the week before the intervention.

Ms. Miller was unemployed, she played with Mike 3 hours more the week after the

intervention than the week before the intervention.

Interview indicators of changes in play frequency

While some parents were reluctant to say that the bags helped them play more, the

majority of interview respondents were comfortable talking about how the bags provided

new materials or approaches to their typical play routines. One example of resistance to a

more assertion, “Saraih is the type of child that you really cannot not play with. But I

think they (the bags) gave us more to do, so I would say, yeah” (Ms. Jimenez). Another

example was shared by a father who was explicit about his strong relationship with his

son, “He loves me. Whatever I’m doing, he wanna do. You know it really don’t matter.

But, you know, he like the puzzles (the Challenge bag) and the paper bags (the Feeling

Words bag)…. Whatever you send, we gonna play with it” (Mr. Garcia).

Page 143: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

131

Other parents were comfortable describing the bags provided “more to do”

(support or impetus). One single mother described that the play bags helped her and her

son play more because she is often distracted by her other responsibilities, sharing “I had

a lot of housework to do…. Being a single parent is tough”. The same mother described

challenges in juggling both children and housework. Ms. Miller used the bags to find

balance.

Moreover, a number of participants gave specific examples of how the

intervention supported more play at home with their children. For example, one parent

described,

I have a lot of house work to do. And then dealing with trying to separate them,

my oldest one, he’s 10….and they clash…being a single parent is tough… the

games (Turn Taking and Focus bags) actually brought them closer together. They

were actually playing good together. Then when it was time to go to bed… me

and him had that bonding time. So it kind of helped too you know. We enjoyed.

We enjoyed all of them (bags). (Ms. Miller)

Another two parents described how the intervention generated an impetus for parent-

child play,

Sometimes, he runs off and does his own thing, but being that we had the bag, I

was like, okay you have to play with me. We are going to play this game, so it

kinda, it helped a little bit (Ms. Boyd)

Similarly, another parent described,

Page 144: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

132

The thing that I liked the most was having you put together that bag and making

us have that time to use it. Because I didn’t realize until we started this program

that I don’t have as much time as I used to, to (play) (Ms. Thompson)

Another parent talked about how they feel like they are losing a battle with technology to

connect with their daughter,

Half the time she’ll get the tablet and be sitting on the couch watching little

videos. And if I wanna do stuff with her, she’ll be like “No, I don’t wanna, I

wanna play on my tablet!” And I’m like, “What?!” You are a four-year-old

dissing her mamma right now….. yeah, she literally wanted. “Mama let’s do the

bags!” (Ms. Brown

Parents interview responses indicate that for many of the families the play bags

encouraged more in-home parent-child play.

Parent Play Involvement

Characteristics of Parent Play Involvement (PPI)

I coded the pre- and post- parent-child play interaction videos using the PPI

(Tejagupta, 1991). The PPI coding process quantitized the qualitative parent-child

interaction video data. Two different coders coded the nine pre and post parent-child

play observation pairs to examine interrater reliability. I coded every parent-child play

observation. Then, I selected and randomly assigned two research team members to

conduct a second coding of the parent child interaction videos. I computed a Kappa

statistic (k) to measure the interrater reliability of the categorical data (Cohen, 1960;

Viera & Garrett, 2005). The Cohen’s Kappa (k) statistic determined interrater reliability

Page 145: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

133

because it considers both disagreement and distance of disagreement between compared

codes. I established interrater reliability at 84% (n= 513 epochs) of the PPI with a kappa

value of .78, substantial agreement (Viera & Garrett, 2005).

I preformed preliminary data analyses on the PPI percentage data. Parents play

involvement styles varied in the parent-child play observations within and across pre and

post videos. Table 5.1 presents the means of the pre and post PPI percentage data. I

explored the relationship between PPI pre and post scores and learning center selection

for parent-child play observation in order to identify any unexpected statistically

significant relationships to parent play involvement and learning area (see Table 5.2),

none were found. I explored the relationship between PPI difference scores and parent

demographics in order to identify any statistically significant correlations among changes

in parent play involvement and parent characteristics (see Table 5.3), none were found

among pre and post scores or learning center choice.

Table 5.1. Mean Percentages of Parental Involvement in Parent-Child Play Interactions, Pre and Post

Pre (n = 9) Post (n = 9)

Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Category

Participation

23.00 9.71 7-35.50 30.06 18.62 11-56

Facilitation

56.50 13.88 43.50-85

48.06 12.46 30-69

Neutral

14.72 7.83 7-31.50 16.22 10.20 0-31

Restriction

2.72 1.97 0-5.00 2.56 3.53 0-10

Noninvolvement

2.28 3.01 0-7.50 3.00 3.19 0-7

Page 146: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

134

Table 5.2. PPI Pre, Post, and Learning Center Selection Correlations (N = 9) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Pre Learning Center -

2. Post Learning Center .42 -

3. Pre Participation .02 -.07 -

4. Pre Facilitation .30 .29 -.62 -

5. Pre Neutral -.46 -.38 -.09 -

.70*

-

6. Pre Restriction .11 -.14 .53 -.80* .58 -

7. Pre Noninvolvement -.10 -.09 -.40 -.35 .66 .11 -

8. Post Participation .28 .20 .14 .08 -.32 -.19 .07 -

9. Post Facilitation .02 .20 -.44 .31 .05 -.10 -.04 -

.89** -

10. Post Neutral -.46 -.10 .43 -.64 .53 .57 -.06 -.75* -.57 -

11 Post Restriction .11 -.18 -.45 .76* -.44 -.58 -.41 .25 -.02 -.57 -

12. Post Noninvolvement -.33 -.36 .01 -.44 .47 .32 .43 -.29 .13 -.27 -.60 -

Page 147: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

135

Table 5.3. Demographics and PPI Difference Scores Correlations 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1.Pre Learning Center -

2. Post LearningCenter .42 -

3. Level of Parent

Education

.00 -.32 -

4. Parent Employment .44 .00 .13 -

5. Hours of Work (Pre) .22 .28 .22 .75* -

6. Hours of Play (Pre) .51 -.08 .11 .05 .13 -

Difference Scores

7. Participation Diff .26 .05 -.22 .49 .05 -.22 -

8. Facilitation Diff -.25 -.10 .19 -.32 -.22 .12 -

.84** -

9. Neutral Diff -.13 .22 .09 -.62 -.03 .02 -.81** .42 -

10. Restriction Diff .04 -.07 -.23 .55 .69 .03 .51 -.76* -.24 -

11. Noninvolvement Diff -.23 -.27 .07 -.63 -.40 .58 -.49 .23 .48 -.24 -

Page 148: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

136

Table 5.4. Case by case of Parental Involvement in Parent-child Play Interactions PID Participation Facilitation Neutral Restriction Noninvol.

Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post

2 35.50 44.50 45.00 30.00 14.50 20.00 3.50 2.00 0 2.50

3 35.50 16.50 52.50 55.00 9.50 27.00 2.50 1.50 0 0

4 27.50 12.50 44.00 52.50 22.50 31.00 5.00 0 1.00 4.00

6 13.50 11.00 43.50 56.50 31.50 24.50 4.00 1.50 7.50 6.50

7 18.50 38.00 60.00 45.00 12.00 10.00 .00 0 5.00 7.00

8 29.00 25.00 54.00 53.00 11.00 15.00 5.00 0 1.00 7.00

9 21.50 56.00 53.00 36.50 16.00 6.50 3.50 1.00 6.00 0

12 19.00 56.00 71.50 35.00 8.50 0 1.00 10.00 0 0

15 7.00 12.00 85.00 69.00 7.00 12.00 .00 7.00 0 0

Changes in parent play involvement. The pre and post PPI scores were not

correlated and there was a very small sample of matched pairs (pre and post). The lack of

correlations coupled with the fact that pre post PPI scores did not seem to show a

difference rendered a t-test comparing mean differences inappropriate. More

appropriately, a case-by-case look at the PPI scores before and after the intervention

among the nine matched pairs are displayed in Table 5.4. A slight increase in

participation and slight decrease in facilitation percentages from pre to post parent play

involvement are noted among a few individual cases (see Table 5.4). The results reveal

no consistent pattern of change in parent involvement in play behaviors after participating

in the intervention. The notion that perhaps parent play involvement didn’t need to be

“fixed” will be considered in the discussion.

Page 149: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

137

Interview indicators of involvement

Parent interview respondents elaborated on the ways that they extended the

intervention materials, themes, and books. This was a mid-intervention mixing point,

when I pulled salient quantitative data and open-ended responses from feedback forms to

prompt parents within interviews to garner further information. Some parents described

examples of extensions within their general descriptions of their experience of the play

bags. These disclosures highlighted ways in which parents extended the intervention

materials into their in-home play contexts, play habits, and unique parent-child

relationship. Moreover, these examples of expressions of the way that the intervention

influenced change in their play involvement. Parents displayed creativity in their

extension of materials and themes. For example, one parent-child dyad added an

experiment approach to the Kerplunk game (Turn Taking bag). The Thompsons decided

to count the number of straws that could be taken out before the content fell and compare

repeated attempts. Another family used the homemade Play Doh in their pretend kitchen

area at home to expand the pretend play outside of pizza. The parent and child continued

a cook-out play scenario together using the dough. Moreover, more than one respondent

described how game play was a new addition to their play repertoire (the Browns, the

Evans, and the Watsons). Parents shared about how they would never have thought to

pair Approaches to Learning themed with books and activities in the ways that they were

presented in the bags, but found this to be useful while interacting and playing with their

children (the Boyds and the Thompsons).

Page 150: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

138

The next section addressed data collected to inform sub-question B, Does the

take-home play bag intervention influence children’s Approaches to Learning Behaviors

within parent-child play interactions? Two coding phases explored children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child interactions.

Approaches to Learning

Patterns of Child Approaches to Learning Behaviors in Parent-Child Play

Interactions

This “bottom up coding” (Saldana, 2015) procedure resulted in the identification

of the following patterns of children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors in parent-child

play interactions. Nine parent-child dyads completed both pre and post video

observations. First, I found that children utilized Approaches to Learning behaviors

within parent-child play interactions in both pre and post play observations. Moreover,

the bottom-up emergent coding of individual parent-child play interactions helped to

reveal unique aspects of each parent-child dyad’s play. While children’s Approaches to

Learning behavioral expressions seemed consistent across learning center (blocks,

manipulatives, or home living), parents’ promotion of Approaches to Learning behaviors

among their children were different. Some parents scaffolded Approaches to Learning

skills, such as cognitive regulation, by interrupting play with quiz like questioning such

as, “what shape is that?” When parents posed these questions within play but out of

context with the activity children had to pause, answer the question, and then return to

their original play goal. Alternatively, other parents scaffolded Approaches to Learning

skills, such as behavioral self-regulation, by actively participating in ways that posed

Page 151: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

139

more natural within-play context challenges for children, such as turn taking that

occurred because of limited materials, persisting with materials that the child selected but

found challenging to work with (bristle blocks), or children matching the behavioral

expectations of their parent in an exciting situation like pretend aggression while playing

with plastic dinosaurs or quickly cleaning up to shift the play in another direction.

Moreover, this bottom up coding provided important new insights into Head Start

children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play. Children

displayed creativity within pretend play scenarios in the parent-child play observations:

Mike Miller and Ms. Miller pretended with plastic dinosaurs, the Boyds and the O’Neals

pretended to cook dinner together, the Browns and the Evans pretended to care for a

baby. Children displayed emotional and behavioral regulation (or interpersonal

responsibility, PLBS; McDermott et al., 2011) in the parent-child play observations

when: Mister O’Neal struggled to open a pretend laptop, persisted for some time,

grimacing with challenge but not becoming angry until Ms. O’Neal assisted, or when

Kalasia took turns within play, when her mom was working with her brother Kalasia did

not interrupt. Other children displayed cognitive regulation in exploratory and pretend

play. Ryan gave his play purpose by declaring he would build a dinosaur house for a

special dinosaur, he looks through the dinosaur bin for a while before he found the one he

was thinking of (purposeful with a clear goal). Mister plays under an implied purposeful

goal as he carefully lined up wooden blocks in order to be able to balance the roof blocks

on top in his exploratory making play. Parent and child play interactions provided

opportunities for Approaches to Learning skill discussion, practice, and challenges.

Page 152: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

140

Influence on Children’s Approaches to Learning. I examined the influence of

the intervention on children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play

interactions across the pre and post parent-child play interactions by transforming the

data. The PALBO quantitized children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within the

parent-child play interactions videos. I computed the Kappa statistic (k) to measure the

inter-observer reliability of the categorical data (Cohen, 1960; Viera & Garrett, 2005). I

established interrater reliability with a second coder at 81% (n = 100 epochs, 3 videos)

with a kappa value of .69, substantial agreement (Viera & Garrett, 2005).

Preliminary analyses showed varied styles of Approaches to Learning behaviors

among children within the parent-child play observations. Means of pre and post PALBO

percentage data (presented in Table 5.5) look hopeful for substantiating significant

changes in children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors as a result of the Approaches to

Learning themed take-home play bags. I explored potential relationships among the

PALBO pre and post scores and learning center selection for parent-child observation in

order to identify any unexpected relationships to children’s Approached to Learning

behaviors and learning area (see Table 5.6). A correlation emerged between post

behavioral regulation and post parent-child play interaction learning center selection, r(7)

= .70, p < .05. I explored potential relationships between the PALBO difference scores

and parent demographics in order to identify any correlations among changes in

children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within play and parent characteristics. A

statistically significant correlation emerged among creativity difference score and

parent’s work hours (r(7) = -.82, p < .05).

Page 153: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

141

Table 5.5. Mean Percentages of Children’s Approaches to Learning Behaviors, Pre and Post Pre (n = 9) Post (n = 9) Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Domain

Behavioral Regulation 23.67 10.17 10-40 42.67 21.89 4-85

Cognitive Regulation 39.78 18.85 10-75 44.22 16.22 5-60

No Regulation

36.56 14.81 10-50 10.56 8.82 0-30

Subdomain

Emotion 24.11 15-40 37.78 18.56 15-70

Creativity 37.00 5-85 61.11 26.43 0-90

Page 154: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

142

Table 5.6. PALBO Pre, Post, and Learning Center Selection Correlations (N = 9) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

1.Pre Learning Center -

2. Post Learning Center .42 -

3. Pre Behavioral Regulation

-.08 -.14 -

4. Pre Cognitive Regulation .09 -.08 -.63 -

5. Pre No Regulation -.06 .19 .11 -.84** -

6. Pre Emotion -.07 -.25 -.14 -.38 .59 -

7. Pre Creativity -.09 -.10 -.40 .35 -.17 -.06 -

8. Pre Sustained Play .64 .11 -.17 .67 -.73 -.32 .08 -

9. Post Behavioral Regulation

.22 .70* .06 -.06 .04 -.20 .26 .17 -

10. Post Cognitive Regulation

.01 -.52 -.49 .46 -.24 .08 -.15 .10 -.75 -

11. Post No Regulation -.24 -.10 -.11 -.38 .56 .73* -.12 -.36 -.32 -.07 -

12. Post Emotion .53 .48 .03 -.13 .14 .24 .36 .29 .72 -.62 -.05 -

13. Post Creativity -.10 -.64 -.33 .38 -.26 -.11 -.01 .03 -.67 .92 -.27 -.67 -

13. Post Sustained Play .22 -.36 .57 -.08 -.29 -.34 .22 .19 -.16 -.10 -.41 .12 .08 -

Page 155: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

143

Despite the small sample size, the case by case pre and post score differences

were promising (see Table 5.7). The small sample size made assumptions of normality

difficult to meet. The behavioral regulation and cognitive regulation PALBO categories

difference scores were substantially negatively skewed, when means the bell curve of the

data points to the left with the mean to the left of the peak. The emotion and creativity

PALBO sub-categories difference scores were moderately skewed, and no-behavioral-

regulation and sustained play periods categories difference scores were symmetrical,

evenly distributed. Because the sample size is so small, and the assumptions of normality

are violated it would be unwise to assert a consistent pattern of change among children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors in parent-child play interactions after participating in

the intervention, but the results are hopeful. And there were significant differences in

three of the PALBO difference category scores. There was a significant difference in the

scores for pre-post behavioral regulation (M = 19.00, SD = 23.54), t(8) = 2.42, p < 0.05.

There was a significant difference in the scores for pre to post no examples behavioral

regulation (M = -26.00, SD = 12.26); t(8) = -6.36, p < 0.001. There was a significant

difference in the length of sustained activity periods for post (M = 3.78, SD = 3.99); t(8)

= 2.84, p < 0.05. These results imply the possibility of significant changes in children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions after receiving

the play bag intervention.

Page 156: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

144

Table 5.7. Case by case percentages of PALBO PID BSR CSR NBR EMO CRE SUS Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post 2 20 40 50 60 30 0 20 20 20 85 3 7 3 15 60 75 40 10 0 15 55 85 60 10 12 4 10 35 40 45 50 30 40 40 45 45 3 4 6 38 4 28 48 34 15 22 15 28 70 3 14 7 25 50 40 40 35 10 15 15 35 70 5 7 8 25 35 55 55 20 10 25 40 5 65 12 9 9 15 40 35 50 50 10 20 40 35 65 5 9 12 25 35 25 55 50 10 35 45 50 90 4 11 15 40 85 10 5 50 10 25 70 30 0 4 10

Research Question 2 Conclusion

The second overarching research question focused on the influence of the Project

Play Pack intervention. I expected that the Project Play Pack intervention would

influence parent-child play frequency, parent play involvement, and children's

Approaches to Learning behaviors.

Qualitative Primary

The four qualitative data sources informing the understanding of the influence of

Project Play Pack were integrated by sub-question. Sub-question A addressed the

influence of the intervention on parent-child play specifically by mixing observation

videos, open-ended weekly feedback form responses and parent post-intervention

interviews to consider common and unique intervention influence on parent play

frequency and parent play involvement. The parent-child play observation videos

provided insight into parent-child play patterns before and after the intervention. The

Page 157: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

145

open-ended weekly feedback from prompts such as “How did you continue this task

throughout the week?” provided unique insights from families about how they used,

extended, and were supported in their play by the take-home play bags. The semi-

structured interview prompts such as “Tell me about a time when you were playing with

the bags at home” provided additional details regarding how families contextualized uses

and extensions of the intervention in their play. I integrated the feedback form prompts

with interview responses to generate depth. Parents described an overall engaged playful

experience in the interview while the open-ended weekly feedback form responses added

from-the-moment details of the experience of the individual bags. When combined, a

picture of how Head Start families' parent-child play was influenced by the intervention

began to emerge. Head Start families were supported to play more or play in new

ways. Sub-question B addressed the influence of the intervention on children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play by studying emergent patterns

of children’s behaviors in the observation videos and comparing the pre and post videos.

The parent-child play observation videos provided new insights into children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions, as opposed to

peer play interactions to the literature (Fantuzzo et al., 1998; McDermott et al., 2011;

McDermott et al., 2014).

This collection of parent reports of engaged playful parent-child interactions as a

result of the intervention tell the rich story of how Project Play Pack supported and

promoted parent-child play as well as how children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors

are being practiced within parent-child play interactions among Head Start families.

Page 158: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

146

Quantitative Complimentary

The three quantitative data sources complimented the qualitative data for both

sub-questions by providing indicators of parent involvement in parent-child play, in-

home play frequency, and indicators of children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors

within parent-child play. Sub-question A addressed the influence of the intervention on

parent’s play involvement. The transformation of the parent-child play observation

videos data (PPI), feedback form data, and the in-home play frequency form data

provided complementary quantitative data for both prongs of parent play involvement

(parent-child play frequency and play involvement). Sub-question B explored and

compared children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play

interactions before and after the intervention, the transformation of the parent-child play

observation videos (PALBO) provided complementary quantitative data for the study of

changes among children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors. There was only one

qualitative source for sub-question B so this mixing occurred between the qualitative and

quantitative pieces, the transformation.

Mixed End

The mixing of the qualitative data sources for sub-question A resulted in a rich

picture of these Head Start families’ parent-child play. The qualitative data regarding

parent-play involvement across data sources revealed strong Head Start family

participation, or intention to participate, in parent-child play. The quantitative data added

complementary definition to the qualitative data contributing important boundaries

between “fixing” and supporting Head Start parent’s parent-child play. Specifically,

Page 159: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

147

parent play involvement scores were high before the intervention. Moreover, I found no

significant changes in parent play involvement after the intervention. In other words,

these Head Start parents’ parent-child play interactions were strong before the

intervention began. Rather than an imposing fix, these data helped to illustrate how the

Project Play Pack intervention provided welcomed supports.

First, these rich qualitative pieces denote the similarities and uniqueness of each

dyad’s parent-child play interaction. Second, the participants’ voices, in post-

intervention interviews, elaborately described where Head Start parents welcomed

“support” rather than “help”. Parent narratives about how the intervention supported

their play were complimented by the PPI data which established strong play patterns

before the intervention. The PPI quant data confirmed that parent play involvement did

not significantly change after the intervention. Rather than “fixing” parent-child play,

these data combine to assert how the intervention made significant contributions to

families by supporting more and new play through the provision of materials (parents

explored new approaches to and types of play inspired by the intervention materials) and

“found” time (the novelty, organization, and delivery of the bags generated fresh impetus

and priority for play, initiative).

The mixing of qualitative data sources for sub-question B resulted in a rich

picture of the influence of the Project Play Pack intervention on children’s Approaches to

Learning behaviors by way of the newly identified Approaches to Learning behaviors

displayed in parent-child interactions. The quantitative data compliment the qualitative

depiction by adding statistical significance to the assertion that the intervention promoted

Page 160: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

148

Approaches to Learning behaviors by providing a systematic way of comparing

children’s Approaches to learning behaviors before and after the intervention.

Page 161: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

149

CHAPTER 6

DISCUSSION

Introduction

The previous chapter detailed the results of each data source and mixing ends

organized by research question. This chapter provides the final discussion of the

overarching research questions of this quasi-experimental embedded mixed method study

(Creswell and Plano-Clark, 2007; Leal et al, 2016). The following sections present the

results and implications of the final mixing phase. Like many other mixed methods

research, this study utilized the “methodological metaphor” of triangulation to clearly

identify the links between research questions, qualitative data, and quantitative data

(Erzberger & Kelle, 2003) for drawing inferences from complementary results (Östlund

et al., 2011). This mixing strategy was the best fit for a quant embedded in qual design

(Erzberger & Kelle, 2003) with regards to the feasibility of the intervention (Figure 6.1)

as well as the influence of the intervention on parent-child play and children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors (Figure 6.2).

Page 162: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

150

Figure 6.1. Complementary Triangulation Model: Feasibility

Figure 6.2. Complementary Triangulation Model: Influence

Page 163: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

151

Feasibility Summary

The Experience

The decline of in-home play among parents and young children in preschool

remains problematic (Singer et al., 2009). Children’s play is a primary avenue for growth

and learning in the preschool years (Bergen, 2002; Fisher et al.,2008; Vygotsky, 1978).

Parent-child play is particularly beneficial for young children (Bennet et al., 1997) and

the parent-child bond (Boyce et al., 2017). Play is threatened by time constraints brought

on by parent work schedule (Milkie et al., 2010), digital media saturation (entertainment

preferred over play, Burdette & Whitaker, 2005; Stanger & Gridina, 1999), and some

parents’ limited understanding of play (see Parker et al., 1999).

Many interventions seeking to support families in poverty are routinely inhibited

by constraints that mirror the threats to play, such as parent’s work scheduling conflict,

lack of child care outside of school hours, and parent fatigue (Mendez, 2010; Webster-

Stratton et al., 2001). Low incomes families face additional challenges to find the time

(Ginsburg, 2007) and the resources (Milteer et al., 2012) to play. The Project Play Pack

intervention was designed in the likeness of established take-home bag designs. This

work was particularly inspired by the Math Pack work described in Chapter 2 (Linder &

Emerson, in revisions). The Math Pack project and Project Play Pack both sought to

support Head Start families in finding time and resources to support their parent-child

play practices. I used experiences from my work on Math Pack, inspiration from the

triadic interaction model, and my interest in non-invasive design features to support Head

Page 164: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

152

Start families with materials to play (resources) and extra impetus to play (found time)

with Approaches to Learning themes.

Math Pack inspired the composition of the take-home play bags (Linder &

Emerson, in revisions). The triadic interaction strengthened my rationale for the design

features of Project Play Pack including the home-based intervention design that both

acknowledged and situated the parent-child relationship at the heart of change (Brooks,

2005; Kaminski et al., 2008; Neville et al., 2013). I situated the take-home play bag

designs of Math Pack (Linder & Emerson, in revisions) within McCollum and Yates

(1994) triadic interaction model and applied Approaches to Learning themes. The triadic

interaction model provided a framework for the shared choice of marking the child, the

parent, and the interventionist as the three partners in the home-based family engagement

intervention. The Project Play Pack intervention used take-home play bags as a proxy for

the interventionist’s presence in the home. Replacing an interventionist’s intrusion into

families’ homes with the take-home play bag but maintaining the equal partnership with

families through open invitations for them to make the activities their own within their

own home context and provide feedback of the experience of each bag were intentional

non-invasive design choices. The triadic interaction model was particularly well suited to

frame Project Play Pack as it sought to promote play and Approaches to Learning

because of their relational nature. Math Pack’s open-ended manipulatives set important

precedence for my decision to utilize primarily open-ended materials. Project Play Pack

contributed new insights into home-based family interventions targeting play.

Page 165: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

153

Mitigating Threats to Play

Non-invasive Project Play Pack design choices included: convenience, novelty,

and fun. The convenience and flexibility of allowing parents to have the bag for a week

to use when and how most appropriate for them was important because of previous work

illustrating how parent training and workshops were particularly challenging for at risk

families (Brand & Jungmann, 2014). Similar to other take-home bag delivery systems

interventions (Brand et al., 2014; Dever & Burts, 2002; Linder, 2017; Zeece & Wallace,

2009), this delivery mode suited Head Start families through convenience (Linder &

Emerson, in revisions).

Similarly, the two-pronged novelty (freshness of the bag materials, themes, and

approaches as well as children’s curiosity for a new bag each week) served invaluable

roles in inciting play. Participants perceptions of intervention support and increased in-

home play frequency were bolstered by bidirectional play initiation, parents using the bag

to initiate play and children using the bags to initiate play. Whether the bag served as a

weekly signpost (reminder) for parents to come together with their children or an exciting

unknown activity for children to explore with their parents, the Project Play Pack

participants provided examples of how the novel elements of the intervention mitigated

established play threats such as parent work schedules, digital media saturation, or parent

uncertainty about play.

Perhaps most importantly, I designed this intervention to be fun. Play is

significant because it is fun (Vickerius & Sandberg, 2006). Parents and children alike

benefit from fun connections. Parent involvement in in-home play through fun and

Page 166: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

154

convenient parent-child play was expected to increase the frequency of in-home play. In

addition, as an incentive for and a byproduct of the intervention fun, parent-child

interactions could have also informally promoted parental self-efficacy through support

of convenient playful interactions with their children (Boyce et al., 2017; Jones & Prinz,

2005). The primary way in which I designed this intervention to be non-invasive, was

designing it to fit within families rather than to encroach upon, to invite them to enjoy.

Rewarding Challenges

Families positive perceptions of Project Play Pack included detailed accounts of

how the take-home bag intervention supported Approaches to Learning parent-child

themed play. Two important elements relating the Approaches to Learning themes

became clear in the mixing for this first research question. My intentional choice to use

Approaches to Learning themes, because they had been identified in previous work as an

area of interest among Head Start families (Linder & Emerson, in press), proved to be an

advantageous choice. Parents’ descriptions of how the bags were novel and useful

included the freshness and utility of using the Approaches to Learning themes within

play. These sentiments were consistent despite reports of the Focus bag and the

Challenge bag being met with some resistance among children. Children initially

struggled to take turns, remain focused, and persist with the board game and the 2 in 1

puzzle. However, parent reports on both the weekly feedback forms and post interviews

regarding these two bags were positive.

Perhaps these themes and activities within the take-home play bag model made

unique space for families to engage in rewarding challenges. The mixed (data type)

Page 167: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

155

parent reports paint a picture of how these two particular bags posed fresh yet challenging

(described by many children’s resistance) Approaches to Learning skill practice while

maintaining play and likability (all families’ rated both bags as enjoyable). Similarly,

Graham, Rodger, and Ziviani (2010) explored families’ experiences with a parent-

directed intervention for occupational therapy with young children and found that their

10-week treatment aimed at supporting parents elicited three themes of parent

experiences: new learning, changes at home, and rewarding challenges. The mixed end

for parent perceptions of Project Play Pack share parallels with Graham et al.’s (2010

rewarding challenge theme. Families in Project Play Pack as well as their parent-directed

intervention for occupational therapy reported that despite difficult beginnings with some

challenging tasks overall the experience was positive. Intentional intervention design

features including situating the intervention within home contexts, sharing power with

parents, and allowing implementation to be flexible to fit unique parent-child dyads

proved successful in influencing parent-child interactions.

Feasibility Conclusions

Because the participants described the experience of the intervention as positive

and perceived the materials as fresh resources, families described their experiences as

appreciated and preferred within the intervention in detail in post-intervention interviews

and weekly feedback form responses. Parents described their perceptions of the purpose

of the intervention as supportive rather than overburdening. Moreover, quantitative

complimentary data reinforced how families found the play packs enjoyable (92%). The

positive reception of the intervention lends support to the feasibility of Project Play Pack,

Page 168: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

156

Math Pack, and other primarily open-ended take-home play bag models. The mixed data

highlights how Head Start families found the intervention novel, useful, enjoyable, and

purposeful. The intervention design features seemed successful in providing a palatable

and therefore utilized intervention for Head Start families. The intentional choice to look

at the experience of the intervention before zooming in on the influence of the

intervention is a contribution to home-based parent engagement intervention literature.

Despite my intervention design choices outlined above and the incorporation of

parent intervention literature suggestions such as: fewer meetings with more focused

goals (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2003; Kaminski et al., 2008), active and engaged

participant learning and skill practice (Brown & Talmi, 2005; Small et al., 2009), and

parent practice with their own child (Brooks, 2005; Kaminski et al., 2008), Head Start

parents found it difficult to participate in all of the Project Play Pack data collection. In

other words, my efforts to develop a feasible intervention or “treatment” seem to be

supported in the mixed data. Project Play Pack did not suffer the average attrition rate of

50% of home visiting programs (Gomby, 2005). However, the data collection,

particularly post-intervention, remained an impediment for parent engagement. The total

data ask included: a brief pre-intervention meeting (demographic survey, play frequency

form, parent-child play observation), weekly feedback forms, a brief post-intervention

meeting (play pack ranking task, post-intervention interview, and parent-child play

observation), and a post-intervention play frequency form completed the week following

the end of the intervention.

Page 169: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

157

It is notable that the Play Frequency form was the most challenging data to collect

pre and post intervention (10 out of 15 responses and 4 out of 13 responses, respectively).

The extremely low response rate was attributed to the inconvenience for families. The

post frequency form was sent home after the return of the last bag, and was to be returned

a week later. Because it was collected after the intervention, families struggled to return

it. The response rates could be a reflection of the burden they placed (remembering to

track amount of play time daily for a week and turn the form back in). Or the response

rates could have been a reflection of parents’ hesitance to disclose not playing enough or

playing more as a result of the intervention. Finally, the task of disclosing the time spent

engaging in parent-child play on paper could be uncomfortable for some parents who

dearly wish they could play more. The future research section provides suggestions for

revisions and adaptations to data sources and collection to make data collection more

feasible for families as well.

Nonetheless, this work contributes to the body of literature regarding the

feasibility of take-home bag delivery systems for parent-child engagement initiatives.

This work moves beyond the truncated emphasis on outcome data resulting from

families’ participation in take-home play bag interventions and provides important

insights into Head Start families’ experiences of the take-home play bag intervention.

The feedback form and post-intervention pairing provide a road map to generating a rich

picture of families’ utilization and experience of take-home play bag interventions that

could be replicated with other take-home bag formats. This work builds on the research

touting positively influence young children’s outcome through take-home bag designs

Page 170: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

158

(Abadiano & Turner, 2003; Barbour, 1998; Brand et al., 2014; Dever, 2001; Dever &

Burts, 2002; Grande, 2004; Kokoski & Patton, 1997; Vukelich, Christie, & Enz, 2001;

Zeece & Wallace, 2009) by providing a rich description of how these outcomes were

generated, through home-based, dynamically parent-guided, supported parent-child

playful interactions.

Influence Summary

Preschoolers learn through play (Broadhead, 2006; Han et al., 2010; Schickedanz,

1978; Schulz & Bonawitz, 2007). Although there is no recognized threshold for enough

in-home play, mounting threats to the time and interest in parent-child play interactions

are evident for families within the literature. Interventions promoting in-home play are

positioned to bring parents and children together with a myriad of beneficial outcomes

(children’s social emotional development: Manz & Bracaliello, 2016; children’s literacy:

Dever & Burts 2002, Zeece & Wallace, 2009; science: Kokoski & Patton, 1997; Parent-

child attachment: Levenstein et al., 2002). However, in-home play interventions

typically focus on manipulating parent interaction styles or bolstering particular outcomes

for children such as literary or math skills. In-home play promotion interventions

focused on changing or fixing parent interactions within play are at risk of overlooking

families’ funds of knowledge (Miller, 2010; Riojas-Cortez, Flores, & Clark, 2003) and

over shooting the main purpose of play, as connection, in exchange for a hyper-focus on

outcome gains. Parent-involvement interventions that attempt to fix parents can be

negatively perceived by families but feasibility data are rarely collected (Nievar,

Jacobson, Chen, Johnson, & Dier, 2011).

Page 171: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

159

In-home play interventions focused on specific children’s outcome changes can

become stressfully exclusive parent-child interactions focused on one content area at risk

of losing important elements of open-ended play and fun. These types of in-home play

interventions run the risk of transforming into homework, home “work”. Instead, Project

Play Pack was designed to support parent-child play through materials and suggestions in

an intentional effort to partner with parents in ways that could increase parent-child in-

home play. The framework for partnership within the Project Play Pack take-home play

bags was rooted in the provision of a fresh space (often novel materials, themes, and

suggestions) for parent-child play interactions to begin. The primarily open-ended nature

of the bags was embodied by avoiding prescriptions for how the play interaction should

end (prescribed rules or goals). The Approaches to Learning themes embodied in the

materials and children’s literature set the stage through fresh space and the purpose of the

primarily open-ended play interactions were to allow children to explore and learn by

creating their own game, rules, and goals (Bekker et al., 2010) within the Approaches to

Learning themes: Waiting, Creativity, Challenge, Turn Taking, Focus, and Feeling

Words. I explored the influence of the Project Play Pack intervention on participating

Head Start families by analyzing data related to play frequency, parent involvement in

parent-child play, and children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors in parent-child play.

This adds to a new wave of work taking closer looks at partnering with parent (Apple,

Farrar, Smith, 2012).

Page 172: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

160

Influence as a result of intervention design

This work contributes to the literature by illuminating the paths between take-

home play bag intervention designs and intervention influence. The two primary avenues

for in-home parent-child play intervention are home visiting (Barton et al., 2014) and

take-home bag designs (Zeece & Wallace, 2009). The literature representing these

intervention designs revealed cost prohibitive home visiting programs and the gaps in our

understanding of how families experience or implement take-home bag designs at home.

This dissertation contributes to the literature by offering new insights into a dynamic

take-home play bag design targeting parent-child in-home play interactions through a

cost efficient convenient delivery design. Promoting play for play’s sake, replacing

outcome focused homework style take-home bags with primarily open-ended take-home

play bags, utilizing Approaches to Learning themes of interest to Head Start parents, and

providing novel play suggestions for families served as intentional dynamic intervention

design choices aimed at influencing parent-child play interactions. My mixed methods

research design allowed for detailed depictions of influence.

Influences on Parent-child outcomes by increasing play

Play habits of preschoolers are often interchangeable with their learning habits.

Children practice persistence when they build and re-build block structures until they

reach the desired height without falling. Children take-turns with each other in playful

interactions such as game play or within a dramatic play discussion. Children practice

sharing objects, time, and space in play. A number of researchers explored and measured

preschoolers’ peer play for indicators of Approaches to Learning behaviors, realizing

Page 173: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

161

play as a hot bed for Approaches to Learning behavioral expressions. However, few

studies have explored parent-child play interactions and Approaches to Learning

(Fantuzzo & McWayne, 2002). Our limited understanding of Approaches to Learning

behaviors within parent-child play interactions constricts opportunities to support

children’s Approaches to Learning habits at home within the context of their families.

This dissertation contributes some of the first descriptions of preschoolers’ Approaches to

Learning behaviors within parent-child play and offers a new coding tool for identifying

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions.

Moreover, this intervention harnessed primarily open-ended playful interactions

to make additional space for children to explore and play with Approaches to Learning

themes and behaviors within parent-child play. The six Approaches to Learning themes

were facilitated through materials for exploratory, pretend, or game play (see Table 3.7).

Initial connections between the intervention intentions and Head Start family play

experiences are evident in the open-ended feedback form responses (unique play action

examples) and post intervention interviews. For example, the Waiting bag targeted

patience an element of emotional and behavioral self-regulation through a children’s

book about a character who has trouble waiting and waiting exploratory tools (sand

timers and binoculars). Parents provided examples within of unique ways they had

played with the materials and themes. The Waiting bag invited parents and children to

engage in open-ended exploratory and game play as many children played with

binoculars for the first time, explored the utility of looking to mitigate the challenges

(regulate behavior) of waiting, or made up their own games for looking and waiting. The

Page 174: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

162

Whitakers wrote about taking the binoculars in the car and playing eye-spy at stoplights.

The Jimenez’s also wrote about child initiated made-up games with the binoculars.

These open-ended interactions were targeted within the take-home play bags

because of the opportunities they provided for children to practice prosocial behaviors

(such as relating, comforting, helping, being creative, encouraging, and attempting to

settle conflicts) (Fantuzzo et al., 1998; Coolahan et al., 2000). Many of the prosocial

behaviors linked to open-ended play within the literature also closely related to

Approaches to Learning, or inclinations, dispositions, and skills children use in learning

(Hyson, 2008; Kagan et al., 1995). Because learning is a social process rooted within

interactions (Vygotsky, 1976), but very few studies have explored children’s Approaches

to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions (Fantuzzo & McWayne,

2002), part of the Project Play Pack intervention exploration included exploring

children’s Approaches to Learning interactions within the primary learning interactions

for young children, parent-child play. I found significant changes among children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play after receiving the

intervention. Some literature suggests that play facilitates children’s purposefulness and

goal direction (Anderson, 2002). Other work highlights play’s utility to support

exploration and persistence (Dennis & Stockall, 2015). The bottom-up coding of

children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions

contribute new insights for Approaches to Learning literature. The PALBO tool and

results highlighting significant changes among children’s Approaches to Learning

Page 175: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

163

behaviors within parent-child play emphasized the importance of parent-child play for

preschool children’s Approaches to Learning skills.

Influence Conclusions

Frequency. New and additional in-home play sessions promoted by Project Play

Pack increased the frequency of quality open-ended playful interactions among parents

and children. In-home play frequency was by far the most difficult data to collect.

Because of challenges to my data collection, I am unable to quantify an assertion of

change among in-home play frequency of intervention participants. However, the mixed

data (post parent interviews, weekly feedback sheets survey and open-ended items)

provided a vivid depiction of the frequency changes in parent-child in-home play as a

result of the take-home play bags. Participating families described in-home play changes

resulting from the intervention as more play or new play.

More play. Seven families were comfortable sharing about how the take-home

play bags incited more in-home play through the provision of opportunities, new ideas

(novelty of topics and materials), fresh incentive, tangible connections between play

themes and real life challenges. These families in-home play frequency increases were

reflected through bold recollections of how their children were dying to play with the

take-home play bag with their parents. While I cannot provide families with more hours

in the day, it seems that Project Play Pack provided new space for more play. These new

spaces appeared as children preferred the bags to digital media, as children used the

Friday bag delivery as a guaranteed play date with their parent, as parents drew upon the

bags to engage children in positive ways despite fatigue from single parenting, or as

Page 176: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

164

parents realized how their parent-child play routines had dissipated after the return of a

new baby as a result of the play bag delivery schedule. Families’ rich description of how

they played more were bolstered by their positive feedback form responses that revealed

patterns of action and creativity within play. Rather than checking a box or completing

the bags once as a “task”, the weekly feedback forms and interview data provided

multidimensional accounts of how parents extended, repeated, and even reveled in the

play bag activities with their children.

New play. Three families were uncomfortable reporting that the take-home play

bags helped them play more but instead described how their in-home play changed to

include new discussions and practice of different skills such as patience, turn taking,

persistence, and creativity. The same three families’ feedback forms supported this proud

resistance. These three families were also reluctant to rate bags as novel, not the first

time they had played something like the bag. However, these three families also reported

that all of the bags were appropriate, enjoyable, clear, and provided opportunities for

practicing Approaches to Learning skills. Each family described different preferences in

their open-ended weekly feedback form responses: all of the books were fresh and

particularly enjoyed (book to play pairings were new); the different and changing

manipulatives were used for fresh parent-child connections (fun to spend time, be silly,

and play together); Approaches to Learning topics were highly valued (played in different

ways and practiced Approaches to Learning behaviors within play). All three families

agreed that they would participate in the intervention if provided the chance again.

Importantly, these three families completed the intervention and expressed their

Page 177: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

165

assertions that they already consistently played with their children. This reflects the

flexibility of the intervention design; it was important to these parents that they were not

being fixed. I provided space for them to tell me so, continued to support them, and

retained them in the project.

Parent Involvement. The feasible home-based parent engagement intervention

targeting in-home play contributed to continued parent-involvement within parent-child

play interactions without the use of a home visitor. Preschoolers parent’s home-based

involvement is important for early learning success (Fantuzzo et al., 2004). Despite

persistently low parental participation in schools among minority and disadvantaged

communities (Davies, 2002; Floyd, 1998; Moles, 1993), research revealed minority

parents’ care greatly about their children’s education (Trumbull, Rothstein-Fisch,

Greenfield, & Quiroz, 2001). The multiple risk factors that face families in poverty

motivated my proposition that more frequent in-home play, via the Project Play Pack

intervention, might foster increased parent involvement in parent-child play. In other

words, more frequent play could serve to support parent play involvement through

practice and routine building. However, pre intervention parent-child play observations

revealed strong parent involvement in parent-child play from the start. Head Start

parents’ strength as players has been noted among other studies. Tamis-LeMonda,

Shannon, Cabrera, and Lamb (2004) found both Head Start mothers and fathers rendered

positive parent scores reflecting sensitivity, positive regard, and cognitive stimulation in

their analysis of free play parent-child interactions. Because parents participated in play

and facilitated play with frequency before the intervention began, it was difficult to find

Page 178: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

166

significant changes between pre and post intervention parent-child play observations.

Perhaps Head Start parents’ play involvement didn’t need to be fixed but simply

supported.

Approaches to Learning. The feasible home-based parent engagement

intervention targeting in-home play contributed to the children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors within parent-child play interactions. The overarching domain of Approaches

to Learning in this study included both cognitive (executive function) and behavioral self-

regulation (socially contextualized regulatory behaviors). The behavioral indicators

related to my conception of Approaches to Learning included: waiting, creativity,

persistence, turn-taking, attention, and emotional regulation. However, these Approaches

to Learning behavioral expressions have not been established with parent-child play

interactions within the literature.

Therefore, the bottom up coding of parent-child play interactions provided

important new insights into Head Start children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors

within parent-child play. A contribution to the literature, these emergent codes paint a

rich picture of the affordances that parent-child play interactions provide to Head Start

children’s approaches to learning skills. Previous literature on children’s Approaches to

Learning behaviors within peer play interactions reinforce the examples of Approaches to

Learning task practice found within this parent-child play. For example, the HSELOF

Approaches to Learning Sub-Domain indicators for creativity could be applied to

instances where children displayed creativity within pretend play scenarios in the parent-

child play observations with plastic dinosaurs (the Millers), cooking dinner (the Boyds

Page 179: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

167

and the O’Neals), taking care of a baby (the Browns and the Evans). Additionally, the

Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale’s indicators could be applied to instances where

children displayed emotional and behavioral regulation (or interpersonal responsibility,

PLBS; McDermott et al., 2011) in the parent-child play observations when: a child

struggles to open a pretend laptop, persists for some time, grimacing with challenge but

not becoming angry until his parent assists (the O’Neals), a child takes turns within play,

when mom is working with brother she does not interrupt (the Evans). These analysis

and results reveal instances where parent-child play interactions provided opportunities

for Approaches to Learning skill discussion, practice, and challenges.

Approaches to Learning supported. The feasible home-based parent engagement

intervention targeting in-home play through Approaches to Learning themed play bags

promoted children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play. Previous

research notes the ample opportunities for children to practice social skills within play,

such as satisfying their wants and needs, managing the behavior of others and

themselves, expressing feelings or ideas, utilizing fantasy, and gathering and sharing

information to and from others (Athanasiou, 2007; Craig-Unkefer & Kaiser, 2002; Zigler

& Bishop-Josef, 2004). These opportunities are easily related to children’s Approaches

to Learning skills (curiosity, creativity, cooperativeness, and persistence). The Project

Play Pack intervention included six take-home play bags created to support six different

Approaches to Learning themes: patience, creativity, persistence, self-regulation,

sustained attention, and emotional regulation. In addition to curated children’s literature

and novel manipulatives, each bag included a tip sheet utilizing casual language to

Page 180: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

168

provide gentle suggestions for play interactions relating to each theme/materials and open

invitations for parents and children to make the experience their own within their home

environment. The bags promoted exploratory, pretend, and game play with open-ended

or flexible avenues for families to explore Approaches to Learning themes in

personalized ways. Similar to the home-school partnerships described in Riojas-Cortez et

al.’s (2003) work, these types of play were prescribed but families were welcomed to

infuse their own expertise or funds of knowledge. The bags provided materials and space

for families to apply their unique ways of knowing and doing to the Approaches to

Learning themes.

The PALBO pre and post comparison along with the bottom up pre and post

coding exploration assert that the Project Play Pack intervention promoted children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play. Children displayed more

Approaches to Learning behaviors within freely selected parent-child play interactions

after receiving the intervention. Although helpful for inciting play, the specific Project

Play Pack materials were not necessary to illicit Approaches to Learning behaviors,

children and parents interacted with toys that they found in Head Start classrooms in both

the pre and post intervention observations. It seems that six week of Approaches to

Learning themed take-home play bags influenced children’s play habits. Children

displayed more frequent behavioral regulation, appropriate expression of emotion, and

creativity among children in the post observations.

Page 181: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

169

Significance

This work contributes to three primary gaps in the literature noted in the first

chapter of this paper. First, this dissertation reveals children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors and skill practice within parent-child play. This work expands beyond the

realm of preschooler’s peer play to take an important look at the relevance of parent-child

play interactions for preschoolers Approaches to Learning skills. The emergent

Approaches to Learning behaviors of Head Start children while playing with family

members were highlighted through bottom up coding and the PALBO. This work asserts

the embedded nature of Approaches to Learning behaviors among Head Start children’s

parent-child play. Second, this dissertation provides new details regarding the efficacy

and feasibility of take-home play bag home-based parent child engagement initiatives for

influencing the play of families in poverty. The Project Play Pack design and data

sources provide original contributions to the literature outlining feasible and responsive

practices for take-home play bag play interventions for families in poverty. The rich

descriptions from families regarding the experience of the intervention supply strong

rationale for researchers to consider the efficacy of designing interventions to support

rather than fix. Finally, the combination of Project Play Pack participants’ positive

experience and the novel exploration of Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-

child play culminated in the conclusion that the take-home play bag interventions

influenced children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors, through support of Approaches

to Learning themed parent-child in-home play.

Page 182: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

170

Moreover, the capstone of this work is the prominence of Head Start parent voices

about their experience of the Project Play Pack intervention. Their rich descriptions

inform new perspectives for interventions aimed at supporting families of color in

poverty. These perspectives are important as culturally and contextually responsive

interventions have a greater likelihood of participant acceptance and attendance (Branson

& Davis, 2007; Julion, Breitstein, & Waddell, 2012). The experiences and insights

shared by the Project Play Pack intervention participants contribute to emergent lines of

research conceptualizing parent involvement among marginalized groups. Mirroring the

Kaomea’s (2012) recommendations for future approaches for indigenous involvement in

education interventions, my work provides support for the appreciation of within-group

variation. Project Play Pack participants were enrolled in Head Start based on a shared

socio-economic need, however families varied by parents’ level of education, work

schedule consistency, take-home play bag preference, and description of take-home play

bag benefits. These demographic and experiential variations among families emphasized

their unique strengths, challenges, and aspirations. The mixed data results underscore the

importance of Project Play Pack’s primarily open-ended activity design for

accommodating these within group differences.

In addition, Kaomea’s (2012) recommendations included aims to support and

strengthen indigenous families, rather than replacing them. Kaomea’s (2012) case study

of Native Hawaiian families with preschool-aged children puts forth powerful narratives

from parents looking for support that respects rather than replaces. I proposed similar

notions in previous chapters, describing how the Project Play pack design was always

Page 183: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

171

intended to support rather than to fix. A main vein of respect and support as opposed to

replacing and fixing was the practice of sharing of power and opportunities for agency

through the suggestion of primarily open-ended activities, the invitation to make

activities their own, and the request for feedback on the experience throughout the

intervention. Goodall and Montgomery (2014) questioned parental involvement versus

engagement in schools with important implications among the two distinctions in terms

of agency. Their reconceptualization of a continuum beginning with parental

involvement and working toward parental engagement utilized a triadic model for

including child, parent, and school was strikingly similar to the triadic interaction model

utilized in the Project Play Pack design. Goodall and Montgomery (2014) asserted that

agency should lie primarily with parents as co-educators. Finally, they stressed the

importance of relational trust, or common understanding of what both parties (parents

and teachers) are trying to achieve (Bryk & Schneider, 2002; Goodall & Montgomery,

2014). The Project Play Pack intervention embodied these new directions in parent

engagement research. Moreover, this dissertation provides a framework for intervention

and mixed methods research designs to ensure these approaches are attempted and

working as intended for Head Start families.

Limitations and Attempts to Control for Challenges

It is difficult to generalize these findings outside of Head Start families in the

Southeastern United States. Nonetheless, one of the primary goals of this work was to

unpack the personalized experiences of a take-home play bag parent-child play

intervention for Head Start families. The resulting rich picture will be helpful beyond

Page 184: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

172

direct generalizations, but rather a full bodied recommendation for designing and

implementing home-based interventions for families of young children in poverty that

prioritize flexibility and power sharing. In addition, the small sample size afforded on a

meager dissertation study budget threatened to limit the ability to extrapolate meaningful

results. However, the quasi-experimental embedded mixed methods design facilitated the

creation of a resonant picture of the intervention experience among the small sample.

The combination of qualitative and quantitative data sources and mixing supported strong

means for triangulation of results.

The Project Play Pack intervention was positively received by participants but

still faced challenges collecting all of the post-intervention data. Positive participant

descriptions of the intervention reinforced the efficacy of the triadic interaction model

design inspirations and novel approaches to non-invasive intervention design features

discussed at length above. These efforts reflect intentional research and intervention

design choices aimed at mitigating the challenges of engaging families of preschool

children in poverty.

Finally, despite research design efforts to generate robust support for the influence

of the Approaches to Learning take-home play bag intervention, some outcome measures

failed to provide adequate support for findings. Specifically, complementary quantitative

data of in-home play frequency did not generate conclusive evidence of change. While

the qualitative parent-child play observation data and transformed PALBO data support

the increase in children’s Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play

Page 185: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

173

after the intervention, a complementary performance measure of children’s Approaches

to Learning would strengthen the results.

Future Research

Future research is needed to continue to assert the utility of take-home play bag

intervention designs for home-based parent engagement interventions. First, replications

of this intervention with larger samples and stronger outcome measures could continue to

contribute to the literature. The inclusion of a larger sample size and a broader

geographical spectrum provided by a multi-site research project could be beneficial in a

future study. Similar convenient delivery modes should continue to be explored. Instead

of the time and cost of delivery bags to Head Start centers by car, I would like to explore

the use of flat rate shipping boxes in the future.

Continued revisions and adaptations to data sources and collection procedures

could also facilitate larger sample sizes and greater feasibility for Head Start families.

Future studies should explore the use of in-home self-recording (audio or video) of

parent-child play interactions, applications providing secure and convenient online data

uploads, post-family interviews over Skype or Google hangout to encourage and facilitate

greater participation in post-intervention data collection. Moreover, new avenues for

measuring in-home play frequency must be sought. Perhaps text message submission of

daily parent hours worked and parent-child play for seven days before and seven days

following the intervention would be preferable to the paper and pencil play frequency

form. Perhaps rephrasing the frequency inquiry would imply less of a question of if Head

Start families are playing and focus more on what/how they are playing. Successfully

Page 186: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

174

reframing the rationale for this type of data collection could elicit more consistent

responses but still inform in-home play frequency and play habit research questions.

Future research should continue to explore dynamic interventions for families of

preschool children experiencing poverty with attention to their experience of the

intervention in comparison with researcher intentions.

Finally, future research is needed to continue to explore preschool children’s

Approaches to Learning behaviors within parent-child play interactions. Replication and

validation studies of the Play Approaches to Learning Behaviors Observation tool,

PALBO, are needed. Moreover, future research should consider children’s Approaches

to Learning behaviors across contexts and from different angles: parent-child play, peer

play, and early learning engagement via observational coding, parent report, teacher

report, and appropriate child performance measures. These new lines of research

asserting the importance of parent-child play for children’s Approaches to Learning

behaviors should be extended and included in rationales to provide additional support

and attention to parent-child in-home play.

Page 187: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

175

APPENDICES

Page 188: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

176

Appendix A Play Pack Tip Sheet

LET’S PLAY! Please feel free to repeat this task as many times as you would like throughout the week. Remember, these tips are just a starting guide, this task should be fun, we encourage you to start with these ideas but then expand and make it your own. If your conversations turn in different directions than what is outlined below, it is okay! Be as creative as you would like with your child! Before returning this Play Pack, please complete the feedback sheet.

1. Read the story, How are you peeling? with your child. As you read, be sure to discuss the silly faces on each page. While reading, you could ask questions like,

• What kind of face is that?! • What does your happy face look like? • What does your sad face look like? • Do any of these silly fruits and veggies look like me?

2. Talk with your child about all of the silly fruits and vegetable faces. The faces are funny! But I like the feeling words. Sometimes it helps to say the way I am feeling out loud. Sometimes I use my face like these fruits and veggies to show my feelings. Sometimes I need to use these words to help other people understand me. Share some exaggerated faces with your child.

3. Share the toy materials from the play pack with your child. Look in the mirror and make a face (pass it to them to try), explore all of the other pieces (google eyes and sunglasses). Have fun exploring facial expressions together, feel free to laugh and be as creative as you like. You might ask questions like:

• What’s the angriest face you can make? (Watch and hold the mirror so that they can see themselves)

• What’s the happiest face you can make? (Watch and hold the mirror so that they can see themselves)

• (make a sad face and ask them) Can you guess what I am feeling? • Is it hard to tell the way that I feel when I’m wearing the sunglasses?

Take turns looking in the mirror, making faces, and guessing emotions. Try to use as many different feeling words that you can think of (you can use the pages of the book if you get stuck). You might ask questions like:

• How do you know when I am sad? • How do you know when your friends are happy? • How do you tell your friends (or your teacher) you are angry?

4. You can continue these games, working with the mirror or the glasses or both for as long as you like. Work together to play and even include siblings in emotion guessing and naming. Maybe try to taking it to the next level, by encouraging your child to create some faces on paper. The paper, glue, and googly eyes can be a fun way to recreate the silly faces of the book and practice even more feeling words.

Page 189: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

177

endix 2 Play Packs: A

Reason to Squeeze-In

Some Play: Feedback Form Waiting Theme

This task was appropriate for my child Agree Disagree We enjoyed playing with this bag Agree Disagree The directions were clear Agree Disagree This provided an opportunity to practice a skill (XXXXX) Agree Disagree This provided an opportunity for us to talk about XXXX in a useful way Agree Disagree This was the first time my child and I did a task like this Agree Disagree What understandings (or misunderstandings) did your child show when participating in this task? What questions or conversations did you add to the task (any examples of ways you personalized the activity to you and your child) as you were playing? Or did your child improvise with the materials, if so, how? How did you continue this task throughout the week (perhaps with different materials or situations)? What was your favorite part of the play interaction? Do you have any other comments or questions about the activity?

Date: ____________

How many times did you and your child play with this bag? _______________

Appendix B Feedback Sheet

Page 190: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

178

Appendix C Semi-Structured Interview Protocol

1. Let’s take a look at this document with all of the bags. What were some things your child liked/disliked about the bags?

What were some things you liked/disliked about the bags?

Can you rank them most liked to least liked? Why did you rank them this way?

Have you ever read these books before?

2. Tell me about a time when you were playing with the bags at home?

- did the bags bring anything new to your play? If yes, please explain.

- did you and your child play more because you had the bags? Why or why not?

3. How do you think children learn things like waiting, pushing through a challenge, focusing, or turn taking behaviors or skills? What about creativity? Or feeling words?

Do you think the bags, books or activities, had anything to do with those labels? Which ones?

Why or Why not?

4. Did the bags allow you to play with your child in ways that you wouldn’t have usually? Can you give me an example? [did you the toys or games you played with had anything to do with social emotional development?]

6. Did the bags allow you to have discussions (or talk about) with your child that you wouldn’t have usually?

Can you give me an example? [did you feel as those these talks had anything to do with social emotional development?]

7. Do you think it is important to talk out loud as you help your child play or solve a problem? Why or why not?

Page 191: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

179

Appendix D Play Bag Preference Rank Task

Page 192: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

180

Appendix E In-Home Play Frequency Form

“How often did you play together this week?”

Jot down about how many minutes of play you had with your child each day

this week.

Jot down about how many minutes of play your child had with another member of your family (parent, grandparent, or sibling).

How much time did you spend playing together each day?

In a different ink, How much time did your child spend playing with another family member?

Fri Sat Sun Mon Tues Wed Thurs

About how much time were you unable to play because of work or other responsibilities? (Note work hours with a “W”; Note other responsibilities with “O”)

Fri Sat Sun Mon Tues Wed Thurs

Page 193: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

181

Appendix F Parental Play Involvement Coding Sheet

Adapted from Tejagupta, 1990; Watts & Barnett, 1973

Codes defined: Participation (PT)—actively joining/engaging in an activity through coordinated actions,

turn taking exchanging vocalizations (ie. Producing sounds relevant to the paly context) or play materials or other forms indicating complementary involvement relevant to such play activities or situations (acting as a play partner, showing equally shared interest or making physical contributions to the activity

Facilitation (FT)—Encouraging an activity through supplementing comments or materials (offering suggestions, play ideas, or themes; teaching; directing or modeling certain play skills, praising, assisting, helping, or locating play objects)

Neutral (NT)--Observing an activity without negative comments or actions (e.g., watching the ongoing play activity, talking with other players about something irrelevant tot the ongoing play situation, paying attention to the paly activity but doing something else)

Restriction (RT)—Disapproving, prohibiting or preventing an activity through distracting interests or expressing negative feelings/comments toward such play situation according to family’s value (e.g., not allowing the child to play with imaginary friends, discontinuing the ongoing play, refusing to help in play, showing physical/verbal restraint to the other players.

Noninvolvement (NI)—Engaging in different activity without paying attention to the ongoing play (reading a book, playing on phone, caring for another child)

Interval Length: 15 seconds Total Video Time: 10 minutes 01.15 01.30 01.45 02.00 02.15 2.30 02.45 03.00 03.15 3.30 PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

03.45 04.00 04.15 04.30 04.45 05.00 0515 05.30 05.45 06.00 PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

06.15 06.30 06.45 07.00 07.15 07.30 07.45 08.00 08.15 08.30 PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

08.45 09.00 09.15 09.30 09.45 10.00 10.15 10.30 10.45 11.00 PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

PT FT NT RT

NI

Page 194: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

182

Appendix G Play Approaches to Learning Behaviors Observation (PALBO) Coding Sheet

WHICH FORM OF SELF REGULATION DOMINATES THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT? Pick one

EBSR: Child manages actions, words, and behaviors with increasing independence o Demonstrates control over actions & words amidst challenges (wanting something

someone else has, frustration about inabilities or mistakes, emotion is clear) o Manages behaviors according to expectations (behaviors appropriately mold to fit

situation) o Waits for turns (waits in lines or for turns with toys) o Refrains from aggressive behavior towards others when angry, frustrated, or sad o Describes the consequences of behavior o Describes the effects their behavior may have been on others

CSR: Cognitive Self-Regulation (Executive Functioning) Child demonstrates an increasing ability to control impulses, maintain focus, and think flexibly & problem solve

o Able to stop an activity to transition to another with adult guidance, calmly o Engages in purposeful play (a goal is clear) o Completes challenging or not preferred tasks (persists) despite frustration, o Returns with focus to an activity after being interrupted o Able to retell a story in the correct order & includes relevant details o Able to follows detailed, multi-step directions, sometimes with reminders o Tries different strategies to solve problems

NBR: No Behavioral Regulation Examples, the child is o Idle, wandering, staring, being aggressive or crying or Monotonous, repeating actions or words (hitting something over and over) o the child is engaged in playing but no regulation examples are evident

01.30 02.00 2.30 03.00 3.30

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

SUS SUS SUS SUS SUS

04.00 04.30 05.00 05.30 06.00 E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n SUS SUS SUS SUS SUS

06.30 07.00 07.30 08.00 08.30 E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n SUS SUS SUS SUS SUS

09.00 09.30 10.00 10.30 11.00 E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n

E.B.S.R C.S.R N.B.R.

EMO y n i

CRE y n SUS SUS SUS SUS SUS

Page 195: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

183

EMO: Emotion: DOES THE CHILD DISPLAY EMOTIONAL BSR AT ANY TIME WITHIN THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT? Y or N Adapted from Emotional BSR: Child manages and expresses emotions Expresses emotions appropriate to situation Seeks assistance when emotions are too intense Utilizes a range of coping strategies (seek support, use words, take deep breaths) CRE: Creativity: DOES THE CHILD DISPLAY CREATIVITY AT ANY TIME WITHIN THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT? Y or N Creativity: Child expresses creativity in making, constructing, or pretending Child uses imagination in play and interactions with others Engages in pretend play Uses imagination with materials to create stories Makes works of art

Uses objectives or materials to represent something else during play (pretend play: complex or simple: assigns roles to self and parent and dictates a story or labels a block tower a castle

SUS: Sustained Attention: DOES THE CHILD MAINTAIN THE SAME LINE OF PLAY ACROSS THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT? If so, draw a dash through the dividing cells. When play changes draw an X over the dividing cells. If a parent redirects play in a way that breaks the line draw an X and write “mom” or “dad” or “other”. Sustained Attention: Maintains focus on activities for extended periods of time Engages in purposeful play (a goal is clear) for extended periods of times When coding is complete go back and count the longest line of play (the largest number of connected 30 second segments) record it. Coding Totals: Count the number of each label and record here:

Code Raw Count % by 20 = Percentage EBSR

CSR NBR Emo Yeses Cre Yeses

Longest Sustained Play, recorded largest # of connected segments =

Page 196: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

184

Appendix H Sample of PALBO Phase Approaches to Learning Behavior Categorization

EBSR: Child manages actions, words, and behaviors with increasing independence

1 Demonstrates control over actions & words amidst challenges (wanting something someone else has, frustration about inabilities or mistakes, emotion is clear)

2 Manages behaviors according to expectations (behaviors appropriately mold to fit situation)

3 Waits for turns (waits in lines or for turns with toys)

4 Refrains from aggressive behavior towards others when angry, frustrated, or sad

5 Describes the consequences of behavior

6 Describes the effects their behavior may have been on others

2 – 2PRE- throwing the Frisbee); ends before full time is up 11:30-12:00

2-3POST- play being facilitated by his mother 7:00-7:30

2-6PRE- Child cleans up current center with intentions of moving to another, accepts gma’s expectations for cleaning up

4- 6PRE- Child makes a counting mistake, gma corrects it, pauses, and re-counts without getting upset

2-7PRE- Child looks for a triangle per mom’s request for 30 seconds, pointing to the wrong shape over and over but continuing to look per her request, her expectations

4- 7PRE-child can’t make bristle blocks fit together after multiple attempts, sighs, and turns to adult for help calmly (x2)emo

3/2- 8PRE- child attends to mom’s quizzing, waits to take puzzle piece, answers, manages mom’s expectations, this feels much more like teaching than playing

8/2 -8POST- child playing restaurant in dramatic play center, seeks help to write down order, spell banana, has to ask and start over more than once, shows frustration in voice but continues [2:30-4:00]

2-12PRE- child is engaged in pretend play and declares, “I want some grapes”, Mom sets expectations, “get you a bowl”, child agrees and finds a bowl, Mom sets another expectation, “now rinse um off in the sink” child does, “now set at the table and eat um” child smiles almost laughing and do so

Page 197: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

185

1/2-9POST- child enthusiastically smiles and engages with mom using magnifying glasses to “look for dinosaurs”, she challenges him to count the dinos, he struggles a little but makes a concerted effort, matching his mom’s play expectations…. [30-1:00]

2-9POST- child manages behaviors according to play scenario, dino play fighting is in line with the pretend plot he is constructing, he fights with mom’s dino, fighting but not too rough, she matches, adds dialogue, he continues the play scenario, fights back, and moves the story along, all the while never exceeding mom’s expectations for roughness, smiling and showing enjoyment, and creatively constructing the story line. [2:00-2:30]

2/1-9POST- child and mom discuss t-rexes and t-rex arms, mom models, child copies, mom sets strict t-rex arms expectations, can’t move your arms from elbow up, they laugh and practice together, child making only one mistake but effort-fully making it work [3:30-4:00] [4:00-4:30]

3-9POST- child waits for a turn to talk and/or play while sister complains to mom, he attends to sister and turns to mom to offer a solution at the end of the 30 seconds [5:00-5:30].

1-9POST- child expresses frustration, mom asks what’s wrong, he says, “I’m trying to make the cage!” she provides a suggestion and he begins again [9:00-9:30]

1-9POST- child expresses frustration, child takes deep breaths as he builds, shoulders rise and fall (without suggestion from mom) [9:30-10:00]

Page 198: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

186

1-9POST- mom encourages child to take his time, he continues slowly focused [10:00-10:30]

CSR:

Cognitive Self-Regulation (Executive Functioning) Child demonstrates an increasing ability to control impulses, maintain focus, and think flexibly & problem solve

7 Able to stop an activity to transition to another with adult guidance, calmly

8 Engages in purposeful play (a goal is clear)

9 Completes challenging or not preferred tasks (persists) despite frustration,

10 Returns with focus to an activity after being interrupted

11 Able to retell a story in the correct order & includes relevant details

12 Able to follows detailed, multi-step directions, sometimes with reminders

13 Tries different strategies to solve problems

2 – 6PRE- Child’s block building is interrupted by gma’s shape and letter quizzing, child attends & answers her questions

10- 3POST- mother interrupts him when he is trying to cook 7:30-8:00

8- 3POST- engages in purposeful play; still being facilitated by mother, attempting to make muffins by himself 9:30-10:00

10- 4POST interrupted by mother but continues to play 1:00-1:30

7-4POST- able to stop an activity to transition 7:00-7:30

8 – 6PRE- Child names baby and says “her thirsty” setting the goal of the dramatic play as babysitting

9-7PRE- Child is building, stands a block up, it falls down, he stands it back up again and make sure it stays this time

10-7POST- child engages in purposeful play, mom quizzes fake food vocab, child responds, and goes back to pretend play scenario

9-7POST- child looks for pretend cheese in his dramatic play for 30 seconds, in two places, until he finds it (persists)

10- 8PRE- child maintains purposeful puzzle play, mom quizzes phonics, child responds, and goes back to puzzle work

10 -8POST-child maintains play focus and theme after an outsider interrupts play, child

Page 199: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

187

waits quietly while mom interacts with adult, once adult leaves the child resumes play with mom

8/13- 12POST – child builds with wooden blocks, stands them on their end, lines them up in order to balance roof pieces on top [purposeful play, careful strategy]

9-9POST- child makes a mistake including lizards in his count, mom corrects and asks him to do it again, he listens and tries again, getting it right the 2nd time and sticking with her in this structured “play” [1:00-1:30]

9-9POST- child shifts play towards pretend play with dinos making the pretend scenario the clear goal of the play, voicing the dinos, having them eat, and beginning a fight with mom’s dino [1:30-2:00]

9-9POST- child pauses (briefly, not for the whole interaction) for sibling to talk to mom and then resumes pretend dino play scenario [2:30-3:00] [3:30-3:30]

8-9POST- child maintains purposeful play goal by selecting a target dino for the dino house he will build, it takes him a while to find the one he wants [6:00-6:30] [6:30-7:00]

8-9POST- child maintains purposeful play goal by finding the target dino for the dino house, expressing happiness that he found it, and beginning to build [7:30-8:00]

8-9POST- child maintains purposeful play goal by silently and intently focusing building dino house [8:00-8:30] [8:30-9:00]

NBR:

No Behavioral Regulation Examples, the child is not engaged

Page 200: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

188

14 Idle, wandering, staring, being aggressive or crying or

15 Monotonous, repeating actions or words (hitting something over and over)

16 child is engaged in playing but no

regulation examples are evident

16- 6PRE – child abruptly suggests a change in play, transitions

14-7PRE – child is silent and aimless for majority of the clip, mom suggests he go get another box of blocks and child obliges but doesn’t seem to have any opinions or preferences. The child’s choice to follow instructions doesn’t seem effortful or an example of regulation.

16- 12PRE- mom is calling out set up for pretend play and gathering materials, child is listening and moving but not contributing or taking any action of his own related to play she is describing

14- 15PRE- child complies with mom’s request once during 30 second segment but is looking around, moving slowly, not engaged

16-9POST- child waits and listens to mom’s idea about what to do next 30 second segment but is mostly idle [5:30-6:00]

16-9POST- child responds mom’s questions once during 30 second segment but is mostly distracted by pulling out new dinos from the box

EMO:

Emotion: DOES THE CHILD DISPLAY EMOTIONAL BSR AT ANY TIME WITHIN THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT?

Adapted from Emotional BSR:

17 Child manages and expresses emotions 18 Expresses emotions appropriate to situation 19 Seeks assistance when emotions are too

intense 20 Utilizes a range of coping strategies (seek

support, use words, take deep breaths)

17- 7PRE-child can’t make bristle blocks fit together after multiple attempts, sighs, and turns to adult for help calmly (x2)

Page 201: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

189

CRE:

Creativity: DOES THE CHILD DISPLAY CREATIVITY AT ANY TIME WITHIN THE 30 SECOND SEGMENT?

Creativity: Child expresses creativity in making, constructing, or pretending

Child uses imagination in play and interactions with others

Engages in pretend play

Uses imagination with materials to create stories

Makes works of art

Uses objectives or materials to represent something else during play (pretend play: complex or simple: assigns roles to self and parent and dictates a story or labels a block tower a castle

6PRE- Child declares, “we will build a big castle”

6PRE- building with wooden blocks, “this is how we make it like a brick (house)”

7PRE- child shows engagement and starts manipulating bristle block on own, begins making

7PRE- child begins manipulating little bristle block people in a pretend play scenario

Page 202: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

190

Appendix I Feedback Form Emergent Theme Development

Focus Question Notes Summary Understanding/misunderstanding

7 sharing and taking turns Child understanding of theme

Respondents display dynamic interactions with the focus bag, choosing to highlight how their child understood the theme (3), the book (2), and the task (1). A third (3/9) of respondents noted trouble with the task (chutes and ladders). However, these descriptions indicate practice and discussion embedded with play related to the bag theme, for example “he didn’t want to do it at first” “She sit and listen before we start playing while I read the rules”. These respondents provide an example of resistance that was worked through and an example of a child attending to (focus) on parent reading rules.

11 he didn't know what to do at first Child trouble with task Child success with task

8

It was hard to get her to understand at first that we had to take turns, she was very excited about taking her turn

Child trouble with task Child success with task Practice another target skill

3 xxxxx understood we had to take turns playing

Child understanding of theme

12

my child understood how all the different animals and people saw the cat differently

Child understanding the book

4 she understands turn-taking but doesn't like it

Child understanding of theme Child trouble with task

6 what was going on with the animals, we didn't get the game

Child understanding the book Family trouble with task

14 she sit and listen before we start playing while I read the rules

15

She enjoyed reading the book and really enjoyed the game she know how to count as we went along and how to understand the game

Child liked materials & book Child liked activity Child understanding the task

Questions or Additions

7 why you have to take turns

Questions parent added to promote task Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking)

Respondents display involvement in their children’s at home play with the focus bag, by providing

Page 203: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

191

11 examples of questions they asked to promote the task (3), theme (1), or other related regulatory theme (2). Two respondents spoke directly to focusing, a child’s trouble focusing and a discussion about strategies for staying focused which invited the child to make suggestions. Respondents tied focus and turn taking closely with the focus bag materials (predictable book and board game). Other respondents reported on how they extended the focus bag to other play experiences such as using the materials to play a different game (2), expanding the game with siblings (1). Belongs below

8

I asked her what other situations required her to wait her turn. When she didn't win at chutes and ladders it was a good opportunity for us to talk about being able to lose without being upset

Questions parent added to promote task Questions parent added to promote related theme Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking & feeling words (emo literacy dealing with losing))

3

Xxxxxx wanted to spin for me when it was my turn and tell me which number I had

Discussion to promote task Unrelated

12

we did not play the game as the instructions stated, we made up our own way

Extended materials to new parent-child created games

4

aalayah changed all the rules to play the way she wants to play so that conversation was interesting

Extended materials to new parent-child created games

6

she was in and out. She would pay attention sometimes then go do something else

Child trouble with task/theme

14 is it my turn now? She asked can her brother play

Questions parent added to promote task Extending activity to include siblings

15

She asked what did the cat do and she described what each animal was doing. I asked how does she pay attention and she was like stay focus. She was like grandma your turn is after me and she won the game

Questions parent added to promote theme Child understood task Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking)

Continued the task

7 practiced taking turns and sharing

Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking)

Respondents reported on how

Page 204: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

192

11 xxxxxxx and his brothers played a lot

Repeating task Practice target skill

they extended the focus bag to other play experiences such: noting how they repeated the activity (5), using the materials to play a different game (2 from above plus 3), expanding the game with siblings (1 from above plus 1), extending the staying-focused theme to other contexts such as care routines (1), or practicing other regulatory themes again throughout the week (3).

8 we played with just the two of us at first then added a third player

Repeating the task Practice target skill Extending task with other players

3

I let elias explain to me how to play the game and he changed the rules a little :)

Extended materials to new parent-child created games

12 we made up games to play, so we could focus on taking turns

Extended materials to new parent-child created games Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking)

4 playing the game the way she wants to play

Extended materials to new parent-child created games

6 did it the same way

Repeated activity/Practice target skill

14

taking turns in the bathroom sink, getting one putting up the forks and spoons

Extending theme to care routines & Practice another target skill (behavioral regulation in turn taking)

15

she enjoyed the game so much we had to play 2 times we also read the book together.

Repeating the task/Practice target skill Rereading the book/Practice target skill

Favorite Part 7 the game Family liked materials

11 playing as a family Parent enjoyed spending time with child (&or siblings

8

3 we loved the story! Family liked book

12 playing chutes and ladders Family liked materials/task

Page 205: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

193

4 how she changed the rules 3 different was amusing

Parent enjoyed seeing child improvise

Parents described their favorite part of the focus bag as spending time playing with their child (3) and watching tier child improvise.

6 she liked reading the book Child liked book

14 well see her smile as we all play together

Parent enjoyed spending time with child (seeing child enjoy play)

15

She favorite part of the book was the cat. I like how she interacted into the game, she continued and told me the coloros. Nd I enjoyed how she helped me read the book

Child liked book Parent enjoyed spending time with child Unrelated

Questions

7 none

11 none

8 none

3 none

12 none

4 none

6 none

14

she is really helpful, this just gave me more reason to have her help much more with the least little things around the house

15

I really enjoyed doing this project with lacyn, the game, the book, and describing the animals

Family liked materials/task

Overall: Respondents display dynamic interactions with the focus bag, choosing to highlight how their child understood the theme (3), the book (2), and the task (1). Respondents display involvement in their children’s at home play with the focus bag, by providing examples of questions they asked to promote the task (3), theme (1), or other related regulatory theme (2). Two respondents wrote about the focus theme referring to, a child’s trouble focusing on the game and a discussion about strategies for staying focused which invited the child to make suggestions. Other respondents reported on how they extended the focus bag to other play

Page 206: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

194

experiences such: noting how they repeated the activity (5), using the materials to play a different game (5), expanding the game with siblings (2), extending the staying-focused theme to other contexts such as care routines (1), or practicing other regulatory themes again throughout the week (3).

A third (3/9) of respondents noted trouble with the focus bag task (chutes and ladders). However, these descriptions indicate practice and discussion embedded within play related to the focus theme, for example “he didn’t want to do it at first” “She sit and listen before we start playing while I read the rules”. These respondents provided examples of resistance that was worked through and a child attending to (focusing) on parent reading rules. Moreover, in addition to liking the activity (4) and the book (5), parents described their favorite part of the focus bag as spending time playing with their child (3) and watching tier child improvise. Discussion Points: Even though respondents noted the difficulty of focusing and taking turns within the chutes and ladders game, their descriptions of what they liked about the task showed that they understood that the activity could be flexible and still work on target or related regulatory skills. It is important to note, respondents tied focus and turn taking closely with the focus bag materials (predictable book and board game). Do I think this is a good, bad, or irrelevant thing?

Page 207: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

195

Appendix J Post Interview Emergent Coding Example

Transcription PID 2 Notes Looking at the bag ranking task…

A: I wonder if there was one that you guys really liked or one that you didn’t like…

M: He LOVED the pizza. That was his favorite

A: Was it his favorite book or his favorite toys?

M: Both of them really. He loved the book and he loved the activity. He had everybody in there dressing up as a pizza man and was delivering pizza. It was fun. I actually had fun with it too.

A: Im so glad to hear that.

M: that would be the second one (Challenge/puzzle) he liked.

A: Really the puzzles?

M: Mmm Hmm he likes puzzles. He loves puzzles

A: How did you guys handle separating the two puzzles out?

M: um, him and grandma worked on one and me and his older brother worked on the other one. And then after we got everything together…. 2:00

M:.. he took them apart and me and him did it again. We had a race.

A: you could tell me too (to the child)

C: me want it

A: you want the pizza book?

C: yes

A: you want the pizza hat?

C: yes

M: he really loved it all

A: Interesting information. Did you think the playdough smelled like pizza?

Child preference (creativity bags) and book. Extension, had “everyone” dressing up Parent preference for creativity bag too Child preference for puzzle (challenge bag) Extension of the challenge bag activity

Page 208: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

196

Appendix K

Post Interview Emergent Coding Scheme Research Question 1: Sub Question A: What are parent perceptions of the take-home play bag intervention? The experience, purpose, and support of the intervention

Preferences Child

Parents

Likes Dislikes Favorites

Likes Dislikes Favorites

Extensions Of play

Of Theme Of Play & Theme

Of Books

Novelty of Books:

All Novel Almost all Novel

Support Working on Approaches to Learning Task

Connecting with siblings

Parent-Child bonding

Purpose To practice approaches to learning skills

To play more (general)

To connect more

[responses about opportunities for

bonding]

Research Question 1: Sub Question B: Does the take-home play bag intervention support/promote parent’s play involvement? Support? More play? More

discussion?

Page 209: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

197

Play More No

Implied yes Yes

How/Why We already play a lot

Resistant agreement Examples of how

Discussed/Talked More

No Not more, but new Yes, more

General Play Involvement Indicators

Quoteable Quotes regarding ongoing inhibitors to in-home play.

Quotable Quotes regarding the play more question. Examples of the resistant agreement and defensiveness.

Quotable quotes of intimate sharing that described children or relationships beyond interview prompts.

Page 210: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

198

REFERENCES

Abadiano, H. R., & Turner, J. (2003). Homeschool partnership: What works? New England Reading Association Journal, 39(3), 58-62.

Algozzine, B., Wang, C., & Violette, A. S. (2011). Reexamining the relationship between

academic achievement and social behavior. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13(1), 3-16.

Almon, J., & Miller, E. (2011). The crisis in early education: A research-based case for

more play and less pressure. College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood, 3, 297-308.

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during

childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82. Anderson, K. J., & Minke, K. M. (2007). Parent involvement in education: Toward an

understanding of parents' decision making. The Journal of Educational Research, 100(5), 311-323.

Annie E. Casey Foundation (2017). Kids count data book state trends in child well-being.

http://www.aecf.org/m/resourcedoc/aecf-2017kidscountdatabook.pdf Anthony, L. G., Anthony, B. J., Glanville, D. N., Naiman, D. Q., Waanders, C., &

Shaffer, S. (2005). The relationships between parenting stress, parenting behaviour and preschoolers' social competence and behaviour problems in the classroom. Infant and Child Development, 14(2), 133-154.

Arnold, D. H., Zeljo, A., Doctoroff, G. L., & Ortiz, C. (2008). Parent involvement in

preschool: Predictors and the relation of involvement to preliteracy development. School Psychology Review, 37(1), 74.

Appl, D. J., Farrar, K. L., & Smith, K. G. (2012). Learning family-centered practices

through a parent–child playgroup practicum. Infants & Young Children, 25(3), 232-243.

Athanasiou, M.S. (2007). Play-Based Approaches to Preschool Assessment. In B.A.

Bracken & R.J. Nagle (Eds.), Psychoeducational assessment of preschool children (4th ed.) (pp. 219-238). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Aureli, T., & Colecchia, N. (1996). Day care experience and free play behavior in

preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 1-17.

Page 211: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

199

Azzi-Lessing, L. (2011). Home visitation programs: Critical issues and future directions. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(4), 387-398.

Baker, C. E. (2013). Fathers' and mothers' home literacy involvement and children's

cognitive and social emotional development: Implications for family literacy programs. Applied Developmental Science, 17(4), 184-197.

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more:

meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological bulletin, 129(2), 195.

Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V.S. Ramachaudran (Ed.). Encyclopedia of human

behavior. (pp. 71–81). New York, NY: Academic Press. Barnett, L. A. (1984). Research note: Young children’s resolution of distress through

play. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25, 477–483. Barnett, L. A., & Storm, B. (1981). Play, pleasure, and pain: The reduction of anxiety

through play. Leisure Sciences, 4, 161–175. Barbour, A. C. (1998). Home literacy bags promote family involvement. Childhood

Education, 75(2), 71-75. Barton, E. E., Steed, E. A., Strain, P., Dunlap, G., Powell, D., & Payne, C. J. (2014). An

analysis of classroom-based and parent-focused social–emotional programs for young children. Infants & Young Children, 27(1), 3-29.

Bekker, T., Hopma, E., & Sturm, J. (2010). Creating opportunities for play: the influence

of multimodal feedback on open-ended play. International Journal of Arts and Technology, 3(4), 325-340.

Bellin, H. F., & Singer, D. G. (2006). My magic story car: Video-Based play intervention

to strengthen emergent literacy of at-risk preschoolers. In D.G. Singer, R.M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Play= Learning: How Play Motivates and Enhances Children's Cognitive and Social-Emotional Growth. (pp. 101-120). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Benedict, E. A., Horner, R. H., & Squires, J. K. (2007). Assessment and implementation

of positive behavior support in preschools. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 27(3), 174-192.

Bennet, N., Wood, L., & Rogers, S. (1997). Teaching through play: Teacher’s thinking in

classroom practice. Buckingham, UK: Open.

Page 212: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

200

Bergen, D. (2002). The role of pretend play in children’s cognitive development. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 4, 1–12.

Bergen, D., & Mauer, D. (2000). Symbolic play, phonological awareness, and literacy

skills at three age levels. In K. A. Roskos & J. F. Christie (Eds.), Play and literacy in early childhood: Research from multiple perspectives. (pp. 45-62). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F.,

... & Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 757.

Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention and intervention:

The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 899-911.

Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: A Developmental

psychobiological approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711. Bodrova, E. (2008). Make-believe play versus academic skills: A Vygotskian approach to

today's dilemma of early childhood education. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 16(3), 357-369.

Bodrova E. & Leong, D.J. (2012). Scaffolding self-regulated learning in young children:

Lessons from Tools of the Mind. In R.C. Pianta, W.S. Barnett, L.M. Justice, & S.M. Sheridan (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood education. (352-396). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Boulatoff, C., & Jump, V. K. (2007). Blueprint of a cost analysis approach for early

intervention: Application to a home visiting program to prevent child abuse and neglect. Journal of Early Intervention, 30(1), 73-84.

Boyce, L. K., Seedall, R. B., Innocenti, M. S., Roggman, L. A., Cook, G. A., Hagman, A.

M., & Norman, V. K. J. (2017). Influence of a parent–child interaction focused bookmaking approach on maternal parenting self-efficacy. Infants & Young Children, 30(1), 76-93.

Bradley, R. (2002). Environment and Parenting. In M. Bornstein (Ed.) Handbook of

parenting. (2nd Ed) (pp. 280-345). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Brand, T., & Jungmann, T. (2014). Participant characteristics and process variables

predict attrition from a home-based early intervention program. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(2), 155-167.

Page 213: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

201

Brand, S.T., Marchand, J., Lilly, E., & Child, M. (2014). Home-school literacy bags for twenty-first century preschoolers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 42, 163-170.

Branson, R. D., Davis, K., & Butler, K. L. (2007). African Americans’ participation in

clinical research: importance, barriers, and solutions. The American Journal of Surgery, 193(1), 32-39.

Bredekamp, S. (2005). Play and school readiness. Educational Perspectives, 38(1), 18-

26. Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). National Association for the Education of Young

Children. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs.

Broadhead, P. (2006). Developing an understanding of young children's learning through

play: the place of observation, interaction and reflection. British Educational Research Journal, 32(2), 191-207.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature

and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development:

Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742. Brooks, R. B. (2005). The power of parenting. In S. Goldstein, & R. B. Brooks (Eds.)

Handbook of resilience in children (pp. 297–314). New York: Plenum. Browne, J. V., & Talmi, A. (2005). Family-based intervention to enhance infant–parent

relationships in the neonatal intensive care unit. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30(8), 667-677.

Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive one-word picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Novato, CA:

Academic Therapy. Brestan-Knight, E. & Salamone, C. A. (2011). Measuring parent-child interactions

through play. In S. Russ & L. Niec (Eds). An Evidence-Based Approach to Play in Intervention and Prevention: Integrating Developmental and Clinical Science. New York, NY: Guilford.

Burdette, H. L., & Whitaker, R. C. (2005). A national study of neighborhood safety,

outdoor play, television viewing, and obesity in preschool children. Pediatrics, 116(3), 657-662.

Page 214: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

202

Bryk, A., & Schneider, B. (2002). Trust in schools: A core resource for improvement. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal Negative Affect in Parent‐Child

Interactions and Children's Peer Competency. Child development, 67(5), 2217-2226.

Casey, A. M., & McWilliam, R. A. (2007). The STARE: The Scale for teachers'

assessment of routines engagement. Young Exceptional Children, 11(1), 2-15. Chen, J. Q., Masur, A., & McNamee, G. (2011). Young children’s approaches to

learning: a sociocultural perspective. Early Child Development and Care, 181(8), 1137-1152.

Chen, J. Q., & McNamee, G. D. (2011). Positive approaches to learning in the context of

preschool classroom activities. Early Childhood Education Journal, 39(1), 71-78. Cheng, Y. J. (2015). Impact of child-centered group play therapy on social-emotional

assets of kindergarten children. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations.

Choi, Y. K. (2013). Two Qualitative Case Studies Examining the Parent-Child

Interaction in Home-Based Musical Play Experiences. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Digital Dissertations.

Christ, T. W., & Makarani, S. A. (2009). Teachers’ attitudes about teaching English in

India: An Embedded mixed methods study. International Journal of Multiple Research Approaches, 3, 73-87.

Chudacoff, H. P. (2007). Children at play: An American history. New York, NY: NYU

Press. Clark, R. (1993). Homework parenting practices that positively affect student

achievement. In N. F. Chavkin (Ed.), Families and schools in a pluralistic society (pp. 53–71). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 20, 37-46. Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific

construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75(2), 317-333.

Page 215: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

203

Comfort, M., & Farran, D. C. (1994). Parent-child interaction assessment in family-centered intervention. Infants & Young Children, 6(4), 33-45.

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners' Teacher Rating Scale--Revised (L). North Tonawanda,

NY: Multi-Health Systems. Connolly, J. A., & Doyle, A. B. (1984). Relation of social fantasy play to social

competence in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20(5), 797-806. Cone, J. D., Delawyer, D. D., & Wolfe, V. V. (1985). Assessing parent participation: The

Parent/family involvement index. Exceptional Children, 51(5), 417-424. Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo J., Mendez, J., & McDermott, P. (2000). School peer interactions

and readiness to learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(3), 458-465.

Cordes, C., & Miller, E. (2000). Fool’s gold: A critical look at computers in childhood.

College Park, MD: Alliance for Childhood. Craig-Unkefer, L.A., & Kaiser, A.P. (2002). Improving the social communication skills

of at-risk preschool children in a play context. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 22(1), 3–13.

Creswell, J. W., & Clark, V. L. P. (2007). Designing and conducting mixed methods

research. California: Sage Publications. Curby, T. W., Brown, C. A., Bassett, H. H., & Denham, S. A. (2015). Associations

between preschoolers' social–emotional competence and pre-literacy skills. Infant and Child Development, 24(5), 549-570.

Cutter-Mackenzie, A., & Edwards, S. (2013). Toward a model for early childhood

environmental education: Foregrounding, developing, and connecting knowledge through play-based learning. The Journal of Environmental Education, 44(3), 195-213.

Datta, L. E. (2001). The wheelbarrow, the mosaic and the double helix: Challenges and

strategies for successfully carrying out mixed methods evaluation. Evaluation Journal of Australasia, 1(2), 33-40.

Davies, D. (2002). The 10th school revisited: Are school-family-community partnerships

on the reform agenda now? Phi Delta Kappan, 83(6), 388-392.

Page 216: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

204

DeLoatche, K. J., Bradley-Klug, K. L., Ogg, J., Kromrey, J. D., & Sundman-Wheat, A. N. (2015). Increasing parent involvement among Head Start families: A randomized control group study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43(4), 271-279.

Denham, S. A. (1998). The Guilford series on Special and emotional development.

Emotional development in young children. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., & Zinsser, K. (2012). Early childhood teachers as

socializers of young children’s emotional competence. Early Childhood Education Journal, 40(3), 137-143.

Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., Zinsser, K., & Wyatt, T. M. (2014). How preschoolers'

social–emotional learning predicts their early school success: Developing theory‐promoting, competency‐based assessments. Infant and Child Development, 23(4), 426-454.

Denham, S. A., & Holt, R. W. (1993). Preschoolers' likability as cause or consequence of

their social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 271. Denham, S. A., Renwick, S. M., & Holt, R. W. (1991). Working and playing together:

Prediction of preschool social‐emotional competence from mother‐child interaction. Child Development, 62(2), 242-249.

Dennis, L. R., & Stockall, N. (2015). Using play to build the social competence of young

children with language delays: Practical guidelines for teachers. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43(1), 1-7.

Denton, K., & West, J. (2002). Children's reading and mathematics achievement in

kindergarten and first grade. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED461438.pdf

DeRosier, M. E., Kupersmidt, J. B., & Patterson, C. J. (1994). Children's academic and

behavioral adjustment as a function of the chronicity and proximity of peer rejection. Child Development, 65(6), 1799-1813.

DeRousie, R. S., & Durham, R. (2008). Processes and factors influencing family

contributions to school readiness. In A. Booth & A.C. Crouter (Eds.), Disparities in school readiness: How families contribute to transitions into school (pp. 299-319). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Dever, M. T. (2001). Issues in Education: Family literacy bags: A Vehicle for parent

Involvement and education. The Journal of Early Education and Family Review, 8(4), 17-28.

Page 217: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

205

Dever, M. T., & Burtis, D. C. (2002). An evaluation of family literacy bags as a vehicle for parent involvement. Early Childhood Development and Care, 172(4), 359-370.

Dickey, K., Castle, K., & Pryor, K. (2016). Reclaiming play in schools. Childhood

Education, 92(2), 111-117. Diener, M. L., Wright, C., Brehl, B., & Black, T. (2016). Socioemotional correlates of

creative potential in preschool age children: Thinking beyond student academic assessments. Creativity Research Journal, 28(4), 450-457.

Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., & Klebanov,

P. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428–1446.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner's manual for the PPVT-III peabody

picture vocabulary test: Form IIIA and Form IIIB. AGS. Drew, W. F., & Rankin, B. (2004). Open-Ended Materials. Young Children, (4), 38-45. Drummond, K. V., & Stipek, D. (2004). Low-income parents' beliefs about their role in

children's academic learning. The Elementary School Journal, 104(3), 197-213. Domina, T. (2005). Leveling the home advantage: Assessing the effectiveness of parental

involvement in elementary school. Sociology of Education, 78(3), 233-249. Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1996). Family involvement in children’s and adolescents’

schooling. In A. Booth & J.F. Dunn (Eds.), Family-school links: How do they affect educational outcomes. (3-34). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Elias, C. L., & Berk, L. E. (2002). Self-regulation in young children: Is there a role for

sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17(2), 216-238. Emerson, A.M. & Linder, S.M. (2018). A Play Bag Intervention: Bridging Home and

School. Kappa Delta Pi Record, 54(2), 78-81. Epstein, J. L. (1987). Toward a theory of family-school connections. In K. Hurrelmann,

F. Kaufmann, & F. Losel (Eds.). Social intervention: Potential and constraints. (pp 121-136). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter & Co.

Epstein, J. L. (1992). School and family partnerships. In M. Aiken (Ed.), Encyclopedia of

educational research (6th ed.) (pp. 1139–1151). New York, NY: Macmillan.

Page 218: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

206

Epstein, J.L., Williams, K.J., & Nesbitt, M.G. (2002). Five-year-study: State and district leadership in developing programs of partnership. In O. Moles (Chair), Five-year-study: School, family, and community partnerships in states, districts, and schools. Symposium contucted at the annual meetin of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.

Epstein, J. L., & Dauber, S. L. (1991). School programs and teacher practices of parent

involvement in inner-city elementary and middle schools. Elementary School Journal, 91, 289–305.

ESRI Business Analyst, 2016. Retrieved from

http://www.greenvillesc.gov/DocumentCenter/View/606 On October 16, 2017. Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor

exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73(4), 1238–1248.

Erzberger, C., & Kelle, U. (2003). Making inferences in mixed methods: The rules of

integration. In: Tashakkori, A., Teddlie, C. (Eds.), Handbook of Mixed Methods in Social & Behavioural Research. (pp. 457–488). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students' academic achievement:

A Meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 1-22. Fantuzzo, J., Coolahan, K., Mendez, J., McDermott, P., & Sutton-Smith, B. (1998).

Contextually-relevant validation of peer play constructs with African American Head Start children: Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(3), 411–431.

Fantuzzo, J., & McWayne, C. (2002). The Relationship between peer-play interactions in

the family context and dimensions of school readiness for low-income preschool children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 79.

Fantuzzo, J., McWayne, C., Perry, M. A., & Childs, S. (2004). Multiple dimensions of

family involvement and their relations to behavioral and learning competencies for urban, low-income children. School Psychology Review, 33(4), 467.

Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., & Tighe, E. (1998). Parental assessment of peer play:

Development and validation of the parent version of the Penn Interactive Peer Play Scale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(4), 659-676.

Fantuzzo, J., Perry, M. A., & McDermott, P. (2004). Preschool Approaches to Learning

and Their Relationship to Other Relevant Classroom Competencies for Low-Income Children. School Psychology Quarterly, 19(3), 212-230.

Page 219: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

207

Fantuzzo, J., Sutton-Smith, B., Coolahan, K. C., Manz, P. H., Canning, S., & Debnam, D. (1995). Assessment of preschool play interaction behaviors in young low-income children: Penn interactive peer play scale. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10(1), 105-120.

Fantuzzo, J., Tighe, E., & Childs, S. (2000). Family involvement questionnaire: A

Multivariate assessment of family participation in early childhood education. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 367.

Fantuzzo, J. W., Tighe, E., & Perry, M. (1999). Relationships between family

involvement in Head Start and children's interactive peer play. NHSA Dialog: A Research-to-Practice Journal for the Early Intervention Field, 3(1), 60-67.

Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An integrative review. Child Development,

52(4), 1095-1118. Filene, J. H., Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., & Cachat, P. (2013). Components associated

with home visiting program outcomes: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 132(2), S100-S109.

Finlon, K. J., Izard, C. E., Seidenfeld, A., Johnson, S. R., Cavadel, E. W., Ewing, E. S.

K., & Morgan, J. K. (2015). Emotion-based preventive intervention: Effectively promoting emotion knowledge and adaptive behavior among at-risk preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4), 1353-1365.

Fisher, D., Flood, J., Lapp, D., & Frey, N. (2004). Interactive Read‐Alouds: Is There a

Common Set of Implementation Practices? The Reading Teacher, 58(1), 8-17. Fisher, K. R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Gryfe, S. G. (2008). Conceptual

split? Parents' and experts' perceptions of play in the 21st century. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(4), 305-316.

Floyd, L. (1998). Joining hands: A parental involvement program. Urban Education,

33(1), 123-135. Fogle, L. M., & Mendez, J. L. (2006). Assessing the play beliefs of African American

mothers with preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(4), 507-518.

Ford, R. M., McDougall, S. J., & Evans, D. (2009). Parent‐delivered compensatory

education for children at risk of educational failure: Improving the academic and self‐regulatory skills of a Sure Start preschool sample. British Journal of Psychology, 100(4), 773-797.

Page 220: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

208

Frost, J. L., Wortham, S. C., & Reifel, R. S. (2008). Play and child development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Galyer, K., & Evans, I. (2001). Pretend play and the development of emotion regulation

in preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 166(1), 93–108. Garmezy, N. (1991). Resiliency and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes

associated with poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34(4), 416-430. Gfellner, B. M., McLaren, L., & Metcalfe, A. (2008). The parent-child home program in

western Manitoba: A 20-year evaluation. Child Welfare, 87(5), 49-67. Giallo, R., Treyvaud, K., Cooklin, A., & Wade, C. (2013). Mothers’ and fathers’

involvement in home activities with their children: Psychosocial factors and the role of parental self-efficacy. Early Child Development and Care, 183(3-4), 343-359.

Giffin, H. (1984). The coordination of meaning in the creation of a shared make-believe

reality. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic Play: The Development of Social Understanding (pp. 73-100). New York, NY: Academic Press, Inc.

Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development

and maintaining strong parent-child bonds. Pediatrics, 119(1), 182-191. Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Singer, D. G. (2006). Why play = learning: A

challenge for parents and educators. In D. G. Singer, R. M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Play = learning: How play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp. 74-100). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Gomby, D. S. (2005). Home visitation in 2005: Outcomes for children and parents.

Washington, DC: Committee for Economic Development. Gonzalez-DeHass, A. (2016). Preparing 21st century learners: Parent involvement

strategies for encouraging students' self-regulated learning. Childhood Education, 92(6), 427-436.

Goodall, J., & Montgomery, C. (2014). Parental involvement to parental engagement: A

Continuum. Educational Review, 66(4), 399-410. Gopnik, A. (2012). Scientific thinking in young children: Theoretical advances, empirical

research, and policy implications. Science, 337(6102), 1623-1627.

Page 221: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

209

Graham, F., Rodger, S., & Ziviani J. (2010). Enabling occupational performance of children through coaching parents: Three case reports. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 30(1), 4–15.

Grande, M. (2004). Increasing parent participation and knowledge using home literacy

bags. Intervention in School and Clinic, 40(2), 120-126. Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., & Apostoleris, N. H. (1997). Predictors of

parent involvement in children’s schooling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 538 –548.

Gross, D., Fogg, L., Webster-Stratton, C., Garvey, C., Julion, W., & Grady, J. (2003).

Parent training of toddlers in day care in low-income urban communities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 261.

Gray, P. (2011). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and

adolescents. American Journal of Play, 3(4), 443-463. Greene, J. C., & Caracelli, V. J. (1997). Defining and describing the paradigm issue in

mixed‐method evaluation. New directions for evaluation, (74), 5-17. Gross, D., Fogg, L., Webster-Stratton, C., Garvey, C., Julion, W., & Grady, J. (2003).

Parent training of toddlers in day care in low-income urban communities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(2), 261-278.

Guralnick, M. J. (1993). Developmentally appropriate practice in the assessment and

intervention of children's peer relations. Topic in Early Childhood Special Education, 13, 344-371.

Gutman, L. M., & McLoyd, V. C. (2000). Parents' management of their children's

education within the home, at school, and in the community: An examination of African-American families living in poverty. The Urban Review, 32(1), 1-24.

Hadeed, J., & Sylva, K. (1999). Behavioral observations as predictors of children's social

and cognitive progress in day care. Early Child Development and Care, 154(1), 13-30.

Han, M., Moore, N., Vukelich, C., & Buell, M. (2010). Does Play Make a Difference?

How Play Intervention Affects the Vocabulary Learning of At-Risk Preschoolers. American Journal of Play, 3(1), 82-105.

Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of

young American children. New York, NY: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Page 222: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

210

Harter, S., & Pike, R. (1984). The pictorial scale of perceived competence and social acceptance for young children. Child development, 55(6), 1969-1982.

Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in childhood. Journal of

child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 84-108. Head Start. (2015). Head Start Early Learning Outcomes Framework: Ages birth to five.

Retrieved from https://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/hslc/hs/sr/approach/pdf/ohs-framework.pdf

Hill, N. E., & Taylor, L. C. (2004). Parental school involvement and children's academic

achievement: Pragmatics and issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(4), 161-164.

Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Bassler, O. C., & Brissie, J. S. (1992). Explorations in parent-

school relations. The Journal of Educational Research, 85(5), 287-294. Hoover-Dempsey, K., & Sandler, H. (1997). Why do parents become involved in their

children’s education? Review of Educational Research, 67(1), 3-42. Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Walker, J. M., Sandler, H. M., Whetsel, D., Green, C. L.,

Wilkins, A. S., & Closson, K. (2005). Why do parents become involved? Research findings and implications. The Elementary School Journal, 106(2), 105-130.

Hughes J.N. & Kwok, O. (2006). Classroom engagement mediates the effect of teacher–

student support on elementary students’ peer acceptance: A prospective analysis. Journal of School Psychology, 43(6), 465–480.

Hyson, M. (2005). Strengthening young children’s positive approaches to learning.

Young Children, 60(6), 68–70. Hyson, M. (2008). Enthusiastic and engaged learners: Approaches to learning in the

early childhood classroom (Vol. 733). New York: Teachers College Press. Ingram, M., Wolfe, R. B., & Lieberman, J. M. (2007). The role of parents in high-

achieving schools serving low-income, at-risk populations. Education and Urban Society, 39(4), 479-497.

International Play Association. (2014). Declaration on the Importance of Play.

http://ipaworld.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/05/IPA_Declaration-FINAL.pdf

Page 223: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

211

Isen, A. M., & Reeve, J. (2005). The influence of positive affect on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Facilitating enjoyment of play, responsible work behavior, and self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 29(4), 295-323.

Jarvis, P., Newman, S., & Swiniarski, L. (2014). On ‘becoming social’: The Importance

of collaborative free play in childhood. International Journal of Play, 3(1), 53-68. Jimerson, S., Egeland, B., & Teo, A. (1999). A longitudinal study of achievement

trajectories: Factors associated with change. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 116-126.

Johnson, R. B., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Turner, L. A. (2007). Toward a definition of

mixed methods research. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 1(2), 112-133. Jones, T. L., & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of parental self-efficacy in parent and

child adjustment: A Review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(3), 341-363. Julion, W. A., Breitenstein, S. M., & Waddell, D. (2012). Fatherhood intervention

development in collaboration with African American non‐resident fathers. Research in Nursing & Health, 35(5), 490-506.

Kagan, S.L., Moore, E., & Bredekamp, S. (Eds.). (1995). Reconsidering children’s early

learning and development: Toward shared beliefs and vocabulary. Washington, DC: National Education Goals Panel.

Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., Filene, J. H., & Boyle, C. L. (2008). A meta-analytic

review of components associated with parent training program effectiveness. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(4), 567-589.

Kaomea, J. (2012). Reconceptualizing indigenous parent involvement in early

educational settings: Lessons from Native Hawaiian preschool families. International Indigenous Policy Journal, 3(4), 1-19.

Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman assessment battery for children.

John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Keith, T. Z., Keith, P. B., Quirk, K. J., Sperduto, J., Santillo, S.. & Killings, S. (1998).

Longitudinal effects of parent involvement on high school grades: Similarities and differences across gender and ethnic groups. Journal of School Psychology, 36(3), 335-363.

Kelly-Vance, L., & Ryalls, B. O. (2005). A systematic, reliable approach to play assessment in preschoolers. School Psychology International 26(4), 398-412.

Page 224: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

212

Kerns, K. A., & Barth, J. M. (1995). Attachment and play: Convergence across components of parent-child relationships and their relations to peer competence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12(2), 243-260.

Kestenbaum, C., Canino, I. A., Wu, W. C., Duch, H., Marti, M., Snow, R., ... &

Benavides, C. (2016). CARING at Columbia Head Start: Promoting Resilience Through Creative Art and Play and a Prevention Model for At-Risk Preschool Children and Families. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(10), S352.

Kiernan, K., & Huerta, M. (2008). Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting

and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. British Journal of Sociology, 59, 783–806.

Knoche, L. L., Sheridan, S. M., Edwards, C. P., & Osborn, A. Q. (2010). Implementation

of a relationship-based school readiness intervention: A multidimensional approach to fidelity measurement for early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(3), 299-313.

Kokoski, T. M. and Patton, M. M. (1997) Beyond homework: Science and mathematics

backpacks. Dimensions of Early Childhood, 25(2), 11–16. Korfmacher, J., Green, B., Staerkel, F., Peterson, C., Cook, G., Roggman, L., ... &

Schiffman, R. (2008). Parent involvement in early childhood home visiting. Child & Youth Care Forum, 37(4), 171-196.

Krafft, K. C., & Berk, L. E. (1998). Private speech in two preschools: Significance of

open-ended activities and make-believe play for verbal self-regulation. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(4), 637-658.

Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2003). Mathematics interventions for children

with special educational needs: A meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 24(2), 97–114.

Ladd, G. W., Birch, S. H., & Buhs, E. S. (1999). Children's social and scholastic lives in

kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development, 70(6), 1373-1400.

Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). Toward a theory of culturally relevant pedagogy. American

Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 465-491. LaForett, D. R., & Mendez, J. L. (2017). Children’s engagement in play at home: a

parent’s role in supporting play opportunities during early childhood. Early Child Development and Care, 187(5-6), 910-923.

Page 225: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

213

Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., & Miller-Loncar, C. L. (2000). Early maternal and child influences on children’s later independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Development, 71(2), 358–375.

Lamb-Parker, F., Piotrkowski, C. S., Baker, A. J., Kessler-Sklar, S., Clark, B., & Peay, L.

(2001). Understanding barriers to parent involvement in Head Start: A Research-community partnership. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 16(1), 35-51.

Landreth, G. L. (2012). Play therapy: The art of the relationship (3rd ed.). New York,

NY: Brunner-Routledge. Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). Toward a theory of culturally relevant pedagogy. American

Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 465-491. Leal, I., Engebretson, J., Cohen, L., Fernandez-Esquer, M. E., Lopez, G., Wangyal, T., &

Chaoul, A. (2016). An Exploration of the Effects of Tibetan Yoga on Patients’ Psychological Well-Being and Experience of Lymphoma An Experimental Embedded Mixed Methods Study. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 12(1), 1-24.

Lee, J. S., & Bowen, N. K. (2006). Parent involvement, cultural capital, and the

achievement gap among elementary school children. American Educational Research Journal, 43(2), 193-218.

LeFevre, A. L., & Shaw, T. V. (2012). Latino parent involvement and school success:

Longitudinal effects of formal and informal support. Education and Urban Society, 44(6), 707-723.

Lennox, S. (2013). Interactive read-alouds—An avenue for enhancing children’s

language for thinking and understanding: A review of recent research. Early Childhood Education Journal, 41(5), 381-389.

Leve, L. D., & Fagot, B. I. (1997). Prediction of positive peer relations from observed

parent–child interactions. Social Development, 6(2), 254-269. Levenstein, P. (1988). Messages from Home: The Mother-Child Home Program and the

Prevention of School Disadvantage. Columbus: Ohio State University Press. Levenstein, P., Levenstein, S., & Oliver, D. (2002). First grade school readiness of

former child participants in a South Carolina replication of the parent–child home program. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 23(3), 331-353.

Page 226: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

214

Levenstein, P., Levenstein, S., Shiminski, J. A., & Stolzberg, J. E. (1998). Long-term impact of a verbal interaction program for at-risk toddlers: An exploratory study of high school outcomes in a replication of the Mother-Child Home Program. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19(2), 267-285.

Levenstein, P., & O’Hara, J. (1993). The necessary lightness of mother-child play. In K.

MacDonald (ED.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications, 221-237. New York, NY: State University of New York Press.

Lewis, J. (2009). 13 As well as words: Congo Pygmy hunting, mimicry, and play. In R.

Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 236-256). New York, NY: Oxford Press.

Li, S., Marquart, J. M., & Zercher, C. (2000). Conceptual issues and analytic strategies in

mixed-method studies of preschool inclusion. Journal of Early Intervention, 23(2), 116-132.

Li-Grining, C. P., Votruba-Drzal, E., Maldonado-Carreno, C., & Haas, K. (2010).

Children’s early approaches to learning and academic trajectories through fifth grade. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1062–1077.

Lillard, A. S. (1993). Pretend play skills and the child's theory of mind. Child

Development, 64(2), 348-371. Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C.

M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children's development: A Review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 1-34.

Lin, T-Y., Chang, K-H., Liu, S-Y. and Chu, H-H. (2006). A persuasive game to

encourage healthy dietary behaviors of young children. Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Ubiquitous Computing, California.

Linder, S.M. 2017. "Math Take-Home Bags: Activities to Support Family Math Play."

Teaching Young Children, 11(1): 29–31. Linder, S.M. & Emerson, A. (In Revisions). Increasing family mathematics play

interactions through a take-home math bag intervention. Lindsey, E. W., Mize, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Mutuality in parent-child play:

Consequences for children's peer competence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14(4), 523-538.

Page 227: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

215

Love, J. M., Kisker, E. E., Ross, C., Raikes, H., Constantine, J., Boller, K., ... & Fuligni, A. S. (2005). The effectiveness of early head start for 3-year-old children and their parents: lessons for policy and programs. Developmental Psychology, 41(6), 885-901.

Lowell, D. I., Carter, A. S., Godoy, L., Paulicin, B., & Briggs‐Gowan, M. J. (2011). A

Randomized controlled trial of child FIRST: A Comprehensive home‐based intervention translating research into early childhood practice. Child Development, 82(1), 193-208.

Luo, M., & Dappen, L. (2005). Mixed-methods design for an objective-based evaluation

of a magnet school assistance project. Evaluation and Program Planning, 28(1), 109-118.

MacDonald, K., & Parke, R. D. (1984). Bridging the gap: Parent-child play interaction

and peer interactive competence. Child Development 55(4), 1265-1277. Mantzicopoulos, P. Y. (1997). The relationship of family variables to Head Start

children's pre-academic competence. Early Education and Development, 8(4), 357-375.

Manz, P. H., & Bracaliello, C. B. (2016). Expanding home visiting outcomes:

Collaborative measurement of parental play beliefs and examination of their association with parents’ involvement in toddler’s learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 157-167

Manz, P. H., Bracaliello, C. B., Pressimone, V. J., Eisenberg, R. A., Gernhart, A. C., Fu,

Q., & Zuniga, C. (2016). Toddlers' expressive vocabulary outcomes after one year of parent–child home program services. Early Child Development and Care, 186(2), 229-248.

Manz, P. H., Gernhart, A. L., Bracaliello, C. B., Pressimone, V. J., & Eisenberg, R. A.

(2014). Preliminary development of the parent involvement in early learning scale for low-income families enrolled in a child-development-focused home visiting program. Journal of Early Intervention, 36(3), 171-191.

Marcon, R. A. (1999). Positive relationships between parent school involvement and

public school inner-city preschoolers' development and academic performance. School Psychology Review, 28(3), 395-412.

Mathieson, K., & Banerjee, R. (2010). Preschool peer play: The Beginnings of social

competence. Educational and Child Psychology, 27(1), 9–20.

Page 228: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

216

Mattingly, D. J., Prislin, R., McKenzie, T. L., Rodriguez, J. L., & Kayzar, B. (2002). Evaluating evaluations: The Case of parent involvement programs. Review of Educational Research, 72(4), 549-576.

McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self‐regulation and academic achievement

in elementary school children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2011(133), 29-44.

McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self‐regulation in early childhood:

Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 136-142.

McClelland, M. M., Morrison, F. J., & Holmes, D. L. (2000). Children at risk for early

academic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15(3), 307-329.

McCollum, J. A., & Yates, T. J. (1994). Dyad as focus, triad as means: A family-centered

approach to supporting parent-child interactions. Infants & Young Children, 6(4), 54-63.

McCurdy, K., & Daro, D. (2001). Parent involvement in family support programs: An

Integrated theory. Family Relations, 50(2), 113–121 McDermott, P. A. (1999). National scales of differential learning behaviors among

American children and adolescents. School Psychology Review, 28(2), 280. McDermott, P. A., Green, L. F., Francis, J. M., & Stott, D. H. (2000). Preschool learning

behaviors scale. Philadelphia, PA: Edumetric and Clinical Science. McDermott, P. A., Fantuzzo, J. W., Warley, H. P., Waterman, C., Angelo, L. E.,

Gadsden, V. L., & Sekino, Y. (2011). Multidimensionality of teachers’ graded responses for preschoolers’ stylistic learning behavior: The Learning-To-Learn scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 71(1), 148-169.

McDermott, P. A., Leigh, N. M., & Perry, M. A. (2002). Development and validation of

the preschool learning behaviors scale. Psychology in the Schools, 39(4), 353-365. McDermott, P. A., Rikoon, S. H., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (2014). Tracing children’s

approaches to learning through Head Start, kindergarten, and first grade: Different pathways to different outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 200.

McLoyd, V.C. (1983) The effects of the structure of play objects on the pretend play of

low-income preschool children, Child Development, 54(3), 626–635.

Page 229: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

217

McNair, J. C. (2011). " It Was Like a Book Buffet!" Parents and Children Selecting African American Children's Literature Together. The Journal of Negro Education, 80(2), 163-175.

McWayne, C. M., Fantuzzo, J. W., & McDermott, P. A. (2004). Preschool competency in

context: an investigation of the unique contribution of child competencies to early academic success. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 633-645.

Mendez, J. L. (2010). How can parents get involved in preschool? Barriers and

engagement in education by ethnic minority parents of children attending Head Start. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(1), 26-36.

Melhuish, E. C., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj‐Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B.

(2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. Journal of Social Issues, 64(1), 95-114.

Myers, H. F., & Taylor, S. (1998). Family contributions to risk and resilience in African

American children. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 215-229. Miedel, W. T., & Reynolds, A. J. (2000). Parent involvement in early intervention for

disadvantaged children: Does it matter? Journal of School Psychology, 37(4), 379-402.

Miller, E. T. (2010). An Interrogation of the “If Only” Mentality: One Teacher’s Deficit

Perspective put on Trial. Early Childhood Education Journal, 38(4), 243-249. Miller, J. (2015). The power of parenting with social and emotional learning. Huffington

Post, April 15, 2015. Milkie, M. A., Kendig, S. M., Nomaguchi, K. M., & Denny, K. E. (2010). Time with

children, children's well‐being, and work‐family balance among employed parents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 72(5), 1329-1343.

Milteer, R. M., Ginsburg, K. R., & Mulligan, D. A. (2012). The importance of play in

promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent-child bond: Focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics, 129(1), e204-e213.

Moles, O. C. (1993). Collaboration between schools and disadvantaged parents:

Obstacles and openings. In N. F. Chavkin (Ed.), Families and schools in a pluralistic society (pp. 21-52). New York, NY: State University of New York Press.

Page 230: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

218

Moore, M. & Russ, S. (2006). Pretend play as a resource for children: Implications for pediatricians and health professionals. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(3), 237-248.

Moore, M., & Russ, S. (2008). Follow-up of a pretend play intervention: Effects on play,

creativity, and emotional processes in children. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 427– 436.

Moschovaki, E., & Meadows, S. (2005). Young children’s spontaneous participation

during classroom book reading: Differences according to various types of books. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 7(1), 1-17.

Myers, W. D. (2014). Where are the People of Color in Children’s Books? The New York

Times, 15. Myhre, S. M. (1993). Enhancing Your Dramatic-Play Area through the Use of Prop

Boxes. Young Children, 48(5), 6-11. National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC). 2009. Position

Statement: Developmentally Appropriate Practice in Early Childhood Programs Serving Children from Birth through Age 8. Retrieved https://www.naeyc.org/sites/default/files/globally-shared/downloads/PDFs/resources/position-statements/PSDAP.pdf

NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (1997). The effects of infant child care on

infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68(5), 860–879.

National Research Council (NRC). (2000). Ecological Indicators for the Nation.

Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press. Nell, M. L., Drew, W. F., & Bush, D. E. (2013). From play to practice: Connecting

teachers' play to children's learning. Washington, D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

Newman, S. B. (1996). Children engaging in storybook reading: The Influence of access

to print resources, opportunity, and parental interaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11(4), 495-513.

Neuman, S. B., & Roskos, K. (1993). Access to print for children of poverty: Differential

effects of adult mediation and literacy-enriched play settings on environmental and functional print tasks. American Educational Research Journal, 30(1), 95-122.

Page 231: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

219

Neville, H. J., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Bell, T. A., Fanning, J., Klein, S., & Isbell, E. (2013). Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(29), 12138-12143.

Nichols, S., & Stich, S. (2000). A cognitive theory of pretense. Cognition, 74, 115 – 147. Nielson, M. (2011). Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: Essential elements in the

evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 170–181.

Nievar, M. A., Jacobson, A., Chen, Q., Johnson, U., & Dier, S. (2011). Impact of HIPPY

on home learning environments of Latino families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26(3), 268-277.

Niklas, F., Cohrssen, C., & Tayler, C. (2016). Home learning environment and concept

formation: A family intervention study with kindergarten children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 44, 419-427.

Nistler, R. J., & Maiers, A. (2000). Stopping the silence: Hearing parents' voices in an

urban first-grade family literacy program. The Reading Teacher, 53(8), 670-680. “Non-invasive.” (n.d.) Retrieved September 27, 2017 from: https://www.merriam-

webster.com/dictionary/noninvasive Noeder, M. M. (2015). The Utility of Parent-Child Play Measures in Understanding

Diagnostic Group Differences. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation.

Noeder, M. M., Short, E. J., Li, S., & Cooper, R. (2011). The impact of parent-child

interaction factors on the play abilities of children diagnosed with attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder, and speech language disorder. Poster session presented at the annual American Psychological Association Conference, Washington D.C.

Nwokah, E., Hsu, H. C., & Gulker, H. (2013). The use of play materials in early

intervention: the dilemma of poverty. American Journal of Play, 5(2), 187-287. O’Shea, M. M. (2004). What do we know about how children learn? The social,

intellectual and cognitive development of children: A guide to the first five years. In E. Chesebrouch, P. King, T. P. Gullotta, & M. Bloom (Eds.). A blueprint for the promotion of prosocial behavior in early childhood (pp. 1-11). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

Page 232: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

220

Östlund, U., Kidd, L., Wengström, Y., & Rowa-Dewar, N. (2011). Combining qualitative and quantitative research within mixed method research designs: A Methodological review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 48(3), 369-383.

Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Johnson, R. B. (2006). The validity issue in mixed

research. Research in the Schools, 13(1), 48-63. Parker, F. L., Boak, A. Y., Griffin, K. W., Ripple, C., & Peay, L. (1999). Parent-child

relationship, home learning environment, and school readiness. School Psychology Review, 28(3), 413-425.

Paulsell, D., Del Grosso, P., & Supplee, L. (2014). Supporting replication and scale-up of

evidence-based home visiting programs: Assessing the implementation knowledge base. American Journal of Public Health, 104(9), 1624-1632.

Pentimonti, J. M., & Justice, L. M. (2010). Teachers’ use of scaffolding strategies during

read alouds in the preschool classroom. Early Childhood Education Journal, 37(4), 241-248.

Peterson, C. A., Luze, G. J., Eshbaugh, E. M., Jeon, H. J., & Kantz, K. R. (2007).

Enhancing parent–child interactions through home visiting: Promising practice or unfulfilled promise? Journal of Early Intervention, 29(2), 119–140.

Phillips, D. A., & Shonkoff, J. P. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The

science of early childhood development. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press.

Plano-Clark, V. L., Schumacher, K., West, C., Edrington, J., Dunn, L. B., Harzstark, A.,

... & Miaskowski, C. (2013). Practices for embedding an interpretive qualitative approach within a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 7(3), 219-242.

Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D. (2007). The how, whom, and why

of parents’ involvement in children’s academic lives: More is not always better. Review of Educational Research, 77(3), 373-410.

Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. S., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). A

Structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 605-819.

Quinlan, E., & Quinlan, A. (2010). Representations of rape: Transcending

methodological divides. Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 4(2), 127-143.

Page 233: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

221

Raikes, H., Green, B. L., Atwater, J., Kisker, E., Constantine, J., & Chazan-Cohen, R. (2006). Involvement in Early Head Start home visiting services: Demographic predictors and relations to child and parent outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21(1), 2–24.

Rallis, S. F., & Rossman, G. B. (2003). Mixed methods in evaluation contexts: A

pragmatic framework. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Handbook of mixed methods in social & behavioral research (pp. 491-512). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ray, D. C., Armstrong, S. A., Balkin, R. S., & Jayne, K. M. (2015). Child‐centered play

therapy in the schools: Review and meta‐analysis. Psychology in the Schools, 52(2), 107-123.

Reynolds, A. J., Mavrogenes, N. A., Bezruczko, N., & Hagemann, M. (1996). Cognitive

and family‐support mediators of preschool effectiveness: A Confirmatory analysis. Child Development, 67(3), 1119-1140.

Richardson, M. V., Miller, M. B., Richardson, J. A., & Sacks, M. K. (2008). Literacy

bags to encourage family involvement. Reading Improvement, 45(1), 3-9. Richgels, D.T., & Wold, L.S. (1998). Backpacking partnerships between school and

home. The Reading Teacher, 52(1), 18-29. Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Wanless, S. B. (2012). Self-regulation and academic

achievement. In R. Pianta, L. Justice, S. Barnett, & S. M. Sheridan (Eds.), The handbook of early education (pp. 299–323). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Riojas-Cortez, M., Flores, B. B., & Clark, E. R. (2003). Valuing and connecting home

cultural knowledge with an early childhood program. Young Children, 58(6), 78-83.

Ritblatt, S. N., Beatty, J. R., Cronan, T. A., & Ochoa, A. M. (2002). Relationships among

perceptions of parent involvement, time allocation, and demographic characteristics: Implication for policy formation. Journal of community Psychology, 30(5), 519-549.

Roethler, J. (1998). Reading in color: Children's book illustrations and identity formation

for Black children in the United States. African American Review, 32(1), 95-105. Roopnarine, J. L., & Davidson, K. L. (2015). Parent-child play across cultures:

Advancing play research. American Journal of Play, 7(2), 228.

Page 234: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

222

Rushton, S., Juola-Rushton, A., & Larkin, E. (2010). Neuroscience, play and early childhood education: Connections, implications and assessment. Early Childhood Education Journal, 37(5), 351-361.

Ryan, C. S., Casas, J. F., Kelly‐Vance, L., Ryalls, B. O., & Nero, C. (2010). Parent

involvement and views of school success: The Role of parents' Latino and White American cultural orientations. Psychology in the Schools, 47(4), 391-405.

Saldaña, J. (2015). The coding manual for qualitative researchers. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage. Schickedanz, J. (1978). " You be the doctor and I'll be sick": Preschoolers learn the

language arts through play. Language Arts, 55(6), 713-718. Schmidt, P. R., & Lazar, A. M. (2011). Practicing What We Teach: How Culturally

Responsive Literacy Classrooms Make a Difference. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Schulz, L. E., & Bonawitz, E. B. (2007). Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more

exploratory play when evidence is confounded. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 1045-1050.

Schulz, L. E., Goodman, N. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Jenkins, A. C. (2008). Going

beyond the evidence: Abstract laws and preschoolers’ responses to anomalous data. Cognition, 109(2), 211-223.

Schultz, D., Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B. P., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2001). Emotion

knowledge in economically disadvantaged children: Self-regulatory antecedents and relations to social difficulties and withdrawal. Development and Psychopathology, 13(1), 53-67.

Scott, D., & Munson, W. (1994). Perceived constraints to park usage among individuals

with low incomes. Journal of Park and Recreation Administration, 12(4), 79-96. Sheridan, S. M., & Kratochwill, T. R. (2007). Conjoint behavioral consultation:

Promoting family-school connections and interventions. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.

Shonkoff, J. P., Phillips, D. A., & National Research Council (Eds). (2000). The

developing brain. In From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Child Development. Washington, D.C.: National Academic Press.

Page 235: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

223

Short, E. J., Noeder, M., Gorovoy, S., Manos, M. J., & Lewis, B. (2011). The Importance of play in both the assessment and treatment of young children. In S. Russ & L. Niec (Eds). An Evidence-Based Approach to Play in Intervention and Prevention: Integrating Developmental and Clinical Science. New York, NY: Guilford.

Shumow, L., Vandell, D. L., & Posner, J. (1999). Risk and resilience in the urban

neighborhood: Predictors of academic performance among low-income elementary school children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 45(2), 309-331.

Singer, D. G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2006). Play= Learning: How play

motivates and enhances children's cognitive and social-emotional growth. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Singer, D. G., & Singer, J. L. (1992). The house of make-believe. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press. Singer, D. G., Singer, J. L., D'Agnostino, H., & DeLong, R. (2009). Children's Pastimes

and Play in Sixteen Nations: Is Free-Play Declining? American Journal of Play, 1(3), 283-312.

Small, S. A., Cooney, S. M., & O’connor, C. (2009). Evidence‐informed program

improvement: using principles of effectiveness to enhance the quality and impact of family‐based prevention programs. Family Relations, 58(1), 1-13.

Smilansky, S., & Shefatya, L. (1990). Facilitating play: A medium for promoting

cognitive, socioemotional, and academic development in young children. Gaithersburg, MD: Psychosocial & Educational Publications.

Smith, M., & Walden, T. (2001). An exploration of African American preschool-aged

children's behavioral regulation in emotionally arousing situations. Child Study Journal, 31(1), 13-13.

Smith-Donald, R., Raver, C. C., Hayes, T., & Richardson, B. (2007). Preliminary

construct and concurrent validity of the preschool self-regulation assessment (PSRA) for field-based research. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22(2), 173-187.

Spence, S. H. (2003). Social skills training with children and young people: Theory,

evidence and practice. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(2), 84-96. Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Early relationships and the development of children. Infant Mental

Health Journal, 21(1-2), 67-74.

Page 236: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

224

St Clair‐Thompson, H., Stevens, R., Hunt, A., & Bolder, E. (2010). Improving children's working memory and classroom performance. Educational Psychology, 30(2), 203-219.

Stadler, M. A., & Ward, G. C. (2005). Supporting the narrative development of young

children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 33(2), 73-80. Stallard, P. (1993). The behavior of 3-year-old children: Prevalence and parental

perception of problem behavior: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(3), 413-421.

Stanger, J.D. & Gridina, N. (1999). Media in the home: The fourth annual survey of

parents and children. Philadelphia: Annenberg Public Policy Center, University of Pennsylvania.

Starkey, P., Klein, A., & Wakeley, A. (2004). Enhancing young children’s mathematical

knowledge through a pre-kindergarten mathematics intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 99-120.

Stephens, K. (2009). Imaginative play during childhood: Required for reaching full

potential. Beginnings workshop. Exchange (186), 53-56. Stevenson, D. & Baker, D. (1987). The family school relation and the child's school

performance. Child Development, 58(5), 1348-1357. Stipek, D., Milburn, S., Clements, D., & Daniels, D. H. (1992). Parents' beliefs about

appropriate education for young children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13(3), 293-310.

Strain, P. S., & Timm, M. A. (2001). Remediation and prevention of aggression: An

evaluation of the Regional Intervention Program over a quarter century. Behavioral Disorders, 26(4), 297-313.

Stroud, J. E. (1995). Block play: Building a foundation for literacy. Early Childhood

Education Journal, 23(1), 9-13. Sturm, J., Bekker, T., Groenendaal, B., Wesselink, R. and Eggen, B. (2008) ‘Key issues

for the successful design of an intelligent interactive playground’, Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Interaction Design and Children, Chicago, Illinois, pp.258–265.

Souto-Manning, M. (2009). Negotiating culturally responsive pedagogy through

multicultural children's literature: Towards critical democratic literacy practices in a first grade classroom. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 9(1), 50-74.

Page 237: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

225

Tamis‐LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2‐and 3‐year‐olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Development, 75(6), 1806-1820.

Taylor, A. R., & Machida, S. (1994). The contribution of parent and peer support to Head

Start children's early school adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(3-4), 387-405.

Teddlie, C., & Tashakkori, A. (Eds.). (2009). Foundations of mixed methods research:

Integrating quantitative and qualitative approaches in the social and behavioral sciences. Sage Publications Inc.

Tejagupta, C. (1991). Mother-father-child play: investigations of preschoolers' home play

behavior, parental involvement and home environment (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation.

Temple, C. A., Martinez, M., & Yokota, J. (2015). Children's books in children's hands:

A brief introduction to their literature. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Thomson, R. N., & Carlson, J. S. (2016). A Pilot Study of a Self-Administered Parent

Training Intervention for Building Preschoolers’ Social–Emotional Competence. Early Childhood Education Journal, 45(3), 419-426.

Thornberry, T., Brestan-Knight, E. (2011). Analyzing the utility of dyadic parent-child

interaction coding system (DPICS-III) warm-up segments. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33(2), 187-195.

Thorp, D., Stahmer, A., & Schreibman, L. (1995). Effects of sociodramatic play training

on children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25(3), 265–282.

Tominey, S. L., & McClelland, M. M. (2011). Red light, purple light: Findings from a

randomized trial using circle time games to improve behavioral self-regulation in preschool. Early Education & Development, 22(3), 489-519.

Torres, M. M., Domitrovich, C. E., & Bierman, K. L. (2015). Preschool interpersonal

relationships predict kindergarten achievement: Mediated by gains in emotion knowledge. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 39, 44-52.

Trawick-Smith, J. (2012). Teacher-child play interactions to achieve learning outcomes:

Risks and opportunities. In R.C. Pianta, W.S. Barnett, L.M. Justice, & S.M. Sheridan (Eds.), Handbook of Early Childhood Education (pp. 259-277). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Page 238: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

226

Trumbull, E., Rothstein-Fisch, C., Greenfield, P.M., & Quiroz, B. (2001). Bridging cultures between home and schools: A guide for teachers. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Tsao, Y. L. (2008). Using guided play to enhance children's conversation, creativity and

competence in literacy. Education, 128(3), 515-520. Uren, N., & Stagnitti, K. (2009). Pretend play, social competence and involvement in

children aged 5–7 years: The Concurrent validity of the child‐initiated pretend play assessment. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 56(1), 33-40.

Van Evra, J. (2004). Television and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Van der Sluis, S., De Jong, P. F., & Van der Leij, A. (2004). Inhibition and shifting in

children with learning deficits in arithmetic and reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(3), 239-266.

Viera, A. J., & Garrett, J. M. (2005). Understanding interobserver agreement: The Kappa

statistic. Family Medicine, 37(5), 360-363. Vickerius, M., & Sandberg, A. (2006). The significance of play and the environment

around play. Early Child Development and Care, 176(2), 207-217. Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding self-regulation: An introduction.

In R.F. Baumeister & K.D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 1-12). New York, NY: Guilford.

Vukelich, C., Christie, J., & Enz, B. (2001). Helping young children learn language and

literacy. Boston, MA: Pearson. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Readings on the

Development of Children, 23(3), 34-41. Vygotsky, L. S. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J.

Bruner, A. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 536–552). New York, NY: Basic Books.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1999). Tool and sign in the development of the child. The collected

works of LS Vygotsky, 6, 3-68. Waanders, C., Mendez, J. L., & Downer, J. T. (2007). Parent characteristics, economic

stress and neighborhood context as predictors of parent involvement in preschool children's education. Journal of School Psychology, 45(6), 619-636.

Page 239: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

227

Walker, J. M. T., Wilkins, A. S., Dallaire, J. R., Sandler, H. M., & Hoover-Dempsey, K. V. (2005). Parental Involvement: Model revision through scale development. The Elementary School Journal, 106(2), 85-104.

Wanless, S. B., & Crawford, P. A. (2016). Reading your way to a culturally responsive

classroom. Young Children, 71(2), 8-15. Watts, J.C. & Barnett, I.C. (1973). Four children: Four environments. In B.L. White &

J.C. Watts (Eds.), Experience and environment: Major influence on the development of the young child (pp. 201-229). Englewood, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in Head Start children:

strengthening parenting competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(5), 715-730.

Webster‐Stratton, C., Reid, J.M., & Stoolmiller, M. (2008). Preventing conduct problems

and improving school readiness: evaluation of the incredible years teacher and child training programs in high‐risk schools. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(5), 471-488.

Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Hammond, M. (2001). Preventing conduct problems,

promoting social competence: A Parent and teacher training partnership in Head Start. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(3), 283-302.

Weinberger, L. A., & Starkey, P. (1994). Pretend play by African American children in

head start. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9(3-4), 327-343. White, J., O’Malley, A., Toso, M., Rockel, J., Stover, S., & Ellis, F. (2007). A

contemporary glimpse of play and learning in Aotearoa New Zealand. International Journal of Early Childhood, 39(1), 93–105.

Yoder, N. (2014). Teaching the Whole Child: Instructional Practices that Support Social

and Emotional Learning in Three Teacher Evaluation Frameworks. Washington, DC: American Institutes for Research Center on Great Teachers & Leaders.

Zeece, P. D., & Wallace, B. M. (2009). Books and good stuff: A strategy for building

school to home literacy connections. Early Childhood Education Journal, 37(1), 35-42.

Zigler, E., & Bishop-Josef, S. (2004). Play under siege: A historical overview. In E. F.

Zigler, D. G. Singer, & S. J. Bishop-Josef Eds.), Children’s play: The roots of reading (pp. 1–13). Washington, DC: Zero to Three/National Center for Infants, Toddlers and Families.

Page 240: Cultivating Head Start Children's Approaches to Learning

228

Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, A. (2008). Motivation: An essential dimension of self-regulated learning. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 1-30). New York: Erlbaum.