12
UNCORRECTED PROOF 1 Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of 2 spatial processing and evidence supporting the efciency 3 hypothesis in females 4 Roberto Q1 Colom a, b, , Jason L. Stein c , Priya Rajagopalan c , Kenia Martínez a, b , David Hermel c , 5 Yalin Wang c , Juan Álvarez-Linera d , Miguel Burgaleta a, b , Mª. Ángeles Quiroga e , 6 Pei Chun Shih a, b , Paul M. Thompson c 7 a Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28049 Madrid, Spain 8 b Fundación CIEN, Fundación Reina Sofía, C/Valderrebollo, 5, 28031 Madrid, Spain 9 c Laboratory of Neuroimaging (LONI), UCLA School of Medicine, 635 Charles E. Young Drive South, Suite 225, Los Angeles, CA 90095-7334, USA 10 d Hospital Ruber Internacional, Fundación CIEN, Fundación Reina Sofía, C/Valderrebollo, 5, 28031 Madrid, Spain 11 e Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, 28223 Pozuelo de Alarcón, Madrid, Spain 12 14 article info 15 abstract 16 Article history: 17 Received 14 October 2012 18 Received in revised form 16 January 2013 19 Accepted 24 January 2013 20 Available online xxxx 21 Here we apply a method for automated segmentation of the hippocampus in 3D high-resolution 22 structural brain MRI scans. One hundred and four healthy young adults completed twenty one 23 tasks measuring abstract, verbal, and spatial intelligence, along with working memory, executive 24 control, attention, and processing speed. After permutation tests corrected for multiple com- 25 parisons across vertices (p b .05), significant relationships were found for spatial intelligence, 26 spatial working memory, and spatial executive control. Interactions with sex revealed significant 27 relationships with the general factor of intelligence (g), along with abstract and spatial 28 intelligence. These correlations were mainly positive for males but negative for females, which 29 might support the efficiency hypothesis in women. Verbal intelligence, attention, and processing 30 speed were not related to hippocampal structural differences. 31 © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 32 Keywords: 33 Q2 Hippocampus 34 Intelligence 35 Working memory 36 Executive control 37 Attention 38 Processing speed 39 Sex differences 40 41 42 43 1. Introduction 44 The hippocampus is a small brain region, involved in cog- 45 nition and emotion, capturing a lot of attention from the 46 scientific community (Pessoa, 2008). Hippocampus structure 47 and function has been related to spatial processing (Frings et 48 al., 2006; Maguire et al., 2000), working memory (Axmacher et 49 al., 2010), episodic memory, and the construction of mental 50 images (Bird & Burgess, 2008; Squire et al., 2010) among other 51 cognitive factors. However, the available evidence is mainly 52 based on narrow cognitive measures, small and heterogeneous 53 samples, or broad anatomical hippocampal estimates. Ob- 54 taining a comprehensive view regarding the relationships 55 between hippocampus structure and human cognition in 56 general requires (a) the analysis of large and well defined 57 samples, (b) diverse measures of the same cognitive factor, and 58 (c) fine-grained hippocampal maps. Neglecting these condi- 59 tions opens the door to incomplete and inconsistent findings. 60 To further complicate things, this brain structure is highly 61 complex. Thus, for instance, with respect to spatial processing, 62 Maguire et al. (2000) found no differences in overall hippo- 63 campal volume between taxi drivers and controls. However, 64 drivers showed larger posterior hippocampi, whereas controls 65 had larger anterior hippocampal volumes. In addition, it has 66 been shown that not all working memory components are Intelligence xxx (2013) xxxxxx Corresponding author at: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28049 Madrid, Spain. Tel.: +34 91 497 41 14; fax: +34 91 497 52 15. E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Colom). INTELL-00757; No of Pages 12 0160-2896/$ see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Intelligence Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidence supporting the efciency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002 Á b d R

Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of ...gsl.lab.asu.edu/archive/INTELL_757.pdf · 2 spatial processing and evidence supporting the efficiency ... the abstract reasoning

  • Upload
    builien

  • View
    225

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF1 Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of

2 spatial processing and evidence supporting the efficiency3 hypothesis in females

4 RobertoQ1 Colom a,b,⁎, Jason L. Stein c, Priya Rajagopalan c, Kenia Martínez a,b, David Hermel c,5 Yalin Wang c, Juan Álvarez-Linera d, Miguel Burgaleta a,b, Mª. Ángeles Quiroga e,6 Pei Chun Shih a,b, Paul M. Thompson c

7 a Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, 28049 Madrid, Spain8 b Fundación CIEN, Fundación Reina Sofía, C/Valderrebollo, 5, 28031 Madrid, Spain9 c Laboratory of Neuroimaging (LONI), UCLA School of Medicine, 635 Charles E. Young Drive South, Suite 225, Los Angeles, CA 90095-7334, USA10 d Hospital Ruber Internacional, Fundación CIEN, Fundación Reina Sofía, C/Valderrebollo, 5, 28031 Madrid, Spain11 e Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Campus de Somosaguas, 28223 Pozuelo de Alarcón, Madrid, Spain

12

14 a r t i c l e i n f o15 a b s t r a c t

16 Article history:17 Received 14 October 201218 Received in revised form 16 January 201319 Accepted 24 January 201320 Available online xxxx

21Here we apply a method for automated segmentation of the hippocampus in 3D high-resolution22structural brain MRI scans. One hundred and four healthy young adults completed twenty one23tasks measuring abstract, verbal, and spatial intelligence, along with working memory, executive24control, attention, and processing speed. After permutation tests corrected for multiple com-25parisons across vertices (pb .05), significant relationships were found for spatial intelligence,26spatial working memory, and spatial executive control. Interactions with sex revealed significant27relationships with the general factor of intelligence (g), along with abstract and spatial28intelligence. These correlations were mainly positive for males but negative for females, which29might support the efficiency hypothesis in women. Verbal intelligence, attention, and processing30speed were not related to hippocampal structural differences.31© 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

32 Keywords:33Q2 Hippocampus34 Intelligence35 Working memory36 Executive control37 Attention38 Processing speed39 Sex differences4041

42

43 1. Introduction

44 The hippocampus is a small brain region, involved in cog-45 nition and emotion, capturing a lot of attention from the46 scientific community (Pessoa, 2008). Hippocampus structure47 and function has been related to spatial processing (Frings et48 al., 2006;Maguire et al., 2000), workingmemory (Axmacher et49 al., 2010), episodic memory, and the construction of mental50 images (Bird & Burgess, 2008; Squire et al., 2010) among other51 cognitive factors. However, the available evidence is mainly

52based on narrow cognitivemeasures, small and heterogeneous53samples, or broad anatomical hippocampal estimates. Ob-54taining a comprehensive view regarding the relationships55between hippocampus structure and human cognition in56general requires (a) the analysis of large and well defined57samples, (b) diversemeasures of the same cognitive factor, and58(c) fine-grained hippocampal maps. Neglecting these condi-59tions opens the door to incomplete and inconsistent findings.60To further complicate things, this brain structure is highly61complex. Thus, for instance, with respect to spatial processing,62Maguire et al. (2000) found no differences in overall hippo-63campal volume between taxi drivers and controls. However,64drivers showed larger posterior hippocampi, whereas controls65had larger anterior hippocampal volumes. In addition, it has66been shown that not all working memory components are

Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

⁎ Corresponding author at: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónomade Madrid, 28049 Madrid, Spain. Tel.: +34 91 497 41 14; fax: +34 91 49752 15.

E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Colom).

INTELL-00757; No of Pages 12

0160-2896/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Intelligence

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

JÁ bJ d R

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

67 related to the hippocampus: it is not relevant for working68 memory binding processes (Baddeley, Allen, & Vargha-69 Khadem, 2010) and it is more active for associating items70 from different cortical regions than from the same cortical71 region (Piekema, Kessels, Rijpkema, & Fernandez, 2009). The72 hippocampus contributes to a temporo-occipital network for73 relational working memory maintenance, but to a fronto-74 parietal network for non-relationalmaintenance (Cashdollar et75 al., 2009; Nee & Jonides, 2011).76 Therefore, it can be stated that the hippocampus is not a77 homogeneous structure (Pantel et al., 2000). The anterior78 region is mainly, but not exclusively, related to emotion – it is79 connected with the prefrontal cortex, the amygdala, and the80 hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis – whereas the posterior81 region is essentially implicated in cognition (Bannerman et al.,82 2004). In addition, regional structural changes across develop-83 ment (age changes) have been documented: the anterior and84 posterior regions are reduced and increased over time re-85 spectively. Volume reduction at the posterior regions appears86 to be greater for females, while volume reduction at the87 anterior region is larger in males (Gogtay et al., 2006). Cahill88 (2006) underscored the need for challenging the presumption89 that sex is not important in neuroscience research, specifically90 discussing data related to the hippocampus. He underscores91 that male and female hippocampi are significantly different in92 anatomical structure, but “whether and how sex influences93 hippocampal function has not been yet systematically exam-94 ined” (p. 3).95 As noted above, most available studies consider isolated96 cognitive tests or tasks, as well as arguable defined general97 estimates, for relating cognitive performance differences to98 hippocampal structure and function. Sometimes a significant99 relationship is found for a given cognitive function, whereas100 other times researchers fail to obtain significant results.101 Contradictory or inconsistent findings may be attributed to102 several factors, such as the specific administered tasks for103 estimating the construct of interest or sample's characteris-104 tics (size, sex, age, etc.). Overcoming this situation involves105 the analysis of large and well defined samples tapping the106 constructs of interest as representatively as possible admin-107 istering several tasks (Barbey et al., 2012;Q3 Colom, Karama,108 Jung, & Haier, 2010; Colom & Thompson, 2011; Colom et al.,109 2009; Colom et al., 2010; Gläscher et al., 2010; Haier et al.,110 2009).111 For this purpose here,we analyze the relationships between112 regional hippocampal structural differences and a wide set of113 cognitive factors, namely abstract-fluid, verbal-crystallized,114 and spatial intelligence, along with working memory, execu-115 tive control, attention, and processing speed. Fluid-abstract116 intelligence (Gf) assesses the complexity level that subjects can117 handle in situations for which previous knowledge is not118 relevant, whereas crystallized-verbal intelligence (Gc) relies in119 the ability to copewith academic types of skills and knowledge,120 such reading or arithmetic (Cattell, 1971). Spatial intelligence121 (Gv) implicates the construction, temporary retention, and122 manipulation of mental images (Lohman, 2000). The general123 factor of intelligence (g) is a source of variance common to a124 variety of abstract, verbal, and non-verbal abilities (Gläscher et125 al., 2010; Karama et al., 2011).Workingmemory can bedefined126 as the ability for the simultaneous storage and processing of127 varied amounts of information (Colom, Rebollo, Abad, & Shih,

1282006). Executive control implicates the ability for regulating129mental processes. Inhibition, shifting or updating are key130components of this type of control (Friedman et al., 2006).131Attention is a broad cognitive function for focusing available132mental resources (Baddeley, 2002). Here, we consider the133control of automatic responses (inhibition). Finally, processing134speed is usually measured by reaction time tasks (Sheppard &135Vernon, 2008), so simple verification tasks are administered in136the present study. Fig. 1 depicts examples of tests and tasks137measuring some of these cognitive factors (detailed descrip-138tions can be found below). While there are published studies139considering some of these functions in isolation, to our140knowledge there is none applying automated mapping of141hippocampal structure to a large and well defined sample of142participants assessing the constructs of interest by diverse143cognitive tests and tasks.

1442. Method

1452.1. Participants and neuroimaging data

146The sample comprised 104 young adults (59 females and 45147males)with amean age of 19.9 (SD=1.6, age range=18–27).1

148They were selected to avoid medical or psychiatric illness,149including a history of head injury and substance abuse. This150was carefully checked by a standardized questionnaire regu-151larly administered where the scanning took place (Fundación152Reina Sofía, Madrid). Written informed consent was obtained153following the Helsinki guidelines (World Medical Association,1542008).155MRIs were obtained with a 3 T scanner.2 The scans were156lineally registered to the ICBM template with a 9 parameter157transformation (3 translations, 3 rotations, and 3 scales) using158the Mintracc algorithm. Aligned images were resampled in an159isotropic space with a voxel size of 1 mm3.160Once the 3D registered volumes were available, twenty161representative cases in terms of sex and variability in cognitive162indices were chosen for manual tracing. These cases were163manually traced by two researchers that worked independently164with the forty images until they achieved appropriate reliability165indices after the delineated labels: sensitivity=.84, specifici-166ty=1, overlay=.78. Next, the remaining eighty four (testing)167brains were submitted to a LONI Pipeline for their automatic168segmentation (Morra et al., 2008).169This Pipeline comprises several steps and produces outputs170for the left and right hippocampus. Afterwards a detailed171quality control (QC)was performed for checking these outputs,

1 This sample was previously analyzed for achieving quite differentresearch goals. Colom et al. (2009) applied a VBM protocol for analyzinggray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence factors(excluding the cerebellum) in 100 participants from this sample. Burgaletaet al. (2012) analyzed sex differences in this same one hundred participantsexploring total, gray, and white matter volumes with respect to abstract,verbal, and spatial intelligence subtests. Bruner, Cuétara, Colom, and Martin-Loeches (2012, 2010); Bruner, Martin-Loeches, and Colom (2010); Bruner,Martín-Loeches, Burgaleta, and Colom (2011), and Martin-Loeches, Bruner,Cuétara, and Colom (in press) based their research in 2D midsagital brainshape variations.

2 GEHC Waukesha, WI, 3 T Excite HDX 8-channels coil. 3D: FSPGR with IRpreparation pulse (TR 5.7 ms, TE 2.4 ms, TI 750 ms, flip angle 12°). Sagacquisition .8 mm thickness, full brain coverage (220 slices), matrix266×266 FOV 24 (isotropic voxels .7 cm3).

2 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

IR

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

Fig. 1. Examples of tests and tasks. Top panel depicts items from the intelligence tests, whereas bottom panel shows some captions for working memory (dotmatrix), executive control (n back), processing speed (spatial recognition speed), and attention (Simon).

3R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

172 as detailed below. A second Pipeline was then applied for173 obtaining hippocampal surface indices. Computed regional174 volumetric differences for the left and right hippocampuswere175 related to the cognitive scores of interest. Probability maps176 were obtained after permutation tests corrected for multiple177 comparisons (pb .05). Specific details regarding manual delin-178 eation of the hippocampus, the automatic recognition algo-179 rithm, and the production of statistical maps can be found180 below.

181 2.2. Psychological measures

182 Twenty one cognitive tests and taskswere administered for183 measuring the psychological constructs of interests defined184 above. Importantly, all these constructs were estimated by185 three different measures for obtaining theoretically represen-186 tative scores.187 Abstract-fluid intelligence (Gf) was measured by the Ad-188 vanced ProgressiveMatrices Test (APM) (Raven, Raven, & Court,189 2004), the abstract reasoning subtests from the Differential190 Aptitude Test (DAT-AR) battery (Bennett, Seashore, &Wesman,191 2005), and the inductive reasoning subtests from the Primary192 Mental Abilities (PMA-R) battery (Thurstone, 1938). The APM193 comprises a matrix figure with three rows and three columns.194 Among eight possible alternatives, the one completing the 3×3195 matrix figuremust be chosen. The screening version comprising196 odd items only was administered (max. score=18). DAT-AR is197 a series test based on abstract figures. Successive figures follow a198 given rule, so the one continuing the seriesmust be chosen from199 several alternatives. The screening version comprising odd200 items only was administered (max. score=20). PMA-R com-201 prises letters' series items. The rule (or rules) underlying a given202 sequence must be extracted for selecting the correct alternative203 (max. score=30).204 Verbal-crystallized intelligence (Gc) was measured by the205 verbal and numerical reasoning subtests from the DAT (VR206 and NR), along with the vocabulary subtests from the PMA207 (V). DAT-VR is based on sentences stated like an analogy. The208 first and last words from the sentence are missing, and a pair209 of words completing the sentence must be selected. The210 screening version comprising odd items only was adminis-211 tered (max. score=20). PMA-V is a synonym test based on212 the meaning of words that must be evaluated against a given213 model word (max. score=50). DAT-NR consists of quantita-214 tive reasoning problems. The screening version comprising215 odd items only was administered (max. score=20).216 Spatial intelligence (Gv) was measured by the spatial217 relations subtests from the DAT (SR), the spatial rotation218 subtests from the PMA (S), and the rotation of solid figures test219 (Yela, 1969). Items from the rotation of solid figures test are220 based on a 3Dmodel figure and several 3D rotated alternatives221 (max. score=21). PMA-S includes a model figure and six222 alternatives, some of whom are simply rotated versions of the223 model figure, whereas the remaining figures are mirror224 imaged. Only rotated figures are to be selected and there225 could be several correct options for each item. The score is the226 total number of correct responses (appropriately selected227 figures — simply rotated) minus the total number of incorrect228 responses (inappropriately selected figures — mirror imaged)229 (max. score=54). DAT-SR is a mental folding test. One230 unfolded figure is shown at the left, whereas figures at the

231right depict folded versions. The folded figure matching the232unfolded figure at the left must be chosen. The screening233version comprising odd items only was administered (max.234score=25).235Working memory (WM) was measured by the reading236span, computation span, and dot matrix tasks ( Q4Colom, Karama237et al., 2010; Colom, Quiroga, et al., 2010). In the reading span238task, participants verify if a set of sentences sequentially239displayed make or make no sense. Each display includes a240sentence and a to-be remembered capital letter. Sentences are24110–15 words long. At the end of a given set, participants recall,242in their correct serial order, each letter from the set. Set sizes243range from 3 to 6 sentence/letter pairs per trial, for a total of 12244trials (4 levels×3 trials=12 trials total). The computation span245task includes a verification task and a recall task. 6 s is allowed246to see the math equation without a time limit for verifying its247accuracy. The displayed solution, irrespective of its accuracy,248must be serially remembered at the end of a given set. Each249math equation includes two operations using digits from 1 to25010. The solutions are always single-digit numbers. Trials range251from three to seven equation/solutions (5 levels×3 trials252each=15 trials total). In the dot matrix task, a matrix equation253must be verified and a dot location displayed in a 5×5 grid254must be retained. The matrix equation is presented during a255maximum of 4.5 s for adding or subtracting simple line256drawings. Once the response is given, the grid comprising the257to-be remembered dot is displayed for 1.5 s. After a given set of258equation–grid pairs, the grid spaces that contained dots must259be recalled clicking with the mouse on an empty grid. Trials260increase in size from two to five equations and dots (4 levels×3261trials=12 trials total). The score for these three WM tasks is262the number of hits in the verification and recalling tasks.263Executive control (updating, UPD) was measured by the 2264back, keep track, and letter memory tasks (Colom, Abad,265Quiroga, Shih, & Flores-Mendoza, 2008). In the 2-back task,266upper and lower case letters are presented in one of eight267equidistant spatial locations around the center of a computer268screen. Stimuli are presented for 200 ms and 1300 ms are269given for responding. There are 75 experimental stimuli of270which 25 are match stimuli. Participants press the space bar of271the keyboard to make a match response (a letter presented in272the same spatial location 2 positions back in the sequence). The273score is the number of hits. In the keep track task, participants274see several categories at the bottom of the computer screen.275Fifteen items, including two or three exemplars from each of276the six possible categories (Odd, even, vowel, consonant, low-277ercase pairs of letters, and uppercase pairs of letters) are then278presented serially and in random order for 1500 ms each, with279the target categories remaining at the bottomof the screen. The280last item presented for each target category must be retained.281Therefore, items must be monitored for updating memory282representations for the appropriate categories. After the283practice trials, participants complete three trials with four284target categories, and three trials with five target categories,285recalling a maximum of 27 items. In the letter memory task286several letters are presented serially for 1000 ms per letter. The287last four letters presented in the list must be remembered.288Instructions require and emphasize rehearsing the last four289letters by mentally adding the most recent letter and deleting290the fifth letter back. Participants perform three practice trials,291and there are six experimental trials of varying length (15, 17,

4 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

1

3

f

ts

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

292 19, 21, 23, and 25) randomly presented, for a total of 24 letters293 recalled.294 Attention (ATT) was measured by verbal and numerical295 versions of the flanker task, along with a spatial variant of the296 Simon task (Q5 Colom, Karama et al., 2010; Colom, Quiroga, et al.,297 2010). The verbal and quantitative tasks require deciding, as fast298 as possible, if the letter/digit presented in the center of a set of299 three letters/digits is vowel/odd or consonant/even. The target300 (e.g. vowel/odd) can be surrounded by compatible (e.g. vowel/301 odd) or incompatible (e.g. consonant/even) letters/digits. The302 spatial task requires deciding if an arrow (horizontally depicted)303 points to the left or to the right of a fixation point. The target304 arrow pointing to a given direction (e.g. to the left) can305 be presented at the left (e.g. compatible) or at the right306 (e.g. incompatible) of the fixation point. There are a total of 32307 practice trials and 80 experimental trials. Half of the trials are308 compatible and they are randomly presented across the entire309 session. Themean reaction time for the incompatible trials is the310 dependent measure.311 Finally, processing speed (PS) was measured by simple312 recognition verbal, numerical, and spatial tasks (Colom et al.,313 2008). In these speed tasks, one or two items (letter, digit, or314 arrows) are sequentially displayed for 650 ms. each. Those315 itemsdefine amemory set that can comprise (a) uppercase and316 lowercase letters, (b) digits, or (c) arrows with different317 shapes. After the last displayed item, a fixation point appears318 for 500 ms. Finally, the probe item appears in order to decide,319 as quickly and accurately as possible (a) if it has the same320 meaning as one of the letters, (b) if the number can be divided321 by one of the digits, or (c) if it has the sameorientation of one of322 the arrows, presented within the memory set. Experimental323 trials range from one to two items (2 levels×30 trials each=324 60 trials total). For example [Verbal speed]: [Memory Set] [First325 screen] A⋙ [Second screen] a⋙[Answer screen, Samemean-326 ing?] ‘Yes’ ‘No’. The score is the mean RT for the correct327 answers.328 All these tests and tasks were always administered in the329 same order. Intelligence tests were administered in sessions 1330 and 2, whereas cognitive tasks were administered in sessions 3331 and 4. Session 1 was devoted to the RAPM (20 min), inductive332 reasoning (PMA-R, 6 min), vocabulary (PMA-V, 4 min), and333 abstract reasoning (DAT-AR, 10 min), while session 2 was334 comprised by verbal reasoning (DAT-VR, 10 min), Rotation of335 solid figures (5 min), numerical reasoning (DAT-NR, 10 min),336 mental rotation (PMA-S, 5 min), and spatial relations (DAT-SR,337 10 min). Session 3 included the working memory and process-338 ing speed tasks, whereas session 4 was comprised by the exec-339 utive and attention tasks.

340 2.3. Manual delineation of the hippocampus

341 The training brains set comprised twenty subjects (half were342 females) chosen for manual delineation of the left and right343 hippocampus (forty images). Manual delineation of the left and344 right structures was performed on the template registered345 images using BrainSuite 2.0 (Shattuck & Leahy, 2002) by two346 researchers working independently. Reliability of the manual347 traces was assessed by sensitivity (the likelihood that a voxel348 defined by researcher A as being within the hippocampus has349 also been defined by researcher B as being within the hip-350 pocampus), specificity (the likelihood that a voxel defined by

351one researcher as not being within the hippocampus has also352not been defined by the other researcher as being within the353hippocampus), and overlay (the percentage of voxels classified354as hippocampus by both researchers divided by the voxels355classified as hippocampus by either researcher).356Hippocampal delineation was conducted according to357guidelines provided by Duvernoy (1998) and Pantel et al.358(2000). The hippocampus includes the cornu ammonis subfields359(CA1–4), dentate gyrus, and subiculum. Manual tracing was360started on the sagittal plane, moving from lateral to medial. The361first slicewas chosenwhere the graymatter of the hippocampus362opened in a pocket of the lateral ventricles. Tracing continued363medially, excluding the ventricles, fimbria, and surrounding364white matter. In anterior portions, the hippocampus was dis-365tinguished from the amygdala through small contrast change of366the alveus surrounding the hippocampal head. The alveus was367excluded whenever possible moving between slices and368between sagittal and coronal views along with references to369neuroanatomical delineations (Duvernoy, 1998).370To reinforce the smoothness of the brain in the manual371delineation, structures were reviewed and edited in the coronal372plane moving from posterior to inferior sections. Jumps in373boundaries between slices could be visualized as jagged borders374in a perpendicular plane. Afterwards, the manual tracing went375back to the sagittal plane for re-editing and ensuring the376pursued smoothness.

Table 1 t1:1

t1:2Means and SDs (standard deviation) for males and females on the cognitivet1:3factors and measures; the effect size (d) is also shown.

t1:4Variable Males(N=45)

Females(N=59)

d

t1:5Mean SD Mean SD

t1:6g (General intelligence) 159 25 155 23 0.16t1:7Gf (Fluid intelligence) 45 8 46 8 −0.21t1:8Gc (Crystallized intelligence) 60 10 57 10 0.38t1:9Gv (Spatial intelligence) 54 15 52 14 0.13t1:10WMC (Working memory) 215 17 211 19 0.22t1:11Executive control 50 9 51 9 −0.17t1:12Attention 551 65 550 55 −0.02t1:13Processing speed 717 176 729 156 −0.07t1:14RAPM 12 3 12 2 −0.06t1:15PMA-R⁎ 18 4 20 4 −0.49t1:16DAT-AR 15 3 14 4 0.17t1:17PMA-V 34 6 32 7 0.34t1:18DAT-VR 14 4 13 3 0.17t1:19DAT-NR 12 3 12 3 0t1:20Rotation of solid figures⁎⁎ 10 4 8 3 0.64t1:21PMA-S 28 10 27 10 0.10t1:22DAT-SR 15 5 17 4 −0.31t1:23Reading span 98 5 98 8 0.10t1:24Computation span 61 14 58 12 0.24t1:25Dot matrix 55 5 55 5 0t1:262 Back 17 5 16 6 0.17t1:27Letter memory 17 4 18 3 −0.04t1:28Keep track⁎ 15 5 18 4 −0.52t1:29Verbal attention 570 76 549 68 −0.29t1:30Numerical attention⁎ 613 105 605 69 −0.08t1:31Spatial attention 469 65 494 54 0.43t1:32Verbal speed 668 212 613 132 −0.32t1:33Numerical speed 860 305 936 272 0.26t1:34Spatial speed 623 175 639 143 0.10

⁎ pb .05. t1:35⁎⁎ pb .01 t1:36

5R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

[

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

Fig. 2. SEM model for the cognitive factors. Gf=abstract-fluid intelligence, Gc=verbal-crystallized intelligence, Gv=spatial intelligence, WM=workingmemory, UPD=executive updating, Speed=processing speed, and ATT=attention.

Fig. 3. Probability maps (p-maps) for the complete group (N=104) controlling for sex, age, and handedness. Significant results were found for general spatialintelligence (Gv), the spatial relations (SR) subtest from theDAT battery (DAT-SR), the spatial workingmemory task (dotmatrix), and the spatial executive control task(n back). Asterisks (*) highlight significant effects (pb .05, corrected for multiple comparisons). Radiological convention is the framework for depicting RH (righthippocampus, on the readers' left) and LH (left hippocampus, on the reader's right) along with views for superior and inferior surfaces. The two latter views arerepresented only when significant findings are found for both.

6 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

377 2.3.1. Automatic recognition378 The 20 left and 20 right training brains were separately used379 to train an automated recognition algorithm to delineate the380 structure in the remaining 84 brains. The so-called auto-context381 model, based on a modified AdaBoost algorithm (Morra et al.,382 2008)was run. Thismethod takes in the binary training scans of383 one researcher, template registered structural MRI images, and384 tissue segmented images of the 20 manually delineated brains.385 It then extracts features from the MRI and tissue segmented386 images based on image intensity, position, image curvatures,387 image gradients, and Haar filters. Combinations of features are388 used to predict the probability of each voxel belonging to a389 defined structure in each of the 84 testing brains. Voxels with390 greater than 0.5 probability are identified as being within the391 structure of interest. Each automated structural definition was392 explicitly checked, so stray voxels and false classifications (i.e.393 hippocampus extending into parahippocampal gyrus, contra-394 lateral putamen being identified, etc.) weremanually corrected.395 Small islands of voxels within slices which were disconnected396 from other voxels of the structure were also removed.

3972.3.2. Statistical maps398Statistical maps are generated to reveal local differences in399radial hippocampal distance and individual differences in these400local surface parameters. At each hippocampal point multiple401regression evaluates whether individual differences at that402point are related to the covariate of interest. The p value de-403scribing the significance of this association controlling for404nuisance covariates (sex, age, and handedness for the complete405group) was plotted onto the surface at each point on the406hippocampus using a color code to produce a statistical map.407Overall p values were assigned to the maps using a permuta-408tion testing approach which measures the distribution of a409given feature in statistical maps (the area with statistics above410a predetermined threshold) thatwould be observed by accident411if subjects were randomly assigned to the covariates of interest.412This distribution is used to compare the features that occurred413empirically with those that occurred by accident in the random414assignments. A ratio is computed describingwhat fraction of the415time an effect of similar or greater magnitude to the real effect416occurs in the random assignments. This is the chance of the

Fig. 4. Sex differences in total hippocampal volumes (top panel, effect sizes – d – are also shown) and regional structural differences between males and females(bottom panel). Asterisks (*) highlight significant effects (pb .05, corrected). Radiological convention is the framework for depicting RH (right hippocampus, onthe readers' left) and LH (left hippocampus, on the reader's right) along with views for superior and inferior surfaces.

7R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

c

) R d

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

8 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

417 observed pattern occurring by accident. This fraction provides418 an overall significance value (p value) for the whole map. The419 proportion of the surface where individual differences are420 related to the covariates of interest at a primary threshold of421 pb0.05 was assessed.422 One sided tests were used, so positive p-maps depict a direct423 relationship — radial distance increases with score increase in424 the covariate of interest, whereas negative p-maps indicate an425 inverse relationship— radial distance decreaseswith increase in426 the covariate of interest.427 Two further contrasts were applied. First, the interaction428 among sex, cognitive scores, and hippocampal structural dif-429 ferences (controlling for age and handedness). Afterwards,430 betamapswere computed for significant findings derived from431 the above interaction. This was intended for interpreting areas432 of significant interaction. The main question to answer here is433 how the hippocampus is associated with cognitive scores434 between males and females.

435 3. Results

436 Results for the psychological constructs/measures separat-437 ed by sex are shown in Table 1. Effect sizes (d) are also reported438 in the last column. It can be seen that females outperform439 males in PMA-R (inductive reasoning), keep track (executive

440updating), and numerical attention,whereasmales outperform441females in the rotation of solid figures test (spatial intelligence)442only. Anyways, most of the differences are not significant, and443certainly, they are not in the most general estimates of444cognition (eight first rows).445Simultaneous relationships among the assessed psycholog-446ical constructs were tested through structural equation model-447ing (SEM) — Fig. 2. Fit indices for this model were reasonable:448CMIN=242.4, DF=168, CMIN/DF=1.4, RMSEA=.065. There449are several noteworthy results. First, working memory (WM)450and updating (UP) show a perfect correlation. Second, fluid451intelligence (Gf) and updating show a very high correlation452(.93). Third, processing speed is related to crystallized and453spatial, but not to fluid intelligence. Fourth, attention is only454related to processing speed and updating. Finally, most of the455correlations are statistically significant (although quite different456in magnitude) but there are also non-significant relationships457(those not shown in Fig. 2).458With respect to imaging analyses, here we report statistical459maps build from significant relations between hippocampal460regional structural differences and the psychological constructs/461measures of interest after the computation of permutation tests462corrected for multiple comparisons (pb .05) as noted above.463First, the complete group (N=104) was considered (con-464trolling for sex, age, and handedness). As depicted in Fig. 3,

Fig. 5. Probability maps (p-maps) showing the interaction among sex, cognition, and hippocampal structure are depicted on the left. Beta maps indicating thedirection of these interactions are shown on the right (yellow and red depict negative values, whereas blue depicts positive values). Significant findings wereobserved for [A] the general factor of intelligence (g) and abstract-fluid intelligence (Gf), [B] the Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) test and the abstractreasoning (AR) subtests from the DAT battery (DAT-AR), and [C] the rotation of solid figures test. Asterisks (*) highlight significant effects (pb .05, corrected).Radiological convention is the framework for depicting RH (right hippocampus, on the readers' left) and LH (left hippocampus, on the reader's right) along withviews for superior and inferior surfaces. These two latter views are represented only when significant findings are found for both. (For interpretation of thereferences to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

9R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

465 significant relationshipsweremainly found in posterior areas for466 general spatial intelligence (Gv), the spatial relations (SR)467 subtest from the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT) battery468 (DAT-SR), the spatial n back task measuring executive control469 (2 back), and the spatial working memory task (dot matrix).470 Secondly, sex differences in hippocampal structure were471 inspected. Top panel of Fig. 4 shows total, right, and left volumes472 formales and females (effect sizes, d, are also depicted), whereas473 the bottompanel depicts regional differences in the right and left474 hippocampus. Males do show larger hippocampi, especially on475 the left side (d=.56). Regionally, sex differences are concentrat-476 ed on the posterior regions of the right hippocampus, but they477 are more widespread on the left side.478 Third, because of the observed sex differences, interac-479 tions among hippocampal structure, cognitive measures, and480 sex were computed (controlling for age and handedness).481 Results can be inspected in Fig. 5.482 Significant findings were observed for the general factor of483 intelligence (g), abstract-fluid intelligence (Gf), and the Abstract484 Reasoning subtest from the DAT battery (DAT-AR) on the left485 posterior hippocampus, as well as for the Advanced Progressive486 Matrices (APM) test and the rotation of solid figures test on the487 right anterior hippocampus. The right posterior hippocampus is488 also relevant for the rotation of solid figures test.489 Betamaps, depicted on the right panel of Fig. 5, showmainly490 positive relationships for males but negative for females. These491 beta maps allow interpreting areas of significant interaction492 regarding how the hippocampus is associated with cognitive493 scores between sexes. Thus, there is a positive association with494 hippocampal shape and cognitive performance in males — the495 larger the radial distance, the better the score, whereas for496 females this association is negative — the shorter the radial497 distance, the better the performance.498 Finally, verbal intelligence, attention, and processing speed499 scores were unrelated to hippocampal regional structural500 differences. Therefore, verbal-crystallized abilities, controlled501 inhibition processing, and mental speed processes, were not502 significantly associated to hippocampal structure in this sample.

503 4. Discussion

504 Here we report findings providing what might be consid-505 ered an interesting general picture. Firstly, varied – but not506 general – measures comprising spatial processing require-507 ments are significantly related to hippocampal structural508 differences. Specifically, spatial intelligence, spatial executive509 control processes, and spatial working memory. It is notewor-510 thy that abstract processing, verbal processing, and numerical511 processing were not significantly related to this brain structure512 for these contrasts controlling for sex, age, and handedness.513 The n back task revealed significant associations with the left514 and right hippocampus, the dot matrix task was circumscribed515 to the left posterior hippocampus, and spatial ability was516 mainly focused on the right hippocampus.517 Secondly, these findings change when analyzing the518 interaction with sex. Now the general factor of intelligence519 (g) – comprised by variance common to abstract, verbal, and520 spatial intelligences – as well as abstract-fluid intelligence521 (Gf) and abstract reasoning (DAT-AR) are associated with the522 left posterior hippocampus only. However, the Advanced

523Progressive Matrices (APM) test and the rotation of solid524figures test are related to the right hippocampus only.525Finally, the computed beta maps revealed that these sig-526nificant associations weremainly positive formales but negative527for females, suggesting higher efficiency for women (Haier et al.,5281988; Neubauer & Fink, 2009a). The efficiency hypothesis states529that people showing better cognitive performance do so along530with lower brain activation than people with worse cognitive531scores. However, the evidence regarding sex is largely confusing,532as noted by Neubauer and Fink (2009b). Haier and Benbow533(1995) failed to find positive evidence for the neural efficiency534hypothesis, but they reported substantial sex differences.535Neubauer, Fink, and Schrausser (2002) reportedneural efficiency536for males in spatial tasks, but for females in verbal tasks. Tang et537al. (2010) found positive correlations between intelligence and538integrity of interhemispheric white matter connections in539females, but these correlations were negative in males. The540findings reported here might help providing some light because,541as discussed by Neubauer and Fink (2009b), functional results542may or may not fit structural ones. At this structural level,543females might show greater efficiency requiring less neural544material for achieving behavioral results on a par with males.545As underscored above, the hippocampus is a small brain546structure, but it is also largely complex. Hippocampus547atrophy is of the utmost interest in Alzheimer research, but548it has been observed that, across time, MCI patients can549improve their mental status, develop AD, or remain within550this diagnostic category (Apostolova et al., 2006). Hippocam-551pus volume is sensitive to aerobic exercise, but the positive552impact is focused on the anterior region of the left and right553sides (Erickson et al., 2011). Children raised in low income554families do show smaller hippocampal volumes than children555from medium and high income homes (Hanson, Chandra,556Wolfe, & Pollak, 2011). These results are just examples557consistent with the complexities related to this brain region.558Hines (2011) noted that “there are many reports of sex559differences in the human brain and many of these are yet560unreplicated (…) some findings of neural sex differences561may prove to be spurious” (p. 77). Haier, Jung, Yeo, et al.562(2005) reported substantial sex differences in the structural563correlates of IQ, but Schmidt et al. (2009) showed that males564and females performance on the n back task is supported by565essentially the same brain networks. These two examples566nicely fit Hines's suspicion, but also our previous claim567regarding the inconsistent findings that might be derived568from the lack of analyses of a wide variety of cognitive569measures with the same sample.570In conclusion, the present comprehensive analyses of the571relationships between hippocampal structural differences and572general human cognition suggests that the posterior sections of573the hippocampus are relevant for several, but not general,574spatial processing requirements. This statement applies to575classic psychometric measures of spatial ability, but also to576spatial working memory, and spatial executive control.577However, spatial processing speed and spatial attention per-578formance is not related to the studied hippocampal differences.579This big picture changes when the interaction with sex is580explicitly addressed. In such a case, the relevance of spatial581processes is circumscribed to the rotation of solid figures test,582and general (g) and abstract-fluid (Gf) intelligence become583significant. Interestingly, for g and Gf significant findings were

10 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

,

FFF

id

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

584 found on the left posterior hippocampus, while this same585 region is relevant for abstract reasoning (DAT-AR) also, but not586 for the advanced progressive matrices test (APM). The fact that587 both the APM and the rotation of solid figures test were related588 to the right hippocampus might be consistent with the widely589 recognized – but frequently neglected – APM spatial re-590 quirements (Ackerman, Beier, & Boyle, 2002).

591 Acknowledgments

592 This work was supported by grants SEJ-2006-07890593 (Ministry of Education and Culture, Spain), PR2008-0038, and594 PSI2010-20364 (Ministry of Science and Innovation, Spain).595 Software for automated segmentation was developed at LONI596 (Laboratory of Neuroimaging), School of Medicine, UCLA.

597 References

598 Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2002). Individual differences in599 working memory within a nomological network of cognitive and600 perceptual speed abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General,601 131(4), 567–589.602 Apostolova, L. G., Dutton, R. A., Dinov, I. D., Hayashi, K. M., Toga, A. W.,603 Cummings, J. L., et al. (2006). Conversion of mild cognitive impairment604 to Alzheimer disease predicted by hippocampal atrophy maps. Archives605 of Neurology, 63, 693–699.606 Axmacher, N., Henseler, M. M., Jensen, O., Weinreich, I., Elger, C. E., & Fell, J.607 (2010). Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory608 in the human hippocampus. PNAS, 107(7), 3228–3233.609 Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psycholo-610 gist, 7, 85–97.611 Baddeley, A., Allen, R., & Vargha-Khadem, F. (2010). Is the hippocampus612 necessary for visual and verbal binding in working memory?613 Neuropsychologia, 48, 1089–1095.614 Bannerman, D.M., Rawlins, J. N., McHugh, S. B., Deacon, R. M., Yee, B. K., Bast, T.,615 et al. (2004). Regional dissociations within the hippocampus — Memory616 and anxiety. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 273–283.617 Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., & Grafman, J.618 (2012). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and execu-619 tive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain, 135, 1154–1164.620 Bennett, G. K., Seashore, H. G., & Wesman, A. G. (2005). Differential Aptitude621 Test (DAT-5). Madrid: TEA, S.A.622 Bird, C. M., & Burgess, N. (2008). The hippocampus and memory: Insights623 from spatial processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 182–194.624 Bruner, E., Cuétara, J. M., Colom, R., &Martin-Loeches, M. (2012). Gender-based625 differences in the shape of the human corpus callosum are associated with626 allometric variations. Journal of Anatomy, 220, 417–421.627 Bruner, E., Martín-Loeches, M., Burgaleta, M., & Colom, R. (2011). Midsaggital628 brain shape correlation with intelligence and cognitive performance.629 Intelligence, 39, 141–147.630 Bruner, E., Martin-Loeches, M., & Colom, R. (2010). Midssagital brain shape631 variation: patterns, allometry, and integration. Journal of Anatomy, 216,632 589–599.633 Burgaleta, M., Head, K., Álvarez-Linera, J., Martínez, K., Escorial, S., Haier, R.,634 et al. (2012). Sex differences in brain volume related to specific skills,635 not to general intelligence. Intelligence, 40, 60–68.636 Cahill, L. (May 10). Why sex matters for neuroscience. Nature Reviews637 Neuroscience, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1909 (AOP, published638 online).639 Cashdollar, N., Malecki, U., Rugg-Gunn, F. J., Duncan, J. S., Lavie, N., & Duzel, E.640 (2009). Hippocampus-dependent and -independent theta-networks of641 active maintenance. PNAS, 106(48), 20493–20498.642 Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston:643 Houghton-Mifflin.644 Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Quiroga, Mª. A., Shih, P. C., & Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008).645 Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but646 why? Intelligence, 36, 584–606.647 Colom, R., Haier, R. J., Head, K., Álvarez-Linera, J., Quiroga, Mª. A., Shih, P. C.,648 et al. (2009). Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial649 intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence, 37, 124–135.650 Colom, R., Karama, S., Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2010). Human intelligence651 and brain networks. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 12, 489–501.

652Colom, R., Quiroga, Mª. A., Shih, P. C., Martínez, K., Burgaleta, M., Martínez-653Molina, A., et al. (2010). Improvement in working memory is not related654to increased intelligence scores. Intelligence, 38, 497–505.655Colom, R., Rebollo, I., Abad, F., & Shih, P. C. (2006). Simple span tasks,656complex span tasks, and cognitive abilities: A re-analysis of key studies.657Memory & Cognition, 34, 158–171.658Colom, R., & Thompson, P. M. (2011). Understanding human intelligence by659imaging the brain. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic, A. Furnham, & S. von660Stumm (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of individual differences661(pp. 330–352). London: Wiley-Blackwell.662Duvernoy, H. M. (1998). The human hippocampus.Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.663Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L.,664et al. (February 15). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and665improves memory. PNAS, 108(7), 3017–3022.666Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J.667K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence.668Psychological Science, 17, 172–179.669Frings, L.,Wagner, K., Unterrainer, J., Spreer, J., Halsband, U., & Schulze-Bonhage, A.670(2006). Gender-relateddifferences in lateralizationof hippocampal activation671and cognitive strategy. Neuroreport, 17(4), 417–421.672Gläscher, J., Rudrauf, D., Colom, R., Paul, L. K., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., et al.673(2010). The distributed neural system for general intelligence revealed674by lesion mapping. PNAS, 107(10), 4705–4709.675Gogtay, N., Nugent, T. F., Herman, D. H., Ordonez, A., Greenstein, D., Hayashi,676K. M., et al. (2006). Dynamic mapping of normal human hippocampal677development. Hippocampus, 16, 664–672.678Haier, R., & Benbow, C. P. (1995). Sex differences and lateralization in679temporal lobe glucose metabolism during mathematical reasoning.680Developmental Neuropsychology, 11, 405–414.681Haier, R. J., Colom, R., Schroeder, D., Condon, C., Tang, C., Eaves, E., et al.682(2009). Gray matter and intelligence factors: Is there a neuro-g?683Intelligence, 37, 136–144.684Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., et al. (2005). The neuroanatomy of general685intelligence: Sex matters. NeuroImage, 25, 320–327.686Q6Haier, R. J., Siegel, B. V., Jr., Nuechterlein, K. H., Hazlett, E., Wu, J. C., Paek, J.,687et al. (1988). Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract688reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography.689Intelligence, 12, 199-21.690Hanson, J. L., Chandra, A., Wolfe, B. L., & Pollak, S. D. (2011). Association691between income and the hippocampus. PLoS One, 6(5), e18712.692Hines, M. (2011). Gender development and the human brain. Annual Review693of Neuroscience, 34, 69–88.694Karama, S., Colom, R., Johnson, W., Deary, I. J., Haier, R. J., Waber, D. P., et al.695(2011). Cortical thickness correlates of cognitive performance accounted696for by the general factor of intelligence in health children aged 6 to 18.697NeuroImage, 55, 1443–1453.698Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In R.699J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). Cambridge:700Cambridge University Press.701Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J.,702Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in703the hippocampi of taxi drivers. PNAS, 97(8), 4398–4403.704Q7Martin-Loeches, M., Bruner, E., Cuétara, J. M., & Colom, R. (in press).705Correlation between corpus callosum shape and cognitive performance706in a sample of healthy young adults. Brain Structure and Function.707Morra, J. H., Tu, Z., Apostolova, L. G., Green, A. E., Avedissian, C., Madsen, S. K.,708et al. (2008). Validation of a fully automated 3D hippocampal709segmentation method using subjects with Alzheimer's disease mild710cognitive impairment, and elderly controls. NeuroImage, 43, 59–68.711Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2011). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal cortex712and the hippocampus to short-term memory: Evidence for a 3-state713model of memory. NeuroImage, 54, 1540–1548.714Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2009a). Intelligence and neural efficiency:715Measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity716in the brain. Intelligence, 37, 223–229.717Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2009b). Intelligence and neural efficiency.Neuroscience718and Biobehavioral Reviews, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.04.001.719Neubauer, A. C., Fink, A., & Schrausser, D. G. (2002). Intelligence and neural720efficiency: The influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ721relationship. Intelligence, 30, 515–536.722Pantel, J., O'Leary, D. S., Cretsinger, K., Bockholt, H. J., Keefe, H., Magnotta, V. A.,723et al. (2000). A newmethod for the in vivo volumetric measurement of the724human hippocampus with high neuroanatomical accuracy. Hippocampus,72510, 752–758.726Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition.727Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 148–158.728Piekema, C., Kessels, R. P. C., Rijpkema, M., & Fernandez, G. (2009). The729hippocampus supports encoding of between-domain associations within730working memory. Article is online at. http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/73110.1101/lm.1283109

11R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002

RRE

RE

04

ory

id

Q

d

20

UNCO

RRECTED P

RO

OF

732 Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2004). Manual for Raven's Progressive733 Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.734Q8 Schmidt, H., Jogia, J., Fast, K., Christodoulou, T., Haldane, M., Kumari, V., et al.735 (2009). No gender differences in brain activation during the N-back736 task: An fMRI study in healthy individuals. Human Brain Mapping.737 Shattuck, D. W., & Leahy, R. M. (2002). BrainSuite: An automated cortical738 surface identification tool. Medical Image Analysis, 8(2), 129–142.739 Sheppard, L. D., & Vernon, P. A. (2008). Intelligence and speed of740 information-processing: A review of 50 years of research. Personality741 and Individual Differences, 44, 535–551.

742Squire, L. R., van der Horst, A. S., McDuff, S. G. R., Frascino, J. C., Hopkins, R. O.,743& Mauldin, K. N. (2010). Role of the hippocampus in remembering the744past and imagining the future. PNAS, 107(44), 19044–19048.745Tang, C. Y., Eaves, E. L., Ng, J. C., Carpenter, D. M., Kanellopoulou, I., Mai, X., et al.746(2010). Brain networks for working memory and factors of intelligence747assessed inmales and females with fMRI and DTI. Intelligence, 38, 293–303.748Thurstone, L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Psychometric Monographs, 1.749Yela, M. (1969). Rotación de Figuras Macizas [Rotation of solid figures]. Madrid:750TEA.

751

752

12 R. Colom et al. / Intelligence xxx (2013) xxx–xxx

Please cite this article as: Colom, R., et al., Hippocampal structure and human cognition: Key role of spatial processing and evidencesupporting the efficiency hypothesis in fem..., Intelligence (2013), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.002