Nahari12Scan

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    1/9

    Does the Truth Come Out in the Writing? SCAN as a Lie Detection Tool

    Galit NahariBar-Ilan University

    Aldert VrijUniversity of Portsmouth

    Ronald P. FisherFlorida International University

    We tested the accuracy of Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN), a verbal lie detection tool that is used

    world-wide by federal law enforcement and military agencies. Sixty-one participants were requested to

    write down the truth, an outright lie or a concealment lie about activities they had just completed. The

    statements were coded with SCAN and with another verbal lie detection tool, Reality Monitoring (RM).

    RM discriminated significantly between truth tellers and outright liars and between truth tellers and

    concealment liars, whereas SCAN did not discriminate between truth tellers and either kind of liar.

    Implications of the findings for the suitability of SCAN as a lie detection tool are discussed.

    Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN), developed by Avinoam

    Sapir, a former polygraph examiner in the Israeli police, is a verbal

    veracity assessment tool. It is used in countries such as Australia,

    Belgium, Canada, Israel, Mexico, UK, US, the Netherlands, Qatar,

    Singapore, and South Africa (Vrij, 2008). It is used by federal law

    enforcement (including the FBI), military agencies (including the

    US Army Military Intelligence), secret services (including the

    CIA) and other types of investigators (including social workers,

    lawyers, fire investigators and the American Society for Industrial

    Security) (Bockstaele, 2008; www.lsiscan.co.il). http://www

    .lsiscan.com/id29.htm provides a full list of past participants of

    SCAN courses. According to (the American version of) the SCAN

    website (www.lsiscan.com) SCAN courses are mostly given in the

    US and Canada (on a weekly basis in those countries). In addition,

    online courses are also available.

    Our impression is that SCAN is used widely. The second author

    gave a seminar on lie detection during the Second International

    Investigative Interviewing Conference, held in July 2006 at the

    University of Portsmouth (UK). That seminar was attended by

    about 100 criminal investigators from many countries including

    Canada, UK, US, and the Netherlands. When asked which lie

    detection tool, if any, they mainly use, the most frequent answer,

    from investigators on both sides of the Atlantic, was SCAN. It was

    reported to be used even more frequently than the well known Reid

    technique in lie detection.

    In the SCAN procedure, the examinee is requested to write a

    detailed description of all his/her activities during a critical period oftime in such a way that a reader without background information can

    determine what actually happened. The handwritten statement is then

    analyzed by a SCAN expert on the basis of a list of predetermined

    criteria. It is thought that some SCAN criteria are more likely to occur

    in truthful statements than in deceptive statements, whereas other

    criteria are more likely to occur in deceptive statements than in

    truthful statements (Sapir, 1987/2000).

    We know of four published studies that examined the validity of

    SCAN as a lie detection tool (Bachenko, Fitzpatrick, & Schonwet-

    ter, 2008; Driscoll, 1994; Porter & Yuille, 1996; Smith, 2001).

    Both Driscoll (1994) and Smith (2001) carried out field studies and

    analyzed statements of actual suspects. They reported high accu-

    racy rates, around 80%. However, Smith, who compared the

    performances of experienced SCAN users with raters not trained in

    SCAN, found no difference in accuracy between the two groups.

    Smith also noticed that different experts used different SCAN

    criteria to justify their decision of whether or not a statement was

    deceptive. In other words, there was a lack of consistency in the

    application of SCAN amongst SCAN users. Both studies, how-

    ever, were limited in that the ground truth, the actual veracity of

    the statements, could not be established. This makes interpreting

    the findings problematic. Porter and Yuille (1996) carried out a

    laboratory SCAN experiment in which participants did or did not

    commit a mock theft and were interviewed about this alleged

    crime. They examined three SCAN criteria: Structure of the state-

    ment; missing information; and first person singular, past tense.

    Truthful and deceptive statements did not differ from each other

    regarding these three criteria. However, this experiment was also

    limited. First, it tested only a small number (three) of SCAN

    criteria. Second, as participants gave oral statements that weresubsequently transcribed, the procedure did not follow one of the

    basic principles of SCAN, the request to write down a statement.

    In a fourth study, some SCAN criteria including verb tense were

    examined (Bachenko et al., 2008). However, the results for the

    individual criteria were not discussed.

    In sum, all four SCAN studies published to date were limited

    and, therefore, not much is known about the accuracy of the SCAN

    tool, despite the fact that it is used by many agencies. In the present

    experiment, we examined whether SCAN could discriminate be-

    tween true and false statements. We compared SCAN with Reality

    Monitoring (RM), another verbal veracity assessment tool. Al-

    This article was published Online First January 21, 2011.

    Galit Nahari, Department of Criminology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat

    Gan, Israel; Aldert Vrij, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK; Ron-

    ald P. Fisher, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Galit

    Nahari, Department of Criminology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan

    52900, Israel, E-mail: [email protected]

    Law and Human Behavior 2011 American Psychological Association2012, Vol. 36, No. 1, 68 76 0147-7307/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/h0093965

    68

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    2/9

    though not used by professionals in the field, RMs validity has

    been widely tested and it appears to discriminate truth tellers and

    liars well above chance level (Masip, Sporer, Garrido, & Herrero,

    2005; Vrij, 2008).

    The underlying assumption of SCAN is that true statements,

    derived from memory of an actual experienced event, differ in

    content and quality from statements that are based on invention orfantasy (Smith, 2001). This is similar to the assumption underlying

    RM. Unlike SCAN, RM provides a theoretical underpinning for

    why specific differences between truth telling and lying will occur.

    RM originally refers to the assessment of ones own memory and

    describes the processes people use to decide for themselves

    whether a given memory for an event was based on experience or

    imagination. According to the reality monitoring theory (Johnson,

    Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988; Johnson & Raye, 1981), memories

    of experienced events are externally derived and originate in

    perceptual experiences. They are characterized by spatial and

    temporal contextual attributes (e.g., where and when the event took

    place), sensory attributes (e.g., what the person felt, smelt, heard or

    saw when the event took place), and affective attributes (e.g.,

    expressions of emotions). In contrast, memories for imagined

    events are internally derived and originate in self-generated

    thought or imagination. They are characterized by cognitive oper-

    ations, such as thoughts and reasons, which probably helped to

    generate these memories. Empirical evidence shows that people

    also use these attributes to judge whether other peoples memories

    are externally or internally derived (Johnson, 2006; Johnson, Bush,

    & Mitchell, 1998; Johnson & Suengas, 1989). Based on these

    findings, it has been argued that RM can be applied to credibility

    judgments (Sporer, 1997, 2004; Vrij, 2008). A statement is con-

    sidered true when it is based on externally derived memories and

    false when it is based on internally derived memories (Sporer,

    1997, 2004).

    Although SCAN and RM share the same underlying assump-tion, that truthful statements differ in quality from deceptive state-

    ments, each tool takes entirely different sets of criteria into account

    to assess the veracity of statements. This deviation may reflect a

    difference in the theoretical underpinnings of the two tools. RM

    focuses on the clarity, realism, and reconstructability of the state-

    ments and examines perceptual, spatial, temporal and affective

    information, and cognitive operations (Sporer, 1997, 2004). The

    SCAN list of criteria is more extensive and includes a variety of

    cues such as denial of allegations, out of sequence information and

    change in language (see, Vrij, 2008). See Table 1 for definitions of

    the 13 SCAN and eight RM criteria.

    A careful examination of both lists of criteria gives the impression

    that SCAN and RM are oriented to different types of lie: conceal-

    ments (SCAN) and outright lies (RM), respectively. To demonstrate

    the distinction between concealment and outright lies (DePaulo,

    Kashy, Kirkendol, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996) consider the following

    hypothetical example. Dan, a teenager, wandered around the city with

    his friends on Friday night. During that night, among other activities,

    he committed an act of vandalism against a school building. The

    police have reasons to believe that Dan committed this act of vandal-

    ism and invites him to the police station. During the interview, Dan

    could use two alternative strategies. He could tell the truth about being

    with his friends, and even could describe truthfully all their activities

    during that night, but omitting the act of vandalism. Alternatively, he

    could deny that he went out on that Friday night, and could make up

    a story pretending that he did something else. If Dan chooses the first

    strategy he provides a concealment lie, and if he chooses the second

    strategy, he provides an outright lie.

    RMs primary concern is assessing the richness in detail, and the

    vividness of the statement. Outright lies are imaginations and

    should therefore be less detailed and vivid than truthful accounts.

    Concealed liars, in contrast, tell the truth about what happened butleave out details that may incriminate them. Therefore, conceal-

    ment lies include descriptions of activities, conversations, and

    events that the examinee actually has experienced. RM thus may

    have difficulty in discriminating between truths and concealed lies.

    We therefore predicted that RM should be able to discriminate

    between truths and outright lies, (as was previously evidenced;

    Masip et al., 2005; Vrij, 2008), but should not be able to discrim-

    inate between truths and concealed lies (Hypothesis 1).

    The SCAN literature does not mention an underlying theme for

    the list of criteria. Examining the criteria listed in Table 1 gives the

    impression to be a motivational approach in which liars attempt

    strategically to select the exact words that reflect their knowledge

    but hide their guilt. For example, at least four (out of 13) criteriaexplicitly deal with what the examinee did not say or concealed.

    Within the missing information criterion, SCAN experts look for

    words or phrases such as finally, later on, some time after,

    which can imply that some information is left out. If someone says

    I started to cook, and some time after I found my father lying on

    the dining room floor, one could wonder what happened after the

    examinee started to cook and before he found his father on the

    floor. Another SCAN criterion is objective and subjective time.

    Here, the proportions of the actual durations of the activities

    (objective time) are compared with the number of words that the

    examinee used to describe these activities (subjective time). When

    the subjective time that was allocated to an activity is shorter than

    the objective time, it may imply that the examinee is attempting toconceal information regarding that activity. Concealments can be

    further reflected by the absence of social introductions in the

    statement. This criterion relates to how the people who were

    involved in the activity are introduced. If an examinee writes we

    and it is not clear who we are, the examinee might be concealing

    something regarding the identity of the other person. Finally,

    extraneous information could indicate concealment. This crite-

    rion relates to whether the examinee reports information that does

    not seem relevant. If so, it could indicate that the examinee is

    trying to hide important information. One could thus expect SCAN

    to be able to distinguish truths from both outright and concealment

    lies (Hypothesis 2).

    In the experiment we also asked participants to reportwhether they had developed a strategy to appear convincing

    and, if so, to describe their strategy. Research into the strategies

    suspects use is emerging only recently, after DePaulo et al.s

    (2003) plea to carry out such research (Colwell, Hiscock-

    Anisman, Memon, Woods, & Michlik, 2006; Hartwig, Granhag,

    & Strmwall, 2007; Hartwig, Granhag, Strmwall, & Doering,

    2010; Strmwall, Hartwig, & Granhag, 2006; Vrij et al., 2009).

    Yet, as Vrij, Granhag and Porter (in press) argued, for success-

    ful lie detection it is important to take differences in strategies

    between liars and truth tellers into account and to develop

    interview protocols that exploit these differences.

    69

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    3/9

    Method

    Participants

    A total of 61 Bar-Ilan undergraduate students (54 females and 7

    males) participated in the experiment for course credits. Their

    mean age was 23.20 (SD 2.17 years).

    Procedure

    Participants arrived individually at the laboratory at a predeter-

    mined time. The experimenter told them that the experiment dealt

    with examining the efficiency of certain tools to detect lies. All

    participants signed a consent form indicating that participation wasvoluntary and that they could withdraw from the experiment at any

    time without penalty. Participants were then assigned randomly to

    one of three experimental conditions: (i) concealment liars (N

    20): mock criminals who concealed their criminal activity but truly

    reported their non-criminal activities, (ii) outright liars (N 20):

    mock criminals who concealed both their criminal and non-

    criminal activities and thus gave a completely false statement; and

    (iii) innocents (N 21): truth tellers who gave a completely

    truthful account of their activities.

    The experimenter read aloud the instructions. The instructions

    for guilty participants (concealment and outright liars) were as

    Table 1

    Definitions of the SCAN and RM Criteria

    Criterion Definition

    SCANSpontaneous corrections The presence of corrections in the statement, such as crossing out what has been written. Although explanations

    and additions are allowed, examinees are explicitly instructed not to cross anything out. A failure to follow thisinstruction could indicate deceit.First person singular past tense The format in which a statement is written. It is thought that truthful statements are written in the first person

    singular, past tense because the writer describes an event that has taken place (I saw the smoke coming out ofthe window). Deviations from this norm should raise suspicion.

    Location of emotions The position of the emotions in the statement. Just before the climax of the story (liars), or throughout the story(truth tellers), and after the climax of the story (truth tellers).

    Change in language Change of terminology or vocabulary in the statement. A change in language indicates that something has alteredin the mind of the writer. For example, if an examinee refers in his statement to all conversations he had asconversations except one conversation which he describes as a discussion, it is likely that he perceived thisconversation differently from the other conversations.

    Lack of conviction and memory The examinee is vague about certain elements in the statement (I believe . . ., I think . . ., kind of . . .) orreports that s/he cannot remember something. Lack of memory indicates deceit.

    Out of sequence Does the statement recount the events in chronological order. A deviation of the chronological order indicatesdeceit.

    Missing information The inclusion of words that indicate that some information has been left out, such as sometime after, finally,later on, and shortly thereafter. Inclusion of those words indicates deceit.

    Extraneous information Does the statement contain information that does not seem relevant. The presence may indicate deceit becauseexaminees could include extraneous information to hide more important information.

    Objective and subjective time Objective time refers to the actual duration of events described in the statement, whereas subjective time refers tothe amount of words spent to describe these events. It is thought that in a truthful statement the objective andsubjective time will correspond with each other, unlike in a deceptive statement. For example, if someonedevotes five lines in a statement to describe a 30-minute period, and then three lines to describe a subsequenttwo- hour period, the objective and subjective time do not correspond and this may indicate deceit.

    Social introductions How are the persons described in the statement introduced. They can be unambiguous (e.g., My wife Lisa . . .)or ambiguous (e.g., We went outside) without saying who we are. Ambiguous introductions indicate a lie.

    Pronouns The use of pronouns (I, my, he, his, they, their etc.) in the statement. Pronouns signal commitment,responsibility and possession. Omitting pronouns (Left the house rather than I left the house) suggestsreluctance of the writer to commit him/herself to the described action and thus indicates deceit.

    Structure of statement The balance of the statement. It is thought that in a truthful statement the first 20% is used to describe activitiesleading up to the event, the next 50% to describe the actual event, and the final 30% to discuss what happenedafter the event. Thus, a ten-line statement is thought to comprise two lines to introduce the event, five lines todescribe the event, and three lines about the aftermath. The more unbalanced a statement, the greater the

    probability that the statement is deceptive.Denial of allegation Does the examinee directly deny the allegation in the statement by stating I did not . . . . Denials indicate truth.RM

    Clarity The statement is clear and vivid.Realism The story is realistic and makes sense.Reconstructability of the story The event can be reconstructed with the information given.Emotions Are there emotions described in the statement.Perceptual information Information about what the examinee felt, smelt, heard or saw when the event took place.Spatial information Information about locations (It happened in a park) or the spatial arrangement of people and/or objects (The

    man was sitting to the left of his wife).Temporal information Information about when the event happened (It was early in the morning) or explicitly describes a sequence of

    events (When the visitor heard all that noise, he became nervous and left).Cognitive operations Thoughts and reasonings (e.g., I must have locked the door, because it was locked when I came home).

    70

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    4/9

    follows: You are requested to commit a mock theft as part of an

    experiment about deception. The mock theft is about stealing a

    statistics exam from a mail box in the Criminology department.

    First, please go to the Social Science library (First floor of this

    building), look for the Prison Journal volume No. 77 (1997). Get

    the article that appears on pp. 125134 and write down the article

    authors names. You will be asked about it later. After the libraryassignment, you should come back to the department, and steal the

    exam. The exam is in mail box No. 50 (Prof Aharonovitz mail

    box). The mailbox is locked, and you can find the key under the

    mail box of the secretariat. At the bottom of the secretariat mail-

    box, you will find the key, which is attached with cellophane tape.

    Take the key, open Prof. Aharonovitzs mailbox, find the exam

    and hide it on you. Make sure that you leave the mailbox in the

    same condition as you found it, so that Prof. Aharonovitz will not

    notice that someone broke into his mailbox. Finally, re-attach the

    key with the cellophane tape to the bottom of the secretariat

    mailbox. When you take the key from the bottom of the secretariat

    mailbox, please make sure that you keep the cellophane tape in

    good condition so that you will be able to use it again. Please makesure that nobody witnesses you committing this crime. Also, do

    not tell anybody that you took the exam.

    You should be back here 30 min from now. In the remaining

    time after you steal the exam, do your normal business. Make sure

    that you carry out more than one activity and do not stay at one

    place for more than 10 min. Please carry out as many activities as

    you can, such as buying a coffee, having a conversation with a

    friend, visiting the book shop, etc. Please do not do anything that

    you may later be unable or uncomfortable to talk about. Please

    return in exactly 30 min from now (the experimenter indicated the

    exact time) to the lab for the second stage of the study.

    The instructions for the innocent participants (truth tellers)

    were the same, excluding the theft of the exam. The assistant

    repeated the instructions to make sure that everything was clear,

    and asked the participants whether they had any questions.

    When the participants indicated that they had understood the

    instructions, they were sent on their mission.

    Upon returning to the lab after exactly 30 min, the experimenter

    told the participant: While you were out a statistics exam had

    been stolen from a mailbox in the Criminology department. You

    are one of the suspects in this theft. In a short while, you will be

    interviewed about it. The interviewer will ask you to write down a

    statement about what you were doing during the last 30 min. She

    does not know whether you are guilty. In case you stole the exam,

    do not admit this under any circumstances.

    At this point the experimenter suggested a different strategy

    to the participants in the guilty conditions (concealment liarsand outright liars). To the concealment liars she said: The best

    strategy will be to give a truthful statement about everything

    you did during the last 30 min, except stealing the exam.

    Therefore, report only your non-criminal activities and skip the

    act of stealing. To the outright liars she said: The best

    strategy will be to give a complete falsely statement about what

    you did during the last 30 min. Therefore, avoid reporting any

    of your activities, both criminal and non-criminal, and give a

    complete falsely report by making-up acts that you did not do

    in this period of time. No strategy was suggested to the

    innocent participants.

    The experimenter continued by saying to all participants:

    While giving your statement, act as convincing as possible. The

    interviewer will use your statement to determine whether you are

    guilty or innocent. If the interviewer believes that you are inno-

    cent, you will receive your course credit and be allowed to leave.

    If the interviewer believes that you are guilty, you will be asked to

    write a second statement about what actually occurred during thelast 30 min. You will be interviewed in 10 min, and the statement

    you will be asked to write down is your only chance to convince

    the interviewer that you are innocent. Please use these 10 min to

    prepare the statement that you are going to write. Finally, the

    experimenter reminded the participants that they should not con-

    fess to the crime, and she repeated the suggested strategies to the

    concealment and outright liars.

    The participants were then left alone in the lab, giving them the

    opportunity to prepare for the interview and plan a strategy. After

    10 min they were asked to complete a pre-interview questionnaire.

    The questionnaire included three main questions: (i) How would

    you describe your level of preparation for writing the statement?

    The participants rated their answer to this question on three 7-point

    scales: (1) Shallow to (7) Thorough; (1) Insufficient to (7) Suffi-

    cient; (1) Poor to (7) Good. The average score on these three scales

    was used as the Quality of Preparation variable ( .85); (ii)

    Have you developed a strategy to appear convincing? Partici-

    pants who answered this question negatively were asked to explain

    why not, and participants who answered it positively were asked to

    describe their strategy; (iii) To what extent are you motivated to

    appear convincing in your statement? Participants rated their

    answer to this question on a 7-point scale (1not at all to

    7completely).

    After completing the questionnaire, participants were brought to

    the interview room. The interviewer (blind to the participants

    condition) sat opposite the participant and gave an instruction that

    follows the standard instructions given by SCAN users in the field:An exam has been stolen from a mail box in the Criminology

    department within the last hour. You are one of the suspects in this

    theft. Please write down in as much detail as possible what you

    were doing during the 30 min that you were out. Start your

    statement from the time you left the lab and end it at the time you

    re-entered the lab. Be sure that you mention all details, all activ-

    ities, all people you have met, all conversations that took place,

    etc. Give as much information as you can, including information

    that seems irrelevant. I will only be asking this single question.

    You will have this one opportunity to give me as much information

    as you can. While writing your statement, you may explain or add

    details as much as you want, but do not cross out anything that you

    have already written.

    After writing their statements, participants were asked to com-

    plete a post-interview questionnaire. They were asked to rate on

    7-point scales (1not at all to 7completely): (i) To what extent

    they were motivated to appear convincing in their statement; (ii)

    To what extent they found it difficult to appear convincing in their

    statement; (iii) To what extent they thought they were convincing

    in their statement; and (iv) To what extent they were concerned

    about being asked to write an additional statement about what

    actually had occurred. In addition, the participants reported

    whether they (v) gave any truthful details in their statement; (vi)

    any false details in their statement, and (vii) concealed any details

    from their statement. Finally, they were asked to indicate what

    71

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    5/9

    percentage of their statement they thought was true. Before

    filling out the post-interview questionnaire, participants were

    told that the experiment had ended, that their answers would not

    influence the assessment of their statements as false or true, and

    that they were requested to answer the questions truthfully.

    Upon completing the questionnaire, all participants were de-

    briefed and given their course credits. None of the participantswas requested to write a second statement.

    Coding of the Statements with SCAN and RM

    Six independent coders assessed the presence of SCAN and RM

    criteria. The coders were not informed that these criteria were used

    to determine true from false statements. Instead, they were told

    that participants were requested to carry out some activities outside

    the lab for 30 min and to provide a detailed statement about these

    activities. The coders received a list of 13 SCAN and 8 RM criteria

    which were mixed together. The SCAN criteria were taken from

    Vrij (2008) and the RM criteria from Sporer (2004). In his list of

    SCAN criteria, Vrij (2008) included the criteria that are most

    emphasized in workshops on the technique (Driscoll, 1994) or areused in research (Porter & Yuille, 1996; Smith, 2001). The coders

    were requested to rate the presence of seven criteria on 10-point

    scales (1not at all to 10 very much) (e.g., to what extent is the

    statement realistic?). They were asked to mark the frequency of

    occurrence of 13 other criteria in the statement (e.g., the frequency

    of occurrence of mentioning emotions in the statement). The

    remaining criterion refers to where emotions appeared in the

    statement (a SCAN criterion). The coders indicated whether or not

    (0 no 1yes) emotions appeared (i) at the beginning of the

    statement; (ii) throughout the statement; or (iii) at the end of the

    statement. This SCAN criterion thus resulted in three dependent

    variables.

    The coders received a written list of the criteria with detailed

    definitions and examples. The trainer (one of the authors) went

    over each criterion, and explained the definitions and examples.

    She answered all questions about the criterion and made sure that

    all coders understood the criterion before she moved on to the next

    one. This training took 90 min. The coders carried out their

    assessments individually at their homes. They took with them thewritten explanations and were asked to read them again before and

    during the coding. Each coder assessed 20 21 statements, and

    each statement was assessed by two coders.

    Inter-Rater Reliability

    Variance was low for several criteria. For example, emotion was

    rated as absent in 84% of the statements. Consequently, Pearson

    correlations were not appropriate for assessing the inter-rater reli-

    ability. We think this is not problematic as the important issue is

    whether or not the coders agreed that the criterion occurred rather

    than whether or not they agreed on its frequency of occurrence.

    Thus, inter-rater reliability was calculated by determining for each

    criterion the percentage of agreement about its presence in thestatement. We dichotomized the original data set: present (1) or not

    present (0). Criteria that were originally measured on 10-point

    scales were recorded as present when the original rating fell in the

    6-to-10 range, and as not present when the original rating fell in the

    1-to-5 range. Criteria that were originally measured by counting

    the number of appearances in the statement were recorded as

    present when they appeared at least once. Table 2 presents the

    agreement percent between coders. High agreement was achieved

    for the RM criteria (ranging between 71% and 98%), except

    cognitive operations which achieved only 44% of agreement.

    High agreement was achieved also for SCAN criteria (ranging

    from 70% to 98%), although low agreement occurred for four

    Table 2

    Inter-Rater Agreement for the SCAN and RM Criteria

    Criterion Method Cue for Coding method % of agreement

    Clarity RM Truth Rating 87Realism RM Truth Rating 98Reconstructability of the story RM Truth Rating 93Emotions RM Truth Counting 84Perceptual information RM Truth Counting 74Spatial information RM Truth Counting 80Temporal information RM Truth Counting 71Cognitive operations RM Lie Counting 44Spontaneous corrections SCAN Lie Counting 66First person singular past tense SCAN Truth Rating 89Location of emotions SCAN

    Beginning Lie 97Throughout Truth 87End Truth 90

    Chang in language SCAN Lie Counting 72Lack of conviction and memory SCAN Lie Counting 74Out of sequence SCAN Lie Counting 98Missing information SCAN Lie Counting 61Extraneous information SCAN Lie Counting 84Objective and subjective time SCAN Truth Rating 70Social introductions SCAN Lie Counting 56Pronouns SCAN Lie Counting 64Structure of statement SCAN Truth Rating 70Denial of allegation SCAN Truth Counting 80

    72

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    6/9

    criteria: Spontaneous corrections (66%), missing information

    (61%), pronouns (64%), and social introduction (56%).

    Since agreement between the coders was not perfect, all dis-

    agreements between the two coders were relegated to a third coder

    who made the final decision regarding the presence of the crite-

    rion. The third coder was blind to the veracity status of the

    statements.We calculated total scores for RM and SCAN, by using the dichot-

    omized variables. A score of 0 was assigned when the criterion was

    absent, and a score of 1 was assigned when the criterion was

    present. Criteria that indicate lies (e.g., cognitive operations, sponta-

    neous corrections, missing information) were re-coded; thus a score of

    1 was assigned when the criterion was absent, and a score of 0

    was assigned when the criterion was present. The total score for each

    method (RM and SCAN) was the sum of all criteria that were

    included in the method. Therefore, the total score for RM could range

    from 0 to 8, and the total score for SCAN from 0 to 15 (As reported

    earlier, location of emotions was divided into three sub-variables, thus

    the SCAN total score could attain 15 points). According to the SCAN

    and RM assumptions, the higher the score, the more likely it is that the

    statement is truthful.1

    Results

    Manipulation Check

    Significant differences emerged between experimental condi-

    tions regarding the percentages of truthful details participants said

    they reported in the statements, F(2, 58) 99.37, MSE 387.27,

    2

    .77, p .001. Scheffe tests revealed that innocents reported

    more truthful details (M 99.52% of the statement, SD 1.50,

    95% CI [98.84, 100.21]) than concealment liars (M 85.90% of

    the statement, SD 18.56, 95% CI [77.21, 94.95]), who reported

    more truthful details than outright liars (M 18.15% of the

    statement, SD 28.90, 95% CI [4.62, 31.68]). This indicates that

    the participants followed the experimenters instructions.

    All innocent participants mentioned that they provided truthful

    details and none of them mentioned that they provided false details

    in their statements. Further, 90.5% (N 19) of the innocents said

    that they did not conceal any information. All outright liars men-

    tioned that they provided false details and concealed information.

    Sixty percent (N 12) of the outright liars said that they did not

    provide any true details, and most outright liars who said that they

    provided true details did this only occasionally (mentioning that

    between 1% and 10% of their statement was true). All concealment

    liars said that they provided true details. Thirty five percent (N

    7) of them also mentioned that they provided false details.

    Participants Motivation

    The motivation of the participants to be convincing was mea-

    sured both before and after providing the statement. The reported

    motivation was high both prior to interviewing (M 6.20, SD

    .99) and after interviewing (M 6.26, SD 1.05). These two

    motivation scores were highly correlated with each other (r .92,

    p .001). Further, twenty eight percent (N 17) of the partici-

    pants were concerned that they might be asked to write a second

    statement (ratings of 6 or 7). There was no difference between the

    three experimental conditions regarding motivation or concerns

    about writing a second statement, all Fs (2, 58) 2.44.

    Pre-Interviewing Preparation

    The self-reported quality of preparation for the interview was

    M 4.69 (SD 1.49). There were no differences among the three

    conditions, F 1. Most of the participants planned a strategy(63.9%, N 39; 13 innocents, 12 concealment liars and 14

    outright liars).

    Innocents reported that they planned to provide details that are

    easy to verify and to mention a potential witness for their alibi

    (N 9), to be as detailed as they could (N 7), or to convince the

    interviewer that they have no criminal record and that they are

    honest people (N 2). Concealment liars planned to be very

    detailed in reporting their non-criminal activities (N 8), and tried

    to inflate their non-criminal activities to compensate for leaving

    out their criminal activities (N 4). Outright liars thought that

    they should mention only activities that are difficult to verify (N

    5), thought of evidence they could provide for activities they did

    not do (N 3), planned to report activities they carried out anotherday (N 2), thought about what they reasonably could have done

    in 30 min (N 2), and tried to convince themselves about their

    own lies, so that they would appear like innocents (N 2).

    Participants who did not develop a strategy said they did not

    need a strategy because they were innocent (N 8); because they

    believe it would be easy to convince the interviewer since s/he can

    (in case of innocents) or cannot (in case of liars) verify their

    statement (N 4); or because planning may interrupt them from

    being natural and from altering their statements if necessary (N

    4).

    Number of Words

    The length of the statements differed between the three exper-imental conditions, F(2, 58) 9.87, MSE 4731.23, 2 .25,

    p .001). Scheffe tests revealed that statements of innocents were

    significantly longer (M 218.43 words, SD 91.35, 95% CI

    [176.84, 260.01]) than statements of concealment liars (M

    144.20 words, SD 52.77, 95% CI [119.50, 168.90]) and outright

    liars (M 129.80 words, SD 53.60, 95% CI [104.71, 154.89]).

    The length of statements of concealment and outright liars did not

    differ significantly from each other.

    Discriminating Between Groups by RM and SCAN

    RM total score. In order to test the validity of RM criteria in

    detecting outright and concealment lies, we first conducted an

    1 After completing their coding, the coders were asked to rate on

    10-point scales how difficult or easy they found it to assess each of the

    criteria (1 very difficult to assess and 10 very easy to assess). The

    coders found most of the criteria relatively easy to apply (about 57% (N

    12) of the criteria were rated as 9 or 10). The criteria they found the

    most difficult to assess were the SCAN criteria: social introductions,

    pronouns, and structure of the statement (Ms are 6.83, 7.50 and 7.83,

    respectively). The correlation between the difficulty in applying the crite-

    rion (Table 3) and the inter-rater percentage agreement regarding that

    criterion (Table 2) was (Spearman) r .50, p .05). That is, the more the

    coders perceived the criteria as easy to apply, the greater was the agreement

    between coders.

    73

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    7/9

    ANOVA with RM total score as the dependent variable, and group

    (innocents, outright or concealment liars) as a factor. This analysis

    revealed a large and statistically significant Group effect, F(2,

    58) 4.33, MSE .51, 2 .13, p .05. Scheffe tests revealed

    that, as predicted in Hypothesis 1, innocents (M 6.71, SD .56,

    95% CI [6.46, 6.97]) obtained higher RM total scores than outright

    liars (M 6.15, SD .75, 95% CI [5.80, 6.50]), representing alarge effect size, d .92. However, contrary to our prediction,

    innocents also obtained higher RM total scores than concealment

    liars (M 6.15, SD .81, d .82, 95% CI [5.77, 6.53]).

    We further tested the validity of RM criteria in discriminating

    between groups. We ran three different discriminant analyses to

    distinguish between (i) outright liars and innocents; (ii) conceal-

    ment liars and innocents; and (iii) outright and concealment liars.

    The objective group-belonging was the classifying variable and the

    RM total score was the predictor. As Table 3 shows, a significant

    discriminant function was found for distinguishing between out-

    right liars and innocents, 2(1) 6.82, Wilks Lambda .84, p

    .01. The function correctly identified 75.0% of the outright liars

    and 66.7% of the innocents, resulting in the total accuracy of

    70.7%. Discriminating concealment liars from innocents was also

    significant, 2(1) 6.15, Wilks Lambda .85, p .05, but

    achieved a lower total accuracy rate (63.4%). Rate of correctly

    classifying innocents was identical as in the previous function

    (66.7%), but the classification of the concealment liars was lower,

    60.0%. Determining outright liars from concealment liars was not

    possible, 2(1) 0.00, ns.

    SCAN total score. Similar to RM, we conducted an ANOVA

    with Group (innocents, outright or concealment liars) as a factor

    and SCAN total score as a dependent variable to test the validity

    of SCAN total score in detecting outright and concealment lies.

    The effect was not significant, F 1, and the SCAN total scores

    of all three groups were highly similar (innocents: M 9.57, SD

    1.29, 95% CI [8.99, 10.16]; outright liars: M 9.55, SD 1.36,95% CI [8.92, 10.18]; concealment liars: M 9.10, SD 1.65,

    95% CI [8.55, 9.65]; all ds for differences between means ranged

    from .01 to .32). Hypothesis 2 is therefore rejected. We further ran

    three discriminant analyses for SCAN, similar to what we had

    done with RM. None of the three discriminant functions were

    significant (2 ranged from .03 to 1.46).

    Scan users do not use total SCAN scores. Instead, they examine

    the individual criteria. We carried out two MANOVAs to accom-

    modate this procedure. In the first MANOVA we compared truth

    tellers with outright liars with the 15 SCAN criteria as dependent

    variables. At a multivariate level the analysis was not significant

    (F 1). At a univariate level only one of the fifteen variables

    showed a significant effect (F[1, 39] 4.99, p .05). In the

    second MANOVA we compared truth tellers with concealment

    liars, and again, the 15 SCAN criteria were the dependent vari-

    ables. The multivariate analysis was not significant (F 1),neither were any of the univariate effects (all Fs 1.72, all ps

    .20).

    RM SCAN. The correlation between RM total score and-

    SCAN total score (r .08) was not significant, supporting our

    observation that they measure different aspects in written state-

    ments. Although SCAN by itself could not discriminate between

    the three experimental groups, we tested whether it contributed to

    the accuracy rate when added to RM. For this purpose, we ran

    three more discriminant analyses, where the objective group-

    belonging was the classifying variable and RM and SCAN total

    scores were the two predictors. We used thestepwisemethod. In

    discriminatinginnocents fromoutright liars or concealment liars

    only RM total score emerged in the function.In other words, the

    discriminant functions obtained in these analyses were identical to

    the discriminant functions described in the RM section, and SCAN

    did not contribute anything beyond the RM score.

    Discussion

    In the present experiment, we failed to find support for SCAN,

    as the total SCAN score did not differ between truth tellers and

    outright liars or between truth tellers and concealment liars. How-

    ever, SCAN users do not examine total SCAN scores; they exam-

    ine the individual SCAN criteria instead. Examining individual

    criteria is problematic as it easily leads to a lack of standardization

    with different SCAN users examining and relying upon different

    criteria. This is exactly what Smith (2001) found in her field study.Even when we examined the individual criteria, however, SCAN

    did not receive empirical support. Only one SCAN criterion dis-

    tinguished between truth tellers and outright liars and none of the

    criteria distinguished between truth tellers and concealment liars.

    Only one out of thirty univariate tests (3.33%) yielded a significant

    result, a percentage that could be expected by chance alone.

    SCAN users may argue that our experimental design was inad-

    equate for SCAN differences to occur between truth tellers and

    liars. This would mean that SCAN cannot be used in all situations.

    Table 3

    Accuracy Rates From the SCAN and RM Tools in Different Types of Lies

    Innocents versus outright Innocents versus concealment Outright versus concealment

    Innocents (%) Outright (%) Total (%) Innocents (%) Concealment (%) Total (%) Outright (%) Concealment (%) Total (%)

    RMHit rate 66.7 75.0 70.7 66.7 60.0 63.4

    SCANHit rate 52.4 65.0 58.5 52.4 45.0 48.8 65.0 55.0 60.0

    RM and SCANHit rate 66.7 75.0 70.7 66.7 60.0 63.4

    Note. Discriminant analysis for RM and RM & SCAN among outright versus concealment could not be obtained (2(1) 0.00). Accuracy rates forsignificant discriminant functions appear in bold.

    74

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    8/9

    SCAN users should then make clear under which circumstances

    SCAN can be used and under which circumstances it cannot be

    used. At present, SCAN users do not discuss this issue. Alterna-

    tively, it could be that we did not conduct the SCAN procedure

    properly. Such a criticism is unlikely, as the SCAN procedure is

    very straightforward. It just invites the examinee to write down

    his/her activities during a critical period of time in enough detail sothat a reader without background information about the examin-

    ees activities could fully grasp the statement (Adams, 1996; Sapir,

    1987/2000). Finally, someone could argue that the null findings

    were the result of a lack of statistical power. However, one should

    note that the largest effect size in the SCAN analyses (between

    innocents and concealment liars) was still small, d .32. Admit-

    tedly, drawing conclusions from null effect is problematic and

    from a statistical standpoint we cannot conclude that SCAN is not

    effective. However, we are still left with the conclusion that SCAN

    has no empirical support to date.

    The poor performance of SCAN is further highlighted by the

    results for RM, the alternative veracity tool we examined. RM did

    discriminate between truth tellers and outright liars as well as

    between truth tellers and concealment liars. The findings compar-

    ing truth tellers and outright liars support the growing body of

    literature showing that RM can be used for lie detection purposes

    (Masip et al., 2005; Vrij, 2008). RM being able to discriminate

    between truth tellers and concealment liars is worth discussing, as

    it was the first time that such a comparison was made in the RM

    deception literature. We were skeptical that RM would effectively

    discriminate between truth tellers and concealment liars. In their

    statements, concealment liars do not include much information that

    is deceptive, as their lie mainly consists of hiding vital informa-

    tion. We argued that RM would have difficulty in spotting con-

    cealments as the RM method examines what the examinee has

    written (mostly truthful) rather than what the examinee has con-

    cealed (a lie). The findings concerning the length of the statement(e.g., number of words) and the strategies the examinees said they

    would use in their concealment lies enables us to explain this

    finding. In order to be successful in concealing information, the

    examinee needs to compensate the hidden information by elabo-

    rating more thoroughly when describing the information they

    present. The concealment liars did not do this. The statements of

    concealment liars were significantly shorter than the statements of

    truth tellers. In addition, only 4 out of 20 concealment liars

    reported as a strategy that they would try to inflate their non-

    criminal activities to compensate for leaving out their criminal

    activities. As a result, the truthful information that concealment

    liars reported was less detailed than the information truth tellers

    reported, and this enabled RM to discriminate between the two

    types of statement.

    SCAN skeptics may argue that we were perhaps too optimistic

    in expecting differences between truth tellers and liars, given the

    lack of an underlying theoretical rationale for SCAN. Linguists, in

    particular, have criticized SCAN for that reason. For example,

    Shuy (1993) is concerned that SCAN does not take into account

    socio-cultural variation in language use, and Heydon (2008) be-

    lieves that SCAN strengthens the mythology of interviewing by

    promoting certain views and beliefs as commonsensical. SCAN

    skeptics may even go one step further: Why bother examining a

    tool that has no theoretical foundation? We disagree with such

    skeptics. First, although SCAN users do not give a theoretical

    foundation for their tool, they may do so in the future. In that

    respect, SCAN could be similar to Criteria-Based Content Analy-

    sis (CBCA), an alternative verbal lie detection tool. CBCA was

    presented (in English) in 1988 by Khnken and Steller, but the first

    publication in English about its theoretical underpinning appeared

    eight years later (Khnken, 1996). Second, we think that it is

    important to examine SCAN since it seems to be used in practiceon a wide scale.

    The self-reported strategies of truth tellers and liars revealed

    more noteworthy findings. First, 8 out of 21 truth tellers did not

    develop a strategy because, as they reported, they were innocent.

    This supports the view that truth tellers are less likely than liars to

    take their credibility for granted (DePaulo et al., 2003; Kassin,

    Appleby, & Torkildson-Perillo, 2010; Vrij et al., in press). Truth

    tellers typically assume that their innocence shines through (Gra-

    nhag, Strmwall & Hartwig, 2007; Kassin et al., 2010; Vrij, Mann,

    & Fisher, 2006), which could be explained with the illusion of

    transparency (Gilovich, Savitsky, & Medvec, 1998), the belief that

    ones inner feelings will manifest themselves on the outside, and

    belief in a just world(Lerner, 1980), the belief that people will get

    what they deserve, and deserve what they get. Second, the most

    frequently cited strategy for truth tellers (9 out of 21) was to

    provide details that can be checked, whereas the most frequently

    cited strategy for outright liars (5 out of 20) was to provide details

    that cannot be checked. Truth tellers strategy makes sense: The

    best way for them to convince the interviewer that they are telling

    the truth is to provide evidence that can be corroborated. Liars, of

    course, cannot do this and need to avoid providing evidence that

    can be corroborated. This differential strategy of truth tellers and

    liars may result in a verbal cue of deceit that has not been

    examined in the verbal lie detection literature so far: the use of

    corroborated cues. That is, to what extent do examinees include

    information in their statements that, in theory, can be checked?

    The hypothesis is that truth tellers include more corroborated cuesin their statements than liars. This hypothesis is worth examining.

    In sum, no support for the use of SCAN was found in the

    experiment, whereas an alternative verbal lie detection tool, RM,

    was successful in discriminating between truth tellers and liars.

    Those findings do not justify the use of SCAN for lie detection

    purposes in real life practice. The results further revealed a narra-

    tive strategy that is worth examining: The use of corroborated

    cues.

    References

    Adams, S. H. (1996). Statement analysis: What do suspects words really

    reveal? FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin, October, 1220. Retrieved from

    http://www.fbi.gov/stats-services/publications/law-enforcement-

    bulletin/leb

    Bachenko, J., Fitzpatrick, E., & Schonwetter. M. (2008). Verification and

    Implementation of Language-Based Deception Indicators in Civil and

    Criminal Narratives. Proceedings of the 22nd international conference

    on computational linguistics (Coling 2008), pp. 4148.

    Bockstaele, M. (2008). Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN). Een nutting

    instrument by verhoren? In L. Smets & A. Vrij (Eds.), Het analyseren

    van de geloofwaardigheid van verhoren (pp. 105156). Brussels: Po-

    liteia.

    Colwell, K., Hiscock-Anisman, C., Memon, A., Woods, D., & Michlik,

    P. M. (2006). Strategies of impression management among deceivers

    and truth tellers: How liars attempt to convince. American Journal of

    Forensic Psychology, 24, 3138.

    75

  • 7/29/2019 Nahari12Scan

    9/9

    DePaulo, B. M., Kashy, D. A., Kirkendol, S. E., Wyer, M. M., & Epstein,

    J. A. (1996). Lying in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 70, 979995. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/

    journals/psp/

    DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. L., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton,

    K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin,

    129, 74118. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74

    Driscoll, L. N. (1994). Validity assessment of written statements fromsuspects in criminal investigations using the SCAN technique. Police

    Studies, 17, 7788.

    Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K., & Medvec, V. H. (1998). The illusion of

    transparency: Biased assessments of others ability to read ones emo-

    tional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 332

    346. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/psp/

    Granhag, P. A., Strmwall, L. A., & Hartwig, M. (2007). The SUE

    technique: The way to interview to detect deception. Forensic

    Update, 88, January, 2529. Retrieved from http://www.bps.org.uk/dfp/

    psychologists/forensic-update.cfm

    Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., & Strmwall, L. (2007). Guilty and innocent

    suspects strategies during interrogations. Psychology, Crime, & Law,

    13, 213227. doi:10.1080/10683160600750264

    Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., Strmwall, L, & Doering, N. (2010). Im-

    pression and information management: On the strategic self-regulation

    of innocent and guilty suspects. The Open Criminology Journal, 3,

    1016 (special issue on deception research). doi:10.2174/

    1874917801003010010

    Heydon, G. (2008). The art of deception: Myths about lie detection in

    written statements. In L. Smets & A. Vrij (Eds.), Het analyseren van de

    geloofwaardigheid van verhoren (pp. 171182). Brussels: Politeia.

    Johnson, M. K. (2006). Memory and reality. American Psychologist, 61,

    760771. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.760

    Johnson, M. K., Bush, J. G., & Mitchell, K. J. (1998). Interpersonal reality

    monitoring: Judging the sources of other peoples memories. Social

    Cognition, 16, 199224. Retrieved from http://wwwguilford.com/

    cgi-bin/cartscript.cgi?pagepr/jnco.htm&dirperiodicals/per_psych

    &cart_id

    Johnson, M. K., Foley, M. A., Suengas, A. G., & Raye, C. L. (1988).

    Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined

    autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,

    117, 371376. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xge/

    Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological

    Review, 88, 6785. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/

    rev/index.aspx

    Johnson, M. K., & Suengas, A. G. (1989). Reality monitoring judgments of

    other peoples memories. Bulletin of the Psycho-nomic Society, 27,

    107110.

    Kassin, S. M., Appleby, S. C., & Torkildson-Perillo, J. (2010). Interview-

    ing suspects: Practice, science, and future directions. Legal and Crimi-

    nological Psychology, 15, 3956. doi:10.1348/135532509X449361

    Khnken, G. (1996). Social psychology and the law. In G. R. Semin & K.

    Fiedler (Eds.), Applied social psychology (pp. 257282). London, Great

    Britain: Sage Publications.

    Khnken, G., & Steller, M. (1988). The evaluation of the credibility of

    child witness statements in German procedural system. In G. Davies &

    J. Drinkwater (Eds.), The child witness: Do the courts abuse children?

    (Issues in Criminological and Legal Psychology, no. 13) (pp. 3745).

    Leicester, United Kingdom: British Psychological Society.

    Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion.

    New York: Plenum.

    Masip, J., Sporer, S. L., Garrido, E., & Herrero, C. (2005). The detection

    of deception with the reality monitoring approach: A review of the

    empirical evidence. Psychology, Crime & Law, 11, 99122. doi:

    10.1080/10683160410001726356

    Porter, S., & Yuille, J. C. (1996). The language ofdeceit: An investigation

    of the verbal clues to deception in the interrogation context. Law and

    Human Behavior, 20, 443459. doi:10.1007/BF01498980

    Sapir, A. (1987/2000). The LSI course on scientific content analysis

    (SCAN) Phoenix, AZ: Laboratory for scientific interrogation.

    Shuy, R. (1993). Language crimes: The use and abuse of language evi-

    dence in the courtroom. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Smith, N. (2001). Reading between the lines: An evaluation of the scientificcontent analysis technique (SCAN). London: UK Home Office, Re-

    search, Development and Statistics Directorate.

    Sporer, S. L. (1997). The less travelled road to truth: Verbal cues in

    deception detection in accounts of fabricated and self-experienced

    events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 373397. doi:10.1002/

    (SICI)1099-0720(199710)11:5373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO;2-0

    Sporer, S. L. (2004). Reality monitoring and detection of deception. In

    P. A. Granhag & L. A. Stromwall (Eds.), The detection of deception in

    forensic contexts (pp. 64102). UK: Cambridge university press.

    A., Hartwig, M., & Granhag, P. A. (2006). To act truthfully: Nonverbal

    behaviour and strategies during a police interrogation. Psychology,

    Crime, & Law, 12, 207219. doi:10.1080/10683160512331331328

    Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities.

    Chichester, England: Wiley.

    Vrij, A., Granhag, P. A., & Porter, S. B. (in press). Pitfalls and opportu-

    nities in nonverbal and verbal lie detection. Psychological Science in the

    Public Interest. Retrieved from http://www.wiley.com/bw/journal

    .asp?ref1529-1006

    Vrij, A., Leal, S., Granhag, P. A., Mann, S., Fisher, R. P., Hillman, J., et

    al. (2009). Outsmarting the liars: The benefit of asking unanticipated

    questions. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 159166. doi:10.1007/

    s10979-008-9143-y

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006). Information-gathering vs accusa-

    tory interview style: Individual differences in respondents experiences.

    Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 589599. doi:10.1016/

    j.paid.2006.02.014

    76

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160600750264http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1874917801003010010http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1874917801003010010http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.760http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/135532509X449361http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160410001726356http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160410001726356http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01498980http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160512331331328http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9143-yhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9143-yhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.014http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9143-yhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9143-yhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160512331331328http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/%28SICI%291099-0720%28199710%2911:5%3C373:AID-CP461[3.0.CO%3B2-0http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01498980http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160410001726356http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160410001726356http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/135532509X449361http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.760http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1874917801003010010http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/1874917801003010010http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160600750264http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74