12
Original Article doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00613.x 28 © 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKCCHChild: Care, Health and Development0305-1862© 2006 The Authors; Journal compilation © 2006 Blackwell Pub- lishing Ltd? 20063312839Original ArticleMeasures of children’s participationG.A. King et al. Correspondence: Dr Gillian King, Research Program Director, Thames Valley Children’s Centre, 779 Base Line Road East, London, Ontario N6C 5Y6, Canada E-mail: [email protected] Original Article Measuring children’s participation in recreation and leisure activities: construct validation of the CAPE and PAC G. A. King,* M. Law,† S. King,† P. Hurley,† S. Hanna,† M. Kertoy‡ and P. Rosenbaum† *Thames Valley Children’s Centre, London, ON, CanChild Centre for Childhood Disability Research, Hamilton, ON, and ‡University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada Accepted for publication 30 November 2005 Abstract There is a need for psychometrically sound measures of children’s participation in recreation and leisure activities, for both clinical and research purposes. This paper provides information about the construct validity of the Children’s Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment (CAPE) and its companion measure, Preferences for Activities of Children (PAC). These measures are appropriate for children and youth with and without disabilities between the ages of 6 and 21 years. They provide information about six dimensions of participation (i.e. diversity, intensity, where, with whom, enjoyment and preference) and two categories of recreation and leisure activities: (i) formal and informal activities; and (ii) five types of activities (recreational, active physical, social, skill-based and self-improvement). This paper presents information about the performance of the CAPE and PAC activity type scores using data from a study involving 427 children with physical disabilities between the ages of 6 and 15 years. Intensity, enjoyment and preference scores were significantly correlated with environmental, family and child variables, in expected ways. Predictions also were supported with respect to differences in mean scores for boys vs. girls, and children in various age groups. The information substantiates the construct validity of the measures. The clinical and research utility of the measures are discussed. Keywords children, disability, measure, participation Introduction The concept of participation is becoming increasingly impor- tant in the field of childhood disability. It is a concept that has face validity – it appears to capture ‘what matters most’ for children with disabilities (Forsyth & Jarvis 2002), as well as their families. The concept of participation figures prominently in the World Health Organization’s (2001) International Classifi- cation of Functioning, Disability and Health, which defines participation as ‘involvement in life situations’, and views par- ticipation as resulting from the interaction of individuals with their social and physical environments. Increasingly, participa- tion is considered to be a key outcome of rehabilitation pro- grammes and services directed at assisting children and families to adapt to the challenges they face (e.g. King et al. 2002). With respect to service delivery, the concept of participation directs attention to issues of person-environment fit (Law & Dunn 1993; Christiansen & Baum 1997) and to the use of experiential and ecological interventions, which provide children and youth with real-world experiences and address barriers in the social and physical environment (King et al. 2005). Relatively little is known about the participation of children with physical disabilities. In comparison with children without disabilities, children with disabilities tend to engage in less varied leisure activities and in more quiet recreation activities and fewer social activities, especially social activities of a spontaneous

Measuring children's participation in recreation and leisure activities: construct validation of the CAPE and PAC

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Original Article

doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00613.x

28

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKCCHChild: Care, Health and Development0305-1862© 2006 The Authors; Journal compilation © 2006 Blackwell Pub-lishing Ltd

? 2006

33

12839

Original Article

Measures of children’s participationG.A. King

et al.

Correspondence: Dr Gillian King, Research Program Director, Thames Valley Children’s Centre, 779 Base Line Road East, London, Ontario N6C 5Y6, CanadaE-mail: [email protected]

Original Article

Measuring children’s participation in recreation and leisure activities: construct validation of the CAPE and PAC

G. A. King,* M. Law,† S. King,† P. Hurley,† S. Hanna,† M. Kertoy‡ and P. Rosenbaum†

*Thames Valley Children’s Centre, London, ON, †

CanChild

Centre for Childhood Disability Research, Hamilton, ON, and ‡University of Western Ontario, London, ON, Canada

Accepted for publication 30 November 2005

Abstract

There is a need for psychometrically sound measures of children’s participation in recreation and

leisure activities, for both clinical and research purposes. This paper provides information about the

construct validity of the Children’s Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment (CAPE) and its

companion measure, Preferences for Activities of Children (PAC). These measures are appropriate for

children and youth with and without disabilities between the ages of 6 and 21 years. They provide

information about six dimensions of participation (i.e. diversity, intensity, where, with whom,

enjoyment and preference) and two categories of recreation and leisure activities: (i) formal and

informal activities; and (ii) five types of activities (recreational, active physical, social, skill-based and

self-improvement). This paper presents information about the performance of the CAPE and PAC

activity type scores using data from a study involving 427 children with physical disabilities between

the ages of 6 and 15 years. Intensity, enjoyment and preference scores were significantly correlated

with environmental, family and child variables, in expected ways. Predictions also were supported

with respect to differences in mean scores for boys vs. girls, and children in various age groups. The

information substantiates the construct validity of the measures. The clinical and research utility of

the measures are discussed.

Keywords

children, disability, measure, participation

Introduction

The concept of participation is becoming increasingly impor-

tant in the field of childhood disability. It is a concept that has

face validity – it appears to capture ‘what matters most’ for

children with disabilities (Forsyth & Jarvis 2002), as well as their

families. The concept of participation figures prominently in

the World Health Organization’s (2001) International Classifi-

cation of Functioning, Disability and Health, which defines

participation as ‘involvement in life situations’, and views par-

ticipation as resulting from the interaction of individuals with

their social and physical environments. Increasingly, participa-

tion is considered to be a key outcome of rehabilitation pro-

grammes and services directed at assisting children and families

to adapt to the challenges they face (e.g. King

et al

. 2002). With

respect to service delivery, the concept of participation directs

attention to issues of person-environment fit (Law & Dunn

1993; Christiansen & Baum 1997) and to the use of experiential

and ecological interventions, which provide children and youth

with real-world experiences and address barriers in the social

and physical environment (King

et al

. 2005).

Relatively little is known about the participation of children

with physical disabilities. In comparison with children without

disabilities, children with disabilities tend to engage in less varied

leisure activities and in more quiet recreation activities and fewer

social activities, especially social activities of a spontaneous

Measures of children’s participation

29

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

nature (Margalit 1981; Brown & Gordon 1987; Sillanpää 1987).

We also know that the diversity of the participation of children

with physical disabilities declines as they move into adolescence

and early adulthood (Brown & Gordon 1987; Stevenson

et al

.

1997). We do not know much about the details of their leisure

and recreational participation – the types of activities they tend

to engage in and to prefer, who they do them with, how much

they enjoy their participation and the extent to which their

participation takes place at home or is community-based. We

also know little about the relative importance of various factors

that may influence the participation of children with disabilities,

including environmental factors such as supportive environ-

ments, family factors such as family preferences for particular

types of activities and child factors such as physical functioning.

Part of the reason for our limited knowledge is that we lack

adequate ways of measuring important aspects of children’s

participation. The development of an adequate measure of par-

ticipation is seen as a challenging but needed task (Forsyth &

Jarvis 2002).

There are relatively few measures of children’s participation

– that is, their involvement in life situations such as personal

maintenance, mobility, social relationships, home life and edu-

cation (World Health Organization 2001). Existing measures

vary in scope, with some focusing on children’s physical activ-

ities (e.g. Hay 1992), others on play (e.g. Henry 2000), and some

including school-based activities (e.g. Diller

et al

. 1981; Hay

1992; Posner & Vandell 1999). Some measures are self-admin-

istered (e.g. Garton & Pratt 1991; Hay 1992; Noreau

et al

. 2003),

some completed by parents (e.g. Diller

et al

. 1981; Achenbach

1991), and others are interviewer-assisted (Henry 2000) or are

based on interviews utilizing children’s accounts of their activ-

ities (Posner & Vandell 1999). These measures typically docu-

ment the number of activities a child performs or the child’s

frequency of engagement in activities [e.g. Diller and colleagues’

(1983) Activity Pattern Indicators]. Some of the measures also

examine children’s enjoyment (e.g. Henry 2000) and/or their

preferences or interest in activities (e.g. Garton & Pratt 1991).

With the exception of Henry’s (2000) Kid Play Profile and

Preteen Play Profile, which capture ‘with whom’ activities take

place, instruments typically do not document whether children

perform activities alone or with others. Whether participation

is solitary vs. done with others is an important aspect of partic-

ipation – one that is thought to be as important as the types of

activities in which children participate (Whiting & Edwards

1988). Capturing the extent to which activities are performed

alone, with family members, or with others in the community

can provide important assessment and outcome information

for the design and implementation of interventions to increase

children’s social participation. Furthermore, existing measures

do not document ‘where’ participation takes place; the degree

to which participation is home- vs. community-based provides

important information about an aspect of participation that

might be the target of change through intervention.

The Children’s Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment (CAPE) and Preferences for Activities of Children (PAC)

This paper provides construct validity information for two self-

report measures of children’s participation in recreation and

leisure activities (i.e. everyday activities outside of mandated

school activities). The CAPE is a 55-item measure of five dimen-

sions of participation (diversity, intensity, with whom, where

and enjoyment) providing three levels of scoring: (i) overall

participation scores; (ii) domain scores reflecting participation

in formal and informal activities; and (iii) scores reflecting par-

ticipation in five types of activities (i.e. recreational, active phys-

ical, social, skill-based and self-improvement activities), which

were determined through principal component analyses (King

et al

. 2004). The PAC is a parallel measure of preference for

activities, which can be scored on the same three levels.

Both measures are appropriate for children and youth with

and without disabilities between 6 and 21 years of age, as the

items reflect activities done by any child. The items were devel-

oped through a review of the literature, expert review and pilot

testing of the CAPE and PAC with children both with and

without disabilities.

The CAPE and PAC are discriminative measurement tools –

tools designed to distinguish between individuals on an under-

lying dimension (Kirshner & Guyatt 1985). They provide a

comprehensive and fine-grained assessment of multiple dimen-

sions of participation in different types of recreation and leisure

activities, including activities falling into formal vs. informal

domains. The distinction between formal and informal activi-

ties is an important one (Medrich

et al

. 1982; Sloper

et al

. 1990;

Larson & Verma 1999). Involvement in formal activities (i.e.

organized or structured activities) is considered to be more

enjoyable to children and to have advantages with respect to the

development of initiative, intrinsic motivation and competence

(Larson 2000).

A number of measures quantify children’s level of accom-

plishment or competence in activities (e.g. Achenbach 1991;

Henry 2000; Noreau

et al

. 2003). In contrast, the CAPE was

designed to be a direct measure of participation. It documents

what

a child does, not the child’s competence in performing an

activity or the degree of support the child requires to take part.

The CAPE does not confound the measurement of participa-

30

G.A. King

et al

.

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

tion with the measurement of aids, assistance or environmental

supports; it reflects the World Health Organization’s (2001)

conceptualization of participation as actual performance of an

activity in the context of an individual’s normal environment.

According to Forsyth and Jarvis (2002), it is important for a

participation measure to capture participation ‘intrinsically’

and separately from other factors, such as aids or assistance.

Other variables should not be used as proxies for actual or

in

vivo

participation.

Previous work involving the CAPE and PAC

The CAPE/PAC manual reports evidence of the test–retest reli-

ability, internal consistency and factor structure of the mea-

sures, as well as correlations among the various levels of the

CAPE participation intensity scores (i.e. overall, formal, infor-

mal and activity type) and among the PAC preference scores

(King

et al

. 2004). This psychometric information was gathered

in the course of a 3-year longitudinal study of the participation

of school-age children with physical disabilities in Ontario,

Canada, which examined children’s patterns of participation

(Law

et al

. 2005) and the relative importance of child, family

and environmental factors as predictors of the extent or inten-

sity of children’s participation (King

et al

. 2006).

As reported in detail in the CAPE/PAC manual, the test–

retest reliabilities for the CAPE activity type participation inten-

sity scores ranged from 0.72 to 0.81, indicating sufficient test–

retest reliability (Kirshner & Guyatt 1985). Internal consistency

reliabilities for the PAC formal, informal and activity type pref-

erence scores also were acceptable (ranging from 0.67 to 0.84).

The CAPE information has been reported in papers examin-

ing differences in participation diversity and intensity for chil-

dren with central nervous system and musculoskeletal disorders

(Law

et al

. 2004), and the participation patterns of boys and

girls with physical disabilities in various age groups (6–8 years,

11 months; 9–11 years, 11 months; 12 years or older) (Law

et al

.

2005). We also have examined the ability of the CAPE to capture

developmental changes in participation intensity over time, by

examining growth curves. Information provided in these papers

indicates that the CAPE scores can be used to make reliable and

valid inferences about children’s activity patterns.

Construct validity evidence for the CAPE and PAC activity type scores

The present paper provides information on the construct valid-

ity of the CAPE and PAC activity type scores. This paper goes

beyond previously reported psychometric information (see King

et al

. 2004). Other than information about underlying factor

structure, nothing has been published previously that pertains to

the construct validity of the PAC preference scores. The data in

this paper come from the longitudinal study of the participation

of school-age children with physical disabilities described above.

How a test relates to external measures provides evidence of

a test’s construct validity (Wiggins 1973). Convergent evidence

is demonstrated when a test score is significantly correlated with

other measures of the same construct, or with measures of other

constructs in expected ways. Discriminant evidence is demon-

strated when two tests designed to measure different constructs

produce results that are not closely related, or when a measure

distinguishes between groups that are expected to perform dif-

ferently on the measure.

As there are few measures of children’s recreational and lei-

sure participation (which was why we decided to develop the

CAPE and PAC), we unfortunately are unable to provide infor-

mation about the relationship between these measures and

other measures of similar constructs. We did, however, collect

information on other constructs with which we expected that

the CAPE and PAC activity types scores would be correlated.

Based on information in the literature about factors associ-

ated with participation, we tested a series of hypotheses about

associations between (i) activity type scores on three participa-

tion dimensions (CAPE intensity, CAPE enjoyment and PAC

preference); and (ii) scores from other measures reflecting char-

acteristics of environments, families and children themselves.

We also examined hypothesized differences in intensity of par-

ticipation, enjoyment and preference for boys vs. girls, and for

children in various age groups. In the following section, we

provide the rationale for specific predictions, some of which

were outlined previously in a conceptual model of the factors

affecting the recreation and leisure participation of children

with disabilities (King

et al

. 2003).

Predictions

Relationships between the CAPE activity type participation intensity scores and other variables

We made the following predictions:

• Intensity of participation in

recreational activities

(e.g. play-

ing with things or toys, playing on equipment) will be asso-

ciated with the family’s active-recreational orientation (i.e.

the family’s extent of participation in social and recreational

activities). In a study involving children with Down Syn-

drome, Sloper and colleagues (1990) found family active-

Measures of children’s participation

31

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

recreational orientation to be associated with the frequency

of children’s participation in both organized activities and

informal play contacts.

• Intensity of participation in

active physical activities

(e.g.

doing team sports, racing or track and field) will be associated

with (i) parents’ perceptions of barriers in the physical-

structural environment (because these activities require the

absence of such barriers; Garton & Pratt 1991); (ii) family

income and financial constraints (because income has been

found to most markedly affect rates of participation in super-

vised sports; Offord

et al

. 1998); (iii) family time constraints

(because participation in organized activities often requires

transportation and a time commitment by parents); (iv) the

family’s active-recreational orientation (Sloper

et al

. 1990);

and (v) children’s general health, athletic competence (Trost

et al

. 1997; DiLorenzo

et al

. 1998) and physical functioning.

• Intensity of participation in

social activities

(e.g. going to the

movies, talking on the phone) will be associated with (i)

children’s perceptions of support from classmates and friends

(because classmates and friends provide companionship for

participation in informal activities; Patterson & Blum 1996;

DiLorenzo

et al

. 1998); (ii) the family’s active-recreational

orientation (Sloper

et al

. 1990); (iii) children’s social compe-

tence, prosocial behaviour, and their social, emotional and

behavioural functioning (Rae-Grant

et al

. 1989); (iv) chil-

dren’s physical functioning (Lepage

et al

. 1998); and (v) chil-

dren’s communicative functioning (because this is required

in social activities).

• Intensity of participation in

skill-based activities

(e.g. learning

to dance, playing a musical instrument) will be associated

with (i) the family’s active-recreational orientation (Sloper

et al

. 1990); and (ii) the family’s intellectual-cultural orienta-

tion (i.e. the family’s level of interest in political, intellectual

and cultural activities). We did not predict an association with

family income or financial constraints because skill-based

activities are comprised of a mix of activities varying in cost,

many of which can be undertaken in more or less costly ways.

• Intensity of participation in

self-improvement activities

(e.g.

going to the public library, reading) will be associated with

(i) the family’s active-recreational and intellectual-cultural

orientations; and (ii) children’s cognitive and communication

functioning.

Relationships of the CAPE enjoyment and PAC preference scores with other variables

It was predicted that children would enjoy and prefer types of

activities related to their self-perceptions in terms of areas of

competence. Significant correlations were predicted between

scholastic competence and both enjoyment of, and preferences

for, self-improvement activities; athletic competence and enjoy-

ment of, and preferences for, active physical activities; and social

competence and enjoyment of, and preferences for, social

activities.

Sex differences

Based on past research, it was predicted that boys would engage

more intensely in active physical activities (and would enjoy

these more and report higher preferences for this type of activ-

ity), whereas girls would engage more intensely in skill-based,

social and self-improvement activities (and would enjoy and

prefer these more than would boys).

These predictions were based on the literature on activity

participation for children without disabilities, in which sex is

generally found to be a major predictor of patterns of partici-

pation diversity (Medrich

et al

. 1982; Garton & Pratt 1991).

Sports participation is generally found to be higher for boys

than for girls (Medrich

et al

. 1982; Larson & Verma 1999), and

other evidence indicates that girls typically are more involved

in skill-based, social and self-improvement activities. For exam-

ple, Offord and colleagues (1998) reported that, among Cana-

dian children aged 6–11 years, boys participated more highly in

sports whereas girls participated more highly in the arts (i.e.

skill-based activities). Similarly, Medrich and colleagues (1982)

reported that sixth grade girls were more likely to participate in

performing and creative arts than were boys. Garton and Pratt

(1991) found that boys aged 13–17 years without disabilities

had a greater intensity of participation in sports than did

girls of this age; girls took part in more vocational and self-

improvement activities, such as going to the library. They also

found that boys were more interested in sports and girls were

more interested in gregarious or social activities. Posner and

Vandell (1999) found that third to fifth grade boys without

disabilities (i.e. boys between approximately 8 and 10 years of

age) were involved in more coached sports than were girls of

this age, whereas girls engaged in more academic and socializ-

ing activities than did boys.

Age differences

It was expected that children’s age would affect their participa-

tion intensity, enjoyment and preference scores. Children were

grouped into three age groupings (6–8 years, 11 months; 9–

11 years, 11 months; and 12 or older). Compared with younger

children, older children were expected to have lower mean par-

32

G.A. King

et al

.

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

ticipation intensity, enjoyment and preference scores on recre-

ational activities, but higher scores on social activities.

There is little literature on the participation of children with

disabilities and for typically developing children in the age

range of interest (ages 6–15 years). The existing literature indi-

cates that socializing increases with age and that there is a

decrease in recreational activities and in watching television.

Posner and Vandell (1999) found involvement in outside

unstructured activities decreased by half between third and fifth

grade (ages 8–10 years approximately) for low-income White

and African American children without disabilities, whereas

time spent socializing increased twofold during that period.

Larson and Richards (1991) reported that girls in ninth grade

(approximately 14 years of age) took part in more social activ-

ities than did girls in fifth grade (approximately 10 years of age).

Brown and Gordon (1987) found that, with increasing age,

children with and without disabilities visited and made tele-

phone calls more frequently and spent more time out of the

house. There is also evidence that watching television peaks in

middle childhood and declines in early and middle adolescence

(Larson & Richards 1991; Larson & Verma 1999).

In summary, examining these predictions will provide infor-

mation about the construct validity of the CAPE and PAC mea-

sures. The information will be useful for service providers and

researchers interested in the ability of the measures to (i) doc-

ument children’s participation intensity, enjoyment and prefer-

ences for various types of activities; and (ii) discriminate in

expected ways between boys and girls, and children of various

ages.

Method

Participants

Participants were 427 children with physical functional limita-

tions, and their families, from the province of Ontario, Canada.

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from McMaster

University in Hamilton, Ontario. Eleven publicly funded

regional children’s rehabilitation centres and one children’s hos-

pital assisted with recruitment.

The organizations compiled lists of all children with physical

disabilities born between 1 October 1985 and 30 September

1994 (inclusive), and provided us with non-identifiable infor-

mation about children’s sex, date of birth, primary diagnosis

and secondary diagnostic data, if available. Using guidelines

developed by the study team, two investigators (a developmen-

tal paediatrician and a methodologist) reviewed the lists and

selected children who fit the criterion of having a ‘physical

functional limitation’. Children with the following primary

diagnoses or conditions were included: amputation; cerebral

palsy; cerebral vascular accident/stroke (vascular brain disor-

ders); congenital anomalies; hydrocephalus; juvenile arthritis;

muscular disorders (non-progressive); neuropathy; ortho-

paedic conditions (e.g. scoliosis); spinal cord injury; spina

bifida; and traumatic brain injury. Children fit into two catego-

ries – those with central nervous system-related disorders and

musculoskeletal disorders (‘structural’ and primary conditions

of muscle tissue).

Families of children who met the inclusion criteria with

respect to age and physical functional limitation were invited

to participate. Lists of ID numbers of these children were sent

to the recruitment sites, which then sent packages from the

research team inviting families to participate. Of 469 consent-

ing families, 28 withdrew prior to data collection, two were

unsuitable (one child was no longer with the family, one child

already had been selected by another site for inclusion in the

study), and 12 were judged to be unsuitable by the interviewer

because of severe cognitive or communicative limitations

(which affected their ability to communicate their responses

and/or raised questions about the reliability of their

responses), leaving 427 children in the study. The convenience

sample included relatively equal cohorts of children (aged 6–

8 years, 11 months; 9–11 years, 11 months; 12 years or older),

and their families.

Data collection

Data were collected through self-administered measures and a

home interview. A package of self-administered questionnaires

was mailed to the family, to be completed prior to the home

visit by the parent who was most knowledgeable about the

child’s daily activities, and then collected at the time of the visit.

This package included: a demographic questionnaire; phase 1

of the CAPE (King

et al

. 2004), which is described below; the

Craig Hospital Inventory of Environmental Factors (Whiteneck

et al

. 2004); the Child Health Questionnaire-50 (Landgraf

et al

.

1996); the Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos 1994); the

Impact on Family Scale (Stein & Riessman 1980); and the

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman 1997).

This package also contained the Activities Scale for Kids (Young

et al

. 2000), which the child completed. As well, parents

completed one measure (the Communication Domain of the

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales; Sparrow

et al.

1984) via a

phone interview prior to the home visit.

Families were contacted by a study interviewer to arrange a

home-based interview to administer phase 2 of the CAPE and

Measures of children’s participation

33

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

PAC, the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition

(Dunn & Dunn 1997), the Self-Perception Profile for Children

(Harter 1985a) or the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents

(Harter 1988), depending on the age of the child/youth, and the

Social Support Scale for Children (Harter 1985b). Fifteen

experienced interviewers were involved in this aspect of data

collection.

Measures

The CAPE and PAC

The factor analytic structure, test–retest reliabilities and inter-

nal consistency reliabilities of these measures are reported in

detail in the CAPE/PAC manual (King

et al

. 2004). The

measures have adequate test–retest and internal consistency

reliabilities.

In this study, the CAPE was administered in two phases. In

phase 1, the child/youth completed a self-administered ques-

tionnaire booklet either alone or, when needed, with the assis-

tance of a parent/guardian. For the previous 4-month period,

they indicated what activities they participated in, and how

often they did each activity.

In phase 2, the interviewer focused on the activities in which

the child reported having participated, and obtained the follow-

ing information for each of these activities: (i) with whom they

typically did the activity (e.g. parent, friend); (ii) where they did

the activity (e.g. home, at a friend’s house); and (iii) how much

they enjoyed doing the activity. Each activity was described

verbally to the child/youth and presented on a card containing

both a drawing of the activity and a written phrase describing

the activity.

The version of the CAPE used in this study had 49 items.

There are three levels of scoring for the CAPE and PAC mea-

sures: overall (one score), domain (two scores) and activity type

(five scores), resulting in eight scores for each dimension of

participation (i.e. diversity, intensity, where, with whom, enjoy-

ment and preference). For the purposes of construct validation,

we focused on the dimensions of intensity, enjoyment and pref-

erence, as measured on the activity type level. Participation

intensity is calculated by dividing the sum of item frequency by

the number of possible activities for a given level of scoring (e.g.

13 activities, in the case of formal participation). Frequency

response options ranged from 1

=

‘1 time in the past 4 months’

to 7

=

‘1 time a day or more’; enjoyment response options

ranged from 1

=

‘not at all’ to 5

=

‘love it’; and preference

response options ranged from 1

=

‘I would not like to do at all’

to 3

=

‘I would really like to do’.

Other measures

Ten other standardized questionnaires were completed by

children or their parents. All measures are widely used and

have evidence of adequate reliability and validity. Table 1

contains information about the scale names of the variables

used in this study and the measures from which they are

derived. The papers cited in the footnote of Table 1 provide

detailed information about the psychometric properties of

the measures.

Analyses

To examine the convergent and discriminant validity of the

CAPE and PAC activity type scores, we correlated these scores

with measures of different constructs. Based on past findings,

we hypothesized that particular correlations would be signifi-

cant. Small to moderate (0.15–0.30), yet statistically significant,

correlations were expected. This is the range of magnitude of

correlations typical in social science research. We also knew that

there are many interrelated determinants of participation (King

et al

. 2003, 2006), and so univariate associations were bound to

be modest in magnitude. To control for type I error because of

the large number of correlations performed, the level for alpha

was set at 0.01, two-tailed. This is a not overly stringent alpha

level, in keeping with the expectation that the magnitude of

correlations would be relatively small.

Analyses of variance were conducted to test study hypotheses

with respect to mean score differences for boys vs. girls, and for

children in three age groups. The level of statistical significance

for these analyses was set at 0.05, two-tailed (error rate per

hypothesis; Kirk 1968). Although there is controversy as to the

correct conceptual unit for error rate, tradition favours the

hypothesis as the proper unit (Kirk 1968).

Results

Evidence for the convergent and discriminant validity of the CAPE activity type intensity scores

We calculated Pearson product-moment correlation coeffi-

cients between the CAPE activity type participation intensity

scores and scores on the other measures. These correlations are

presented in Table 1; significant correlations are shown in bold

print and predicted correlations are indicated by the shaded

cells. The expected correlations are positive in nature, except for

two variables indicated by asterisks in Table 1 (the physical-

structural environment and financial constraints).

34

G.A. King

et al

.

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd,

Child: care, health and development

,

33

, 1, 28–39

As expected, there were small to moderate correlations in

predicted directions between the CAPE participation intensity

scores and the environmental, family and child variables; the

majority of the significant correlations were in the 0.10–0.20

range. All predicted correlations reached statistical significance

(

P

<

0.01, two-tailed). Of the 33 significant correlations, 23

(69.7%) were predicted.

Evidence for the concurrent validity of the CAPE enjoyment scores and PAC preference scores

Table 2 presents evidence for the convergent and discriminant

validity of the CAPE activity type enjoyment scores and the

PAC activity type preference scores. It was predicted that

children would enjoy and prefer types of activities related to

their self-perceptions in terms of areas of competence, as

measured by the Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter

1985a) or the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter

1988). Table 2 shows small yet significant correlations in the

expected pattern. Significant correlations are shown in bold

print and predicted correlations are indicated by shaded cells

in Table 2.

As shown in Table 2, four out of the six predicted correlations

were significant. Children’s enjoyment of particular types of

activities and their preferences for these activities were related

to their areas of competence. For example, enjoyment of active

physical activities was significantly related to children’s self-

perceptions of their athletic competence (

r

=

0.15), and prefer-

ences for social activities were significantly related to children’s

perceptions of their social competence (

r

=

0.17). Significant

correlations were predicted, but not found, between self-

improvement activities and scholastic competence.

Group differences in participation intensity, enjoyment and preference

We predicted that mean intensity, enjoyment and preference

scores for the five types of activities would differ depending on

children’s sex and age.

Sex differences

Table 3 provides the mean scores for boys and girls for partici-

pation intensity, enjoyment and preference, on the level of

Table 1.

Correlations between the CAPE activity type participation intensity scores and other variables (

n

=

427)

VariablesRecreationalactivities

Activephysicalactivities

Socialactivities

Skill-basedactivities

Self-improvementactivities

Environmental variables*Physical-structural environment (CHIEF)

0.08

-

0.17

0.10 0.05 0.03Classmate support (SSSC) 0.12

0.18 0.15

0.03 0.04Close friend support (SSSC) 0.09 0.08

0.18

0.08 0.06

Family variablesTotal family income

0.01

0.21 0.15

0.11

0.18

*Financial constraints (IOF)

0.11

-

0.21

-

0.13

0.04 0.02Time constraints (CHQ-50) 0.03

0.15 0.13

0.00

0.01Active-recreational orientation (FES)

0.17 0.27 0.23 0.20 0.28

Intellectual-cultural orientation (FES) 0.03 0.08 0.05

0.17 0.30

Child variablesGeneral health (CHQ-50)

0.01

0.13

0.00 0.07

0.05Athletic competence (SPPC) 0.10

0.29

0.08 0.05

0.02Social competence (SPPC) 0.06 0.08

0.16

0.01

0.01Prosocial behaviour (SDQ) 0.08 0.06

0.18

0.07

0.14

Social, emotional & behavioural functioning (CHQ-50) 0.03 0.07

0.14

0.04

0.01Physical functioning (ASK)

0.15 0.42 0.30

0.07 0.12Cognitive functioning (PPVT-III) 0.08

0.19 0.22

0.07

0.24

Communicative functioning (VABS) 0.07

0.21 0.21

0.14

0.26

Bolded correlations are significant at the 0.01 level or lower (two-tailed). Shaded cells indicate predicted correlations. Asterisks indicate variables for which correlations were expected to be negative. ASK, Activities Scale for Kids (Young

et al

. 2000); CHIEF, Craig Hospital Inventory of Environmental Factors (Whiteneck

et al

. 2004); CHQ-50, Child Health Questionnaire-50 (Landgraf

et al

. 1996); FES, Family Environment Scale (Moos & Moos 1994); IOF, Impact on Family Scale (Stein & Riessman 1980); PPVT-III, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Third Edition (Dunn & Dunn 1997); SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman 1997); SPPC, standardized scores from the Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter 1985a) or the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter 1988); SSSC, Social Support Scale for Children (Harter 1985b); VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (Sparrow

et al

. 1984).

Measures of children’s participation 35

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 33, 1, 28–39

activity type. Table 3 shows that boys reported that they partic-

ipated significantly more intensively in active physical activities

than did girls, whereas girls participated more intensively in

social, skill-based and self-improvement activities, as predicted.

Girls reported significantly higher enjoyment of social, skill-

based and self-improvement activities than did boys. Boys pre-

ferred active physical activities more than did girls; girls pre-

ferred skill-based activities and self-improvement activities

more than did boys. Boys’ and girls’ preferences therefore mir-

rored the data for enjoyment and for participation intensity, but

slightly less so.

Age differences

Table 4 provides the mean participation intensity, enjoyment

and preference scores for children with disabilities of different

ages, on the level of activity type.

As predicted, with increasing age, children participated less

intensely in recreational activities. These recreational activities

included some items more relevant for younger children, such

as doing crafts and playing with toys, but this category also

included watching television. Children in the older age group

also had a higher mean participation intensity score for social

activities, as expected. As well, enjoyment and preference scores

decreased across the age groups.

Discussion

This paper utilized data from a large sample of children with

varied levels of function and functional limitation (427 children

with physical disabilities between the ages of 6 and 15 years) to

provide evidence for the construct validity of the activity type

scores of the CAPE and its companion measure, PAC (King

et al. 2004). The findings indicated that participation intensity,

enjoyment and preference scores for five types of activities were

associated in predicted ways with scores on selected environ-

mental, family and child variables. Predictions also were sup-

ported with respect to differences in mean scores for boys vs.

girls, and for children in various age groups. Analyses revealed

a number of significant relationships that confirmed a priori

hypotheses and therefore serve to validate both the CAPE and

PAC as measurement instruments. The findings indicate that

the dimensions of activity participation examined in this study

– intensity, enjoyment and preference – are distinct from one

Table 2. Correlations between children’s areas of competence and enjoyment and preference scores for types of activities (n = 427)

Competencevariables

Recreationalactivities

Active physicalactivities

Socialactivities

Skill-basedactivities

Self-improvementactivities

Enjoyment scoresScholastic competence 0.07 0.08 0.05 −0.04 0.12Athletic competence 0.10 0.15 0.10 −0.01 0.10Social competence 0.18 0.12 0.22 0.07 0.10

Preference scoresScholastic competence −0.01 0.12 0.04 0.07 0.06Athletic competence 0.03 0.24 0.02 0.01 0.00Social competence 0.07 0.16 0.17 0.09 0.00

Bolded correlations are significant at the 0.01 level or lower (two-tailed). Shaded cells indicate predicted correlations.

Table 3. Mean participation intensity, enjoyment and preference scores for boys and girls with physical functional limitations (n = 427)

SexRecreationalactivities

Active physicalactivities

Socialactivities

Skill-basedactivities

Self-improvementactivities

Intensity scores (on a scale of 0–7)Boys 4.16 1.89a 3.11b 0.95c 2.99d

Girls 4.17 1.61a 3.33b 1.26c 3.17d

Enjoyment scores (on a scale of 1–5)Boys 3.98 4.24 4.14e 3.96f 3.14g

Girls 4.05 4.11 4.26e 4.20f 3.40g

Preference scores (on a scale of 1–3)Boys 2.43 02.42h 2.58 2.01i 2.02j

Girls 2.47 02.25h 2.64 2.33i 2.16j

The data are from 229 boys and 198 girls.Pairs of means with the same superscript are significantly different at P < 0.05, two-tailed.

36 G.A. King et al.

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 33, 1, 28–39

another, meaningful and interpretable. The CAPE and PAC

provide detailed information about participation on these

dimensions with respect to five types of activities, and are sen-

sitive to variations in children’s participation across these types

of activities.

Study limitations

This paper provides evidence of the psychometric properties of

the CAPE and PAC based on a sample of children with physical

disabilities. It is therefore important to consider how this sam-

ple may have affected the evidence reported for the psychomet-

ric properties of the CAPE and PAC. There is, however, evidence

that the measures are appropriate for, and provide meaningful

information about, the participation patterns of typically devel-

oping children.

The correlations reported in this paper as evidence of con-

struct validity were smaller in magnitude than expected (most

falling in the 0.10–0.20 range). Although the correlations were

small in magnitude, 70% of the significant correlations were

predicted.

Another study limitation concerns the clinical meaningful-

ness of the differences in the participation intensity, enjoyment

and preference scores reported for boys and girls, and for chil-

dren between 6–8, 9–11 and 12 years of age or older. More data

from different samples of children and youths are required to

consider the meaning and utility of these differences in a clinical

setting. The differences do, however, indicate the ability of the

CAPE and PAC activity type scores to differentiate in predicted

ways among different groups of individuals.

Clinical and research utility of the CAPE and PAC

The psychometric evidence provided in this paper supports the

clinical and research utility of the CAPE and PAC. As there are

few self-report measures of children’s recreation and leisure

participation, these measures fill an important gap for service

providers, teachers, service managers and researchers.

In terms of clinical utility, the CAPE and PAC are useful in

providing fine-grained assessments of children’s everyday activ-

ity patterns (because they provide more than just an overall

participation score) and in determining activity preferences.

Service providers can use the CAPE to conduct a comprehensive

assessment of a child’s direct activity engagement, including the

number of activities the child is involved in, the extent or inten-

sity of his/her involvement in activities of different types, his/

her enjoyment of these activities and information about the

context of participation – with whom and where each activity

takes place.

Information from the PAC about children’s activity prefer-

ences is particularly useful in identifying activities that children

might like to try, and to assist in collaborative goal setting with

children and their families. Service providers could discuss the

match between activities and the children’s skill level with chil-

dren and families to ensure that selected activities provide

opportunities for children’s skill development, should that be a

goal. The five activity types in the PAC allow service providers

to explore with clients the acceptability of substituting one

activity for another, as at least some activities within each activ-

ity type grouping can be considered to be approximate func-

tional equivalents (Medrich et al. 1982). A child who cannot

Table 4. Mean participation intensity, enjoyment and preference scores by age groupings of children with physical functional limitations (n = 427)

Age groupingRecreationalactivities

Active physicalactivities

Socialactivities

Skill-basedactivities

Self-improvementactivities

Intensity scores (on a scale of 0–7)6–8 years 4.59a 1.77 3.05b 1.16 3.069–11 years 4.34a 1.86 3.23b 1.13 3.0812 or older 3.50a 1.61 3.35b 0.98 3.08

Enjoyment scores (on a scale of 1–5)6–8 years 4.19c 4.27 4.29d 4.14 3.48e

9–11 years 3.99c 4.18 4.21d 4.13 3.20e

12 or older 3.88c 4.09 4.07d 3.92 3.13e

Preference scores (on a scale of 1–3)6–8 years 2.57f 2.36g 2.58 2.29h 2.20i

9–11 years 2.49f 2.38g 2.64 2.17h 2.06i

12 or older 2.27f 2.25g 2.59 2.00h 2.01i

There were 125 children aged 6–8 years, 176 children aged 9–11 years, and 126 children aged 12 years or older.Groups of means with the same superscripts denote significant main effects for age (P < 0.05, two-tailed) in two-way analyses of variance (age grouping by sex).

Measures of children’s participation 37

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 33, 1, 28–39

fulfil a need with one activity can potentially meet that need

through a similar activity. For example, a child might express a

strong preference to take part in a social activity, such as visiting

or going to a party, but may not have the opportunity to do so.

In this case, a service provider could propose an alternate social

activity from among those covered in the PAC that might pro-

vide the social contact that the child is looking for, and be under

the child’s control to initiate (e.g. talking on the phone).

The CAPE and PAC have a number of features contributing

to their usefulness for research or evaluation purposes. They are

conceptually grounded, provide a comprehensive assessment of

a broad range of everyday activities, are appropriate for children

with and without disabilities across a wide age range, and assess

multiple dimensions of participation (i.e. diversity, intensity,

enjoyment, location, with whom and preference).

The measures distinguish between formal vs. informal activ-

ities, and among five types of activities, which is of interest to

researchers. These classifications of activities are useful in terms

of prediction specificity as they allow fine-tuned testing of pre-

dictions about determinants of children’s participation in activ-

ities of particular types. For example, the measures could be

used to determine the impact of clinical interventions on par-

ticipation in different types of activities, or to explore the role

of various environmental or family factors in facilitating or

hindering children’s participation in different types of activities.

Researchers also could use the measures to gain important

information about how variables of interest relate to different

dimensions and types of activity participation, and to examine

how different populations of children vary with respect to these

aspects of participation.

Another important feature of the CAPE is that it is a direct

measure of participation. The CAPE does not confound partic-

ipation with competence or with amount of assistance: it cap-

tures what children do, rather than their skill or the amount of

support they require to take part in an activity. This feature

contributes to the CAPE’s utility for research and evaluation

purposes (Forsyth & Jarvis 2002). As the CAPE provides a direct

measure of participation, it can be used to examine the influ-

ence of skill-, support- and opportunity-based interventions on

children’s participation, as well as the influence of impairments

and environmental factors.

As the CAPE and PAC are applicable for children, youth and

young adults (ages 6–21 years), the measures show promise for

use in longitudinal research. Although they were designed to be

discriminative measures, they can be used to examine changes

in various dimensions of participation as a result of interven-

tion or to examine changes over time, as we have done in a

longitudinal study of the factors affecting children’s participa-

tion. More information is required about the ability of the

CAPE and PAC scores to detect change over time, for both

individual children and groups of children, before we can make

definitive statements about the responsiveness of the measures.

We believe, however, that the instruments will be useful in

determining the effectiveness of clinical trials (i.e. as a research

tool) and to measure outcomes of recreational programmes and

other therapeutic programmes designed to enhance children’s

community participation (i.e. as a programme evaluation tool).

In conclusion, this paper has provided evidence for the con-

struct validity of two measures of the recreation and leisure

participation of children and youth with and without disabili-

ties. As evaluating the validity of measurement tools is an ongo-

ing process (American Educational Research Association,

American Psychological Association, and National Council on

Measurement in Education 1999), the future use of the CAPE

and PAC will provide more information about the validity of

the measures, as well as their utility as clinical and research

tools.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the National Institutes of Health for funding

the research project on which this paper is based (Grant

HD38108-02). Appreciation is extended to Susan Roberts,

Mary Forhan, and the many interviewers, organizations, and

families who contributed their time and commitment to this

research project. CanChild Centre for Childhood Disability

Research is funded in part by an operating grant from the

Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-term Care.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991) Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist 4–18,

1991 Profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry, Bur-

lington, VT, USA.

American Educational Research Association, American Psychological

Association, and National Council on Measurement in Education.

(1999) Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. American

Psychological Association, Washington, DC, USA.

Brown, M. & Gordon, W. A. (1987) Impact of impairment on activity

patterns of children. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,

68, 828–832.

Christiansen, C. & Baum, C. (1997) Person-environment occupational

performance: a conceptual model for practice. In: Occupational

Therapy: Enabling Function and Well Being (eds C. H. Christiansen

& C. M. Baum), pp. 46–79. Slack Incorporated, Thorofare, NJ, USA.

Diller, L., Fordyce, W., Jacobs, D. & Brown, M. (1981) Activity Pattern

Indicators: Self-Administered Inventory. Rehabilitation Indicators

Project. NYU Medical Center.

38 G.A. King et al.

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 33, 1, 28–39

Diller, L., Fordyce, W., Jacobs, D., Brown, M., Gordon, W., Simmens,

S., Orazem, J. & Barret, L. (1983) Rehabilitation Indicators Project

Final Report (Project No. G008003039). U.S. Department of Educa-

tion, Washington, DC, USA.

DiLorenzo, T. M., Stucky-Ropp, R. C., Vander Wal, J. S. & Gotham,

H. J. (1998) Determinants of exercise among children. II.

A longitudinal analysis. Preventive Medicine, 27, 470–477.

Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1997) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test –

Third Edition. (PPVT-III). American Guidance Service, Inc., Circle

Pines, MN, USA.

Forsyth, R. & Jarvis, S. (2002) Participation in childhood. Child: Care,

Health and Development, 28, 277–279.

Garton, A. F. & Pratt, C. (1991) Leisure activities of adolescent school

students: predictors of participation and interest. Journal of Adoles-

cence, 14, 305–321.

Goodman, R. (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a

research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–

586.

Harter, S. (1985a) Self-Perception Profile for Children. Department of

Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA.

Harter, S. (1985b) Social Support Scale for Children. Department of

Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA.

Harter, S. (1988) Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. Department of

Psychology, University of Denver, Denver CO, USA.

Hay, J. A. (1992) Adequacy in and predilection for physical activity in

children. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 2, 192–201.

Henry, A. D. (2000) Pediatric Interest Profiles: Surveys of Play for

Children and Adolescents. Therapy Skill Builders, San Antonio,

TX, USA.

King, G. A., Baldwin, P. J., Currie, M. & Evans, J. (2005) Planning

successful transitions from school to adult roles for youth with dis-

abilities. Children’s Health Care, 34, 193–216.

King, G., Tucker, M. A., Baldwin, P., Lowry, K., LaPorta, J. & Martens,

L. (2002) A Life Needs Model of pediatric service delivery: services

to support community participation and quality of life for children

and youth with disabilities. Physical and Occupational Therapy in

Pediatrics, 22, 53–77.

King, G., Law, M., King, S., Rosenbaum, P., Kertoy, M. & Young, N.

(2003) A conceptual model of the factors affecting the recreation and

leisure participation of children with disabilities. Physical and Occu-

pational Therapy in Pediatrics, 23, 63–90.

King, G., Law, M., King, S., Hurley, P., Hanna, S., Kertoy, M., Rosen-

baum, P. & Young, N. (2004) Children’s Assessment of Participation

and Enjoyment (CAPE) and Preferences for Activities of Children

(PAC). Harcourt Assessment, Inc., San Antonio, TX, USA.

King, G., Law, M., Hanna, S., King, S., Hurley, P., Rosenbaum, P.,

Kertoy, M. & Petrenchik, T. (2006) Predictors of the leisure and

recreation participation of children with physical disabilities: a

structural equation modeling analysis. Children’s Health Care (in

press).

Kirk, R. E. (1968) Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral

Sciences. Wadsworth, Belmont, CA, USA.

Kirshner, B. & Guyatt, G. (1985) A methodologic framework for assess-

ing health indices. Journal of Chronic Diseases, 38, 27–36.

Landgraf, J. M., Abetz, L. & Ware, J. E. Jr (1996) Child Health Ques-

tionnaire (CHQ): A User’s Manual. The Health Institute, New

England Medical Center, Boston, MA, USA.

Larson, R. W. (2000) Toward a psychology of positive youth develop-

ment. American Psychologist, 55, 170–183.

Larson, R. & Richards, M. H. (1991) Daily companionship in late

childhood and early adolescence: changing developmental contexts.

Child Development, 62, 284–300.

Larson, R. W. & Verma, S. (1999) How children and adolescents spend

time across the world: work, play, and developmental opportunities.

Psychological Bulletin, 125, 701–736.

Law, M. & Dunn, W. (1993) Perspectives on understanding and chang-

ing the environments of children with disabilities. Physical and

Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 13, 1–17.

Law, M., Finkelman, S., Hurley, P., Rosenbaum, P., King, S., King, G.

& Hanna, S. (2004) Participation of children with physical disabili-

ties: relationships with diagnosis, physical function, and demo-

graphic variables. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 11,

156–162.

Law, M., King, G., King, S., Kertoy, M., Hurley, P., Rosenbaum, P.,

Young, N. & Hanna, S. (2005) Patterns of participation in recre-

ational and leisure activities among children with complex physical

disabilities. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology (in press).

Lepage, C., Noreau, L. & Bernard, P. M. (1998) Association between

characteristics of locomotion and accomplishment of life habits in

children with cerebral palsy. Physical Therapy, 78, 458–469.

Margalit, M. (1981) Leisure activities of cerebral palsied children. Israel

Journal of Psychiatry and Relational Science, 18, 209–214.

Medrich, E. A., Roizen, J., Rubin, V. & Buckley, S. (1982) The Serious

Business of Growing Up: A Study of Children’s Lives Outside School.

University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, USA.

Moos, R. H. & Moos, B. S. (1994) Family Environment Scale Manual,

3rd Edn. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Palo Alto, CA,

USA.

Noreau, L., LePage, C., Boissiere, L., Picard, R., Fougeyrollas, P.,

Mathieu, J., Desmarais, G. & Nadeau, L. (2003) Social participation

in children with cerebral palsy: measurement issues and applica-

tions. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45 (Suppl. 94),

43–44.

Offord, D., Lipman, E. & Duku, E. (1998) Which Children don’t Par-

ticipate in Sports, the Arts and Community Programs? Report from

the National Research Conference Titled ‘Investing in Children:

Ideas for Action’. Human Resources Development Canada, Ottawa,

ON, Canada.

Patterson, J. & Blum, R. W. (1996) Risk and resilience among children

and youth with disabilities. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent

Medicine, 150, 692–698.

Posner, J. K. & Vandell, D. L. (1999) After-school activities and the

development of low-income urban children: a longitudinal study.

Developmental Psychology, 35, 868–879.

Rae-Grant, N., Thomas, B. H., Offord, D. R. & Boyle, M. H. (1989)

Risk, protective factors, and the prevalence of behavioral and emo-

tional disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 262–268.

Measures of children’s participation 39

© 2006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Child: care, health and development, 33, 1, 28–39

Sillanpää, M. (1987) Social adjustment and functioning of chronically

ill and impaired children and adolescents. Acta Paediatrica Scandi-

navica Suppl. 340, 1–70.

Sloper, P., Turner, S., Knussen, C. & Cunningham, C. (1990) Social life

of school children with Down’s syndrome. Child: Care, Health and

Development, 16, 235–251.

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A. & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984) Vineland Adaptive

Behavior Scales. American Guidance Service, Inc., Circle Pines, MN,

USA.

Stein, R. E. K. & Riessman, C. K. (1980) The development of an

impact-on-family scale: preliminary findings. Medical Care, 18, 465–

472.

Stevenson, C. J., Pharoah, P. O. D. & Stevenson, R. (1997) Cerebral

palsy – the transition from youth to adulthood. Developmental Med-

icine and Child Neurology, 39, 336–342.

Trost, S. G., Pate, R. R., Saunders, R., Ward, D. S., Dowda, M. & Felton,

G. (1997) A prospective study of the determinants of physical

activity in rural fifth-grade children. Preventive Medicine, 26, 257–

263.

Whiteneck, G. G., Harrison-Felix, C. L., Mellick, D. C., Brooks, C. A.,

Charlifue, S. B. & Gerhart, K. A. (2004) Quantifying environmental

factors: a measure of physical, attitudinal, service, productivity, and

policy barriers. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85,

1324–1335.

Whiting, B. B. & Edwards, C. P. (1988) Children of Different Worlds:

The Formation of Social Behavior. Harvard University Press, Cam-

bridge, MA, USA.

Wiggins, J. S. (1973) Personality and Prediction: Principles of Personality

Assessment. Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA, USA.

World Health Organization. (2001) The International Classification of

Functioning, Disability and Health. Author, Geneva, Switzerland.

Young, N. L., Williams, J. I., Yoshida, K. K. & Wright, J. G. (2000)

Measurement properties of the Activities Scale for Kids. Journal of

Clinical Epidemiology, 53, 125–137.