Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    1/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 11111

    American Sociological Review , 2002, Vol. 67 (February:129) 1

    guaranteeing that humanitys future will un-fold there. Shortly thereafter, surely duringthe second decade of the century, the lasthunter-gathers will cease to exist, ending 6million years of dedication to what Diamond(1992:191) called the most successful andlong-persistent lifestyle in the career of ourspecies. Sociology should be well-poised tounderstand the nature and meaning of theseincredible transitions; but I believe it is not,owing to three interrelated conceits.

    The first is our elevation of the social overthe biological. Somehow we have allowedthe fact that we are social beings to obscurethe biological foundations upon which ourbehavior ultimately rests. Most sociologistsare woefully ignorant of even the most el-ementary precepts of biological science. If we think about biology at all, it is usually interms of discredited eugenic arguments andcrude evolutionary theorizing long since dis-carded in the natural sciences.

    Direct correspondence to Douglas S. Massey,Department of Sociology, University of Pennsyl-vania, 3718 Locust Walk, Philadelphia, PA19104-6299 ([email protected]). The au-thor thanks Mary Fischer and Sid Seale for theirassistance in preparing the graphics for thisarticle.

    Douglas S. Massey University of Pennsylvania

    Human socie ty emerged over 6 million years of hominid evolution. During this time group size steadily increased,and to maintain group cohesion human beings graduallyevolved a well-developed social intelligence based on thedifferentiation and refinement of emotions. The neurologi-cal structures for emotional expression are part of the

    primitive brain and developed long before the cognitiveequipment for rational intelligence evolved. Indeed, full rationality came rather latein human evolution, and it has only been within the last 100 years that the socialconditions emerged for a mass culture based on rationality. A review of the evolutionof human society and human cognition illustrates the creation and workings of thehuman emotional brain and show how it operates independently of and stronglyinfluences the rational brain. If sociology is to advance, research and theory must grapple with both rational and emotional intelligence and focus particularly on theinterplay between them.

    arly in the twenty-first century,two momentous events will occur.

    Somewhere around 2007 humanity will

    cross a demographic Rubicon: For the firsttime, more than half of all human beings willlive in cities. From that point on, the bulk of population growth will occur in urban areas,

    E

    A Brief History of Human Society:The Origin and Role of Emotionin Social Life

    2001 PRESIDENTIAL ADDRESS

    As you know, sir, in the heat of action menare likely to forget where their best interestslie and let their emotions carry them away.

    Sydney Greenstreet inThe Maltese Falcon

    Written and directed by John Huston

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    2/29

    22222 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    The second conceit is our focus on themodern rather than the traditional. So-ciology came of age in the late nineteenthcentury as an attempt to comprehend newsocial forms arising out of urban industrial-ism, leaving the study of so-called primi-tive societies to our colleagues in anthro-pology. But a science of human society is ascience of human society , and as such itshould span all communities, from smallbands of hunter-gatherers to large urban ag-glomerations. I can think of no more obvi-ous relic of nineteenth-century colonialistthinking than the continuing division be-tween anthropology and sociology.

    Finally, sociologists have unwisely el-evated the rational over the emotional in at-tempting to understand and explain humanbehavior. Its not that human beings are not

    rationalwe are. The point is that we arenot only rational. What makes us human isthe addition of a rational mind to a preexist-ing emotional base. Sociologys focusshould be on the interplay between rational-ity and emotionality, not on theorizing theformer while ignoring the latter or posingone as the opposite of the other. Attemptingto understand human behavior as the out-come of rational cognition alone is not onlyincorrectit leads to fundamental misunder-standings of the human condition.

    In this address I seek to explicate and am-plify these three critiques by undertaking abrief review of human society from its ori-gins to the present. I date the origins of hu-manity from the point at which we began towalk upright, which freed our hands for themanufacture of tools and our brains for ab-stract thought. In tracing our origins fromthe remote past to the present, I seek to illu-minate what sort of beings we really are, andto project how we might be expected tofunction in the dense, urban environments of

    the future.

    THE PATH TO THE PRESENT

    We are the survivors of a host of bipedal pri-mates, known as hominids, who oncewalked the earth. The path of descent fromthe first biped to ourselves resembles not somuch a tree as a shrub, with many branchesdrifting off to extinction and others extend-ing toward the present. Considering this

    shrub from its origins to the present and pay-ing attention to the fundamentals of popula-tion, community, technology, subsistence,and culture, I identify seven basic eras of societal development.

    Prehabiline Society Recent archaeological finds have pushed theorigins of the hominid line back to 6 millionyears ago, when Ardepithecus ramidus andlater Australopithecus africanus descendedfrom the trees to spend their days walkingupright on the ground, which enabled themto exploit an emerging ecology of mixed for-est and grassland (Wrangham 2001). Ourearliest ancestors were small, averaging 1.5meters in height and maybe 70 kilograms inweight, but they exhibited a pronounced

    sexual dimorphism that left males 60 percentlarger than females. Among primates, suchdimorphism is associated with a pattern of female out-marriage into a group composedof a dominant male plus subordinates, con-sorting females, offspring, parents, and sib-lings (Goodall 1986, 1990). As such, dailylife was probably similar to that of modern-day chimpanzees, which is organized aroundforaging, scavenging, and the occasionalhunting of small prey (de Waal 1998;Dunbar 1988; Goodall 1986, 1999).

    There is no evidence that the early Aus-tralopithecines manufactured permanenttoolshence their society is labeled pre-habiline . Like modern chimps, they prob-ably modified perishable materials to use astools (such as sticks to fish termites or leavesto soak up water), and they may havewielded unworked stones for a variety of purposes. The teeth of the Australopith-ecines were large, their jaws were adaptedfor crushing, and their guts were capacious,suggesting a diet centered on vegetable mat-

    ter. Some have argued that the Australopith-ecines descended from the trees precisely toexploit new, ground-based food sources(protein-rich roots and tubers) that were be-coming available on the expandingsavannahs (Wrangham 2001).

    Groups of early hominids, like modernchimps, were probably loosely structuredsocially (Maryanski and Turner 1992; Turner2000). Chimpanzee communities are heldtogether by emotional bonds between indi-

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    3/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 33333

    viduals differentiated by sex, age, and rank in a dominance hierarchy. Within groupsrather strong ties exist between mothers andchildren, and these ties may persist even af-ter the children are grown, especially formale offspring. Weaker ties exist betweenadult males, and ties between other commu-nity members are generally weak or absent.Females transfer out to join other groups atpuberty, preventing the formation of strongstructures based on intergenerational matri-

    lines of related females and male dominancehierarchies. Among apesand among ourlast common ancestorssocial structure isfluid, leaving individuals at the micro levelto seek out their own supportive friend-ships that reflect personal likes and dis-likes, which creates an overall sense of community at the macro level (Turner2000:9).

    Emotional bonds of kinship and friendshipare maintained by mutual grooming, whilerank is established by threat displays occa-

    sionally backed up by force. Social relationsare complex, requiring each individual torecognize and form alliances with others.Chimps must cultivate webs of influence andmutual obligation, form coalitions to defeatcommon adversaries, remember the personalattributes and past behaviors of others, andknow of relationships between others (Byrne1995, 2001; de Waal 1998, 2001; Goodall1999). Studies and field observations showthat chimpanzees are self-aware and able to

    infer the intentions of others and fully ca-pable of social deception and manipulation(de Waal 1998, 2001; Gallup 1970, 1982;Goodall 1986, 1999; Yerkes [1943] 1988). Itis quite likely that Australopithecines pos-sessed similar capacities (Maryanski 1987,1992,1993).

    Compared with modern-day chimps, how-ever, early hominids lived in larger groups.Whereas Chimps normally exist in groups of about 50, calculations by Dunbar (1996)

    suggest that Australopithecines lived inbands of 60 to 70 individuals. Like chimpan-zees, therefore, the earliest hominid speciesmust have possessed a well developed socialintelligence. The number of possible dyadsamong any N individuals is given by the for-mula (N 2 N) / 2, which is graphed as afunction of group size in Figure 1. Whereaschimps (at N = 50) must keep track of 1,225dyadic relationships, Australopithicines (atN = 65) had to manage 2,080, which requiresgreater cognitive ability (Dunbar 2001). As

    a result, the average cranial capacity of Aus-tralopithecines, about 450 cc, was largerthan that of chimps, which averages about400 cc (Napier and Napier 1985).

    The increase in cranial capacity reflects anexpansion of the neocortex, the outermostlayer of the brain, yielding a higher degreeof encephalization (Jerison 1973; Passing-ham and Ettlinger 1974). Despite their keensocial intelligence, however, there is no evi-dence Australopithecines possessed much in

    35,000

    30,000

    25,000

    20,000

    15,000

    10,000

    5,000

    0

    Group Size

    N u m

    b e r

    o f D y a

    d s

    0 50 100 150 200 250

    Chimpanzees: N = 50; dyads = 1,225

    Australopithicines: N = 65; dyads = 2,080

    Homo habilus: N = 75; dyads = 2,775

    Homo erectus: N = 110; dyads = 6,000

    Homo neanderthalensis: N = 140; dyads = 9,730

    Figure 1. Number of Dyadic Relationships Graphed by Group Size

    Note : The number of dyads is a function of group size. It is determined by the formula (N 2 N ) / 2.

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    4/29

    44444 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    the way of what we would call rationality.The frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes of the brainthe loci of abstract reasoning andlanguageremained relatively undeveloped,and there is little evidence of any asymme-try between the right and left hemispheres, adefinitive characteristic of hominids whomanufacture tools. Expansion of the neocor-tex occurred mainly in those portions thatdealt with sensory processing and probablyinvolved a rewiring to enable greater cor-tical control of emotional expression (Turner1997, 2000).

    Like modern chimps, the early Australop-ithecines could probably solve simple prac-tical problems and learn through observationand imitation, but communication was re-stricted to simple vocalizations and gestures.Although modern chimps can be taught 150

    to 200 symbols in the laboratory and canlearn to string them together in sets of twoor three (Snowdon 2001), they display noawareness of word order (syntax), and overmillions of years they have never used sym-bols on their own (Lenneberg 1980). Chimpsdo not even engage in spontaneous pointing,a behavior displayed by the youngest humanchildren (Bruner 1986).

    Prehabiline cognition is associated withwhat Donald (1991) calls episodic culture,in which perceptions and behaviors flow

    largely in the present moment. Individualshave memories of concrete past episodes,they can recall prior experiences, and theycan perceive ongoing social situations; butthey lack semantic memory and abstract rea-soning. Prehabiline society and its episodicculture dominated human life for 3.5 millionyears. During all this timesome 175,000generationsno stone tools were produced.Hominid life was confined to Africa andyielded a population that numbered only inthe thousands or tens of thousands of beings

    dispersed into small roaming groups (Coale1974). The essential features of prehabilinesociety are summarized in Table 1 and arecompared with succeeding societies.

    Oldawan Society

    The first revolution in human history oc-curred with the appearance of stone toolsabout 2.5 million years ago and was associ-ated with the appearance of a new genus of

    hominids. Homo habilis made crude stoneartifacts that most of us would not even rec-ognize as tools. Named for the East Africangorge where they were first discovered,Oldawan tools are pieces of rock that havebeen sharpened on one side by flaking to cre-ate crude choppers and scrapers. Thesesimple tools are associated with the first evi-dence of patterned human living in the formof centralized butchering sites and indicate agrowing reliance on hunting over gathering.

    Although the height of Homo habilis didnot differ dramatically from Aust ra lo -

    pithecus africanus , body weight was largeras their skeletons were more robust, and cra-nial capacity averaged about 550 cc (Stringer1992)a 20 percent increase associated withan expansion of group size to 70 or 80 indi-viduals (Dunbar 2001). The increase in group

    size was accompanied by a greater cognitivecapacity to monitor the increased number of interpersonal dyads (2,775 at N = 75); it alsonecessitated a concomitant increase in theamount of time spent grooming. Primatescultivate and maintain dyadic bonds throughmutual grooming, which causes the releaseof natural opiates in the brain, which in turnpromote feelings of well-being and attractionand lead to social cohesion (Keverne,Martensz, and Tuite 1989). For maximumgroup cohesion to be achieved, every group

    member must groom or be groomed by ev-eryone else.As a result, group size is naturally limited

    by the amount of time individuals can affordto spend grooming each other rather thanengaging in other essential behaviors such assleeping, feeding, courting, and mating(Dunbar 2001). Across primates 20 percentseems to represent an upper threshold fortime spent grooming. The group size char-acteristic of Homo habilis for the first timepushed grooming time above this threshold,

    suggesting that other social mechanismsmust have come into play to maintain rela-tionships (Aielo and Dunbar 1993). Turner(2000) suggests these mechanisms involveda more complex communication of emotion.

    Beyond increased group size, the exist-ence of stone tools, and a larger brain, littleelse changed cognitively, culturally, or so-cially for hominids during the Oldawan pe-riod, which lasted around 1 million years, or50,000 generations. The total human popu-

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    5/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 55555

    T a

    b l e 1

    . E r a s

    i n t h e

    E v o

    l u t i o n o

    f H u m a n

    S o c i e t y

    K i n d o f

    S o c

    i e t y

    V a r

    i a b l e /

    C h a r a c t e r

    i s t i c

    P r e -

    H a b

    i l i n e

    O l d a w a n

    P a l e o

    l i t h i c

    N e o

    l i t h i c

    A g r a r

    i a n

    I n d u s t r i a l

    P o s

    t - I n d u s t r i a l

    B e g

    i n n i n g

    d a t e

    6 m i l

    l i o n

    B . P .

    2 . 5 m

    i l l i o n

    B . P .

    1 . 5 m

    i l l i o n

    B . P .

    5 0 , 0

    0 0 B

    . P .

    1 0 , 0

    0 0 B

    . P .

    2 0 0 B

    . P .

    2 0 B

    . P .

    D u r a t

    i o n

    ( i n y e a r s )

    3 . 5 m

    i l l i o n

    1 m

    i l l i o n

    1 . 5

    m i l l i o n

    4 0 , 0

    0 0

    1 0 , 0

    0 0

    1 8 0

    2 0 +

    G e n e r a t

    i o n s

    1 7 5 , 0 0 0

    5 0 , 0

    0 0

    7 5 , 0

    0 0

    2 , 0 0 0

    5 0 0

    9

    1 +

    I n h a b i t a n t s

    A . a

    f r i c a n u s

    H . h

    a b i l i s

    H . e

    r e c t u s

    H . s

    a p i e n s

    H . s

    a p i e n s

    H . s

    a p i e n s

    H . s

    a p i e n s

    C r a n i a l c a p a c i

    t y

    4 5 0 c c

    5 5 0 c c

    1 , 1 0 0 1 , 4 0 0 c c

    1 , 4 5 0 c c

    1 , 4 5 0 c c

    1 , 4 5 0 c c

    1 , 4 5 0 c c

    S u s

    t e n a n c e

    F o r a g i n g

    H u n

    t e r - g a

    t h e r e r

    H u n t e r - g a t

    h e r e r

    H u n

    t e r - g a

    t h e r e r

    F a r m

    i n g

    M a n u f a c

    t u r i n g

    K n o w

    l e d g e

    T o o

    l s

    P e r i s h a b l e

    C r u

    d e s t o n e

    R e f

    i n e d s t o n e

    F l i n t a n

    d b r o n z e

    B r o n z e a n

    d i r o n

    S t e e l a l

    l o y s

    S i l i c o n , p

    l a s t

    i c

    K i n d o f c u

    l t u r e

    E p i s o d

    i c

    E p i s o

    d i c

    M i m e t

    i c

    M y t

    h i c

    M y t

    h i c

    T h e o r e t

    i c

    T h e o r e t

    i c

    S e t

    t l e m e n

    t t y p e

    M o b

    i l e c a m p

    B u t c h e r

    i n g s i

    t e

    B a s e c a m p

    C a m p / v i

    l l a g e

    C i t y

    M e t r o p o

    l i s

    M e g a l o p o l

    i s

    C o m m u n

    i t y s i z e

    6 5

    7 5

    1 4 5

    1 5 5

    1 m

    i l l i o n

    2 0 m

    i l l i o n

    3 0 m

    i l l i o n

    H u m a n p o p u

    l a t i o n

    < 5 0 , 0 0 0

    < 1 0 0 , 0 0 0

    < 1 , 0 0 0 , 0 0 0

    6 m

    i l l i o n

    9 7 0 b i l l i o n

    6 b i l l i o n

    9 b i l l i o n

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    6/29

    66666 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    lation at the time still numbered only in thetens of thousands, all lived in Africa, andthere is no evidence of any use of languageor symbolic culture (see Table 1). Hominidculture remained episodic, albeit with an in-crease in social complexity and a concomi-tant expansion of cranial capacity to handlegreater demands for social intelligence.

    Paleolithic Society

    About 1.5 million years ago the appearanceof another species and a new technology sig-naled the emergence of a new kind of soci-ety. The species was Homo erectus and thetechnology was the Acheulean tool culture(named for French village, Saint Acheul,where the tools were first found). The pro-gression from habilis to erectus marked an

    abrupt and very important shift in humanevolution (Donald 1991). Although the up-per heights of the two species differed rela-tively little, the size of Homo erectus becameconsiderably more uniform (1.3 to 1.5meters)sexual dimorphism was reduceddramatically from a 60-percent male/femaledifferential to one on the order of 15 percent,about the average for modern humans. Ana-tomically, full shoulder rotation disappeared(which is why modern humans cannot bra-chiate well), indicating a final break from

    arboreal life. Both teeth and guts grewsmaller, suggesting the increased nutritionalimportance of meat. And with the emergenceof Homo erectus we find evidence of the con-trolled use of fire (Brain 1983) and the cre-ation of seasonal base camps (Donald 1991),suggesting the emergence of a nomadic life.

    The sharp decrease in sexual dimorphismstemmed not from a reduction in the size of males, but from an increase in the size of fe-males, thus raising the average height of hominids. The increase in the size of females

    was necessary to accommodate the birth of these new creatures with large heads and fatbrains, as the progression from Homo habilisto Homo erectus was accompanied by a dou-bling of cranial capacityfrom about 550 ccto 1,000 cc to 1,100 cc. The dome of the cra-nium rose, and the face flattened to accom-modate expansion of the frontal, temporal,and parietal lobes of the brain. Homo erectusalso provides the first evidence of brain lat-eralitya differentiation in size between

    right and left brain hemisphereswhich isassociated with handedness and communica-tive abilities (Donald 1991). Rather than thecrude stone choppers of the Oldawan cul-ture, the Acheulean tool kit evinced consid-erable manual dexterity (which goes alongwith handedness). Tools now consist of sym-metrically shaped, carefully worked handaxes, cleavers, and knives with sharper andmore effective cutting surfaces.

    The decline in sexual dimorphism and theneed to care for large-brained children whowere helpless in infancy, vulnerable in child-hood, and dependent through adolescenceproduced a radical change in human socialstructure and mating patterns (Ellison 2001).It was probably during this era that the hu-man practice of pair bonding appeared, ac-companied by a decline in intermale con-

    flict, the disappearance of rigid dominancehierarchies, and a growing investment by fa-thers of their time and resources for the ben-efit of their mates and children (Lovejoy1980). Male commitment to pair bondingwas reinforced (though not guaranteed) byan entirely new reproductive biology, inwhich females were continuously sexuallyreceptive, breasts were enlarged and dis-played, and the timing of estrous was hid-den. The distinctively human and unprimate-like practice of copulating in private prob-

    ably also emerged at this time. Homo erectuss increased brain size en-abled another expansion in group size, to 90to 120 individuals, yielding somewhere inthe neighborhood of 6,000 dyadic relation-ships (N = 110). Despite greater encephal-ization, however, evidence suggests that theintelligence of Homo erectus was still pre-linguistic. It was, however, different fromthe episodic intelligence that preceded it.The basic cognitive function for Homoerectus was mimesis imitation or mim-

    icryin which vocalizations, facial expres-sions, eye movements, body postures, andmanual signs were used to communicate andto transmit skills from person to person.

    The emergence of gestural communicationreflected a significant increase in the rangeand complexity of emotional expression, asthe cognitive bases for primary emotionssuch as fear, anger, disgust, happiness, andsadness were rewired and interconnected viathe cortex to produce new sets of emotions

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    7/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 77777

    conducive to social cohesion and solidarity,such as shame, guilt, anticipation, and hope(Turner 2000).

    Learned physical skills among Homoerectus were an essential adaptive resourceand were passed from generation to genera-tion by demonstration and imitation (Donald1991). Although mimetic culture remainedconcrete, visual, and episode-bound, it nev-ertheless brought about an increase in thesharing and accumulation of knowledge,yielding a more stable social structure basedon ritual and custom, which served toheighten the emotional content of social re-lationships and promote bonding (Turner2000). It was probably during this phase of human social evolution that facial expres-sions attained their current importance as ameans of signaling emotion. Patterns of non-

    verbal facial expression recur across culturesand are interpreted similarly by humans inall societies, suggesting a hard wired abil-ity to read emotions that are displayed on thehuman face (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1989; Ekmanand Rosenberg 1997; Goleman 1995). Theresulting mimetic culture provided humanswith a powerful adaptive tool that enabledthem to leave their ancestral homeland forthe first time and survive in a wider varietyof habitats. Around 1 million years ago,

    Homo erectus moved out of Africa and oc-

    cupied the southern portions of Asia and Eu-rope, a move which brought about the firstsignificant growth of the human population(Coale 1974).

    Late in the paleolithic period, around300,000 years ago, several new species of hominid arrived on the scene to competewith Homo erectus , who eventually disap-peared. The new species were Homoheidelbergensis and the better-known Homoneanderthalensis , also known as the Nean-derthals (Tattersall 1995). The latter ap-

    proximated modern humans in height, butwere much heavier and more powerfullybuilt, with thicker bones and a more robustmusculature. The size of the Neanderthalcranium also expanded to reach volumescharacteristic of our own species, althoughthe physical organization of the brain re-mained distinct, with small and constrictedfrontal lobes. Nonetheless, the increase inaverage cranial capacity to around 1400 ccenabled group size to expand further, reach-

    ing 120 to 160 individuals and implying aneed to manage some 9,730 separate dyadicrelationships (N = 140; see Figure 1)

    With groups this large, the maintenance of cohesion could not possibly have relied onmutual grooming, as servicing so many rela-tionships would have required around 40percent of the groups entire time to be spentgrooming, a percentage that is far too largeto be feasible (Dunbar 2001). Culture, there-fore, must by then have been the centralmechanism maintaining social cohesion.Nonetheless, both the fossil and archaeologi-cal evidence suggest that Neanderthals weremimetic and nonverbal (Diamond 1992;Donald 1991; Tattersall 1995): Their con-stricted frontal lobes and a skull flexure in-capable of accommodating a modern vocalapparatus suggest that Neanderthals could

    not speak (Lieberman 1975, 1984).The Neanderthals nonetheless evinced a

    more advanced material culture than theirhominid predecessors, as indicated by theemergence of a new tool kit, the Mousterian(again named for a French village), whichconsisted of flint or stone blades haf ted ontowood or bone handles. These are the firstcomposite toolsthose composed of at leastthree materials. The new tool kit yielded 10times more cutting edge per kilo of raw ma-terial than the former Acheulean technology.

    With the Neanderthals we also begin to ob-serve stone hearths and postholes, indicatingthe construction of permanent shelters. Wealso observe migration and settlement in thecolder regions of northern Europe, and nu-merous bone middens indicate a heavy reli-ance on hunting for food. With the emer-gence of semipermanent settlements of 120to 160 persons, social structure probably be-gan to move beyond small bands differenti-ated by age and sex to form larger collectivessuch as kin-based clans (Tattersall 1995).

    Neolithic Society

    Fully modern human beings emerged in Af-rica somewhere around 150,000 years agoand rapidly migrated outward to occupy allcorners of the globe. Homo sapiens reachedEurope and Asia around 50,000 years beforepresent (B.P.), Australia by 40,000 B.P., theArctic by 20,000 B.P., the Americas by10,000 B.P., and most of the islands of

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    8/29

    88888 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    Polynesia by 2,000 B.P. (Balter 2001; Dia-mond 1997; Gibbons 2001a, 2001b).Whereas the Australopithecines and habi-lines had remained in Africa for 5 millionyears without moving, and Homo erectus andthe Neanderthals had not expanded beyondsouthern Europe and Asia over 1 millionyears, within the space of just 50,000 years

    Homo sapiens fully populated the entireearth. Clearly they must have gained accessto some remarkable new survival tools.Given that the maximum density achieved byhunters and gatherers in the richest habitatsis a little under .5 persons per square kilome-ter (Hassan 1981), demographers estimatethat the prehistoric human populationreached roughly 6 million persons (Coale1974; Landers 1992; Livi-Bacci 1992;).

    In Homo sapiens , the brain reached its

    present size and structure with a full expan-sion of the frontal lobes that created the po-tential for a new and revolutionary kind of culture (Knight, Dunbar, and Power 1999;Watts 1999). The importance of the humanbrain as an adaptive organ is indicated by thefact that it constitutes 2 percent of bodyweight but uses 20 percent of the bodys to-tal energy (Donald 1991). Nonetheless, theappearance of anatomically modern humanbeings was not initially accompanied by dra-matic cultural changes.

    The crucial period of cultural changeseems to been around 50,000 years ago, inan era that has variously been called thegreat leap forward (Diamond 1992), thecreative revolution (Tattersall 1998) andthe symbolic revolution (Chase 1999).From this point onward, human technologyundergoes rapid change that is disconnectedfrom further changes in brain size oranatomy. Whereas earlier technologies pre-vailed for hundreds of thousands or evenmillions of years and only changed with the

    arrival of a new species, after 50,000 B.P.material culture evolves rapidly and differ-entiates without any corresponding physicalchanges. Human cognitive capacity had ap-parently reached a state of dynamism andflexibility where it could innovate adapta-tions ad infinitum.

    In addition to the hafted stone and woodtools characteristic of Mousterian culture,

    Homo sapiens now began to fashion a hostof new and more delicate tools from bone

    and antler, creating hooks, needles, awls,and harpoons. They wove fibers into ropesand clothing; tanned hides for clothing andcoverings; created jewelry and other formsof personal adornment; and built permanentshelters that not only included hearths, butlamps and kilns as well. They inventedspear throwers, bows, and arrows to alloweffective hunting from a distance, thusminimizing risks to personal safety. Late inthe neolithic era, bronze began to replaceflint in tools and weapons, ushering in abronze age.

    Perhaps not immediately, but sometimeafter the appearance of Homo sapien s ,speech developed and gave human beings acapacious verbal memory and new capacitiesfor cognition and analysis (Gregory 1981).The development of language was enabled

    by important changes in the size, structure,and function of the brain compared with theNeanderthals. Expansion of an area locatedtoward the bottom of the frontal lobes(Brocas area) permitted phonology , the mak-ing and controlling of sounds. The develop-ment the temporal lobes (Wernickes area)allowed audition , the ability to hear and dif-ferentiate sounds; and expansion of the par i-etal and frontal lobes permitted conceptual-ization , the ability to use and manipulate ar-bitrary sound symbols in meaningful ways

    to think of words before uttering them and toorganize them into large units of meaning.All of these expanded brain functions wereconnected into a larger articulatory loop thatfunctioned in a newly coordinated fashion(Carter 1998; Panksepp 1998).

    Language emerged not to give humans acapacity for rational or abstract analysis perserational thought was more of a by-prod-uctbut to enhance their social intelligenceto enable them to get along in large groups(Dunbar 1996; Maryanski 1996). Language

    is the ultimate social arbiter, allowing peopleto maintain interpersonal relationshipsthrough conversation, to monitor the socialinteractions of others through gossip, and toreach collective decisions through discus-sion. In Dunbars (2001) words, humanshave bigger, more complexly organizedgroups than other species simply because wehave a larger onboard computer (the neocor-tex) to allow us to do the calculations neces-sary to keep track of and manipulate the

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    9/29

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    10/29

    1 01 01 01 01 0 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    mechanisms to distribute food and preventthe accumulation of surplus. They employ auniversal classification of people accordingto kinship and have a symbolic structurebased on bipolar opposites (good/bad, male/ female, hot/cold, etc.). Their metaphoric nar-ratives associate animal identities with thoseof humans, and much of human action is in-fused with ritual (Collins 1987).

    Although possessing a capacity for ratio-nal thought, elements of rational cognitionand culture are not very well developed inmythic cultures. Rather, the new cognitiveapparatus created by words was morestrongly connected to emotional centers of the brain through conditioning and learningthat were deliberately reinforced by ritual,ceremony, and spirituality (Collins 1987;Turner 2000). The emergence of cultural

    forms more strongly connected to rational-ity required something that hunter-gathersnever achieveda food surplusand thatonly became possible with the domesticationof plants and animals.

    Agrarian Society

    The signature characteristic of hunter-gath-erer societies is that, except for the veryyoung and the very old, everyone producesfood: Each day all people must work to en-

    sure the groups caloric intake. Until quiterecently, human societies had no specialists,and certainly none devoted to full-time cog-nition. This only became possible between12,000 and 10,000 years ago with the emer-gence of the first settled villages, which, inturn, were rendered feasible by the cultiva-tion of crops and the herding of animals. Forthe first time humans had created a food sur-plus. The accumulation of food in excess of one-days needs enabled a dramatic accel-eration of human population growth, higher

    population densities, and the existence of anew class of peoplealbeit a small classwho did not have to produce food for theirown survival (Coale 1974; Livi-Bacci 1992;Sjoberg 1960).

    Agriculture, herding, and cities emerged atdifferent times in different places dependingon access to plants and animals that werepotentially domesticable (Diamond 1997);but they first materialized in a geographicarc extending from the eastern Mediterra-

    nean upward through Anatolia and downinto the valley of the Tigris and Euphratesrivers. Known as the Fertile Crescent, itwas here that the ancient ancestors of sev-eral modern cereal crops and herd animalswere found in foothills adjacent to a flat al-luvial plain whose soil was annually replen-ished and watered by seasonal floodingtheperfect environment for the emergence of in-tensive agriculture. Wheat, barley, and peaswere domesticated in Mesopotamia by10,000 B.P., along with sheep and goats; by6,000 B.P. several fruit-bearing trees andbushes had also been tamed (notably thoseyielding olives and grapes). Agriculture wasindependently invented in China, Meso-america, and perhaps India, and spread toEurope and other locations in Asia and theAmericas through diffusion (Diamond 1997;

    Sjoberg 1960).The domestication of plants and animals

    set the stage for a new kind of human soci-ety based in cities. The first cities emergedin Mesopotamia around 8,000 years ago,with populations in the 5,000 to 25,000range (Sjoberg 1960). Under a preindustrialtechnology, the amount of food that could beproduced per hectare was limited and the to-tal size of the food surplus was necessarilymeager. While intensive agriculture mayhave permitted cities to exist, they couldnt

    be numerous and only a few people couldlive in them. Prior to the industrial revolu-tion, no more than 5 percent of any societylived in cities and the total population of asingle city never exceeded 1 million (Chan-dler and Fox 1974; United Nations 1980).

    With yields per hectare fixed by techno-logical limits, the only means for a city tobecome larger was to gain control of moreland and thereby to commandeer a largersurplus. Mesopotamian cities were limited insize by internecine rivalries and political

    fragmentation. It was not until the Pharaohsconsolidated their control over the NileRiver Valley around 5,000 years ago that cit-ies really began to expand. The first settle-ment to exceed 100,000 inhabitants wasThebes, in Egypt, around 3,400 years ago. Itwas succeeded a thousand years later byBabylon, which surpassed 200,000, and fi-nally by Rome, which during the reign of Caesar Augustus was the first city to surpass500,000. The 100,000-person threshold was

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    11/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 1 11 11 11 11 1

    crossed in China 2,600 years ago and inMesoamerica about 2,000 B.P. (Chandlerand Fox 1974).

    Wherever it occurred, agrarianism dis-placed hunting and gathering as a way of lifethrough direct conquest, demographicabsorption, or ecological marginalization.This victory occurred not because agrarian-ism provided people with less work, betternutrition, and longer lives. On the contrary,the residents of early agrarian societiesworked longer hours, consumed fewer calo-ries, lived in poorer health, and within citiesthey died in larger numbers (Diamond 1997;McNeill 1976). What gave agrarian societ-ies their decided advantage in confrontationswith hunter-gatherers was their demographicweight, which allowed them to possess anadvanced technology, specialized fighters,

    and deadlier germs.With the advent of agriculture, the stone

    age gave way to the bronze and iron agesmetals replaced flint to create more effectivetools and more lethal weapons. Urban dwell-ers and the agrarian empires they maintainedevinced new instrumental needs, specialiststhought up new tools to satisfy them, tradersobtained the necessary raw materials, arti-sans made them, a political elite controlledtheir distribution, and a religious elite justi-fied their deployment. As a result, in any en-

    counter urban-led agrarian societies wereable to wield weapons and technologies farsuperior to anything that hunter-gathererscould muster. Agrarian societies also couldspare more people from the necessity of foodproduction to wield these weapons, whichled to the emergence of professional soldiersand the first standing armies. In any compe-tition over land and resources, populationsenumerated in the tens or hundreds of thou-sands held a clear advantage over smallbands numbering in the dozens or hundreds,

    especially if the former fielded an army of well-equipped, well-fed, and well-trainedprofessional fighters.

    Finally, the density of preindustrial cities,the lack of sewage facilities, the contamina-tion of drinking water, and the close asso-ciation of humans with animals gave one fi-nal advantage to agrarian-urbanism: germs(Diamond 1997). Until the nineteenth cen-tury, in fact, health conditions in cities weresuch that infectious diseases were endemic,

    and life expectancies were short, muchshorter than among peasants in the country-side (McNeill 1976; Preston and Haines1991). In fact, were it not for continuous ru-ral in-migration, preindustrial cities wouldnot have been able to sustain themselves de-mographically. Nevertheless, although deathrates were high and life expectancies short,those urban dwellers who survived either in-herited or acquired an immunity to bacteria,viruses, and other parasites that hunter-gath-erers could never attain, given their low den-sities. As a result, throughout history initialcontacts between hunter-gathers and agrari-anism-urbanism have invariably led to theformers decimation through a sudden expo-sure to diseases for which they had no natu-ral immunity (Diamond 1997; McNeill1976; Tierney 2000).

    From the rise of the first city-states around10,000 years ago up to the year 1800, agrar i-anism was the dominant form of human so-ciety, occupying ever larger portions of theworlds geography with ever larger popula-tions, both rural and urban. The rough tra-

    jectory of world population growth is shownin Figure 2, which graphs the estimated sizeof the human population from 600 B.C. to1800 A.D. At the end of the paleolithic era,the worlds population stood at around 8 mil-lion; but over the next 8,000 years it grew to

    reach about 252 million in the year zero.Under the Pax Romana, population growthreached a plateau and with the collapse of the Roman Empire it declined to around 200million before rebounding slowly after theyear 600. Beginning in the year 1000, a va-riety of technological and social innovationscame together to unleash a demographicboom in Europe, which proceeded apace un-til the Black Death struck in the middle1300s (Levine 2001). As the foundations fora global economic order based on mass pro-

    duction, trade, and investment were estab-lished after 1600 (Braudel 1982; Wallerstein1974), the human population entered an eraof sustained exponential growth that accel-erated until the dawn of the nineteenth cen-tury, when the worlds population stood ataround 954 million (Livi-Bacci 1992).

    In the 500 generations preceding 1800, theproportion of people living in urban areasnever exceeded 5 percent (United Nations1980), and before that year only 65 cities in

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    12/29

    1 21 21 21 21 2 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    the entire history of the world had everachieved populations greater than 100,000(Chandler and Fox 1974). The vast majorityof people in the civilized agrarian societ-ies of the past were illiterate peasants wholived in small villages and isolated hamletsnot much bigger than the hunting and gather-ing communities of the paleolithic era. Work was governed by the rhythms of the seasons,and most people lived and died within a few

    kilometers of where they were born, spend-ing all their lives within a community of friends and relatives they knew personally.In such a society, culture remained substan-tially mythic. The only real difference com-pared with the mythic cultures of hunter-gathers was the patina of formal structureimposed from above by a priestly class.

    Through the innovation of written lan-guage, however, agrarianism created thefoundation for a new kind of culture. Theorigins of writing lay in the desire of

    Sumerian merchants for an accounting sys-tem to keep track of their sales and ship-ments. Some 10,000 years ago, urban trad-ers began using a stylus to mark clay tokensof different shapes, thus indicating the quan-tity and nature of goods being shipped orstored (Harris 1986; Lawler 2001). Over themillennia, these clay tokens expanded intotablets, and the markings were elaborateduntil about 5,000 years ago, when they be-came a syllabic script for the Sumerian lan-

    guage. Then, about 4,000 years ago thePhoenicians evolved the first phonetic alpha-bet, which later was borrowed and modifiedby the Greeks and eventually the Romans,whose system we use today (Diamond1997). Writing emerged independently inChina and Mesoamerica at later dates.

    The advent of writing created the first ex-ternal memory devicestablets, scrolls, andstone monumentswhich dramatically re-

    duced the demand on biological memory andfreed up human cognition for other tasks.Whereas biological memory is inherentlylimited by a fixed number of neurons (notall of which we use), writing created a stor-age system of unlimited size. Moreover,through both written and oral connectionsbetween individual brains within dense ur-ban environments, cities dramatically in-creased the size of the human cognitive ap-paratus (Wright 2000). In many ways, theyfunctioned as macro-social brains, and the

    use of symbols externalized thought and dra-matically increased information processing(Diamond 1997).

    Writing thus created the potential for anew theoretic culture a logical system of thought grounded in experience and rational-ity and capable of making accurate predic-tions about events in the natural and socialworlds (Donald 1991). Almost by definition,access to such a culture was limited to thesmall minority of people who were literate

    1,000

    900

    800

    700

    600

    500

    400

    300

    200

    100

    0 600 400 200 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 1800

    Year

    P o p u

    l a t i o n i n

    M i l l i o n s

    Black Death

    Figure 2. World Population, 600 B.C. to 1800 A.D.

    Sources : Coale (1974); Landers (1992); Livi-Bacci (1992).

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    13/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 1 31 31 31 31 3

    and freed from the burdens of food produc-tion, and even among those who could readand write there were two competing modesof thought (Bruner 1986; Snow 1959). Nar-rative thought is basically a written exten-sion of oral culture expressed as literature:

    plays, poems, stores, novels, operas, and soon. Analytic thought , while dependent onwords, is a rational construction grounded informal systems of argument, taxonomy, andconceptualization. Analytic thought followslogical rules based on induction and deduc-tion and makes frequent use of formal quan-titative measurement. Although theoreticculture gained adherents and influence inhuman affairs during the agrarian era, it nec-essarily remained a minor part of the totalhuman experience, given the small number

    of people who were literate.

    Industrial Society

    Agrarian food surpluses allowed a smallnumber of people to devote their time tothinking and innovating, and even though the

    percentage of such people remained tiny, thesteady increase in population from Romantimes to 1800 meant that the absolute num-ber of such people steadily grew. Whereas

    no more than 5 percent of the world lived incities during the eras of both CaesarAugustus and Queen Victoria, for Romantimes this implied a population of 13 millionurbanites, while for the Victorian era therewere 48 million city-dwellers. The growing

    quantity of people with the time and freedomto think new thoughts promoted more andfaster innovation, which caused more popu-lation growth, which increased innovation,and so on.

    Beginning around 1800, human societyentered a remarkable new period of demo-graphic growth the likes of which the worldhad never seen. Whereas hunting and gath-ering had been the dominant mode of soci-ety for 6 million years and agrarianism for10,000 years, industrialism flowered and

    matured in less than 200 years. From a baseof 954 million people in 1800, the humanpopulation grew to 1.6 billion in 1900, 2.5billion in 1950 and 6.1 billion in 2000. Inthe past 200 years, more people have beenborn than in all previous human generationscombined. Even more remarkable has beenthe shift of the human population from ruralto urban areas.

    As can be seen in Figure 3, in 1800 thepercentage urban was still roughly at the up-

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    1800 1825 1850 1875 1900 1925 1950 1975 2000 2025

    Year

    P e r c e n

    t a g e

    Percentage living in cities

    Percentage living in cities withpopulations > 1 million

    50 percent will live in cities

    Figure 3. Trends in World Urbanization, 1800 to 2000

    Sources : United Nations (1980, 1999).

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    14/29

    1 41 41 41 41 4 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    per limit for an agrarian societyabout 5percent of total population. Slowly at first,and then accelerating in the late nineteenthand early twentieth centuries, there was amassive geographic shift. In the industrial-izing societies of Europe and North Americathe transition to urbanism was rapid and

    complete by the mid-twentieth century(Brockerhoff 2000; Preston 1979). Althoughdeveloping countries lagged behind, they arenow also rapidly urbanizing, and by 2025the proportion of city-dwellers worldwide isprojected to reach nearly 60 percent.

    In this century, not only will a majority of human beings be living in cities, but a grow-ing fraction will reside in extremely largeplaces. Beginning in the mid 1920s, the per-centage of human beings living in cities of 1million or more rose sharply. As world ur-

    banization passes the 50 percent mark to-ward the end of this decade, the number liv-ing in large metropolises will surpass 20 per-cent; and by the year 2025, one-quarter of all humanity will live in cities of 1 millionor more. Increasingly, most of these large ur-ban agglomerations will be in the ThirdWorld. Whereas the five largest cities in1900 were London, New York, Paris, Berlin,and Chicago, in 2015 they will be Tokyo,Bombay, Lagos, Dhaka, and Sao Paolo, fol-

    lowed by Karachi, Mexico City, New York,Jakarta, and Calcutta (United Nations 1999).

    The urbanization of society has been ac-companied by a remarkable acceleration of technical, economic, social, and culturalchange. Technologically, inanimate sourcesof power replaced animate sources, as horses

    and oxen gave way to wind and water, thento coal and steam, and finally to petroleum,hydroelectricity, nuclear energy, solar, andgeothermal power. Economically, the pro-ductive base of society shifted in rapid suc-cession from agriculture to manufacturing toservices, with each transition accompaniedby a startling increase in the productivity of land, capital, and labor. The creation of massmarkets yielded economies of scale thatwere accompanied by an unprecedented in-crease in social differentiation and stratifi-

    cation. As the distance between the top andthe bottom of society grew, the interveningspace was filled with a complex array of in-creasingly variegated social strata. Cultur-ally, gender roles changed, childhood wasextended, fertility levels fell, and survivalrates increased to create a demographicstructure in which the old outnumbered theyoung.

    Although the foundations for theoreticculture were laid in the agrarian era, the

    0

    25

    50

    75

    100

    1800 1825 1850 1875 1900 1925 1950 1975 2000

    Year

    P e r c e n

    t L i t e r a

    t e

    United States

    Europe

    Worldwide

    Figure 4. Trends in Literacy, 1800 to 2000

    Sources : Cipolla (1969); Graff (1987); Lestage (1982); and Wagner (1993).

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    15/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 1 51 51 51 51 5

    technical constraints of preindustrial farmingnaturally limited the size of the food surplusand hence the percentage of people whocould live in cities, thus placing an upperlimit on literacy. As long as literacy re-mained the prerogative of a small minorityof people who were liberated from the land,the prevailing cultural mix would beweighted toward the mythic rather than thetheoretical. The full flowering of rationalculture, in other words, awaited the adventof mass literacy.

    Figure 4 shows trends in the literacy of adults from 1800 to 2000 in Europe, theUnited States, and worldwide; data are fromCipolla (1969), Lestage (1982), Graff (1987),and Wagner (1993). In 1800, the vast major-ity of human beings would not have hadmuch access to the fruits of theoretic culture,

    as 85 percent of the worlds population waspreliterate. Even in Europe, only about 40percent could read and write in 1800, owingmainly to mass illiteracy in the Russian Em-pire. The United States fared better, with lit-eracy rates approaching 60 percent.

    In Europe and the world, the degree of lit-eracy changed slowly during the nineteenthcentury. As late as 1900 half of all Europe-ans and two-thirds of the worlds populationstill could not read or write. After 1900,however, the extension of public schooling

    in Europe rapidly increased literacy there tolevels above 90 percent. Among nations of the Third World, meanwhile, national devel-opment programs began to extend universalschooling, and in the late 1940s world lit-eracy broke the 50 percent barrier. By theyear 2000, three-quarters of the worldspopulation had been given instruction in atleast the fundamentals of reading and writ-ing. Nonetheless, nearly one-quarter of allhumanity remained disenfranchised from thebasic requirement for a rational, theoretic

    culture.

    The Emerging Post-Industrial Order

    Despite the incredibly rapid change since1900, there is evidence of an even greateracceleration beginning around 1980 with themass production of the silicon chip and theconsequent introduction of computers intovirtually all aspects of social and economic

    life. In the new economic order, wealth restson the creation of knowledge rather than themanufacture of goods, and status derivesfrom the control and manipulation of infor-mation. Although markets are increasinglyglobal in scale, they are also increasinglyfragmented, being divided into segmentedniches rather than mass populations of con-sumers. Within the global economy, capital,labor, goods, commodities, and informationare increasingly mobile and transnational,yielding a new internationalization of bothproduction and culture (Harvey 1990).

    Compared with the roughly 300,000 gen-erations that humans spent as hunter-gather-ers and the 500 generations they spent asagrarians, the 9 generations passed in the in-dustrial era and the 1 generation so far spentin the emerging post-industrial era are a drop

    in the bucket of time. As organisms, we can-not possibly have adapted to the environ-ment in which we now find ourselves. Fu-ture adaptations will come from institu-tional, technological, and cultural changes,not from physiological adaptation. The na-ture and success of our adaptation will de-pend heavily on what sort of organisms wearehow we think and how we function so-cially.

    PAST IS PRESENT

    In his play, Requiem for a Nun , WilliamFaulkner noted,The past is never dead. Itsnot even past (act 1, scene 3). Our brainshave evolved over millions of years as achanging mixture of emotionality and ratio-nality, and each evolutionary step has builtupon what came before. Emotionality clearlypreceded rationality in evolutionary se-quence, and as rationality developed it didnot replace emotionality as a basis for hu-man interaction. Rather, rational abilities

    were gradually added to preexisting and si-multaneously developing emotional capaci-ties. Indeed, the neural anatomy essential forfull rationalitythe prefrontal cortexis avery recent evolutionary innovation, emerg-ing only in the last 150,000 years of a 6-mil-lion-year existence, representing only about2.5 percent of humanitys total time on earth.To the extent we possess rational cognition,it necessarily rests on a preexisting emo-tional foundation.

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    16/29

    1 61 61 61 61 6 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    This evolutionary history is reflected inthe basic structure of our brains, which issummarized in Figure 5. Natural selection isconservative: It doesnt build new capacitiesfrom scratch, but acts upon structures andfunctions that already exist. As a result, hu-man beings have come to possess whatMacLean (1973, 1990) has labeled a triunebrain: three different layers of neural

    anatomy laid down on top of one anotherduring different phases of evolution. Theoldest and deepest layer consists of the brainstem and the cerebellum (basal ganglia);these structures control autonomic functions,such as heartbeat and breathing, as well asinstinctual behaviors, such as the sucking re-flex. It is known as the reptilian brain be-cause it closely resembles the structure andorganization of the neural anatomy found inreptiles today, and presumably in the past.

    The next layer is called the mammalian

    brain (Panksepp 1998). It surrounds the rep-tilian brain and consists of a set of neuralstructures collectively known as the limbicsystem. The most important organ in thissystem is the amygdala, which is crucial inregistering incoming emotional stimuli andstoring emotional memories. Other limbicsystem organs include the thalamus, whichreceives stimuli and relays them to otherparts of the brain; the hypothalamus, whichworks with the pituitary gland to keep the

    body chemically regulated and attuned to itsenvironment; the hippocampus, which is re-sponsible for laying down memory; and asurrounding layer of tissue known as the cin-gulate cortex. Together these cerebral organswork together unconsciously to coordinateinputs from the senses and to generate sub-

    jective feelings and emotional states that in-fluence subsequent cognition and behavior

    (for a detailed account of the neurology of emotion, see Turner 1999, 2000).

    The outermost layer of the brain is theneocortex, also known as the neomammalianbrain (Panksepp 1998). This consists of anouter layer of gray matter mostly given overto the conscious processing of sensorystimuli. The neocortex has four basic sectorsreplicated symmetrically across two hemi-spheres of the brain. At the back, the occipi-tal lobes do visual processing. Along the topand sides, the parietal lobes focus on move-

    ment, orientation, and calculation. Behindthe ears, the temporal lobes deal with soundand speech; and the lower part of the frontallobes are devoted to taste and smell. The toppart of the frontal lobes is known as the pre-frontal cortex.

    The foregoing organization is common toall mammals, although different lobes maybe more or less developed in different spe-cies. Dogs, for example, have a better senseof smell than humans, and consequently

    Neomammalian (neocortex)

    Mammalian (limbic system)

    Reptilian (basal ganglia)

    Amygdala

    Figure 5. The Triune Structure of the Human Brain

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    17/29

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    18/29

    1 81 81 81 81 8 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    quick cost-benefit calculation and decided itwas worth the expenditure of energy to jumpout of the way to avoid being run over bythe car. Given what we now know about howthe brain works, however, this account of human action is completely incorrect(Goleman 1995; LeDoux 1996; Panksepp1998). By the time the rational brain has re-ceived the information about the oncomingthreat, the emotional brain has alreadyspurred your body into action and you will

    have begun to jump out of the way. Yourheart will be pounding, and your breathingwill have accelerated. If you later try to ex-plain your behavior, you may say it all hap-pened so fast that you acted without think-ing.

    But the action was a result of thinking; itwas unconscious thinking undertaken in thelimbic system. At the same time that thethalamus sent the visual and auditory infor-mation to the cortex for high-order process-ing, it also dispatched it along a low road

    to the amygdala, for what LeDoux (1996)calls quick and dirty processing. In con-trast to the high road through the neocor-tex, this lower route is shorter and faster.The amygdala immediately recognizes thethreat to survival posed by the oncoming carand sends a message to the hypothalamus tospur the body into immediate action.Adrenaline is released, your heart rate rises,your muscles contract, and in a split secondyou jump out of the way.

    Laboratory studies indicate that informa-tion reaches the amygdala about a quarter of a second before it reaches the prefrontal cor-tex. Thus, the emotional brain perceives thedanger and acts before the rational brainknows what is happening. In this case, hu-man behavior actually derived from theworkings of the older mammalian brainrather than the newer, more rational, butmuch slower neomammalian brain: If yourfrontal cortex were severed from the rest of

    your brain, thus disabling your rational fac-ulties, you would still jump out of the wayof the car.

    In explaining human behavior it is thusimportant to distinguish between rationalityand rationalization. In the foregoing episode,a rational-theoretical account that explainsthe act of jumping out of the way as the re-sult of a rational cost-benefit decision is in-correct. This explanation is a rationalizationof behavior whose trajectory had alreadybeen determined in the emotional brain. The

    limbic system was the locus of action, notthe prefrontal cortex. An innate feature of the rational brain, however, is to rationalizeevents and behaviors. Thus, even though thebehavior, jumping away from the car,stemmed from prerational cognition, we feelcompelled to tell ourselves a logical storyabout what happened.

    The power of rationalization has beendemonstrated in a famous experiment inwhich women were presented with a series

    Amygdala(Emotional Brain)

    (Rational Brain)

    Response

    Sensory Cortex

    ThalamusStimulus

    Somatic

    Frontal Cortex

    Figure 6. Schematic Representation of the Cognitive Processing of Emotion

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    19/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 1 91 91 91 91 9

    of nylon stockings and asked to select thepair they liked best (Gazzaniga 1992). Whenasked why they had made their selection, allsubjects provided a lucid account referringto differences in quality, texture, color, size,workmanship, or styleeven though thestockings were, in fact, identical. Accordingto one neuroanatomist, we like to think of . . . action as the result of a rational decision,but it is really just the fulfillment of an im-pulse (Carter 1998:41).

    Rational and Emotional Memory

    Not only does the emotional brain offer hu-man beings a second, parallel system of per-ception, it also provides a second memorysystem in which emotional states and feel-ings are recorded independently from infor-

    mation is consciously stored in the neocor-tex (Carter 1998; LeDoux 1996). That emo-tional memory exists is illustrated by thecase of a woman who, as a result of braindamage, had lost all ability to create newmemories. Every time her doctor saw her hehad to reintroduce himself. One day, how-ever, he held a pin in his hand before he in-troduced himself. When the woman shook his hand, she recoiled in pain as the pinpunctured her skin. The doctor then left theroom and after a while came back. Once

    more he offered his hand in introduction. Al-though the woman did not recognize him,she refused to shake his hand. She could notsay why; she just wouldnt do it:

    [The doctor] was no longer just a man . . .but had become a stimulus with a specificemotional meaning. Although the patient didnot have a conscious memory of the situa-tion, subconsciously she learned that shak-ing [the doctors] hand could cause herharm, and her brain used the stored infor-mation, this memory, to prevent the unpleas-antness from occurring again. (LeDoux1996:181)

    As mentioned before, the brain is highlylateralized, and in most people the left sideis devoted to analytic, logical, verbal think-ing, and the right side to emotional process-ing. That is, the left neocortex is used for ra-tional thought whereas the right amygdala isemployed for emotional processing (thereare right- and left-side amygdalas, like allbrain organs except the putamen). Each eye

    receives and transmits information to bothsides of the brain at once: The right side of both eyes sends information to the right sideof the brain and the left side of both eyessends information to the left side. Brain re-searchers have developed special contactlenses that refract incoming light to one sideof the retina or the other, so that quite liter-ally, the left half of the brain does not knowthat the right side sees and vice versa. In oneexperiment, LeDoux, Wilson, and Gazzaniga(1977) showed disturbing images of peoplebeing thrown into flames to subjects wear-ing right-refracted lenses, which channeledthe information to the emotional brain butnot to the rational brain. Afterward the sub-

    jects had no dec larative memory of whatthey had seenthey could only describe avague awareness of light and flashingyet

    they felt quite upset and disturbed. Althoughthey could not say why, they no longer likedthe experimenter or felt comfortable in hispresence.

    Such unconscious learning reflects a basicbut powerful psychological process identi-fied long ago by the Russian physiologistIvan Pavlov (1927). In his experiments onclassical conditioning , an unconditionedstimulus (in the above example, a pin prick)is paired with a conditioned stimulus (thedoctors hand) to generate an unconditioned

    response (pulling away from the pin prick).As a result of the pairing, the conditionedstimulus (the doctors hand) becomes asso-ciated with the unconditioned stimulus (thepin prick) in the subjects memory. Becausethe memory is stored in the amygdala, how-ever, it is not conscious, representing whatLeDoux (1996) calls an implicit memory.

    Implicit memories exist independently of and apart from explicit verbal memories andcontribute an underlying emotional valenceto the information that the neocortex stores.

    It is thus a basic feature of human cognitionthat all perceptions and memories have bothan implicit and an explicit content. Explicitcontent is laid down in conscious memory bythe hippocampus and stored in the cerebralcortex, whereas implicit content is filtered bythe thalamus and stored unconsciously in theamygdala. Indeed, it is only because objec-tive perceptions carry different emotionalvalences that we are even able to use ratio-nality to choose between them (Turner 2000).

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    20/29

    2 02 02 02 02 0 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    Implicit memories are created through thepairing of stimuli with hard-wired emotionalor motivational states. Once an implicitmemory has been created and stored, it is re-markably durable and difficult to eliminate.

    For our purposes, unconditioned stimulimay be divided into appetites and emotions.The former are internal conditions within thebody that send messages to the brain to in-stigate particular survival-related behaviors.Classical conditioning was first studied us-ing an appetite (hunger) in which Pavlovsdogs were conditioned to salivate in re-sponse to a bell, which sounded just beforefood was presented. In the past few years,scientists have identified specific neuralstructures, pathways, and transmitters asso-ciated with basic appetites such as hunger,thirst, tiredness, lust, playfulness, and curi-

    osity (Carter 1998; LeDoux 1996; Panksepp1998).

    There also appear to be specific neuralassemblies associated with the fundamentalhuman emotions such as fear, anger, dis-gust, nurturing, and liking (Panksepp 1998).Although some of the neurological mecha-nisms (such as fear) are better worked outthan others (such as disgust), all appear tobe mediated through specific neural struc-tures within the limbic system, thus creat-ing the possibility for classical conditioning

    by systematically pairing external stimuliwith emotional reactions to generate im-plicit memories that are stored in theamygdala. I believe that such unconsciousmemories created by intentional or inad-vertent conditioning lie behind much hu-man social behavior.

    THE LIMITS OF RATIONALITY

    My review of human evolution and cogni-tion yields six basic facts which, I believe,

    bring to question our overreliance on ratio-nality in explaining human behavior. (1)Emotionality preceded rationality in humanevolution. Our ancestors lived in structuredsocial communities grounded in emotionlong before they developed rational facul-ties. (2) The ability to evaluate potentialcosts and benefits of hypothetical actionsand to use these evaluations in planning forthe futurethe essence of what most theo-retical models mean by rationality

    emerged very late in human evolution. Wehave only possessed the neurological equip-ment for such mental operations for less than3 percent of our time on earth. (3) Once weacquired the physiological capacity for ratio-nal thought, it took additional time to de-velop the mental devices necessary for ratio-nal cognition and analysis. Although ana-tomically modern human beings appeared150,000 years ago, symbolic thoughtevolved a full 100,000 years later than that.(4) It took another 45,000 years for spokenlanguages to be systematized in writing, cre-ating the possibility for the external storageof information and the emergence of an in-cipient rational culture. (5) Even after theseartifacts had been invented, it took another5,000 years for them to be inculcated withina majority of human beings, offering for the

    first time the possibility of a mass societybased on rationality. (6) And finally, despitethe recent flowering of rationality, evolutionhas bequeathed us a cognitive structure withtwo mentalitiesone emotional and one ra-tional. While these two processors are inter-connected, emotional cognition precedes ra-tional cognition in evolutionary time and inreal time, and feedback between the two issuch that emotional traffic into the rationalbrain dominates that going in the reverse di-rection.

    These six facts lead me to the inescapableconclusion that human decisions, behaviors,and social structures cannot be modeledsolely as a function of rationality. My read-ing of human evolution and cognition sug-gests that rationality, rather than being adominant and deep-rooted force in humanaffairs, is quite recently arrived and ratherfragile. To the extent that rationality mani-fests itself in social life, it is only because of cultural practices and cognitive habits thathave been painstakingly acquired over mil-

    lennia and deliberately passed on to succeed-ing generations. Emotionality remains astrong and independent force in human af-fairs, influencing perceptions, coloringmemories, binding people together throughattraction, keeping them apart through ha-tred, and regulating their behavior throughguilt, shame, and pride. By failing to theo-rize emotion and by ignoring interactionsbetween rational and emotional cognition,sociologists derive an incomplete and mis-

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    21/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 2 12 12 12 12 1

    leading view of human social behavior. As Isuggest in the following examples, a seriousconsideration of the nature and working of the emotional brain has much to offer soci-ology.

    The Origin of Norms and Values Sociology used to explain behavior as stem-ming from norms and values. While theseconcepts have not disappeared from the dis-cipline, the thrust of recent theorizing hasbeen to explicate their rational foundationsand see them as fundamentally logical pro-cesses (see Becker 1996; Coleman 1990).Work on subjective social meanings has in-creasingly been left to our friends in anthro-pology, who unfortunately have taken a diveoff a postmodernist cliff. From my point of

    view, however, the study of norms and val-ues should really be about interactions be-tween social structure and the emotionalbrain, and particularly about how familiesand communities act to create implicitmemories that link behaviors, places, ob-

    jects, experiences, and thoughts to subjec-tive emotional states, thus shaping future ra-tional behavior.

    The way that parents teach values to chil-dren, for example, implicitly recognizes thecognitive structure pictured in Figure 6 (see

    page 18), where emotional cognition pre-cedes rational discrimination and where,owing to the asymmetry of neural connec-tions (indicated by the heavier line goingfrom the amygdala to the prefrontal cortex),the emotional brain is in a better position toinfluence the rational brain than vice versa.Consider, for example, thoughts about sex.Although thinking about sex naturally leadsto the release of endorphins in the humanbrain to create pleasurable feelings, parentswho wish their children to avoid early sexual

    contact endeavor to pair sexual stimuli andsymbols with negative emotions such asshame, guilt, and even pain to create a con-ditioned response that will cause the child,in adolescence, to remain chaste. If the con-ditioning is successful, the emotional brainwill send out negative, inhibiting messagesto the cortex when confronted with sexualstimuli. Of course, such conditioning carriesthe risk that it maybe all too successful,leading to sexual dysfunction in adulthood.

    Consumer Markets

    Neoclassical economics assumes that tastesand preferences are given and that peopleenter markets to satisfy them through the ra-tional process of utility maximization (for arecent attempt to move beyond this simplifi-cation, see Becker and Murphy 2000). Al-though professors of economics may acceptthese assumptions and use them to derivecomplicated behavioral models, their col-leagues in business schools do not. On thecontrary, professors of marketing for yearshave sought to teach students specificallyhow to influence preferences, tastes, andmotivations through advertising and publicrelations efforts. It has been years since theoverlords of Madison Avenue have tried toappeal to our rational brains in marketing

    consumer products.For example, what is the rational connec-

    tion between an entity labeled the Swedishbikini team and a popular brand of beer?Objectively there is none, of coursetheimage is not intended to appeal to the ratio-nal brain. By systematically and repeatedlypairing the beer with attractive blond womenin bikinis, the TV ad seeks to create a condi-tioned response among young mentheprime demographic category of beer con-sumers. If the conditioning is successful

    and in the course of any sporting event thepairings will surely be relentlessthe sightof the relevant six-pack (the conditionedstimulus) will trigger an implicit memory of the endorphins released by the sight of vo-luptuous Swedish women to generate a feel-ing of attraction toward the product that willlead to its purchase.

    Influencing Political Behavior

    One frequently hears the disparaging epithet

    social engineering applied to liberals whoadvocate using government power to redis-tribute resources in society. In truth the realsocial engineers are todays political con-sultants. These individuals, mostly conserva-tive, apply the methods of social science tomanipulate the emotional brain and thusshape perceptions of political actors andideas. They pair their candidate or positionwith positive emotional stimuli while link-ing opposing figures and ideas with negative

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    22/29

    2 22 22 22 22 2 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

    stimuli (Jamieson 1996). Given the inherentsurvival value of emotional reactions such asfear and ragewhich prepare an organismto flee or fightnegative emotions tend tobe more powerful as conditioning stimulithan positive emotions, which yields agreater prevalence of negative ads in politi-cal campaigns. Attack ads would probablyalso predominate in commercial advertisingwere it not for libel laws.

    A persistent question is why do candidatescontinue to rely so heavily on negative ad-vertising when surveys repeatedly show thatvoters disapprove of and dislike the tactic?The simple reason is that the attack adswork. Although voters express a rational dis-approval of negative campaigning, theiremotional brains nonetheless pay attentionto negative material, and the pairing of the

    negative emotions they arouse with candi-dates and political positions shifts votes inthe expected direction by creating implicitmemories lodged in the amydgala that latershape rational cognition.

    Increasingly political consultants use themethods of social sciencesurveys and fo-cus groupsnot to hone rational argumentsbut to identify emotional triggers that can bepaired with people and platforms to createimplicit memories within the emotionalbrain. The 1994 Republican Contract with

    America, for example, was less a coherentpolitical program than a list of 8 vagueprinciples and 10 contradictory legislativeproposals that surveys and focus groups hadrevealed would appeal to different segmentsof the electorate. Their votes were thusstitched together to eke out a majority inthe house of representatives. What modernpolitical consultants have done is simply toroutinize and refine the tactics of panderingthat astute politicians have naturally em-ployed for generations (see Berezin 1997;

    Emirbayer and Goldberg 2001).

    Fear, Prejudice, and Stereotyping

    Increasingly, we have become a fearful soci-ety, harboring a variety of apprehensions andsuspicions about people and events that arecompletely out of proportion with the actualrisks in society (Davis 1998; Glassner 1999).These fears reflect rampant fear condition-ing achieved inadvertently by the media and,

    sadly, often intentionally by political actorsin our image-driven postmodern world. As aspecies, we are programmed to pay close at-tention to violent and threatening events. Inthe natural world, violence is rarebutwhen it occurs our brains instantly tune in.Fear-inducing stimuli proceed directly to theamygdala and unleash a variety of somaticand emotional reactions that focus our atten-tion and put us on high alert before our ra-tional brain has had a chance to act (Carter1998; LeDoux 1996; Panksepp 1998). Eventhough our rational brain may quickly dis-cern that there is no particular threat, thechemicals released into the blood stream(e.g. adrenaline) persist for some time andyield extended feelings of excitation andarousal (Goleman 1995; LeDoux 1996).

    Television programmers and movie pro-

    ducers have long understood the inherent at-traction of violent images, and over the yearsthey have steadily ratcheted up the incidenceand severity of violence in a variety of me-dia, from movies to the internet. Possibly themost important escalation of violence hasbeen on television news. Whereas seriouscrime dropped by 3 percent through the1980s and early 1990s, and while the mur-der rate fell by 9 percent and overall crimi-nal victimization dropped by 21 percent,news coverage of crime by the three major

    networks doubled and the reporting of mur-ders tripled (Freeman 1994; Zucchino 1994).In a typical news night in 1995, 29 percentof news time was devoted to violence andwell over half to crime, war, or disaster(Klite, Bardwell, and Setzman 1995). AsOrfield (1997) put it, Stations are pushedtoward crime coverage because it representsattention-grabbing material while requiringlittle or no labor costs spent on backgroundresearch and because crimes sensationalismgarners high Nielsen ratings in a competitive

    market (p. 23).Thus, despite constant or declining crimerates in the United States, Americans are ex-posed, albeit vicariously, to more crime andviolence than ever before, and consequentlyin a safer world they feel more vulnerableand threatened. What is important sociologi-cally, however, is the potential for condition-ing inherent in the repeated association of the emotional responses of fear and ragewith people, events, and actions that regu-

  • 8/12/2019 Clase 1.1 a Brief History of Human Society the Origin and Role of Emotion in Social Life

    23/29

    THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY THE ORIGIN AND ROLE OF EMOTION IN HUMAN SOCIETY 2 32 32 32 32 3

    larly appear on the screen. As Glassner(1999) documents in his study of the cul-ture of fear, Americans have come to har-bor a host of irrational fears, not just of crime, but of dangerous drugs, monstermoms, killer kids, mutant microbes, planecrashes, and road rage.

    Probably the most important fear condi-tioning that is accomplished by TV program-mers, however, is the implicit associationbetween violence and minorities, especiallyAfrican Americans. The repeated pairing of minorities with fearful circumstances ontelevision cannot help but create highlynegative and largely unconscious memoriesassociated with groups such as blacks andHispanics, and the existence of these im-plicit memories can only serve to strengthenracial and ethnic stereotyping and perpetu-

    ate racism (Glassner 1999). The existence of such manufactured fears and racist feelingsalso provide politicians with powerful emo-tional levers they can use to mobilize thewhite electorate (Edsall and Edsall 1991).

    The most notorious example, but certainlynot the only one, is the famous Willie Hortoncommercial during the 1988 presidentialcampaign, in which the natural human ab-horrence to violence was systematicallypaired with Governor Michael Dukakis via aclearly racialized image. The commercial

    was powerful and effective not only becauseit tapped deep into the fear system hard-wired in the brain but also because it drewupon racialized images established throughpast fear conditioning. The frequent deliber-ate use of racist imaging for short-term po-litical gain is certainly one of the most dis-graceful practices to emerge in modern poli-tics, whether in the United States, in theBalkans, or in Rawanda.

    The Effects of Urbanism

    For my last example of how emotion influ-ences human affairs, I return to the fact thatwe are on the verge of becoming, for the firsttime, a fully urbanized society. Increasinglyhuman beings will not only live in cities butthose cities will be very large, and the larg-est among them will be in poor countries of the Third World. As a result, among both de-veloped and developing societies, povertywill increasingly be urbanized and geo-

    graphically concentrated. Within nations, thebulk of the poor will be housed in large ur-ban agglomerations, and within these areasthe poor will increasingly concentrate inpoor neighborhoods, thus driving the spatialconcentration of poverty to new heights(Massey 1996).

    Sociologists have long been fascinatedwith the influence of urbanism on social life.A classic statement was offered by Wirth(1938), who argued that the size, density,and heterogeneity of industrial cities wouldundermine social cohesion to promote indi-vidual alienation and social malaise. Al-though subsequent research has failed toconfirm his hypothesis, (Fischer 1975, 1982,1995), that didnt stop a second round of theorizing about the pathological effects of population density based on animal models

    (Calhoun 1962; Christian 1963). Once again,however, the expected relationship failed tomaterialize (Carnahan, Gove, and Galle1974; Gove and Huges 1983; Winsborough1970).

    More recent work, however, has confirmeda clear relationship not between populationdensity and social maladies but between theconcentration of poverty and delete