33
Once again about the science-policy interface

Once again about the science-policy interface

  • Upload
    cato

  • View
    30

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Once again about the science-policy interface. Q R A. Open risk management: overview. Human-human interface. There are really interesting new interfaces for transmitting information from person to person: Facebook: How are you? Wikipedia: What is thing X? Opasnet: What should we do? - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: Once again about the science-policy interface

Once again about the science-policy interface

Page 2: Once again about the science-policy interface

Open risk management: overview

QRA

Page 3: Once again about the science-policy interface

Human-human interface

• There are really interesting new interfaces for transmitting information from person to person:

– Facebook: How are you?– Wikipedia: What is thing X?– Opasnet: What should we do?

• A universal interface for communication about decisions and decision support is urgently needed.

• When that problem is solved, the communication problem between science and policy is solved as well.

There is no need for a separate science-policy interface.

Page 4: Once again about the science-policy interface

Introduction to probability theoryJouni Tuomisto

THL

Page 5: Once again about the science-policy interface

Probability of a red ball

• P(x|K) = R/N, – x=event that a red ball is picked– K=your knowledge about the situation

Page 6: Once again about the science-policy interface

Probability of an event x

• If you are indifferent between decisions 1 and 2, then your probability of x is p=R/N.

p

1-p

Red

x does not happen

x happens

White ballDecision 1

Red ball

Decision 2

Prize

100 €

0 €

100 €

0 €

Page 7: Once again about the science-policy interface

The meaning of uncertainty

– Uncertainty is that which disappears when we become certain.

– We become certain of a declarative sentence when (a) truth conditions exist and (b) the conditions for the value ‘true’ hold.– (Bedford and Cooke 2001)

– Truth conditions:– It is possible to design a setting where it can

be observed whether the truth conditions are met or not.

Page 8: Once again about the science-policy interface

Different kinds of uncertainty– Aleatory (variability, irreducible)– Epistemic (reducible), actually the difference

only depends on its purpose in a model.– Weights of individuals is aleatory if we are

interested in each person, but epistemic if we are interested in a random person in the population.

– Parameter (in a model): should be observable!

– Model: several models can be treated as parameters in a meta-model

Page 9: Once again about the science-policy interface

Different kinds of uncertainty: not really uncertainty

– Ambiguity: not uncertainty but fuzziness of description

– Volitional uncertainty: “The probability that I will clean up the basement next weekend.”– Uncertainties about own actions cannot be

measured by probabilities.

Page 10: Once again about the science-policy interface

What is probability?– 1. Frequentists talk about probabilities only when

dealing with experiments that are random and well-defined. The probability of a random event denotes the relative frequency of occurrence of an experiment's outcome, when repeating the experiment. Frequentists consider probability to be the relative frequency "in the long run" of outcomes.[1]

– 2. Bayesians, however, assign probabilities to any statement whatsoever, even when no random process is involved. Probability, for a Bayesian, is a way to represent an individual's degree of belief in a statement, or an objective degree of rational belief, given the evidence.

– Source: Wikipedia

Page 11: Once again about the science-policy interface

Significance and confidence

• Significance level: – The probability of some aspect of the data, given H is

true.

• Probability:– Your probability of H, given data.

• Confidence:– Probability that the interval includes θ (θ is given).

• Probability:– Probability that θ is included in the interval (data is

given).

Page 12: Once again about the science-policy interface

Positions of Bayesian approach

• Statistics is the study of uncertainty.

• Uncertainty should be measured by probability.

• Data uncertainty is so measured, conditional on the parameters.

• Parameter uncertainty is similarly measured by probability.

• Inference is performed within the probability calculus, mainly by Bayesian rule.

Page 13: Once again about the science-policy interface

Probability and conditional probabilities

• The totality of possible states of the world

P(A)

P(B)

Page 14: Once again about the science-policy interface

Probability rules

• Rule 1 (convexity):– For all A and B, 0 ≤ P(A|B) ≤ 1 and P(A|A)=1.– Cromwell’s rule P(A|B)=1 if and only if A is a logical

consequence of B.

• Rule 2 (addition): if A and B are exclusive, given C, – P(A U B|C) = P(A|C) + P(B|C).– P(A U B|C) = P(A|C) + P(B|C) – P(A ∩ B|C) if not exclusive.

• Rule 3 (multiplication): for all A, B, and C,– P(AB|C) = P(A|BC) P(B|C)

• Rule 4 (conglomerability): if {Bn} is a partition, possibly infinite, of C and P(A|BnC)=k, the same value for all n, then P(A|C)=k.

Page 15: Once again about the science-policy interface

Binomial distribution

– You make n trials with success probability p. The number of successful trials k follows the binomial distribution.– Like drawing n balls (with replacement) from

an urn and k being red.

– P(n,k|p) = n!/k!/(n-k)! pk (1-p)(n-k)

Page 16: Once again about the science-policy interface

Example

– You draw randomly 3 balls (with replacement) from an urn with 40 red and 60 white balls. What is the probability distribution for the number of red balls?

Page 17: Once again about the science-policy interface

Answer

– P(n,k|p) = n!/k!/(n-k)! pk (1-p)(n-k)

– 0 red: 3!/0!/3! *0.40*(1-0.4)3-0 – = 1*1*0.63 = 0.216– 1 red: 3!/1!/2! *0.41*(1-0.4)3-1 = 0.432– 2 red: 3!/2!/2! *0.42*(1-0.4)3-2 = 0.288– 3 red: 3!/3!/0! *0.43*(1-0.4)3-3 = 0.064

Page 18: Once again about the science-policy interface

Binomial distribution (n=6) P(•|p,n)

0 1 2 3 4 5 6

0.000

0.100

0.200

0.300

0.400

0.500

0.600

0.700

0.800

0.900

1.000

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

0.25

0.3

0.35

0.4

0.45

0.5

0.55

0.6

0.65

0.7

0.75

0.8

0.85

0.9

0.95

1

Page 19: Once again about the science-policy interface

Binomial distribution: likelihoods P(k|•,n)

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1

0.000

0.100

0.200

0.300

0.400

0.500

0.600

0.700

0.800

0.900

1.000

01

23

45

6

p(k red balls|theta)

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

theta=probability of single red ball

number of red balls

Page 20: Once again about the science-policy interface

Bayes’ theorem

– P(θ|x) = P(x|θ) P(θ) / P(x)– Proof:

– P(θ, x) = P(θ|x)P(x) = P(x|θ) P(θ)

– P(x) is often difficult to determine, but it is independent of θ and thus a constant over θ. Therefore:

– P(θ|x) ~ P(x|θ) P(θ) (proportionality)

Page 21: Once again about the science-policy interface

Bayes with words

• Likelihood: P(x|θ)

• Prior: P(θ)

• Posterior: P(θ|x)

• Posterior ~ Prior*Likelihood

Page 22: Once again about the science-policy interface

Getting rid of nuisance factors

– P(θ,α|x) = P(x ,α |θ) P(θ ,α) / P(x ,α)– P(θ|x) = ∫P(θ ,α |x) dα

Page 23: Once again about the science-policy interface

Example of Bayes’ rule with balls

– You draw randomly 3 balls (with replacement) from an urn with 40 red and 60 white balls. What is the probability distribution for the number of red balls?

– How do you update your prior if you draw three red balls in row?– Probability of 3 red given prior = likelihood =0.43

= 0.064– Prior = 0.4– Posterior=P(θ|R) = P(R|θ) P(θ) / P(R)

=0.064*0.4/0.064=0.4 !!

Page 24: Once again about the science-policy interface

Why doesn’t the probability change with new data?

– Because the prior is not uncertain, although it is a probability.

– P(p=0.4)=1, P(p<>0.4)=0

– Therefore, it is unaffected by any data, even if you get, say, five red balls in row P=0.45=0.010.

– You talk about your unlikely results with the guy who sold the urn to you. He replies: ”Did I say it has less red balls? Maybe it was more red balls. I really don’t remember, but the ratio is 40:60 for sure.”

– How does this change your model?

Page 25: Once again about the science-policy interface

New Bayes model with uncertain prior: red ball drawn

Guy was right in the first place? Yes

Guy was right? No

Sum

Next ball is red

0.2 0.3 0.5

Next ball is white

0.3 0.2 0.5

Sum 0.5 0.5 1

P(Y|R)=P(R|Y) P(Y)/P(R)

=0.4*0.5/(0.4*0.5+0.6*0.5) =0.2/0.5 = 0.4

Page 26: Once again about the science-policy interface

New Bayes model with uncertain prior: second ball is red

Guy was right in the first place? Yes

Guy was right? No

Sum

Next ball is red

0.16 0.36 0.52

Next ball is white

0.24 0.24 0.48

Sum 0.4 0.6 1

P(Y|R)=P(R|Y) P(Y)/P(R)

=0.4*0.4/(0.4*0.4+0.6*0.6)=0.16/0.52=4/13≈0.308

Page 27: Once again about the science-policy interface

New Bayes model with uncertain prior: third ball is red

Guy was right in the first place? Yes

Guy was right? No

Sum

Next ball is red

1.6/13 5.4/13 7/13

Next ball is white

2.4/13 3.6/13 6/13

Sum 4/13 9/13 1

P(Y|R)=P(R|Y) P(Y)/P(R)

=0.4*(4/13)/((1.6+5.4)/13) = 1.6/7 = 8/35≈0.229

Page 28: Once again about the science-policy interface

Conclusion from the red ball study

– We are not modelling the reality directly; we are modeling our understanding of reality.

– It might be useful to think of the Bayes rule as a 2*2 table.

– The principle is the same, even if there are more than 2 rows or columns.

– The principle is the same, even if there are more than two dimensions in the table.

Page 29: Once again about the science-policy interface

Bayes’ rule in diagnostics

– Imagine there is a clinical test for narcolepsy with 0.99 sensitivity and 0.99 specificity.

– A man was worried about his 6-year-old daughter who got the swine flu vaccination. He took the daughter to a private laboratory for the test.

– Now he comes to you with the daughter. The test result is positive.

– Does the daughter have narcolepsy?

Page 30: Once again about the science-policy interface

Narcolepsy diagnostics?

– What is sensitivity?– N(true positive)/N(disease) =P(test+|disease)

– What is specificity?– N(true negative)/N(healthy) =P(test-|healthy)

Page 31: Once again about the science-policy interface

Narcolepsy diagnosticsNarcolepsy? Yes

Narcolepsy? No

Sum

Data is positive

0.00099 0.01 0.01099

Data is negative

0.00001 0.989 0.98901

Sum 0.001 0.999 1

P(N|t+)=P(t+|N) P(N)/P(t+)

=0.99*0.001/0.01099 ≈ 0.0901

Page 32: Once again about the science-policy interface

Narcolepsy: importance of anamnesis. sensitivity=specificity=0.95

Narcolepsy? Yes

Narcolepsy? No

Sum

Data is positive

0.0856 0.0455 0.1311

Data is negative

0.0045 0.8644 0.8689

Sum 0.0901 0.9099 1

P(N|t+)=P(t+|N) P(N)/P(t+)

=0.95*0.0901/0.1311 ≈ 0.6528

Page 33: Once again about the science-policy interface

Narcolepsy: importance of negative anamnesis. Sens.=spec.=0.95

Narcolepsy? Yes

Narcolepsy? No

Sum

Data is positive

0.0856 0.0455 0.1311

Data is negative

0.0045 0.8644 0.8689

Sum 0.0901 0.9099 1

P(N|t-)=P(t-|N) P(N)/P(t-)

=0.05*0.0901/0.8689 ≈ 0.0052