400
THE INFLUENCE OF ONLINE VISUAL MERCHANDISING ON CONSUMER EMOTIONS: MODERATING ROLE OF CONSUMER INVOLVEMENT DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Young Ha, M.S. * * * * * The Ohio State University 2006 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Professor Sharron J. Lennon, Advisor Professor Leslie Stoel Professor Susan Zavotka Professor Michael Browne Advisor College of Human Ecology

Osu 1135134346

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Osu 1135134346

THE INFLUENCE OF ONLINE VISUAL MERCHANDISING ON CONSUMER EMOTIONS: MODERATING ROLE OF CONSUMER INVOLVEMENT

DISSERTATION

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the

Graduate School of The Ohio State University

By

Young Ha, M.S.

* * * * *

The Ohio State University 2006

Dissertation Committee:

Approved by

Professor Sharron J. Lennon, Advisor Professor Leslie Stoel Professor Susan Zavotka Professor Michael Browne

Advisor

College of Human Ecology

Page 2: Osu 1135134346

Copyright by Young Ha

2006

Page 3: Osu 1135134346

ii

ABSTRACT

The current research consists of two studies. The purpose of Study 1 was to

investigate 1) the effects of peripheral cues presented in the apparel websites on

consumers’ emotions (pleasure and arousal) under low situational involvement, 2) the

influence of product involvement (personal relevance of clothing products) as a

moderator of the relationship between peripheral cues and emotions under the low

involvement situation, 3) the influence of emotions on consumer response behaviors, and

4) the mediating effects of emotions on the relationship between peripheral cues and

response behaviors (purchase intention and approach behaviors). A convenience sample

of 157 female college students participated in an online experiment using a mock website

for Study 1. In a between-subjects experiment with one factor (peripheral cues) having

two levels (presence vs. absence), Study 1 found: 1) main effects for peripheral cues on

consumer pleasure and arousal, 2) a stronger effect for peripheral cues on pleasure and

arousal for consumers with a low level of clothing product involvement rather than with a

high level of clothing product involvement, 3) direct effects of consumer emotions on

purchase intention and approach behaviors, and 4) indirect effects of peripheral cues on

purchase intention and approach behaviors via consumer pleasure and arousal.

The purpose of Study 2 was to examine 1) the effects of web cues – central cues

(product-related stimuli) and peripheral cues (stimuli not directly related to the product) –

Page 4: Osu 1135134346

iii

on emotions, 2) the influence of emotions on consumer response behaviors (satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors), 3) the effects of situational involvement

(e.g., purchase situation vs. browsing situation) as a moderator of the relationship

between web cues and emotions (pleasure and arousal), and 4) the mediating effects of

emotional states on the relationship between web cues and response behaviors. A

random sample of 1634 female undergraduate students participated in an online

experiment using a mock website for Study 2. Employing a 2 (situational involvement:

high vs. low) x 2 (central cues: medium amount vs. high amount) x 2 (peripheral cues:

presence vs. absence) between subjects’ factorial design, Study 2 revealed: 1) direct

effects for central cues on pleasure and for peripheral cues on arousal, 2) the influence of

pleasure and arousal on satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors, 3) the

effects for central cues on consumer pleasure and arousal under high situational

involvement (purchasing situation) and effects for peripheral cues on consumer emotions

under low situational involvement (browsing situation), and 4) the mediating effects of

consumer emotions on the relationship between web cues and consumer response

behaviors.

The findings of Study 1 and Study 2 1) provide valuable information for apparel

online retailers developing successful apparel online stores using various web cues that

may attract both online browsers and purchasers, 2) extend online visual merchandising

research by empirically investigating how various web cues presented in apparel websites

Page 5: Osu 1135134346

iv

influence consumer emotions that in turn affect consumer response behaviors under

different involvement conditions, and 3) combine the ELM and the S-O-R paradigms to

explain and predict consumer responses to online visual merchandising under different

involvement conditions.

Page 6: Osu 1135134346

v

Dedicated to my parents and my loving husband, Jin Nam

Page 7: Osu 1135134346

vi

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I first and foremost thank my advisor Dr. Sharron J. Lennon from the bottom of

my heart for her constant support, encouragement, assistance, and advice, which made

this dissertation possible. Her enthusiasm and inspiration have always stimulated me to

take steps forward in research and teaching. Throughout my whole life, she has been the

best mentor. Without her endless support during my graduate program at this university,

the completion of my degree was not possible.

I would also like to express my special thanks to my committee members for their

thoughtful and valuable suggestions for my dissertation. My heartfelt thanks go to Dr.

Leslie Stoel for her sincere support during the past five years. She has supported me to

make steady progress in my research as well as teaching. Her incessant guidance and

encouragement allowed me to gain confidence in my ability to complete my dissertation.

I also wish to express my appreciation to Dr. Susan Zavotka for her valuable suggestions

and feedback on my dissertation research, particularly on the website design. My

gratitude is extended to Dr. Michael Browne for his invaluable lectures on factor analysis

and structural equation modeling and for his helpful comments on my dissertation. His

brilliant insight and lessons were exceptional.

I am greatly indebted to Dr. Nancy A. Rudd for her thoughtful concern and help

during my graduate program. Her continuous guidance and comments on my teaching

Page 8: Osu 1135134346

vii

were extremely precious for my career development. My appreciation is also extended to

Dr. Gong-Soog Hong who has provided helpful advice and support for my teaching. My

thanks also go to Dr. Loren Geistfeld for his constant encouragement for my teaching and

work.

I wish to express my thanks to my best friends Jihye Park at Iowa State University,

Jungmin Ha, and Juhee Kim in Korea for their genuine support and love. I am also

thankful to my colleagues who have contributed to my dissertation in numerous ways:

Sejin Ha, Hunju Im, Hyejeong Kim, Junghwan Kim, Minjeong Kim, Wisuk Kwon,

Jiyoung Lim, Jeesun Park, and Minjung Park. My special gratitude goes to Hyunju Im

and Jeesun Park for their special companionship and assistance during the last quarter of

my graduate study. I would also like to thank Ann Glenn for sharing joys and difficulties

with me while teaching.

My sincere thanks go to my parents and sister in Korean and my parents-in-law

for their endless love and encouragement through my life. I owe perhaps the greatest

thanks to my husband, Jin Nam, who has given me his unwavering support for my work.

The completion of my dissertation would have been unfeasible without his continuing

support and patience.

Page 9: Osu 1135134346

viii

VITA

February 14, 1974..………………………………… Born in Seoul, Korea

February 1998..…………………………………….. B.A. Hanyang University Major: Textiles and Clothing Seoul, Korea September 1999 – August 2002…………………… M.S. The Ohio State University Major: Textiles and Clothing Columbus, OH March 2001 – August 2005 ……………………….. Graduate Teaching Associate

Department of Consumer Sciences The Ohio State University Columbus, OH

July 2004 – September 2004……………………….. Graduate Research Associate Department of Consumer Sciences The Ohio State University Columbus, OH September 2005 – December 2005 …………………Lecturer

Department of Consumer Sciences The Ohio State University Columbus, OH

Page 10: Osu 1135134346

ix

PUBLICATIONS

Research Publication 1. Ha, Y., & Stoel, L. (2004). Internet apparel shopping behaviors: The influence of general innovativeness. International Journal of Retail and Distribution Management, 32 (8), 377-385. 2. Ha, Y., & Lennon, S. (2005). Effects of satisfaction with local shopping condition in rural areas on Internet apparel shopping behavior. Abstract published in Proceeding of the International Textiles and Apparel Association, available at: www.itaaonline.org

3. Lennon, S., Ha, Y., Johnson, K., Damhorst, M. L., Jasper, C., Lyons, N. (2005). Online shopping for apparel, food, and home furnishings products as a form of outshopping. Abstract published in Proceeding of the International Textiles and Apparel Association, available at: www.itaaonline.org.

4. Ha, Y., & Lennon, S. (2005). Effects of apparel website atmospherics on consumer emotions and purchase intention. Abstract published in Proceeding of 2005 Korean Society of Clothing and Textiles and the Japan Society of Home Economics [CD-ROM].

5. Ha, Y., & Lennon, S (2005). Rural consumers’ Internet apparel shopping: Innovativeness and beliefs. Abstract published in Proceedings of American Collegiate Retailing Association [CD-ROM].

6. Ha, Y., & Lennon, S. (2005). Effects of attractive model and image movement in apparel websites on judgments of clothing fashionability and purchase intention. Abstract published in the first Global Symposium for Consumer Sciences (GSCS), available at: www.consumersciences.org

7. Ha, Y., Kwon, W., & Lennon, S. (2004). Online visual merchandising: A cross national approach. Abstract published in Proceeding of the International Textiles and Apparel Association, available at: www.itaaonline.org.

Page 11: Osu 1135134346

x

8. Ha, Y. & Lennon, S. (2003). Rural Ohio consumers’ Internet apparel shopping: Innovativeness and perceptions of the Internet and Internet shopping. Abstract published in Proceeding of the International Textiles and Apparel Association, available at: www.itaaonline.org.

9. Lee, K. H., Ha, Y., & Read, E. (2002). Improving reference citing skills and pre-class learning activities via image assignments in a beginning aesthetics course. Abstract published in Proceeding of the International Textiles and Apparel Association, available at: www.itaaonline.org.

FIELD OF STUDY

Major Field: Human Ecology Area of Specialization: Textiles and Clothing Minor Field: Quantitative Psychology

Page 12: Osu 1135134346

xi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

Abstract ............................................................................................................................... ii

Dedication ........................................................................................................................... v

Acknowledgments.............................................................................................................. vi

Vita................................................................................................................................... viii

List of Table................................................................................................................... xviii

List of Figure................................................................................................................... xxii

Chapters

1. Introduction............................................................................................................. 1

1.1. Introduction...................................................................................................... 1 1.2. Problem Statement ........................................................................................... 4 1.3. Purpose of the Study ........................................................................................ 6 1.4. Significance of the Study ................................................................................. 7 1.5. Definition of Terms.......................................................................................... 9

2. Literature Review.................................................................................................. 11

2.1. Background Literature ................................................................................... 12 2.1.1. Visual Merchandising in Store........................................................ 12 2.1.2. Visual Merchandising in Apparel Store.......................................... 13 2.1.3. Visual Merchandising in General Websites.................................... 16

Page 13: Osu 1135134346

xii

2.1.4. Visual Merchandising in Apparel Websites ................................... 17 2.2. Theoretical Framework.................................................................................. 21 2.2.1. S-O-R Paradigm.............................................................................. 21 The Effects of Emotions on Consumer Behaviors:

The Effects of Store Environment ................................................ 23 The Effects of Emotions on Consumer Behaviors: The Effects of Site Environment................................................... 25

2.2.2. Elaboration Likelihood Model: Involvement and Persuasion ........ 27 Websites as Persuasion ................................................................. 27 Central and Peripheral Routes ...................................................... 28 Involvement .................................................................................. 28 Product Involvement..................................................................... 29 Situational Involvement ................................................................ 31

2.2.3. Applying the S-O-R Paradigm and Involvement into Internet Shopping Context....................................................... 34

2.3. Proposed Models and Hypotheses ................................................................. 39 2.3.1. Main Study 1................................................................................... 41

The Effects of Peripheral Cues on Emotional States .................... 41 The Moderating Effect of Product Involvement between S-O ..... 42 The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors............. 44 The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R ............ 45

2.3.2. Main Study 2................................................................................... 48 Part I: The Effects of Web Cues on Emotional States .................. 48 Part II: The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors ...................................................................... 52 Part III: The Moderating Effects of Situational Involvement between S-O.................................................................................. 55 Part IV: The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R.................................................................................. 59

3. Pilot Studies .......................................................................................................... 64 3.1. Pilot Study 1................................................................................................... 66 3.2. Pilot Study 2................................................................................................... 71 3.3. Pilot Study 3................................................................................................... 76 3.4. Content Analysis............................................................................................ 79

Page 14: Osu 1135134346

xiii

4. Main Study 1......................................................................................................... 84 4.1. Method ........................................................................................................... 85 4.1.1. Research Design and Experimental Manipulations ........................ 85 4.1.2. Instrument Development................................................................. 87 4.1.3. Procedure ........................................................................................ 88 4.2. Analyses and Results ..................................................................................... 90 4.2.1. Description of Participants.............................................................. 90 4.2.2. Manipulation Check........................................................................ 92 4.2.3. Dependent Variables....................................................................... 93 Emotional States: Pleasure and Arousal ....................................... 93 Purchase Intention......................................................................... 94 Approach Behaviors...................................................................... 95 4.2.4. Assessment of Measurement Properties ......................................... 97 Convergent Validity.................................................................... 102 Unidimensionality....................................................................... 104 Discriminant Validity.................................................................. 105 Assessment of Reliability ........................................................... 108 Model Specification .................................................................... 110 Data Screening ............................................................................ 110 4.2.5. Hypothesis Testing........................................................................ 114 Hypothesis 1................................................................................ 114 Hypothesis 2................................................................................ 117 Hypotheses 3 and 4 ..................................................................... 126 Model fit.......................................................................... 126 Hypothesis 5................................................................................ 134 The direct effects of peripheral cues on

response behaviors .......................................................... 135 The mediating effects of emotional states ...................... 136

5. Main Study 2....................................................................................................... 138 5.1. Method ......................................................................................................... 139 5.1.1. Research Design and Experimental Manipulations ...................... 139 5.1.2. Instrument Development............................................................... 143 Emotional States ......................................................................... 143 Satisfaction.................................................................................. 144 Purchase Intention....................................................................... 144

Page 15: Osu 1135134346

xiv

Approach Behavior ..................................................................... 144 Perceived Amount of Information .............................................. 145 Perceived Quality of Web Appearance....................................... 145 Situational Involvement .............................................................. 146 Demographic Information and Prior Experiences ...................... 147 5.1.3. Website Development................................................................... 147 Apparel Stimuli Preparation for the Websites ............................ 148 Mock Website Development for the Main Study 2 .................... 148 5.1.4. Recruitment of Participants........................................................... 150 5.1.5. Experiment Procedure for Study 2................................................ 151 5.2. Analysis and Results .................................................................................... 153 5.2.1. Description of Participation .......................................................... 153 5.2.2. Manipulation Check...................................................................... 158 Situational Involvement .............................................................. 158 Central Cues................................................................................ 159 Peripheral Cues ........................................................................... 160 5.2.3. Dependent Variables..................................................................... 163 Emotional States: Pleasure and Arousal ..................................... 163 Satisfaction.................................................................................. 164 Purchase Intention....................................................................... 164 Approach Behaviors.................................................................... 164 5.2.4. Assessment of Measurement Properties ....................................... 166 Convergent Validity.................................................................... 169 Unidimensionality....................................................................... 171 Discriminant Validity.................................................................. 172 Assessment of Reliability ........................................................... 175 Testing Invariance of Measurement Model over Groups ........... 177 Model Specification .................................................................... 180 Data Screening ............................................................................ 184 5.2.5. Hypotheses Testing....................................................................... 185 Part One ...................................................................................... 187 Hypothesis 1.................................................................... 187 Hypothesis 2.................................................................... 194 Part Two...................................................................................... 200 Hypothesis 3.................................................................... 200 Part Three.................................................................................... 208

Hypothesis 4.................................................................... 208 Part Four...................................................................................... 224 Hypothesis 5.................................................................... 224

Page 16: Osu 1135134346

xv

6. Discussion and Conclusions ............................................................................... 244 6.1. Discussion .................................................................................................... 245 6.1.1. Findings from Study 1 .................................................................. 245 The Effects of Peripheral Cues on Emotional States .................. 245 The Moderating Effect of Product Involvement between S-O ... 248 The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors........... 251 The Mediating Effects of Emotional States Between S-R.......... 254 6.1.2. Findings from Study 2 .................................................................. 258 The Effects of Web Cues on Emotional States........................... 260 The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors........... 263 The Moderating Effect of Situational Involvement

between S-O................................................................................ 266 The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R .......... 269

6.2. Implications.................................................................................................. 274 6.2.1. Managerial Implications for Study 1 ............................................ 274 6.2.2. Managerial Implications for Study 2 ............................................ 277 6.2.3. Theoretical Implications ............................................................... 285 6.3. Limitations ................................................................................................... 288 6.3.1. Homogeneity of the Sample Population ....................................... 288 6.3.2. Simulated Situational Involvement............................................... 289 6.3.3. Limited Product Category............................................................. 289 6.4. Recommendations for Future Studies.......................................................... 290 List of References ........................................................................................................... 292 APPENDICES ................................................................................................................ 303 Appendix A: Pilot Study 1.................................................................................. 303 Recruitment Letter .................................................................................. 304 Questionnaire for Low Involvement....................................................... 305 Questionnaire for High Involvement ...................................................... 307 Appendix B: Pilot Study 2 – Websites ............................................................... 310

Page 17: Osu 1135134346

xvi

Appendix C: Pilot Study 2 – Apparel Stimuli .................................................... 313 Thirty Four Pairs of Pants Rated in the Pilot Study 2............................. 314 Two Items Selected for Pilot Study 4 ..................................................... 317 Five Items Selected for the Main Experiment ........................................ 318 Appendix D: Pilot Study 3 – Website and Questionnaire................................... 319 Appendix E: Main Study 1 – Website: Manipulations for Peripheral Cues ....... 323 Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence .................................................. 325 Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence.................................................. 326 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence .............................................. 327 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence.............................................. 328 Peripheral Cues: Presence – Flashing Image and Colorful Icons ........... 329 Appendix F: Main Study 1 – The Questionnaire ................................................ 330 Appendix G: Main Study 2 – Invitation Email................................................... 333 Invitation Email for High Involvement................................................... 334 Invitation Email for Low Involvement ................................................... 336 Appendix H: Main Study 2 – Instruction Page................................................... 338 Instruction Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence ......................................... 339 Instruction Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence......................................... 340 Appendix I: Main Study 2 – Scenario Page........................................................ 341 Scenario Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence, Involvement – Low........... 342 Scenario Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence, Involvement – Low .......... 343 Scenario Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence, Involvement – High .......... 344

Scenario Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence, Involvement – High.......... 345 Appendix J: Main Study 2 – Main Page ............................................................. 346 Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence .................................................. 347 Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence.................................................. 348 Appendix K: Main Study 2 – Product Page........................................................ 349 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence,

Central Cues – Medium Amount ............................................................ 350 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence, Central Cues – High Amount.................................................................. 351 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence, Central Cues – Medium Amount ............................................................ 352 Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence, Central Cues – High Amount.................................................................. 353 Peripheral Cues: Presence – Flashing Image and Colorful Icons ........... 354 Product Page: Size Chart – Peripheral Cues – Absence ......................... 355 Product Page: Size Chart – Peripheral Cues – Presence......................... 356

Page 18: Osu 1135134346

xvii

Appendix L: Main Study 2 – Purchase Page for High Involvement .................. 357 Purchase Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence ............................................ 358 Purchase Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence............................................ 359 Appendix M: Main Study 2 – The Questionnaire............................................... 360 Appendix N: Standardized Residual Evaluated in Main Study 1 ....................... 364 Appendix O: Data Screening for Normality Test ............................................... 366 Appendix P: Standardized Residual Evaluated in Main Study 2........................ 368 Appendix Q: Data Screening for Normality Test ............................................... 370 Appendix R: Human Subjects Approval Form for Study 1................................ 372 Appendix S: Human Subjects Approval Form for Study 2 ................................ 374

Page 19: Osu 1135134346

xviii

LIST OF TABLE Table Page 2.1. Summary of hypotheses in Study 1 and Study 2 .................................................. 63 3.1. Frequencies of web cues listed in Pilot Study 1 ................................................... 70 3.2. Descriptive statistics and reliabilities of apparel items selected in

Pilot Study 2 for Study 1 and Study 2 .................................................................. 74

3.3. Web cues listed in Pilot Study 2 that participants reported paying attention to when they purchased clothing online................................................. 75

3.4. The popular apparel websites participants listed in Pilot Study 2 ........................ 75 3.5. The extent to which web cues are product related rated in Pilot Study 3............. 78 3.6. Coding categories used in Content Analysis and frequencies of the

product related web cues presented in apparel websites....................................... 82 3.7. Number of product related web cues available in the 15 apparel

websites analyzed in the Content Analysis........................................................... 83 4.1. Demographic profile of participants ..................................................................... 91 4.2. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables ........................................................ 96 4.3. Final measurement items for each of four latent constructs ............................... 101 4.4. Factor loadings, t-values, and item reliability for convergent validity ............... 103 4.5. Chi-square difference tests for discriminant validity.......................................... 106 4.6. Correlations and confidence intervals for discriminant validity......................... 107 4.7. Composite reliability and AVE of latent constructs ........................................... 109

Page 20: Osu 1135134346

xix

4.8. Descriptive statistics for clothing involvement items......................................... 118 4.9. Mean differences for pleasure influenced by peripheral cues and product

involvement interaction ...................................................................................... 124 4.10. Mean differences for arousal influenced by peripheral cues and product

involvement interaction ...................................................................................... 124 4.11. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit for

Hypotheses 3 and 4 ............................................................................................. 129 4.12. Multiple regression analysis for purchase intention in Hypothesis 5 ................. 137 4.13. Multiple regression analysis for approach behaviors in Hypothesis 5 ............... 137 5.1. The eight treatments in Study 2 .......................................................................... 142 5.2. Demographic descriptions of participants .......................................................... 156

5.3. Participants’ prior Internet usage and online browsing/purchasing

Experiences ......................................................................................................... 157 5.4. Descriptive statistics for manipulation check items in Study 2 .......................... 162 5.5. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables ...................................................... 165 5.6. Final measurement items for each of five latent constructs................................ 168 5.7. Factor loading, t-values, and item reliability for convergent validity................. 170 5.8. Chi-square difference tests for discriminant validity.......................................... 173 5.9. Correlations and confidence intervals for discriminant validity......................... 174 5.10. Composite reliability and AVE of latent constructs ........................................... 176 5.11. The results of testing the invariance of the measurement model........................ 179 5.12. Summary of the model fit for the proposed model in Hypothesis 1................... 189

Page 21: Osu 1135134346

xx

5.13. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 1 ................................. 193 5.14. Summary of the model fit for the proposed model in Hypothesis 2................... 195 5.15. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 2 ................................. 199 5.16. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in Part 2 .......... 202 5.17. Summary of the model fit for Model 1 (high involvement) in

Hypothesis 4a...................................................................................................... 211 5.18. Summary of the model fit for Model 2 (low involvement) in

Hypothesis 4a...................................................................................................... 212 5.19. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 4a ............................... 216 5.20. Summary of the model fit for Model 1 (high involvement) in

Hypothesis 4b...................................................................................................... 218 5.21. Summary of the model fit for Model 2 (low involvement) in

Hypothesis 4b...................................................................................................... 219 5.22. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 4b ............................... 223 5.23. Summary of the model fit for the model tested the direct effects of

central cues on response behaviors ..................................................................... 228 5.24. Estimated means of satisfaction, purchase intention, and

approach behaviors ............................................................................................. 229 5.25. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in

Hypothesis 5a...................................................................................................... 231 5.26. Estimated intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors in Hypothesis 5a ................................................................................. 234

5.27. Summary of the model fit for the model tested the direct effects of peripheral cues on response behaviors................................................................ 237

Page 22: Osu 1135134346

xxi

5.28. Estimated means of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors ............................................................................................. 238

5.29. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in Hypothesis 5b...................................................................................................... 240

5.30. Estimated intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors in Hypothesis 5b................................................................................. 243

6.1. Summary of hypotheses testing results in Study 1 ............................................. 257 6.2. Summary of manipulations used in Study 2 ....................................................... 259 6.3. Summary of hypotheses testing results in Study 2 ............................................. 273

Page 23: Osu 1135134346

xxii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page 2.1. The S-O-R paradigm............................................................................................. 23 2.2. The S-O-R paradigm in the online shopping context ........................................... 38 2.3. The proposed model in the main study 1 .............................................................. 47 2.4. Part one of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 1 and 2)........................................ 51 2.5. Part two of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 3a to 3f)....................................... 54 2.6. Part three of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 4a to 4b) .................................... 58 2.7. Part four of the hypothesized model for Study 2 (Hypotheses 5a and 5b) ........... 61 2.8. The proposed model for Study 2........................................................................... 62 3.1. A summary of pilot tests ....................................................................................... 65 4.1. Model specification for Study 1.......................................................................... 112 4.2. The proposed model in Study 1 .......................................................................... 113 4.3. Effects of peripheral cues and product involvement on pleasure ....................... 123 4.4. Effects of peripheral cues and product involvement on arousal ......................... 125 4.5. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for

Hypotheses 3 and 4 ............................................................................................. 130

4.6. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Hypotheses 3 and 4 ....................................................................................... 131

5.1. Model specification for Parts 1 and 3 in Study 2................................................ 182

Page 24: Osu 1135134346

xxiii

5.2. Model specification for Part 2 and Part 4 in Study 2.......................................... 183 5.3. The proposed model in Study 2 .......................................................................... 186 5.4. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for

Hypothesis 1........................................................................................................ 190

5.5. Completely standardized parameter estimates for Hypothesis 1 ........................ 191 5.6. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for

Hypothesis 2........................................................................................................ 196

5.7. Completely standardized parameter estimates for Hypothesis 2 ........................ 197 5.8. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Part 2.............. 203 5.9. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the proposed model

for Part 2 ............................................................................................................. 204

5.10. Unstandardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4a ....... 213 5.11. Completely standardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for

Hypothesis 4a...................................................................................................... 214

5.12. Unstandardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4b ....... 220 5.13. Completely standardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for

Hypothesis 4b...................................................................................................... 221

5.14. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5a ................ 232 5.15. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the model for

Hypothesis 5a...................................................................................................... 233

5.16. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5b................ 241 5.17. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the model for

Hypothesis 5b...................................................................................................... 242 6.1. The results of Hypotheses 1a and 1b .................................................................. 247

Page 25: Osu 1135134346

xxiv

6.2. The results of Hypotheses 2a and 2b .................................................................. 250 6.3. The results of Hypotheses 3 and 4 ...................................................................... 253 6.4. The results of Hypothesis 5................................................................................. 256 6.5. The results of Hypotheses 1 and 2 in Part 1........................................................ 262 6.6. The results of Hypothesis 3 in Part 2 .................................................................. 265 6.7. The results of Hypothesis 4 in Part 3 .................................................................. 268 6.8. The results of Hypothesis 5 in Part 4 .................................................................. 272

Page 26: Osu 1135134346

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

1.1. Introduction

Internet users are increasing in the world as well as in the U.S. According to

Computer Industry Almanac Inc, about 934 million were global Internet users in 2004

and global users are projected to reach 1.35 billion in 2007 (“Population Explosion,”

2005). The rate of American households with computers increased from 51% in 2000 to

61.8% in 2003 and 54.6% of American households had Internet access in 2003 (“A

Nation Online: Entering the Broadband Age,” 2004). U.S. Internet population reached

185.6 million in 2005 (“Population Explosion,” 2005) and 140.6 million are considered

active users who go online at least once a month (Burns, 2005a). In addition, remarkable

changes were made in the relative distribution of the various types of Internet access in

the U.S. American households with high-speed Internet or broadband Internet access

increased from 9.1% in 2001 to 19.9% in 2003 while dial-up Internet connections

decreased by 12.7% during the same period (“A Nation Online: Entering the Broadband

Age,” 2004). These statistics show a change in Internet connections from dial-up service

Page 27: Osu 1135134346

2

to high-speed Internet connections in American households. Internet users with high-

speed Internet connections also engaged in more types of online activities in the areas of

entertainment, banking, purchasing products, or gathering information than those with

dial-up connections (“A Nation Online: Entering the Broadband Age,” 2004).

As the Internet population grows, the Internet influences the shopping patterns of

in-home shoppers. Internet shopping has grown rapidly, and Internet shoppers can find

product information easily using the Internet (Ward and Lee, 2000). Compared to 15%

of the global average, the U.S. led the world with the highest proportion of online

shoppers at 32% of all Internet users (Greenspan, 2002). U.S. online sales reached

$141.4 billion in 2004 and are predicted to approach $172.4 billion in 2005, representing

a growth of 22% from 2004 (Burns, 2005b). Eighty-one percent of adults with Internet

access have purchased online since they started using the web (“Statistics: US Online

Shoppers,” 2002). Thirty-two percent of Internet users have used the Internet for

shopping (Greenspan, 2002) and apparel is one of the most frequently purchased online

merchandise categories (Greenspan, 2004). U.S. online apparel sales during the 2003

holiday season reached more than $3.7 billion—representing a 40% increase over the

same period in 2002—followed by toys and video games with $2.2 billion (Rush, 2004).

Visual merchandising is an important strategic tool in fashion marketing (Lea-

Greenwood, 1998). Visual merchandising as the total store environment including

merchandise presentation, store design and image, mannequins, props and materials,

lighting, graphics, and signage influences product sales and store image in the retail

setting (Cahan & Robinson, 1984; Diamond & Diamond, 2003). Previous research found

that store environments (e.g., lighting, color, and music) influenced consumers’

Page 28: Osu 1135134346

3

emotional states such as pleasure and arousal that in turn influenced consumer response

behaviors (Baker, Levy, & Grewel, 1992; Bitner, 1992; Buckley, 1991; Donovan &

Rossiter, 1982; Donovan, Rossiter, Marcoolyn, & Nesdale, 1994; Spies, Hesse, & Loesch,

1997). Higher levels of pleasure and arousal induced by store environments increased

purchase intention (Babin, Hardesty, & Suter, 2003; Fiore & Kimle, 1997) and approach

behaviors (Donovan & Rossiter, 1982). This indicates the importance of visual

merchandising in retail stores for increasing purchase intention and approach behaviors.

As the population of Internet shoppers grows, the effects of visual merchandising

in websites have gained attention from researchers. Previous studies showed that site

designs and merchandising attract customers and influence their satisfaction with Internet

shopping (Harris, 1998; Szymanski & Hise, 2000). More attractive and pleasurable site

stimuli may influence consumers’ purchase decisions (Menon & Kahn, 2002). When a

website creates pleasure for consumers, there is a positive effect on approach behaviors

(Menon & Kahn, 2002). Website designs make consumers return to the websites (Rice,

1997). In addition, ease of site navigation (Rice, 1997; Szymanski & Hise, 2000) and

entertaining experiences (Rice, 1997) make people enjoy and come back to the websites

later. Extensive and higher quality product information also affects consumers’

satisfaction in Internet shopping (Szymanski & Hise, 2000).

The inability to try on apparel products before purchase is a major concern for

consumers when purchasing apparel using an in-home method (Kim & Lennon, 2000;

Kwon, Paek, & Arzeni, 1991; Park, Lennon, & Stoel, 2005). Kim and Lennon (2000)

found that the amount of product and service information in a television shopping

program segment reduced perceived risk and increased purchase intention. Fabric and

Page 29: Osu 1135134346

4

color information and product details can also reduce perceived risk in the catalog

shopping context (Kwon et al., 1991). In an Internet shopping context, Then and Delong

(1999) suggested that more information regarding visual aspects of apparel products such

as a variety of images and of different product views can generate higher purchase

intention for consumers and in turn, increase sales for e-business. Viewing apparel

products in a variety of combinations can help consumers assess how they might look

wearing those items (Allen, 1999). Mix and match suggestions in apparel sites may also

increase consumers’ purchase intentions that in turn increase apparel sales on the Internet

(Allen, 2000; Then & Delong, 1999).

1.2. Problem Statement

Although the effects of website visual merchandising have gained attention from

previous researchers (Allen, 1999; Menon & Kahn, 2002; Szymanski & Hise, 2000; Then

& Delong, 1999), it is surprising that so little empirical research related to visual

merchandising in apparel websites has been conducted. Previous Internet apparel

shopping studies have focused on demographic issues (Goldsmith & Goldsmith, 2002;

Kim, Damhorst, & Lee, 2002), purchasers vs. browsers (Goldsmith & Goldsmith, 2002;

Lee & Johnson, 2002), and the effect of prior experiences with the Internet or Internet

shopping on Internet apparel shopping (Goldsmith & Goldsmith, 2002; Yoh & Damhorst,

2003).

The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996), developed

in the context of persuasion, may be a useful guide for research into online visual

Page 30: Osu 1135134346

5

merchandising. According to the ELM, central cues are defined as factors that influence

consumer attitudes under a high involvement condition and that require cognitive effort

to process (Petty et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). Consumers under the high

involvement condition focus on analysis of product relevant information such as the

message argument. Alternatively, peripheral cues are defined as factors that are

sufficient to produce an initial attitude change with minimal cognitive processing. In the

low involvement condition consumer attitudes are influenced by the persuasive peripheral

cues such as a pleasant environment or attractive sources (Petty et al., 1983; Petty &

Cacioppo, 1996). Based on the assumption that online purchasing is analogous to a

persuasion situation, conceptualizing web cues as central or peripheral may offer some

useful insights. Applying the ELM in Internet shopping context, Eroglu, Machleit, and

Davis, (2003) found that peripheral cues presented in apparel websites influence pleasure

and arousal that in turn affect consumer response behaviors in low situational

involvement (browsing situation). However, the effects of central cues available on

apparel websites on consumers’ emotional reactions and behavioral responses have not

been investigated.

In the retail setting, consumers examine apparel products using visual and tactile

senses. However, Internet shoppers can rely only on visual information available on the

screen (e.g., verbal descriptions and product images). Because apparel cannot be

physically experienced online, Internet shopping is a riskier way to purchase apparel

products than in-store shopping. Therefore, it is necessary to understand whether or not

certain types of web cues (both central cues and peripheral cues) on apparel websites

have an impact on consumers’ emotions (pleasure and arousal) that in turn influence

Page 31: Osu 1135134346

6

consumer response behaviors (e.g., satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors). In addition, situational involvement or product involvement may have a

significant effect on the relationship between different types of web cues and consumers’

emotional states. Consumers browsing the websites with a purchasing goal may be more

likely to attend to central cues (product related verbal and pictorial information) rather

than peripheral cues (background color, colorful icons, and flashing images), while those

without a purchasing goal may be more affected by peripheral cues. In other words,

highly involved consumers may be influenced by central cues whereas low involved

consumers may be affected by peripheral cues. Thus, it is also essential to investigate

how the effects of different web cues on emotional states are changed by the levels of

situational or product involvement.

1.3. Purpose of the Study

The purpose of Study 1 is to 1) investigate the effects of peripheral cues presented

in the apparel websites on consumers’ emotional states (pleasure and arousal) under low

situational involvement, 2) examine the influence of product involvement (personal

relevance of clothing products) as a moderator of the relationship between peripheral

cues and emotional states (pleasure and arousal) under the low involvement situation, 3)

assess the influence of emotional states on consumer response behaviors (purchase

intention, and approach behaviors), and 4) examine the mediating effects of emotional

states on the relationships between peripheral cues and response behaviors (purchase

intention and approach behaviors).

Page 32: Osu 1135134346

7

The purpose of Study 2 is to 1) examine the effects of web cues—central cues

(product-related stimuli) and peripheral cues (stimuli not directly related to the

product)— on emotional reactions, 2) assess the influence of emotional states on

consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors),

3) investigate the effects of situational involvement (e.g., purchase situation vs. browsing

situation) as a moderator of the relationship between web cues and emotional reactions

(pleasure and arousal); e.g., how central cues and peripheral cues influence consumers’

emotions under different levels of involvement, and 4) assess the mediating effects of

emotional states on the relationships between web cues and response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors).

1.4. Significance of the Study

The findings from this study will provide valuable information for apparel online

retailers. Since it is impossible to closely examine apparel products using the tactile

sense in an online context, consumers planning to buy apparel items from online retailers

may rely on verbal or pictorial cues (central cues) that describe an apparel item. Due to

the inability to inspect items on the Internet, more descriptive central cues (high amount

of verbal information, larger views with various angles, and complete mix and match

suggestions) may help consumers evaluate apparel products before making an online

purchase decision. More detailed verbal information, larger views from various angles

(front, back, side, and detail views), and complete mix and match suggestions may induce

more pleasure and arousal that in turn influence consumers’ satisfaction, purchase

Page 33: Osu 1135134346

8

intention, and approach behaviors. This effect is expected to be stronger for consumers

in the high situational involvement (e.g., purchasing situation) than in the low situational

involvement (e.g., browsing situation). For consumers browsing the websites without a

purchasing goal, peripheral cues such as the presence of background color, colorful icons,

and flashing images may have significant effects on consumers’ pleasure and arousal that

consequently influence consumers’ satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors.

Based on the results, this study will provide a comprehensive model that describes

the relationship among various web cues presented on apparel websites, emotional states

(pleasure and arousal) experienced while browsing the websites, and consumers’

response behaviors. The model will also explain how consumers in different situations

react to various web cues and how product involvement influences the relationship

between peripheral cues and consumer emotions. Therefore, the results of the study may

help online apparel retailers develop new strategies for visual merchandising of their

websites to attract various consumer groups in different situations or with different levels

of product involvement.

Page 34: Osu 1135134346

9

1.5. Definition of Terms

The following terms are used in this study and defined as follows:

1. Approach behaviors: In this study, approach behaviors are defined as desire to

shop or explore in the website or the likeability of the website.

2. Central cues: Central cues include both verbal and pictorial cues that could be

directly related to a purchasing goal (e.g., product related web cues and service

related web cues). In this study, central cues are manipulated by different

amounts of product related web cues.

3. Emotion: Emotion is defined as affective states that focus on pleasure and arousal

as expected reactions to various web stimuli that in turn influence consumer

response behaviors.

4. Internet browsing: Internet browsing is defined as information search activities on

the Internet without a particular purpose of product purchasing.

5. Internet purchasing or shopping: Internet purchasing or shopping is defined as an

online buying activity as a consequence of browsing the websites.

6. Peripheral cues: Peripheral cues include both verbal and pictorial cues that are not

directly related to the purchasing goal (e.g., brand logo, background color, icons,

and layout). In this study, peripheral cues are manipulated by the presence or

absence of background color, various colorful icons, and a flashing brand logo.

7. Product involvement: Product involvement is defined as the personal relevance of

clothing products in this study. It is more a permanent personal involvement than

situational involvement.

Page 35: Osu 1135134346

10

8. Product related web cues: Product related web cues are defined as the information

that describes an individual apparel item (e.g., product care information, fabric

information, color information, country of origin information, style information,

size information, fit information, larger views, mix and match, and price

information).

9. Purchase intention: In this study, purchase intention is defined as consumers’ plan

to buy apparel products from the websites. The decision may be affected by their

browsing or shopping experience in the apparel websites.

10. Satisfaction: In this study, satisfaction is defined as consumers’ behavioral

responses as a result of the fulfillment of their shopping or browsing experience at

the apparel website.

11. Service related web cues: Service related web cues are defined as the information

related to services provided from online stores such as delivery information,

contact information, customer services information, security information, return

policy information, and account information.

12. Situational involvement: Situational involvement is defined as personal relevance

for a specific situation. In this study, a purchasing situation is considered to

induce high situational involvement and browsing a website is considered to

induce low situational involvement (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Page 36: Osu 1135134346

11

CHAPTER 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

This chapter describes background literature, theoretical framework, and

hypotheses development for Study 1 and Study 2. In the first section, previous studies

addressing the effects of emotions induced by store or site environments on consumer

behaviors and reporting the effects of visual merchandising in retail stores and in the

websites are reviewed. In the second section of the chapter, the Stimuli-Organism-

Response paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974) and the Elaboration Likelihood Model

(Petty & Cacioppo, 1996) are discussed as theoretical frameworks for this study. In the

third section, hypotheses development is presented along with the proposed models for

Study 1 and Study 2.

Page 37: Osu 1135134346

12

2.1. Background Literature

2.1.1. Visual Merchandising in Store

According to textbook authors, visual merchandising is defined as “the

presentation of a store and its merchandise in ways that will attract the attention of

potential customers and motivate them to make purchases” (Diamond & Diamond, 2003,

p. 5). Before using the term, visual merchandising, display was used to describe the job

of a “window trimmer” who produced visual or artistic aspects of the merchandising

presentation (Cahan & Robinson, 1984; Diamond & Diamond, 2003). Today’s visual

merchandising emphasizes the total store environment including merchandise

presentation, store design and image, mannequins, props and materials, lighting, graphics,

and signage (Cahan & Robinson, 1984; Diamond & Diamond, 2003). Visual

merchandising is therefore concerned with both the visual and the marketing functions of

the store environment to enhance the store image and to increase sales (Cahan &

Robinson, 1984; Diamond & Diamond, 2003). Walters and White (1987) emphasized

that visual merchandising should arouse consumers’ interest about products and stimulate

them to buy more products. Using theme-oriented props in the retail store, visual

merchandising may increase mood which can influence purchasing (Tyreman & Walton,

1998). For example, Sears stores displayed men’s dress shirts with typewriters, globes,

and old law books to create the feeling of the professional office (Tyreman & Walton,

1998). Theme-oriented props help consumers understand how and/or where they will use

Page 38: Osu 1135134346

13

the products; the way the products are displayed influences consumers’ feeling about the

product (Tyreman & Walton, 1998).

In addition, visual merchandising must coordinate the entire merchandising, in-

store design, and space allocation (Walters & White, 1987). Sometimes, improper

elements incompatible with consumers’ expectations can hurt store image (Cahan &

Robinson, 1984; Walters & White, 1987). Thus, the quality of visual merchandising is

thought to be very important to increase sales as well as to enhance the image of the store

(Cahan & Robinson, 1984; Walters & White, 1987).

2.1.2. Visual Merchandising in Apparel Stores

Some researchers have also studied visual merchandising. Lea-Greenwood (1998)

emphasized the importance of visual merchandising as a strategic tool in fashion

marketing. Visual merchandising can attract consumers to enter stores and can

communicate brand image (Lea-Greenwood, 1998). Kerfoot, Davis, and Ward (2003)

investigated the effect of visual merchandising stimuli on consumers’ brand recognition,

liking for displays, browsing, and purchase intention in women’s retail apparel stores.

Through interviews they found a number of elements which influenced consumers’

perceptions of the apparel retail stores. The elements included merchandise color,

manner of presentation, awareness of fixtures, path finding, sensory qualities of materials

and the effects of lighting. The results showed that the coordination of merchandise color

influenced purchase intention. The use of a variety of colors was perceived as attractive

and appealing and it influenced respondents’ browsing tendency. In addition, four

Page 39: Osu 1135134346

14

different methods of presentation influenced respondents’ perceptions. Hanging

merchandise presentation was perceived as the most attractive presentation method

because it was visible, suggested mix and match items, and helped people visualize

outfits. Mannequin presentation also generated a positive perception in that it showed

entire designs and suggested how a garment might fit. However, a folding presentation

and the use of rails led to some negative perceptions in terms of difficulty of assessing.

Neatness of folded clothing made respondents hesitate to disturb the display. Also, the

tendency to browse and the perceptions of the quality of the store were related to path

finding. Path finding is “the provision of a clear route” and “noticeably affected

propensity to browse” (Kerfoot et al., 2003, p. 149). Path finding shows where to start

and provides a natural way to navigate in the store. Path finding is influenced by

merchandise density and display density. Respondents tended to perceive more browsing

and display space as indicative of better quality and more expensive brands. Sensory

quality of materials such as flooring, fixtures, and hangers and lighting also affected

respondents’ perceptions. The results showed that there was a significant relationship

between consumers’ perceptions of visual merchandising and the development of

approach and avoidance behavior (Kerfoot et al., 2003).

A recent study found significant effects for merchandise coordination on product

evaluation and purchase intention in the apparel retail store setting (Lam & Mukherjee,

2005). Well coordinated apparel items induced higher aesthetic response toward two

complementary products as a whole (enjoyable, nice-looking, pleasing, attractive, good

appearance, and beautiful) than poorly coordinated items. The study also revealed that

the social impressions (socially acceptable, fashionable, popular, high in status, desired

Page 40: Osu 1135134346

15

impression, and approved by others) of apparel items were influenced by the coordination

of apparel items. Well coordinated items conveyed a more positive impression than

poorly coordinated items. Aesthetic response and social impression had positive effects

on product evaluation (good, favorable, desirable, like, and very useful) that in turn

influenced consumers’ purchase intention for the product (Lam & Mukherjee, 2005).

Supporting the previous research (Kerfoot et al., 2003), the study suggests that well

coordinated apparel items may enhance consumers’ aesthetic response and product

evaluation and consequently, improve store/brand image and sales.

Sen, Block, and Chandran (2002) investigated the effect of window displays in

clothing stores on consumers’ shopping decisions such as store entry and product

purchase. The results showed that consumers looking for store image and product fit

information from window displays were more likely to enter a store than those looking

for merchandise, promotional, and fashion information from window displays. The study

suggests that window displays in clothing stores should present better product fit

information and convey store image to draw more customers into the store. However, in

terms of purchase behavior, consumers looking for fashion information and fit

information from displays tended to purchase products in the store, but consumers

looking for store image displays were less likely to buy products in the store. Results

indicate that consumers looking for product fit information (i.e., information that allows

consumers to assume the fit of the product with consumers’ physical or symbolic selves)

from window displays may enter the store to examine and purchase the products on

display while consumers looking for store image from displays may enter the store to

browse and gain additional information related to store image but may not purchase the

Page 41: Osu 1135134346

16

products (Sen et al., 2002). This result suggests that product fit information in displays is

the most important cue to draw more customers into a store as well as to make them

purchase in the store. Based on the results, Sen et al. (2002) recommend that clothing

retailers should use mannequins with idealized body types representing their target

consumers’ body types.

2.1.3 Visual Merchandising in General Websites

In the retail store, visual merchandising means floor layout, interior design,

signage, in-store promotion, and product mix that facilitate purchasing (Harris, 1998).

Applying this proven notion to the Internet environment can offer a completely new

standpoint for designing websites to be more profitable. Harris (1998) suggested some

ideas that give direction for applying those proven concepts in the retail store to website

designs. Instead of a floor plan and signage, online graphics, photos, and other design

elements can be used to attract customers to websites and to get them to the products.

Merchandise categories available on the homepage (e.g., the order of the list) may lead

customers in the right direction on the retail websites. In terms of display and music, a

large colorful photo image of the product and price presented right next to the image of

the product will attract customers’ attention and also music on the opening page can

create an exciting mood for the customers (Harris, 1998).

Rice (1997) researched website design factors that make customers keep returning

to a website. Based on a pretest, the survey finally had 12 questions that fell into two

areas: the evaluation of design and technology of the websites and the emotional

Page 42: Osu 1135134346

17

experience of the user during the visit. The survey also included a question about site

patronage intention and an open-ended question tapping the most important factors that

affect decisions to return or not return to the site. The results showed that making

websites with design a top priority was the most important factor to make people return

to the websites. The second was making people enjoy the websites: making it easy to

find what they were looking for and offering them a novel and entertaining experience.

The next factors in order were the quality of the organization/layout of the site, the degree

of uniqueness, ease of finding information, excitement, visual attractiveness, ease of

navigation, and the speed of the websites (Rice, 1997). The study offers some important

guidelines to make websites attract more attention from customers.

Szymanski and Hise (2000) studied determinants of customer satisfaction in

Internet shopping. The results showed that satisfaction with Internet shopping was

influenced by perceptions of site design and merchandising (including product assortment

and product information). More extensive and higher quality product information

affected consumers’ satisfaction in Internet shopping. Sites designed to be fast,

uncluttered, and easy to navigate played an important role in consumers’ satisfaction.

2.1.4. Visual Merchandising in Apparel Websites

For apparel related websites, the significance of the layout and design of the

websites has been emphasized by Then and Delong (1999) and Allen (2000). According

to Then and Delong, (1999), visual design on Internet apparel sites can be considered

analogous to retail store layout. Thus, the main goal for website design is similar to that

Page 43: Osu 1135134346

18

for store design in that both websites and stores want consumers to come in, to enjoy the

environment, and to purchase products.

Perceived risk related to the inability to try on apparel products before purchase is

a major concern for consumers when purchasing apparel in-home (Kim & Lennon, 2000;

Kwon et al., 1991; Park et al., 2005). Park et al. (2005) found that there was a negative

relationship between perceived risk and apparel purchase intention in Internet apparel

shopping. This means that if perceived risks are reduced in Internet apparel shopping,

then consumers’ purchase intentions will be increased. Kim and Lennon (2000) found

that the amount of product and service information was negatively related to perceived

risk and positively related to purchase intentions in a television shopping context.

Therefore, to reduce perceived risk and enhance purchase intentions in Internet shopping,

apparel websites should offer richer and more intensive product information using a

variety of sources of product presentation.

Then and Delong (1999) indicated that if Internet retailers offer more information

through the visual display of apparel products using a variety of images, then consumers

will purchase more apparel products in the Internet. Visual aspects of product

presentation such as images of the online product in its closest representation of end use,

displayed in conjunction with similar items, and from various angles such as front, back,

and side view can generate higher purchase intentions for consumers and in turn, increase

higher selling for e-business (Allen, 2000; Then & Delong, 1999).

Images including both static and kinetic graphics can make a website page look

more interesting (Rowley, 2002). Then and Delong (1999) also suggested that a three-

dimensional apparel display can be helpful in minimizing the uncertainties of apparel

Page 44: Osu 1135134346

19

shopping on the Internet. Apparel product presentation can be enhanced with 3D views

(Allen, 1999). Viewing apparel products in a variety of combinations can help

consumers examine how they might look wearing those items (Allen, 1999). Park et al.

(2005) tested the effects of product movement (kinetic image) in apparel websites on

mood, perceived risk, and purchase intention. The study found that people exposed to a

product in motion (kinetic image) tended to have more positive mood and greater

purchase intention than those exposed to the product not in motion (static image). The

results also showed that kinetic image reduced consumers’ perceived risk (Park et al.,

2005).

Apparel sites often have some mix and match suggestions to create a complete

look by combining pants, shirts, and accessories (Allen, 2000). J. Crew is the best

example of excellent merchandising in that they combine individual items to suggest a

complete look. Customers are not likely to purchase all those combined outfits but

seeing the suggested coordination may increase sales (Allen, 2000). Mix and match

suggestions for each item may increase consumers’ purchase intention in Internet

shopping (Then & Delong, 1999).

For apparel products, fabric and color information and product details are

considered important determinants of consumers’ response behaviors such as satisfaction,

site patronage, and purchase intentions. Kwon et al. (1991) suggested that to reduce

perceived risk for apparel products in catalog shopping, retailers should offer accurate

and complete pictures of products and describe products in detail especially for color,

texture, and fabric description. Fiore and Yu (2001) found that catalog pages with fabric

samples enhanced attitude toward the apparel product in comparison to those without

Page 45: Osu 1135134346

20

fabric samples. Inaccurate color information causes the loss of sales and increases

product returns and complaints in online apparel stores (Nitse, Parker, Krumwiede,

Ottaway, 2004). Consumers dissatisfied with apparel products delivered in a color that

was different from what was expected would not purchase products from online apparel

stores in the future (Nitse et al., 2004).

According to Park and Stoel (2002), more than two-thirds or more of the apparel

websites they analyzed provided some type of color information (21 out of 31 merchants

provided both visual and verbal color descriptions). However, sensory types of product

information were not really available in apparel websites. For example, fabric

construction (e.g., woven and knit), texture/fabric hand (comfortable, soft, and heavy),

and mix and match suggestions were offered by only about half or fewer of apparel

websites analyzed. In addition, only 55 % (17 out of 31) of the sites provided larger

views to see more detail and only one apparel site offered a 3D product rotation

presentation. Finally, this study suggested that sensory and experiential product

information should be added to apparel websites to increase sales (Park & Stoel, 2002).

Then and Delong (1999) indicated the importance of using human models for

apparel product presentation to show the natural drape of the garment on the human body.

Given choices, respondents chose mannequin display as the best, flat display as the

second, and sketches as the last for apparel product presentation on websites. This result

shows that if a human body is not available, then consumers will prefer mannequins, as

the best form for apparel product presentation. Thus, on apparel websites, a human

model, at least a mannequin, should be used for apparel product presentation to offer

better information to consumers and as a result, to improve their sales.

Page 46: Osu 1135134346

21

2.2. Theoretical Framework

2.2.1. S-O-R Paradigm

According to Mehrabian and Russell (1974), much of the research in

environmental psychology has focused on the effects of physical stimuli (e.g., things of

the everyday physical environment) on human emotions (e.g., pleasure, arousal, and

dominance, or ‘PAD’) and the effects of physical stimuli on a variety of behaviors (e.g.,

satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach-avoidance behavior). In terms of physical

stimuli, most research has relied on the sensory variables such as color, sound,

temperature, and texture. Consumer emotions are conceptualized as three dimensions:

pleasure, arousal, and dominance. Pleasure and arousal can be easily measured by self-

report or by observation of positive facial expressions. Emotional pleasure refers to the

degree to which a consumer feels happy, pleased, satisfied, contented, or hopeful while

arousal is consumer emotion that refers to the extent to which a consumer feels

stimulated, excited, or aroused. Dominance can be measured through verbal reports or by

observations of body posture or facial expression. Although the S-O-R paradigm

suggests three dimensions to measure consumer emotions, previous research has found

that dominance had only little or no effect on consumer behaviors (Donovan & Rossiter,

1982; Donovan et al., 1994) and therefore was not used in recent environmental research

(Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn; 2002; Sherman, Mathur, & Smith, 1997). In

addition, because participants will be controlled by instructions in an experimental study,

it is expected that dominance experienced by participants will not differ by various web

Page 47: Osu 1135134346

22

cues. Thus, in the present study pleasure and arousal will be used to measure consumer

emotions felt by people while browsing the websites.

As mediators, these emotional states cause various consumer response behaviors

(Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Physical stimuli (e.g., store design) in the environment

influence consumer emotions (e.g., pleasure and arousal) that serve as mediating

variables in determining a variety of consumer response behaviors such as satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors in a retail setting. A high level of pleasure

and arousal elicited by environmental stimuli in retail stores and in websites enhance

satisfaction (Eroglu et al., 2003; Machleit & Mantel, 2001; Spies, Hesse, & Loesch,

1997), purchase intention (Babin & Babin, 2001; Fiore et al., 2005; Spies et al, 1997),

and approach behaviors such as desire to explore and desire to shop (Eroglu et al., 2003;

Menon & Kahn, 2002).

Mehrabian and Russell (1974) proposed the theoretical framework with the

outline of the important variables that take place in most situations; it is called the Stimuli

(S)—Organism (O)—Response (R) paradigm (See Figure 2.1). This S-O-R paradigm has

been examined and developed by researchers applying it to the retail context (Baker et al.,

1992; Baker, Parasuraman, & Grewal, 2002; Bitner, 1992; Buckley, 1991; Donovan &

Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al., 1994; Spies et al., 1997), who found that the paradigm

worked well in retail situations. Applying the S-O-R model to an Internet shopping

context, a recent study (Eroglu et al., 2003) found that the effects of consumer emotions

as intervening variables between various web cues and consumer response behaviors in

an Internet retail setting are analogous to those in an in-store retail shopping context.

Emotions (pleasure and arousal) induced by atmospheric stimuli presented in the website

Page 48: Osu 1135134346

23

had a significant effect on consumer response behaviors such as satisfaction and approach

behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Stimulus Organism Response

Figure 2.1. S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974).

The Effects of Emotions on Consumer Behaviors: The Effects of Store Environment

Several researchers have examined the S-O-R model in retail store environments

and found that consumers’ emotions induced by environmental stimuli influenced

consumers’ response behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors. Higher levels of pleasure and arousal from the store environment enhanced

shoppers’ purchase intentions (Fiore & Kimle, 1997). Donovan and Rossiter (1982)

Environmental stimuli -- Things of the everyday physical environment --sensory cue (color, music, temperature)

Emotional Responses Pleasure Arousal Dominance

Behavioral Responses Approach-avoidance behavior

Page 49: Osu 1135134346

24

tested the S-O-R model in retail settings, especially focusing on three emotional

experiences—pleasure, arousal, and dominance—and suggested that this framework is a

good starting point for studying consumer response behaviors in the retail context. They

found that retail environmental stimuli influenced consumers’ emotions such as pleasure,

arousal, and dominance and these emotional experiences were powerful determinants of

consumer behaviors in the retail store. Pleasure induced by store stimuli such as lighting

and music had an effect on consumer behaviors including positive attitudes toward store

environment, enjoyment of shopping, intention to revisit the store, intention to spend, and

intention to browse more. In a later study, Donovan et al. (1994) extended Donovan and

Rossiter’s (1982) study in that the later one used a broader sample, measured emotions

during the shopping rather than after the shopping, and recorded the effects of the store

stimuli on actual shopping behaviors rather than behavioral intention. They found that

pleasure elicited by the store environment was a significant determinant of unplanned

time spent in the store and unplanned purchasing in the store (Donovan, et al., 1994).

Baker et al. (1992) examined various aspects of store atmospherics—ambient

cues and social cues—and their effects on the retail patronage decision based on the S-O-

R model. They found that ambient factors such as music and lighting significantly

influenced consumers’ pleasure for the low social factor (e.g., only one employee) but not

for the high social factor (e.g., three employees and greeting of one employee). Social

cues alone influenced consumers’ arousal in the store. These affective states—arousal

and pleasure—had a great impact on consumers’ purchase intentions (Baker et al., 1992).

Sherman et al. (1997) studied the effects of various stimuli (social factor, overall

image, design factor, and ambient factor) in apparel retail stores on consumer emotions

Page 50: Osu 1135134346

25

(pleasure and arousal) which in turn influence consumers’ purchasing behavior. The

results showed positive effects of social factors and design factors on pleasure and a

positive effect of ambient factors on arousal. Consumer emotions induced by a variety of

factors in the store environment enhanced the amount of money and time spent in the

store and the number of items purchased in the store (Sherman et al., 1997).

Color is one of the most important design elements to communicate style and

mood in retail store environments since it is the first thing a customer notices in the store

(Colborne, 1996). Exciting colors attract our eye and make a store look alive (Colborne,

1996). Color in retail stores appears to influence consumer emotions such as pleasure

and arousal (Bellizi & Hite, 1992; Crowley, 1993) that in turn influence consumer

behavioral intentions. Several studies have been conducted to examine the influence of

store colors on consumers’ shopping behaviors such as approach behavior and purchase

intention (Babin et al., 2003; Bellizi, Chrowley, & Hasty, 1983). Consumers’ evaluations

and excitement induced by the color of store environments were positively related to

store patronage intentions and purchase intentions (Babin et al., 2003).

The Effects of Emotions on Consumer Behaviors: The Effects of Site Environment

Based on the S-O-R model suggested by Mehrabian and Russell (1974), Menon

and Kahn (2002) examined the effects of consumers’ emotions, induced by atmospheric

stimuli in websites, on consumers’ later shopping behaviors. The results showed that if

the websites created pleasure for consumers, then there was a positive effect on approach

behaviors (e.g., browsing more, engaging in unplanned purchasing, and seeking out more

stimulating products). The study suggested some important insights for website

Page 51: Osu 1135134346

26

designers and Internet retailers. Accordingly, Internet retailers should be concerned

about consumers’ emotions stimulated by the initial website/page since the initially

encountered webpage and products may shape consumers subsequent shopping behaviors

(Menon & Kahn, 2002).

Eroglu et al. (2003) examined the S-O-R model suggested by Eroglu, Machleit,

and Davis (2001) in online store environments. They created two new apparel websites

(including t-shirts and sweatshirts with custom imprinting options), one only with high

task relevant cues and another with both low task relevant and high task relevant cues.

The two websites basically contained the same high task relevant cues but one website

added low task relevant cues such as a dark green (instead of black color) for the text, a

pale gray background for a sweatshirt with the logo on it (instead of no background),

photographs of the designers with their profiles, an affiliation graphic, and animated

Visa/Mastercard logo on the ordering page. Results showed that site atmospheric cues

(background color, text color, and other colorful graphics) influenced the level of

pleasure which in turn influenced consumer attitude. In addition, consumer emotions

(pleasure and arousal) as mediators had significant effects on response behaviors such as

satisfaction (including likelihood of revisit) and approach behaviors (intention to stay and

explore).

In the online apparel shopping context, consumers’ emotions elicited by product

presentation (Park et al., 2005) and image interactivity (Fiore, Jin, & Kim, 2005) also

influence consumer response behaviors. Park et al. (2005) found that a positive mood

induced by product movement (product views in motion) shown in apparel websites

influenced purchase intention. Fiore et al.’s (2005) study revealed that image

Page 52: Osu 1135134346

27

interactivity was a stimulating experience while browsing apparel websites and positively

influenced consumer emotions (pleasure and arousal) that consequently affected

consumers’ approach behaviors.

2.2.2. Elaboration Likelihood Model: Involvement and Persuasion

Websites as Persuasion

Advertisements primarily serve two basic roles: to inform and to persuade (Singh

& Dalal, 1999). In other words, advertisements provide information related to a

particular product or service that in turn persuades consumers to buy the product or

service. Websites as a mass medium resemble traditional advertisements in that they

perform the same fundamental roles (to inform and to persuade) (Joines, Scherer, &

Scheufele, 2003; Singh & Dalal, 1999). Websites are designed to provide information

and consequently to persuade consumers to purchase products from the websites. The

ability to purchase products is a unique advantage of websites (Joines et al., 2003). The

function of some websites is informational, while others have a commercial purpose and

sell products in addition to providing information. In both cases websites serve as a form

of advertising (Berthon, Pitt, & Watson, 1996; Joines et al., 2003; Singh & Dalal, 1999).

Using the Internet consumers can access and browse websites from any place at any time

and purchase products directly from commercial websites. Based on the assumption that

online purchasing is analogous to persuasion situations, the Elaboration Likelihood

Model (ELM) (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996) developed in the context of persuasion, may

provide some useful insights for research in online shopping contexts.

Page 53: Osu 1135134346

28

Central and Peripheral Routes

According to the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996), there are two different routes to

persuasion or attitude change—a central and a peripheral route. Under the central route

people’s attitude change will depend on cognitive processing while under the peripheral

route people’s attitude change will depend on minimal cognitive processing. Therefore,

central route processing tends to create more permanent attitude change than peripheral

route processing. The peripheral route is not very permanent nor very successful for

changing attitudes especially under high involvement conditions (Petty & Cacioppo,

1996).

Under the high involvement condition central cues such as the number of issue-

relevant arguments in the message influences attitude change but source credibility as a

peripheral cue has no significant effect. However, under the low involvement condition

attitude change is determined basically by peripheral cues such as attractiveness of the

source, but not by the number of issue-relevant arguments in the message. Persuasion

arises from careful thinking about the issue or message via the central route and

persuasion results from non-issue-relevant cues such as source attractiveness via the

peripheral route (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). The effects of different types of involvement

on persuasion have also been studied in the consumer behavior context (Zaichkowsky,

1986).

Involvement

Zaichkowsky (1986) categorized the antecedents of involvement into three

factors: person factors, stimulus factors, and situation factors. Personal factors include

Page 54: Osu 1135134346

29

one’s inherent needs, importance, interest, and values related to a particular object.

Different people may have inherently different levels of involvement for a particular

product (i.e., product involvement). Stimulus factors are related to the physical

characteristics of the stimulus such as differentiation of alternatives, source of

communication, and content of communication. Situational factors also influence the

levels of involvement. Different situational factors such as a purchasing vs. no

purchasing situation may have an impact on consumer’s level of involvement related to a

particular product. When a purchase is perceived as important, consumers may be

motivated to make a careful decision based on the quality of information (Zaichkowsky,

1986). For example, consumers may pay greater attention to car information on

advertisements if they are thinking of buying a car. However, if consumers are not

looking for a new car, then they may not pay attention to car advertisements.

Zaichkowsky (1985) developed the personal involvement inventory (PII) with 20 items to

measure different levels of involvement. Although these items were originally developed

to measure personal involvement with products, the study suggested that it can be used to

measure situational involvement. Previous research (Garlin & McGuiggan, 2002; Shao,

Baker, & Wagner, 2004; Stafford & Stern, 2002; Zaichkowsky, 1986; Zaichkowsky,

1994) demonstrated the sensitivity of the measure toward different types of situational

involvement (e.g., purchase vs. no purchase) for the same product.

Product involvement

Product involvement is a more enduring involvement (Wells & Prensky, 1996)

and levels of involvement with a same product vary greatly across people (Zaichkowsky,

Page 55: Osu 1135134346

30

1985). Therefore, consumers with high product involvement experience constant high

involvement with a particular product category (e.g., clothing product) as a result of

personal factors that generate greater needs, importance, interest, and value related to the

product (Zaichkowsky, 1986). For example, consumers with greater interest, needs,

value, and importance for clothing products have an enduring involvement with clothing

products. By contrast, the average or low involved consumers become highly involved

with clothing products only when they are in a specific purchasing situation, but are less

involved in clothing products when they are not in a purchasing situation. Kinley,

Conrad, and Brown (1999) found that consumers who had a high level of clothing

involvement were more likely to use a magazine, a television, a store window, and in-

store displays to obtain information related to clothing products than those who had a low

level of clothing involvement.

In comparison to low involved consumers, high involved consumers were found

to search for more information about the product before they purchased to process

relevant information in detail (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983; Petty & Cacioppo,

1996; Zaichkowsky, 1985). Macias (2003) found that consumers with high product

involvement had higher comprehension of a website than those with low product

involvement. Because interactivity is often thought to be more effortful, consumers with

high product involvement are more likely to interact with features of a website (e.g.,

clicking icons in order to request more information) than those with low product

involvement (Cho, 1999, 2003; Macias, 2003). Cho’s (1999) study further revealed that

consumers with low product involvement tended to click a banner ad in a larger size or

with dynamic animation more than one of average size or with no dynamic animation.

Page 56: Osu 1135134346

31

However, the size of ads or dynamic animation had no effect for consumers with high

product involvement (Cho, 1999). The results support the ELM developed by Petty and

Cacioppo (1996). Consumers in the low involvement condition were more likely to

attend to peripheral cues such as icons with dynamic animation, image sizes, and

background color, while those in the high involvement condition tended to have high

motivation to request more information and contents related to the product (central cues)

in order to examine the product in detail (Petty, et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996).

Consumers with low product involvement are less likely to seek and utilize information

and to compare different brands than those with high product involvement (Mittal, 1989)

Situational involvement

Situational involvement such as purchase or usage occasions tends to enhance the

level of involvement with a particular product (Wells & Prensky, 1996). Consumers who

are highly involved with a particular situation (e.g., browsing with a purchasing goal)

may engage in different behaviors in comparison to those who are low involved with the

situation (e.g., browsing without a purchasing goal). People are more motivated to

allocate the cognitive effort required to evaluate the true merits of an issue or a product

under a high involvement situation rather than under a low involvement situation (Petty,

et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). Attention and elaboration are expected to be

enhanced by increasing the personal relevance of the products at a particular time and

situation (Petty et al., 1983; Zaichkowsky, 1986).

Previous research examined the moderating effect of situational involvement in

brand evaluation or attitude (Karson & Korgaonkar, 2001; Kokkinaki & Lunt, 1999;

Page 57: Osu 1135134346

32

Maoz & Tybout, 2002; Park & Hastak, 1995), in advertising effectiveness (Dotson &

Hyatt, 2000; Petty et al., 1983), in expectations of service quality (Shao et al., 2004), and

in consumer emotions (Eroglu et al., 2003; Mano, 1997). Park and Hastak (1995) studied

the effects of involvement as a moderator on persuasion during message exposure. The

results showed that involved subjects tended to spend a longer amount of time for brand

evaluation than uninvolved subjects. Involved subjects remembered more information in

the advertisement than uninvolved subjects. In addition, message quality had a greater

impact on brand attitude under the high involvement situation rather than under the low

involvement situation, while source credibility had a greater impact on brand attitude

only under the low involvement situation (Park & Hastak, 1995).

Dotson and Hyatt (2000) found that high involved people had a more favorable

attitude toward the brand advertised with strong arguments and had greater purchase

intentions for the ad with strong arguments. The results are consistent with the prediction

of the ELM that under high involvement situations people process product information

more carefully than under low involvement situations (Dotson & Hyatt, 2000).

Argument quality as a central cue has more effect on persuasion under high situational

involvement, whereas expertise or an attractive model as a peripheral cue has more effect

on persuasion under low situational involvement (Petty et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo,

1984). Petty et al. (1983) found that argument quality tended to be a more important

determinant of purchase intentions under high situational involvement than under low

situational involvement. Involvement also influenced consumers’ recall of brand name.

Strong arguments increased brand name recall and brand name recognition under high

involvement. However, under low situational involvement consumers were more likely

Page 58: Osu 1135134346

33

to recall the brand name of the product when they were exposed to famous endorsers

(Petty et al., 1983).

Shao et al., (2004) investigated the effects of appropriateness of service contact

personnel dress on expectations of service quality. Consistent with the ELM, the study

found that the appropriateness of personnel dress as a peripheral cue had a greater impact

on consumer expectations of service quality and purchase intention under the low

involvement condition rather than under the high involvement condition (Shao et al.,

2004).

In Internet apparel shopping contexts, Eroglu et al.’s (2003) study supports the

tenets of the ELM in that only under the low involvement situation low task relevant cues

(peripheral cues such as background color, text color, animated icons) influenced

consumer emotions. However, under the high involvement situation low task relevant

cues (peripheral cues) had no effects on consumer emotions (Eroglu et al., 2003). The

results demonstrated the effects of situational involvement as a moderator on the

relationship between peripheral cues and consumer emotions.

Page 59: Osu 1135134346

34

2.2.3. Applying the S-O-R Paradigm and Involvement into Internet Shopping Context

Online store environments are different from traditional store environments.

Applying the S-O-R paradigm into an online shopping context, Eroglu et al. (2001)

suggested a conceptual model for atmospheric qualities of online retailing. Eroglu et al.

(2001) developed different terms to explain online store environments using the S-O-R

paradigm. The stimuli on the Internet consist of visible and audible cues to Internet

shoppers. Those cues may be divided into those that are high task relevant and those that

are low task relevant. High task relevant cues include site descriptors such as verbal and

pictorial information directly relevant to consumers’ shopping goal. Information on the

products, price, sale, delivery, and return policies are examples of verbal content related

to shopping goals. Pictures of the products and navigational support (e.g., site map,

search tool, menu bar of page) are other examples of high task relevant cues in an

Internet shopping context. Whereas low task relevant cues include site information not

directly relevant to shopping goals such as colors, borders and background patterns, fonts,

animation, music, icons, pictures for decorative purposes, and even amount of white

space. Low task relevant cues can create a mood or image of the site and can make the

verbal content easy or difficult to read. Verbal content directly related to the shopping

goal is typically the most task relevant cue in the Internet. However, in the case of

clothing, a picture of the product is also a very important high task relevant cue because

consumers buy clothing based on style or design (Eroglu et al., 2001).

As a whole, these task relevant cues will influence shoppers’ affect and cognition

that finally influence their shopping outcomes (Eroglu et al., 2001). Affective states have

Page 60: Osu 1135134346

35

three different dimensions—Pleasure, Arousal, and Dominance (PAD)—as expected

reactions to the stimuli (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Eroglu et al. (2001) add the

cognitive state to the model that describes consumer’s internal mental processes and

includes attitudes, beliefs, attention, comprehension, memory, and knowledge. In the

Internet shopping context, the cognitive state concerns “how online shoppers interpret

information provided on the screen, choose from alternative sites and products as well as

their attitudes toward the virtual stores, and so forth (Eroglu et al., p. 181, 2001).”

During the shaping of these attitudes, Internet shoppers deal with questions about

whether they like Internet shopping as an alternative shopping method and whether they

like the processes of Internet shopping. Therefore, affective and cognitive states serve as

mediators between atmospheric stimuli of the online store and consumers’ behavioral

responses (Eroglu et al., 2001).

In addition, Eroglu et al. (2001) add two moderators between the S-O

relationships in the online shopping context. Those are 1) involvement—the degree of

personal relevance—and 2) atmospheric responsiveness—the tendency to be influenced

by the qualities of immediate physical environment. The idea of involvement is also

found in the ELM proposed by Petty et al. (1983). High task relevant cues influence

highly involved shoppers to pursue a central cognitive process that creates a positive

affective and attitudinal state. Low involved shoppers may be more interested in and

influenced by the low task relevant cues (Eroglu et al., 2001). This means that high task

relevant cues may require more thinking to process information about the products than

low task relevant cues. Therefore, under the high involved Internet apparel shopping

condition (e.g., browsing with a purchasing goal), persuasion may arise from thinking

Page 61: Osu 1135134346

36

about the high task relevant cues that influence consumers’ emotional states that in turn,

influence consumers’ shopping behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intention,

approach behaviors. Under the high involved shopping condition, people may have more

favorable attitudes toward the apparel website with high task relevant cues in comparison

to the website with low task relevant cues, while under the low involvement condition

people may have more favorable attitudes toward low task relevant cues presented in the

apparel website.

As responses to the S-O-R paradigm, Eroglu et al. (2001) suggest different

avoidance-approach behaviors in the context of Internet shopping. Approach behaviors

consist of positive outcomes such as intention to stay, explore, spend more money, and

revisit the sites. On the other hand, avoidance behaviors are negative outcomes such as a

desire to leave and not return (Eroglu et al., 2001). Figure 2.2 shows the S-O-R paradigm

suggested by Eroglu et al. (2001) to explain the role of atmospheric characteristics in the

online store setting. In a later study, Eroglu et al. (2003) empirically tested this model

and the results supported this S-O-R model in online store environments.

Eroglu et al. (2003) investigated the effects of atmospheric web cues (low task

relevant cues) on affective states (pleasure and arousal) that affect cognitive states

(attitude) and examined both affective and cognitive states as mediators between

atmospheric web cues and consumer behavioral responses (satisfaction and approach

behavior). They also assessed the moderating effects of involvement and atmospheric

responsiveness between S-O. The results showed a positive effect of atmospheric web

cues on pleasure that in turn positively influenced consumer attitude toward the website.

Both affective (pleasure and arousal) and cognitive states (attitude) had positive impacts

Page 62: Osu 1135134346

37

on consumer response behaviors such as satisfaction and approach behaviors. The results

also showed that the effect of the atmospheric web cues (low task relevant cues) on

consumer response behaviors were mediated by the emotions felt by consumers. High

levels of pleasure and arousal elicited by site atmospheric cues increased consumers’

satisfaction and approach behaviors. In addition, the relationship between web cues and

consumer emotions were moderated by involvement. The effect of atmospheric web cues

(low task relevant cues) on pleasure was significant only under the low involvement

condition (e.g., browsing without a purchasing goal) (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Applying the S-O-R paradigm and the ELM to the Internet shopping context the

model proposed and tested by Eroglu et al. (2001, 2003) provides valuable insight into

the effects of peripheral cues (low task relevant cues) on consumer emotions, thereby

influencing consumer response behaviors. Although the importance of central cues (high

task relevant cues: web cues directly related to consumer’s purchasing goal) in increasing

purchase intention for and satisfaction with apparel in the online context has been

emphasized in previous research (Allen, 2000; Eroglu et al., 2001; Park & Stoel, 2002;

Then & Delong, 1999), the effects of central cues on the emotions and behavioral

responses were not empirically tested in Eroglu et al.’s (2003) study.

Page 63: Osu 1135134346

38

Stimulus Organism Response

Figure 2.2. S-O-R paradigm in the online shopping context (Eroglu et al., 2001, p. 179).

Online Environment

Cues • High Task Relevant • Low Task Relevant

Internal States

• Affect • Cognition

Shopping Outcomes

• Approach • Avoidance

Involvement

Atmospheric Responsiveness

Page 64: Osu 1135134346

39

2.3. Proposed Models and Hypotheses

According to the S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974), stimuli in

websites influence consumers’ emotions in terms of pleasure and arousal that in turn

influence consumers’ response behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intentions, and

approach behaviors (e.g., desire to explore or shop). The ELM explains the effects of

involvement on persuasion and attitude change (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). According to

the ELM, under a high involvement condition, central cues (e.g., product related web

cues) provided in the websites will affect persuasion, whereas under a low involvement

condition, peripheral cues will affect persuasion. Thus, the present study hypothesizes

that consumers under the high involvement condition will likely be affected by central

cues (e.g., different product views such as front, side, back, and details, the amount of

verbal information, and mix and match suggestions) shown in apparel websites, while

those in the low involvement condition will likely be affected by peripheral cues (e.g.,

colorful icons and background color) in the websites.

Blending the S-O-R paradigm and the ELM, the present study hypothesizes 1)

different effects for central and peripheral web cues on consumer emotions that in turn,

influence consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors) and 2) the effect of involvement (product involvement and situational

involvement) as a moderator of the S-O relationship.

The current research consists of two studies. Study 1 proposes 1) the effects of

peripheral cues presented in the apparel websites on consumers’ emotions (pleasure and

arousal) under low situational involvement, 2) the influence of product involvement

Page 65: Osu 1135134346

40

(personal relevance of clothing products) as a moderator of the relationship between

peripheral cues and emotions (pleasure and arousal), 3) the impact of emotions on

consumer response behaviors (purchase intention, and approach behaviors), and 4) the

mediating effects of consumer emotions between peripheral cues and response behaviors

(purchase intention and approach behaviors). Figure 2.3 describes the hypotheses and the

proposed model for Study 1.

The second study suggests 1) the effects of web cues—central cues (product-

related web cues) and peripheral cues (web cues not directly related to product)— on

consumer emotions, 2) the influence of emotions on consumer response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors), 3) the effects of situational

involvement (purchase situation vs. browsing situation) as a moderator of the relationship

between web cues (central cues and peripheral cues) and consumer emotions (pleasure

and arousal), and 4) the mediating effects of emotions between web cues and response

behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors). Hypotheses (See

Table 2.1) and proposed models for Study 1 (Figure 2.3) and Study 2 (Figure 2.8) will be

developed and discussed in detail in the following sections.

Page 66: Osu 1135134346

41

2.3.1. Main Study 1

The Effects of Peripheral Cues on Emotional States

According to the S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974), stimuli in

websites influence consumers’ emotions in terms of pleasure and arousal. Previous

research supports the effects of stimuli in retail stores (Babin et al., 2003; Baker et al.,

1992; Bellizi & Hite, 1992; Crowley, 1993; Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al.,

1994) and websites (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et al., 2005; Menon & Kahn, 2002) on

consumers’ emotions. Store environmental cues such as music (Baker et al., 1992;

Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al., 1994), lighting (Baker et al., 1992; Donovan

& Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al., 1994), and color (Babin et al., 2003; Bellizi & Hite,

1992; Crowley, 1993) influence consumers’ pleasure and arousal.

In an online shopping context, web environmental cues such as background color,

text color, animated logo, moving image, and image interactivity influence consumers’

pleasure and arousal while they are browsing the websites (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et

al., 2005; Menon & Kahn, 2002). According to the ELM, these web cues are categorized

as peripheral cues that influence consumer emotions under low involvement situations

(Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). Therefore, based on previous research, the following

hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 1. Under the low involvement situation site atmospheric cues (peripheral

cues) will influence emotional reactions.

Page 67: Osu 1135134346

42

Hypothesis 1a. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

pleasure.

Hypothesis 1b. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

arousal.

The Moderating Effect of Product Involvement between S-O

Consumers high in product involvement have greater needs and interest in a

particular product, rate it as more important, and assign greater value to it than those low

in product involvement (Zaichkowsky, 1986). For example, consumers high in clothing

involvement may have consistent needs and interest in clothing, rate it as more important,

and assign greater value to it across situations. As a type of enduring involvement (Wells

& Prensky, 1996), product involvement may influence consumers’ attention to web cues

while browsing apparel websites. For example, highly involved consumers search for

more information about the product (Petty et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996;

Zaichkowsky, 1985).

Consumers high in product involvement are more likely to interact with website

features (e.g., by clicking icons in order to obtain more information) than those low in

product involvement (Cho, 1999, 2003; Macias, 2003). Supporting the ELM, Cho (1999)

found that consumers with low product involvement tended to click a large banner ad or

one with dynamic animation more than an average size banner ad or one with no dynamic

animation. However, the size of ads or dynamic animation had no effect for consumers

Page 68: Osu 1135134346

43

high in product involvement (Cho, 1999). Consumers in the low involvement condition

tended to attend to peripheral cues such as icons with dynamic animation, larger image

sizes, and background color, while those in the high involvement condition tended to be

affected by central cues such as product or service related information (Petty, et al., 1983;

Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). Consumers with a low level of product involvement are less

likely to seek and utilize information and to compare different brands than those with a

high level of product involvement (Mittal, 1989). Hence, based on previous research, it

is hypothesized that peripheral cues will have a greater effect on consumers with a low

level of product involvement rather than those with a high level of product involvement.

This rationale led to the second hypothesis.

Hypothesis 2. Product involvement (i.e., personal relevance of clothing products) will

moderate the relationship between peripheral cues and emotional reactions.

Hypothesis 2a. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on pleasure for people

with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Hypothesis 2b. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on arousal for people

with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Page 69: Osu 1135134346

44

The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors

According to Mehrabian and Russell (1974), consumer emotions as mediators

affect various consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). High levels of

pleasure and arousal induced by the environmental stimuli enhance purchase intention

(Babin & Babin, 2001; Baker et al., 1992; Spies et al, 1997) and approach behaviors such

as desire to explore and desire to shop (Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2002).

Previous research examined the influence of store colors on consumers’ shopping

behaviors such as approach behavior and purchase intention (Babin et al., 2003; Bellizi,

Chrowley, & Hasty, 1983). Consumers’ evaluations and excitement induced by the color

of store environments were positively related to store patronage intentions and purchase

intentions (Babin et al., 2003). Applying the S-O-R paradigm to the Internet apparel

shopping context, Eroglu et al. (2003) found significant effects of emotions (pleasure and

arousal) on consumer approach behaviors. Based on this literature the third and fourth

hypotheses were developed.

Hypothesis 3. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal experienced from the

apparel website will influence purchase intention.

Hypothesis 3a. Pleasure will be positively related to purchase intention.

Hypothesis 3b. Arousal will be positively related to purchase intention.

Hypothesis 4. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal experienced from the

apparel website will influence approach behaviors.

Page 70: Osu 1135134346

45

Hypothesis 4a. Pleasure will be positively related to approach behaviors (desire

to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 4b. Arousal will be positively related to approach behaviors (desire to

explore or shop and likability of the websites).

The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R

The S-O-R paradigm illustrates the mediating effects of consumer emotions

between environmental stimuli and consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell,

1974). Based on the S-O-R paradigm, previous research investigated the effects of

consumers’ emotions as mediators between atmospheric stimuli and consumers’

shopping behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2003; Sherman et al, 1997;

Spies et al., 1997). Pleasure and arousal tend to mediate the relationship between

atmospheric stimuli and consumer purchasing behaviors in retail stores (Sherman et al,

1997) and in online stores (Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2003). Consumer

emotions elicited by atmospheric stimuli positively influenced approach behaviors (e.g.,

browsing more, engaging in unplanned purchasing, and seeking out more stimulating

products) in the Internet shopping context (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et al., 2005; Menon

& Kahn, 2003). This suggests that Internet retailers should think about consumers’

emotions experienced while browsing the websites that may shape consumers’

subsequent shopping behaviors. Therefore, it is predicted that consumer emotions

(pleasure and arousal) will mediate the relationship between peripheral cues presented in

apparel websites and consumer response behaviors such as purchase intention and

approach behaviors. This argument led to the development of the following hypothesis.

Page 71: Osu 1135134346

46

Hypothesis 5. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the

relationship between peripheral cues and consumers’ response behaviors (purchase

intention and approach behaviors).

Page 72: Osu 1135134346

47

Figure 2.3. The proposed model in the main study 1.

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

Product Involvement

Approach Behavior

Patronage Intention H1a

H1b

H2a and H2b

H3a

H4a

H4b

H3b

H5

Stimulus Organism Response

Page 73: Osu 1135134346

48

2.3.2. Main Study 2

Part I: The Effects of Web Cues on Emotional States

The S-O-R paradigm developed by Mehrabian and Russell (1974) describes the

effects of various stimuli available in retail stores and in websites on consumers’

emotions. Supporting the S-O-R paradigm, Donovan and Rossiter (1982) found that

retail environmental stimuli such as lighting and music influenced consumers’ emotions

such as pleasure and arousal. In a later study, using a broader sample Donovan et al.

(1994) measured emotions during shopping rather than after shopping as Donovan and

Rossiter had done and found a significant effect for store environment on consumer

emotions.

Various stimuli (social factors, overall image, design factors, and ambient factors)

available in apparel retail stores tend to increase consumers’ pleasure and arousal

(Sherman et al., 1997). As one of the most important design elements that communicates

style and mood in retail store environments, color influenced consumer emotions such as

pleasure and arousal in retail stores (Bellizi & Hite, 1992; Crowley, 1993).

Given that the online store environment is different from a traditional store

environment, Eroglu et al. (2001) developed different terms to explain online store

stimuli—high task relevant cues and low task relevant cues. High task relevant cues

include site descriptors such as verbal and pictorial information directly relevant to

consumers’ purchasing goal. Information related to the products, price, sale, delivery,

and return policies, pictures of the products, and navigational support (e.g., site map,

Page 74: Osu 1135134346

49

search tool, menu bar of page) are examples of high task relevant cues in the Internet

shopping context. Low task relevant cues include web cues not directly relevant to

shopping goals, for example colors, borders and background patterns, fonts, animation,

music, icons, pictures for decorative purpose, and even amount of white space. Verbal

content directly related to the shopping goal are typically the most task relevant online

cues. However, in the case of clothing, a picture of the product is also a very important

high task relevant cue because consumers buy clothing based on style or design (Eroglu

et al., 2001). In relation to the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996), high and low task

relevant cues can be categorized as central cues and peripheral cues, respectively. As a

whole, these web cues (both central cues and peripheral cues) may influence shoppers’

pleasure and arousal (Eroglu et al., 2001). In a later study, Eroglu et al. (2003) found a

positive effect for atmospheric web cues on consumer emotions (pleasure and arousal) in

the apparel online shopping context. Based on the S-O-R paradigm and previous

research, it is hypothesized that the amount of central cues1and the presence or absence of

peripheral cues presented in apparel websites will influence consumer pleasure and

arousal experienced while browsing the websites (See Figure 2.4 for the proposed model

in Part 1). This rationale led to the development of the following hypotheses.

1 All websites must have a minimal amount in order for consumers to buy

Page 75: Osu 1135134346

50

Hypothesis 1. Central cues (e.g., number of different product views-front, back, sides,

details, amount of verbal information, and amount of mix and match suggestions) will

influence consumers’ emotional reactions experienced from an apparel website.

Hypothesis 1a. As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium

amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of

central cues will experience more pleasure.

Hypothesis 1b. As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium

amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of

central cues will experience more arousal.

Hypothesis 2. Peripheral cues (e.g., pictorial icons and background colors) will

influence consumers’ emotional reactions experienced from the apparel website.

Hypothesis 2a. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, consumers exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

pleasure.

Hypothesis 2b. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, consumers exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

arousal.

Page 76: Osu 1135134346

51

Stimulus Organism

Figure 2.4. Part one of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 1 and 2).

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues Arousal

Pleasure H1a

H1b

H2a

H2b

Page 77: Osu 1135134346

52

Part II: The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors

According to the S-O-R paradigm, consumer emotions cause various consumer

response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Pleasure and arousal induced by the

store or site environmental cues enhance satisfaction (Eroglu et al., 2003; Machleit &

Mantel, 2001; Spies et al, 1997), purchase intention (Babin & Babin, 2001; Baker et al.,

1992; Spies et al, 1997), and approach behaviors such as desire to explore and desire to

shop (Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2002). For example, consumers’ evaluations

and excitement elicited by the colors of store environments were positively related to

store patronage intentions and purchase intentions (Babin et al., 2003). Positive emotions

induced by the servicescape influenced behavioral intentions (Hightower, Brady, &

Baker, 2002). Consumers with a positive mood elicited by the pleasant store

environment were likely to spend more money in the store and were highly satisfied with

the store (Spies et al., 1997). Applying the S-O-R paradigm to the Internet apparel

shopping context, Eroglu et al. (2003) investigated the effects of consumer emotions on

consumer satisfaction and approach behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of

the websites). High levels of pleasure and arousal induced by various web cues tend to

increase consumers’ satisfaction and approach behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Accordingly, it is predicted that consumer emotions such as pleasure and arousal will be

positively related to various consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors) (see Figure 2.5 of the proposed model for Part 2).

Based on this rationale, the following hypothesis was developed.

Page 78: Osu 1135134346

53

Hypothesis 3. Emotional states will be positively related to consumers’ response

behaviors.

Hypothesis 3a. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

Hypothesis 3b. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ purchase

intention.

Hypothesis 3c. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ approach

behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 3d. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

Hypothesis 3e. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ purchase

intention.

Hypothesis 3f. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ approach

behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Page 79: Osu 1135134346

54

Organism Response

Figure 2.5. Part two of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 3a to 3f).

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Approach behavior

Satisfaction H3a

H3b

H3d

H3c

H3e

H3f

Page 80: Osu 1135134346

55

Part III: The Moderating Effect of Situational Involvement between S-O

According to the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996), there are two different routes to

persuasion—a central and a peripheral route. Persuasion arises from careful thinking

about an issue or message via the central route or from non-issue-relevant cues such as

source attractiveness via the peripheral route (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). Under the high

involvement condition central cues such as the number or strength of issue-relevant

arguments in the message influence persuasion but source credibility as a peripheral cue

has no significant effect. Under the low involvement condition persuasion is determined

basically by peripheral cues such as attractiveness of the source, but not by the number of

issue-relevant arguments in the message (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996).

The level of involvement is influenced by different situations (Wells & Prensky,

1996). For example, consumers who browse apparel websites with a purchasing goal

may have higher situational involvement than consumers who browse websites without a

purchasing goal. People are more motivated to allocate the cognitive effort required to

evaluate the true merits of a product under the high involvement situation rather than

under the low involvement situation (Petty et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996).

Attention is expected to be enhanced by increasing the level of involvement at a

particular time and situation (Petty et al., 1983; Zaichkowsky, 1986).

Supporting the ELM, previous research has found that situational involvement

moderated the effect of central cues and peripheral cues on brand evaluation or attitude

(Karson & Korgaonkar, 2001; Kokkinaki & Lunt, 1999; Maoz & Tybout, 2002; Park &

Hastak, 1995), on advertising effectiveness (Dotson & Hyatt, 2000; Petty et al., 1983), on

Page 81: Osu 1135134346

56

expectations of service quality (Shao et al., 2004), and on consumer emotions (Eroglu et

al., 2003; Mano, 1997). Park and Hastak (1995) studied the effects of involvement as a

moderator on persuasion during message exposure and found that message quality as a

central cue had a greater impact on brand attitude under the high situational involvement

condition rather than under the low situational involvement condition, while source

credibility as a peripheral cue had a greater impact on brand attitude only under the low

situational involvement condition (Park & Hastak, 1995). Under high involvement

situations, people process product information more carefully than under the low

involvement situation (Dotson & Hyatt, 2000). Increasing consumer’s involvement

causes them to attend to central cues and affects attitude formation (Petty & Cacioppo,

1996).

Thus, central cues have more effect on persuasion in the high situational

involvement condition, whereas peripheral cues have more effect on persuasion in the

low situational involvement condition (Petty et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1984;).

Consistent with the ELM, Shao et al. (2004) found that the appropriateness of personnel

dress as a peripheral cue had a greater impact on consumer expectations of service quality

and purchase intention under low involvement rather than under high involvement (Shao

et al., 2004). Eroglu et al.’s (2003) study also supports the prediction of the ELM in that

only under low situational involvement, low task relevant cues (peripheral cues such as

background color, text color, animated icons) influenced consumer emotions. However,

under high situational involvement, low task relevant cues (peripheral cues) had no effect

on consumer emotions (Eroglu et al., 2003). Based on the ELM and previous research, it

is hypothesized that central cues will have a greater impact on consumer pleasure and

Page 82: Osu 1135134346

57

arousal under high situational involvement rather than under the low situational

involvement. In addition, in the low situational involvement condition, peripheral cues

will have a greater influence on consumer emotions (pleasure and arousal) (Figure 2.6

describes the proposed model in Part 3). Based on this rationale the following hypothesis

was developed.

Hypothesis 4. Situational involvement will moderate the relationship between web cues

and emotional reactions.

Hypothesis 4a. Central cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions

under a high involvement situation than under a low involvement situation.

Hypothesis 4b. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions

under a low involvement situation than under a high involvement situation.

Page 83: Osu 1135134346

58

Stimulus Organism

Figure 2.6. Part three of the model in Study 2 (Hypotheses 4a to 4b).

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

H4a and H4b

Situational Involvement

Page 84: Osu 1135134346

59

Part IV: The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R

The S-O-R paradigm predicts the mediating effects of consumer emotions

between environmental stimuli and consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell,

1974). According to the S-O-R paradigm, environmental stimuli (e.g., store design)

influence consumer emotions (e.g., pleasure and arousal) that serve as mediating

variables in determining various consumer response behaviors such as satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors in retail stores (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974).

Supporting the S-O-R paradigm, previous research revealed significant mediating effects

of consumers’ emotions between environmental cues (in stores and in websites) and

consumers’ response behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2003; Sherman et al,

1997; Spies et al., 1997).

In the Internet shopping context, Eroglu et al. (2001, 2003) predicted that various

web cues (central and peripheral cues) would affect consumer emotions that consequently

influence consumer response behaviors, such as satisfaction and approach behaviors.

Eroglu et al. (2003) found that the effect of web cues on consumer response behaviors

were mediated by consumer emotions experienced while browsing the websites. Pleasure

and arousal elicited by various web cues presented in websites positively influenced

satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors (e.g., browsing more, engaging

in unplanned purchasing, and seeking out more stimulating products) (Eroglu et al., 2003;

Fiore et al., 2005; Menon & Kahn, 2003). This suggests that Internet apparel retailers

should consider emotions felt by consumers while browsing the websites that may

influence consumers’ consequent shopping behaviors.

Page 85: Osu 1135134346

60

Based on the S-O-R paradigm and previous research, it is proposed that consumer

emotions such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the relationship between type of cue

(central and peripheral cues) and consumers’ response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors) (see Figure 2.7 for the proposed model for Part 4 in

Study 2). Based on the previous rationale, the following hypothesis was developed.

Hypothesis 5. Emotional states will mediate the relationship between type of cue and

consumers’ response behaviors.

Hypothesis 5a. The relationships between central cues and response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) will be mediated by

emotional states.

Hypothesis 5b. The relationships between peripheral cues and response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) will be mediated by

emotional states.

Page 86: Osu 1135134346

61

Figure 2.7. Part four of the hypothesized model for Study 2 (Hypotheses 5a and 5b).

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Situational Involvement

Approach behavior

Satisfaction

H5a and H5b

Stimulus Organism Response

Page 87: Osu 1135134346

62

Figure 2.8. The proposed model for Study 2.

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Situational Involvement

Approach behavior

Satisfaction H1a

H1b

H2a

H2b

H4a and H4b

H3a

H3b

H3d

H3c

H3e

H3f

Part Four

Part One Part Two

Part Three

H5a and H5b

Stimulus Organism Response

Page 88: Osu 1135134346

63

Hypotheses

Study 1 Study 2

As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more pleasure.

H1a H2a

As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more arousal.

H1b H2b

As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of central cues will experience more pleasure.

N/A H1a

As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of central cues will experience more arousal.

N/A H1b

Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on pleasure for people with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

H2a N/A

Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on arousal for people with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

H2b N/A

Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

N/A H3a

Pleasure will be positively related to purchase intention.

H3a H3b

Pleasure will be positively related to approach behaviors.

H4a H3c

Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

N/A H3d

Arousal will be positively related to purchase intention.

H3b H3e

Arousal will be positively related to approach behaviors.

H4b H3f

Central cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions under a high involvement situation than under a low involvement situation.

N/A H4a

Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions under a low involvement situation than under a high involvement situation.

N/A H4b

Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the relationship between central cues and consumers’ response behaviors.

N/A H5a

Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the relationship between peripheral cues and consumers’ response behaviors.

H5 H5b

Table 2.1. Summary of Hypotheses in Study 1 and Study 2.

Page 89: Osu 1135134346

64

CHAPTER 3

PILOT STUDIES

This chapter describes pilot studies conducted to develop manipulations for the

two main studies. Three pilot studies and one content analysis were conducted to select

appropriate stimuli (e.g., apparel items, situational involvement, central cues, and

peripheral cues) for the two main experiments. The purpose of the first pilot study was to

develop and examine the situational involvement manipulations, the purpose of the

second pilot study was to select apparel stimuli for Study 1 and Study 2, and the purpose

of the third pilot study and content analysis were to develop manipulations for central

cues. Figure 3.1 presents the summary of each pilot test in detail.

Page 90: Osu 1135134346

65

Pilot Study 1: Develop Manipulations for Situational Involvement

1. Develop situational involvement manipulations based on previous research (Eroglu et al., 2003; Zaichkowsky, 1986) and check the effectiveness.

2. Check web cues that gain more attention under different situational involvements.

Pilot Study 2: Select Apparel Stimuli

1. Select apparel stimuli for Study 1 (2 items) and Study 2 (5 items). 2. Check web cues that gain more attention when purchasing clothing online

(high involvement situation) and compare the results with Pilot Study 1. 3. Check the popular apparel websites among participants for Content

Analysis.

Pilot Study 3: Prepare for Content Analysis

1. Based on Pilot Study 1, Pilot Study 2, and previous research (Eroglu et al., 2001), the list of web cues representing both product related central cues and peripheral cues was developed.

2. Check the extent to which the web cues were product related. 3. Results of Pilot Study 3 were used to develop a coding frame for Content

Analysis.

Content Analysis: Develop Manipulations for Central Cues

1. Develop a coding frame representing product related central cues based on Pilot Study 3.

2. Fifteen apparel websites listed in Pilot Study 2 were content analyzed. 3. Based on the results, central cues (medium amount vs. high amount) were

selected for Study 1 and Study 2.

Figure 3.1. A summary of pilot tests.

Page 91: Osu 1135134346

66

3.1. Pilot Study 1

Pilot Study 1 was conducted to develop and test situational involvement

manipulations and to determine which web cues are attended to in different involvement

situations. According to Zaichkowsky (1986), consumers use different cues to evaluate

products depending on their level of involvement. Eroglu et al. (2001) proposed that

under a high involvement situation (e.g., online apparel purchasing) consumers use high

task relevant cues such as verbal content or pictures of the merchandise related to

shopping goals (i.e., central cues). The authors further suggest that under the low

involvement situation (e.g., online browsing without a purchasing goal) consumers may

be more influenced by low task relevant cues such as colors, icons, and decorative

pictures (i.e., peripheral cues) not directly related to the purchasing goal. To check which

web cues gain more attention under different levels of situational involvement,

participants were asked to list what they recalled from the website after browsing.

The design of Pilot Study 1 was a between-subjects experiment with one factor

(situational involvement) having two levels (low vs. high). One British apparel website

(www.asos.com) was selected to use as a template for Pilot Study 1 to eliminate the

effects of participants’ previous experiences with U.S. apparel websites on the results.

The pilot study was conducted in a laboratory setting with computers. Seventy four

students from a consumer science class participated in Pilot Study 1 for extra credit.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups (high vs. low

involvement situation).

Page 92: Osu 1135134346

67

Participants were asked to read a scenario (high or low involvement situation)

before they started browsing. According to Zaichkowsky (1986), different situational

factors such as purchasing (high involvement) versus no purchasing (low involvement)

may have an impact on consumer’s level of involvement. Eroglu et al. (2003)

manipulated situational involvement with two different scenarios: a purchasing situation

and a browsing situation. Thus, based on previous research two different scenarios

manipulating situational involvement were created in Pilot Study 1. For the low

involvement situation, participants were asked to read the following scenario: “Imagine

that you find a clothing website today. Now, you are going to visit one clothing website.

Browse and look around the website for a while,” and to fill out a survey. Participants in

the high involvement situation read the following scenario: “Now, you are going to visit

one clothing website. Imagine that you have been given a $100 gift certificate to

purchase apparel products from the website. Remember! After browsing the website, you

should be able to identify and describe two items that you would like to purchase from

the website.” See Appendix A for more information.

Since situational involvement was manipulated in the pilot study, participants

were asked to rate their level of involvement while browsing the website using 7-point

scales (1-not at all and 7-very much so). The mean scores for low involvement (N = 31)

and high involvement (N = 43) were 5.31 and 5.72 respectively. Univariate analysis of

variance (F (1, 72) = 4.183, p < .05) revealed that participants in the high involvement

situation felt more involved than those in the low involvement situation. Therefore, the

results indicated that the manipulation for situational involvement was successful.

Page 93: Osu 1135134346

68

To determine web cues which participants paid attention to under different levels

of situational involvement (high or low) participants were asked to list what they

remembered after browsing the website. Types of recalled web cues were classified as

central or peripheral cues based on previous research (Eroglu et al., 2001). Eroglu et al.

(2001) indicated that a picture of products, a verbal description of products, size

information, the price, and terms of ordering and shipping could be central cues directly

relevant to consumers’ shopping goal (e.g., buying a pair of pants) that in turn influence

consumers’ emotions and behaviors. Whereas more decorative web cues such as

beautiful background colors and graphics, graphical images, fonts, animation, music, and

icons, and pictures other than products could be peripheral cues not directly related to

consumers’ shopping goals. Central cues were further classified as service related or

product related. Table 3.1 shows the list of web cues mentioned by participants in low or

high involvement situations. Number of web cues recalled were summed and entered

into chi-square difference tests to check for significant differences between the two

involvement groups (See Table 3.1). Under the high involvement situation, participants

tended to recall more web cues relevant to a purchasing goal (i.e., central cues such as

service and product related information), whereas participants under the low involvement

situation were more likely to remember web cues related to peripheral cues such as

different kinds of icons and the layout of the website. According to Zaichkowsky, 1986),

under the high involvement situation (e.g., purchasing) consumers are more likely to pay

attention to central cues (e.g., the quality of information) rather than peripheral cues (e.g.,

attractive model). Results of Pilot Study 1 also showed that participants recalled more

cues related to products rather than cues related to services in both low and high

Page 94: Osu 1135134346

69

involvement situations. Chi-square difference tests showed significant differences

between the number of recalled product related central cues and service-related central

cues in both low (χ2 = 11.842, p < .005) and high involvement (χ2 = 23.701, p < .001).

This could indicate that consumers are more likely to pay attention to product related

cues rather than service related cues.

Page 95: Osu 1135134346

70

Web Cues Frequency

High involvement

(N = 39)a

Low involvement

(N = 31)

χ2(1)

Central Cues

Service related account info 9 1 4.764* company info 4 2 n/a

contact info 5 1 n/a customer service 5 3 n/a delivery info 17 11 .284 Faq (Frequently asked questions) 2 0 n/a return policy 3 4 n/a security info 3 1 n/a

Total 48 23 4.069*

Product related colors for products 21 10 1.818 descriptions of products 16 5 3.569 different views of products (larger views, back views)

7 7 .185

fabric 6 2 n/a pictures of products 11 2 4.401* prices 17 8 1.529 size (size, size chart) 10 9 .073 mix and match suggestions 17 7 2.223 wash, care instruction 4 3 n/a

Total 109 53 8.789** Peripheral Cues

Web cues not related to a purchasing goal

brand logo 2 4 n/a layout 2 7 n/a icons (France icon, in stock icon, check out icon, payment icon)

2 15 13.308***

news (news letter subscription) 3 3 n/a Total 9 29 15.800***

Note. a Different sample size for the high involvement treatment group is due to missing data, * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 Table 3.1. Frequencies of web cues listed in Pilot Study 1.

Page 96: Osu 1135134346

71

3.2. Pilot Study 2

The second pilot study was conducted to select appropriate apparel items for the

mock apparel websites used in Study 1 and Study 2. The purpose of the second pilot

study was twofold. The first was to choose apparel stimuli that minimized variance as a

result of style. Since the study is interested in consumers’ responses to the various web

cues (central cues and peripheral cues) during shopping or browsing, apparel items

should be in fashion and attract the participants of the study (i.e., young female college

students) to keep them browsing and shopping. In the high involvement situation (e.g.,

purchasing situation) participants were asked to select one item that they would like to try

or buy from the website. The use of unattractive products could have influenced the

results of the study. Therefore, choosing apparel items that were appealing to

participants was important. The second purpose of this pilot study was to check web cues

that attract attention when purchasing clothing products online (i.e., high involvement

situation) and compare with the results of Pilot Study 1.

Images of 34 pairs of pants were downloaded from several commercial apparel

websites (See Appendix C). All items were presented on a mannequin form (lower torso

only) and images were prepared and edited using Adobe Photoshop 7 to retain the

consistency of image quality in terms of resolution, background color, brightness, and

contrast. Any noticeable brand logos were removed from the images to avoid any

possible effects of brand on the results of the study. All items were shown in front and

back views on white background and the resolution of images was retained to be 300 x

360 pixels. Thirty four pairs of pants were evaluated to select seven final apparel items

Page 97: Osu 1135134346

72

for the two main studies (two items for Study 1 and five items for Study 2) (See

Appendix C).

Pilot Study 2 was conducted as an online survey (See Appendix B). In addition to

evaluating the pants, respondents indicated whether or not they had purchased clothing

online, websites they had purchased clothing from (open-ended format), and what they

paid attention to on clothing websites (open-ended format). One hundred twenty seven

female undergraduate students from two consumer science classes participated in the

pilot study for extra credit. Thirty four pairs of pants were randomly ordered in the

survey. Participants viewed all 34 pairs and evaluated each pair in terms of attractiveness,

fashionability, and likability using 7-point unipolar scales (e.g., Unattractive – Attractive).

Reliabilities of the three items for all 34 apparel items were found to be adequate

(Cronbach’s αs > .86) (See Table 3.2). Scores for the three items were summed to select

the final apparel items for Study 1 and Study 2. Five apparel items with the highest mean

and median scores were selected for Study 2 and two items with the next two highest

scores were selected for Study 1 (See Table 3.2 and Appendix C).

Eighty-one participants had purchased clothing from the Internet and 114

participants had browsed for clothing on the Internet. Participants who had purchased

clothing from online stores were asked to write what they paid attention to on the apparel

websites when they purchased clothing on the Internet. Table 3.3 describes web cues

mentioned by participants. Thirteen out of 16 items mentioned were types of product

related web cues such as color of products, style, fit information, and fabric. The other

three items, not product related, were brand name, security of websites, and shipping

information which could also influence consumers’ behavior when purchasing clothing

Page 98: Osu 1135134346

73

products online. A chi-square difference test was performed to check if the difference

between the number of product related web cues and other web cues listed in Pilot Study

2 was significant and it was found to be significant, χ2 = 8.895, p < .01. Results of Pilot

Study 1 (See Section 3.1 and Table 3.1) and Pilot Study 2 (Table 3.3) show that

participants are more likely to attend to product related central cues rather than service-

related central cues in purchasing situations (i.e., high involvement situation).

Participants were also asked to list the apparel websites from which they had

purchased clothing products. Table 3.4 shows the list of the popular apparel websites

provided by participants. Content analysis of these apparel websites was conducted to

identify central cues used on existing apparel websites (See Section 3.1.4).

Page 99: Osu 1135134346

74

Apparel

Items

Cronbach’s α

Mean

Median

S.D.

Min.

Max.

Item 1 ** .86 14.20 14.00 3.45 3 21 Item 2 * .95 15.58 16.00 3.66 3 21 Item 3 .96 10.14 10.00 4.61 3 21 Item 4 .96 12.41 12.00 4.24 3 21 Item 5 .97 11.43 11.00 5.14 3 21 Item 6 * .95 14.96 15.00 3.95 3 21 Item 7 * .96 14.81 15.00 4.17 3 21 Item 8 .96 12.48 12.50 4.43 3 21 Item 9 * .97 14.52 16.00 5.01 3 21 Item 10 .96 11.39 12.00 4.62 3 21 Item 11 .98 11.50 11.00 5.78 3 21 Item 12 * .97 14.33 15.00 4.63 3 21 Item 13 .95 12.53 13.00 6.01 3 21 Item 14 .97 12.21 12.00 5.20 3 21 Item 15 .94 7.47 6.00 4.79 3 21 Item 16 .95 8.17 6.00 4.83 3 21 Item 17 .97 13.25 14.00 5.70 3 21 Item 18 ** .96 13.53 14.00 4.70 3 21 Item 19 .96 13.10 14.00 5.14 3 21 Item 20 .95 11.49 12.00 5.06 3 21 Item 21 .95 11.36 11.00 5.98 3 21 Item 22 .97 8.93 8.00 4.70 3 21 Item 23 .92 7.18 5.00 4.87 3 21 Item 24 .96 12.29 12.00 4.90 3 21 Item 25 .96 11.11 10.00 6.27 3 21 Item 26 .94 5.74 3.00 4.27 3 21 Item 27 .96 11.67 12.00 5.10 3 21 Item 28 .97 9.01 9.00 4.72 3 21 Item 29 .97 11.11 12.00 5.90 3 21 Item 30 .95 11.16 12.00 4.43 3 21 Item 31 .97 12.22 12.00 5.75 3 21 Item 32 .97 9.67 10.00 5.27 3 21 Item 33 .97 11.75 12.50 5.39 3 21 Item 34 .95 9.80 10.00 5.63 3 21

Note. * Items selected for the main study 2, ** Items selected for the main study 1. Table 3.2. Descriptive statistics and reliabilities of apparel items selected in Pilot Study 2 for Study 1 and Study 2.

Page 100: Osu 1135134346

75

Web Cues Frequencies

Brand name 3 Color of products 15 Descriptions (details, cut) 10 Detail pictures (larger views in many angles and pictures of products) 10 Fit information (fit on the model) 12 Materials (fabric) 10 Price 25 Product variety 1 Product availability 1 Quality of products 5 Sales 9 Security of websites 2 Shipping information 3 Size (size chart, measurement) 15 Style (style, look, design, fashion) 18 Theme (mix and match) 2

Table 3.3. Web cues listed in Pilot Study 2 that participants reported paying attention to when they purchased clothing online.

Websites

Frequencies

www.victoriasecret.com 36 www.gap.com 17 www.jcrew.com 14 www.abercrombie.com 13 www.delias.com 13 www.bananarepublic.com 12 www.nordstrom.com 11 www.ae.com 9 www.urbanoutfitter.com 9 www.forever21.com 8 www.bebe.com 8 www.alloy.com 7 www.guess.com 6 www.oldnavy.com 6 www.ardenb.com 5

Table 3.4. The popular apparel websites participants listed in Pilot Study 2.

Page 101: Osu 1135134346

76

3.3. Pilot Study 3

The purpose of Pilot Study 3 was to check if the web cues classified as product

related by participants in Pilot Studies 1 and 2 are actually perceived to be product related

by a different set of participants. The result of Pilot Study 3 was used to develop a coding

frame representing product related central cues for content analysis of popular websites

(See Section 3.1.4). First, based on Pilot Study 1 (Table 3.1), Pilot Study 2 (Table 3.3),

and previous research (Eroglu et al., 2001), 22 web cues representing both central cues

(16) and peripheral cues (6) were generated (See Table 3.5). The web cue mentioned as

‘descriptions of products’ in Pilot Studies 1 and 2 was subdivided as pockets, waist,

inseam, and enclosure details to be more specific. Then, the 22 web cues were evaluated

in terms of the extent to which they were product related. According to Pilot Studies 1

and 2, people tend to pay more attention to product related central cues (e.g., fabric, color

of products, design details etc.) rather than service related central cues (e.g., return policy,

shipping and delivery information). In addition, since the focus of the main study was on

products, central cues related to products rather than services were evaluated in Pilot

Study 3 and the results were used to develop a coding frame for content analysis.

Pilot Study 3 was conducted as an online survey (Appendix D). Fifty nine female

undergraduate students in a consumer science class participated in the survey for extra

credit. Using 5-point Likert-type scales with endpoints ranging from 1 (very unlikely) to

5 (very likely) participants were asked to indicate the extent to which a list of web cues

were product related. Results are shown in Table 3.5.

Page 102: Osu 1135134346

77

As expected, product related central cues classified in Pilot Studies 1 and 2 were

evaluated as more product related (see Table 3.5) than any of the web cues. Mean and

median scores for all product related web cues were close to 4 or above, with detail views

as the highest, whereas mean and median scores for peripheral web cues were close to 3

or lower. Results of this pilot study suggest that 16 web cues (e.g., detail views, price

information, design details, waist, fit information, inseam, style information, color

information, size information, product care information, fabric information, enclosure

details, larger views, side views, mix and match suggestions, and originality of the

product) are types of product related central cues and that 6 web cues (e.g., placement of

images, images other than products on the background, colors used on the website, font

size, background color, font color) are types of peripheral web cues (Table 3.5). The 16

product related central cues were used to develop a coding frame for content analysis

(See Section 3.1.4).

Page 103: Osu 1135134346

78

Web Cues Mean

Median S.D. Min. Max.

detail views 4.61 5.00 .68 2 5 price information 4.60 5.00 .78 2 5 design details (e.g., pockets) 4.60 5.00 .78 2 5 waist (e.g., low or high) 4.58 5.00 .78 2 5 fit information 4.56 5.00 .76 2 5 inseam (e.g., length) 4.54 5.00 .76 2 5 style information 4.52 5.00 .74 3 5 color information 4.51 5.00 .89 2 5 size information 4.49 5.00 .78 3 5 product care information (e.g., machine wash, hand wash etc.) 4.49 5.00 .93 1 5

fabric information 4.47 5.00 .93 2 5 enclosure details (e.g., zipper, buttons) 4.47 5.00 .89 2 5

larger views 4.35 5.00 .88 2 5 side views 4.33 5.00 .87 2 5 mix and match suggestions (e.g., suggested coordinated items such as sweaters, handbag, shoes etc.)

3.93 4.00 1.04 1 5

originality of the product (e.g., country of origin) 3.67 4.00 1.17 2 5

placement of images 3.07 3.00 1.25 1 5 images other than products on the background 2.86 3.00 1.20 1 5

colors used on the website 2.63 2.00 1.21 1 5 font size 2.40 2.00 1.33 1 5 background color on the website 2.40 2.00 1.18 1 5 font color 2.39 2.00 1.32 1 5

Table 3.5. The extent to which web cues are product related rated in Pilot Study 3.

Page 104: Osu 1135134346

79

3.4. Content Analysis

To finalize selection of appropriate central cues for the main experiments and to

increase reality of the mock websites, existing apparel websites were analyzed to check

which types of product related central cues are available in existing apparel online stores.

This information was used to develop mock websites for the main studies. The 15

popular apparel websites identified in Pilot Study 2 were analyzed in terms of the

existence of various product related central cues.

A coding frame with 15 categories for content analysis was developed based on

web cues reflecting product related central cues rated in Pilot Study 3. Design details

(pockets and other details) and larger views (front, back, and side view) were divided into

subcategories (See Table 3.6). Thus, the final coding frame had 18 categories (See Table

3.6).

Two graduate students were trained by the researcher and coded 15 U.S. apparel

websites according to the coding frame working independently for the entire coding

procedure. First, coders logged onto each apparel website and then went to the ‘women’s

pants’ section. Next, they clicked on each pair of pants to go to the individual product

page. On each product page, coders used the categories established in the coding frame.

They were asked to browse at least ten pairs of pants, if available, on each apparel

website to check the consistency of the content across product pages. Each category

could be coded as ‘present’ or ‘absent’. If more than five product pages contained the

same content, then those were coded as ‘present’. Reliability of coding was calculated

using Perreault and Leigh’s (1989) reliability index which takes chance agreement into

Page 105: Osu 1135134346

80

account (Kolbe & Burnett, 1991). The inter-coder reliability between the two coders

was .97 (97 %). Disagreements between the two coders were due to one coder’s failure

to find an occurrence of a particular content. After the coding was complete, results from

the two coders were compared and disagreements were discussed. After the discussion,

agreements between the two coders reached 100%.

Ten categories of web cues were present on 10 or more of the content analyzed

websites (see Table 3.6). Those 10 (out of the 18) categories of web cues, most frequently

presented in existing apparel websites, were used to create a baseline amount of central

cues for the mock websites used in Study 2. These cues were: color information,

enclosure details, fabric information, inseam (length), front larger view, one mix and

match item, country of origin, price information, size information, and style information.

All 18 categories of web cues were used in the high amount of central cues condition.

Thus, the mock websites with the medium amount of central cues contained 10 types of

product related web cues while the mock websites with the high amount of central cues

contained all 18 types of product related web cues. Although mix and match suggestions

were presented in more than 14 of the analyzed apparel websites, only seven websites

provided mix and match suggestions for all products. The number of suggested items for

each product varied from one item to three items and those items could be tops, shoes,

handbags, and other accessories. The other seven websites presented mix and match

suggestions for selected items only. Due to the inconsistencies in products for which mix

and match suggestions were presented and also in numbers of mix and match suggestions

within and across the websites, ‘mix and match suggestions’ in the mock websites used in

Study 2 were manipulated by the number of suggested items (one for the medium amount

Page 106: Osu 1135134346

81

of central cues and three for the high amount of central cues). So both conditions had

some level of mix and match items suggested.

As shown in Table 3.7, the number of available product related web cues in the

product page for each apparel website was also counted. The Jcrew website contained

the largest amount of product related web cues (N = 18) in the product page followed by

Bananarepublic website (N = 17). These two websites are examples of websites with a

high amount of central cues. Abercrombie, alloy, ardenb, delias, and bebe are examples

of the websites with a medium amount of central cues in the product page. When

consumers are shopping for apparel products, they like to physically inspect the products

in terms of fit, color, size, design, and fabric. Due to the nature of the online apparel

purchase process, the inability to examine apparel products and the uncertainty about

color, fabrics, and fit can cause high risk perceptions related to in-home shopping

(Bhatnagar, Misra, & Rao, 2000). Previous research suggested that in nonstore shopping

ample product related information should be provided by using a variety of sources (Kim

& Lennon, 2000; Then & Delong, 1999). Thus, commercial apparel websites should

provide at least a medium amount of product related central cues to reduce uncertainty

about the products and consequently to sell products. As shown in the results of the

content analysis, the popular commercial apparel websites listed in Pilot Study 2 offer at

least a medium amount of central cues (e.g., delias, bebe, Abercrombie, alloy, and

ardenb). To increase the reality of the two main studies, the mock websites used in Study

1 and Study 2 contained at least a medium amount of product related central cues.

Page 107: Osu 1135134346

82

Coding categories Frequencies (N = 15)

Color information 12 * Design details (e.g., pockets) Pockets 7 Other details

(belt loops, stitching)

6

Detail views (e.g., close-up views for front, back, and side, zoom options) 4

Enclosure details (e.g., zipper, buttons) 11 * Fabric information 15 * Fit information 7 Inseam (e.g., length) 11 * Larger views Front 15 * Back 7 Side 1 Mix and match suggestions (e.g., suggested coordinated items such as sweaters, handbag, shoes etc.)

14* Consistent a = 7

Inconsistent b = 7 Originality of the product (e.g., country of origin) 14 * Price information 15 * Product care information (e.g., machine wash, hand wash etc.) 9

Size information 15 * Style information 12 * Waist (e.g., low or high) 6

Note. a all product pages have at least one mix and match suggestion, b some product pages have mix and match suggestions and others do not, * items included in the mock websites for Study 2 with the medium amount of central cues. Table 3.6. Coding categories used in Content Analysis and frequencies of the product related web cues presented in apparel websites.

Page 108: Osu 1135134346

83

Websites

Number of product related information cues

www.jcrew.com 18 www.bananarepublic.com 17 www.gap.com 13 www.nordstrom.com 13 www.ae.com 13 www.urbanoutfitter.com 13 www.forever21.com 13 www.oldnavy.com 13 www.victoriasecret.com 12 www.guess.com 11 www.delias.com 10 www.bebe.com 10 www.abercrombie.com 9 www.alloy.com 9 www.ardenb.com 9

Table 3.7. Number of product related web cues available in the 15 apparel websites analyzed in the Content Analysis.

Page 109: Osu 1135134346

84

CHAPTER 4

MAIN STUDY 1

This research contains two main studies. This chapter presents the method,

analyses, and results of Study 1. The purpose of Study 1 was to examine 1) the effects of

peripheral cues on emotional reactions (pleasure and arousal) under a low involvement

situation, 2) the effects of emotions on consumers’ response behaviors (purchase

intention and approach behaviors), 3) the effects of product involvement as a moderator

between S-O, and 4) the mediating effect of emotions between peripheral cues and

response behaviors. The method part illustrates research design and experimental

manipulations, data collection procedure, and instrument development for main study 1.

Analyses and results describe demographics of participants, dependent variables,

preliminary analyses, and hypotheses testing for Study 1. This research was exempted

from IRB review (Protocol number # 2004E0259, see Appendix R).

Page 110: Osu 1135134346

85

4.1. Method

4.1.1. Research Design and Experimental Manipulations

The design of Study 1 was a between-subjects experiment with one factor

(peripheral cues) with two levels (presence vs. absence). Two mock apparel websites

each with a different amount of peripheral cues were created. Each website consisted of

an instruction page, a scenario page to control the situational involvement, a main page

showing two apparel items together selected in Pilot Study 2, a product page for each of

two apparel items, and a survey page. Each product page had a link to the larger view

(See Appendix E).

Two apparel stimuli selected in Pilot Study 2 were edited for the mock websites

used in Study 1. Front views of two items were prepared in a small size (150 x 190

pixels) and a medium size (225 x 300 pixels). Items in the small size were displayed in

the main page and ones in the medium size were displayed in each product page

(Appendix E). The resolution of front and back larger views was retained to be 450 x

450 pixels for the two apparel items.

Eroglu et al. (2001) indicated that web cues such as beautiful background colors

and patterns, fonts, animation, and graphical icons to click rather than simple underlined

textual hyperlinks could be examples of peripheral cues that might influence consumers’

emotion or the image of an online store. In a later study, Eroglu et al. (2003) used text

with a dark green color rather than black, a background image of a product with the brand

logo, and a green graphic for the links for the website with peripheral cues. In the present

Page 111: Osu 1135134346

86

study, to manipulate peripheral cues in the mock website, the website with the peripheral

cues had text in a blue color (rather than black), a flashing pink brand logo (i.e., heart dot

is blinking), pink and yellow-green icons with roll-over images, and a pink background

with an E-Style logo pattern (See Appendix E). Since the mock online stores used in the

study were targeting young female consumers that are characteristic of the potential

participants for the study, a pink color thought to be more appealing to the target market

was used as a main color in the mock websites with the presence of peripheral cues. In

western culture, pink is traditionally perceived as a feminine color (Clark, 2003). In

addition, pink has been one of the most popular colors in women’s clothing since 2003

(Pink Chic, 2005). The most popular apparel online store listed in Pilot Study 2 (See

Table 3.4), ‘Victoria’s Secret’, introduced a lingerie line ‘Pink’ in 2003 targeting 18 to 22

year old female consumers and now the ‘Pink’ line is found in more than 1000 Victoria’s

Secret stores (Prior, 2005). Victoria’s Secret brand targeting female consumers also uses

pink background for its website. In addition to the brand logo and the background pattern,

previous research also accentuated the importance of the amount of “white space” that

surrounds pictures of the products (Chatterjee, 2001; Eroglu et al., 2001; Eroglu et al.,

2003). Thus, the mock website with the peripheral cues had plenty of white background

surrounding pictures of the products to make the products stand out on the webpage.

White background (about 5 inches x 5.5 inches in 1024 by 786 resolution) surrounded the

descriptions as well as the pictures of the products. On the other hand, the website

without peripheral cues had a static brand logo with a black color, no color icons (i.e.,

text icons with an underline), and white background without any pattern (See Appendix

E).

Page 112: Osu 1135134346

87

To control the effects of central cues in Study 1, the two websites contained the

same amount of product related central cues. All product related cues listed in Table 3.6

excluding detail and side views and mix and match suggestions were included in the

mock website. Detail and side views were excluded because those were hardly found in

existing apparel online stores (See Table 3.6). Due to the inconsistency across existing

apparel websites, mix and match suggestions were also excluded in Study 1 (See Section

3.1.4).

According to previous research (Eroglu et al., 2003; Zaichkowsky, 1986),

browsing without a purchasing goal generates a low level of situational involvement.

Thus, to induce a low involvement situation, all participants were asked to read the same

scenario: “Now, you are going to visit one clothing website. Browse and look around the

site for a while.” This scenario was developed based on Eroglu et al.’s (2003) study and

Pilot Study 1. After browsing the website, participants were asked to finish the survey.

4.1.2. Instrument Development

The dependent measures in Study 1 (See Appendix F) contained five parts. Part 1

assessed respondents’ emotional states (pleasure and arousal) using 12 7-point semantic

differential scales (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). In Part 2, intention behaviors were

measured by four items using 5-point Likert type scales ranging from 1 (unlikely) to 5

(likely) (Park, Lennon, & Stoel, 2005) and approach behaviors were assessed by four

items using 5-point Likert type scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so)

(Huang, 2003). Personal involvement with clothing products using 10 7-point semantic

Page 113: Osu 1135134346

88

differential scales (Zaichkowsky, 1994) were assessed in Part 3. Part 4 checked the

perceived amount of information using 5 items with 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) (Kim & Lennon, 2000) and user-perceived

quality of web appearance using 5 items with 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) (Aladwani & Palvia’s, 2002). Part 5 asked for

respondents’ demographic information such as age (open-ended format) and ethnicity

(selecting a category). Reliability of each dependent measure was established in previous

research. See Section 5.1.2 for detailed information about each instrument.

4.1.3. Procedure

Study 1 was conducted as an online experiment. A convenience sample of 157

female undergraduate students from three different consumer science classes at the Ohio

State University participated in this study for extra credit. Female undergraduate students

were recruited for the study because women are significant Internet apparel purchasers

and browsers (Lee & Johnson, 2002). Apparel stimuli and the mock websites were

developed to target young female consumers.

An instruction and the URL for the survey were given to students during the class.

When participants logged on to the website, they were randomly assigned to one of two

treatment groups (presence or absence of peripheral cues). All participants were asked to

read the instructions on the first page of the website and then read the scenario (low level

of situational involvement) on the next page. After reading the scenario, participants

were asked to browse the two pairs of pants selected in Pilot Study 2. Upon the

Page 114: Osu 1135134346

89

completion of browsing, participants went to the survey page to finish the dependent

measures. See Appendix E for more information.

Page 115: Osu 1135134346

90

4.2. Analyses and Results

This part explains the research analyses and results of Study 1. Study 1 examined

how the peripheral cues and the product involvement influence consumer emotions that

in turn, influence consumer behaviors. Analyses and results of Study 1 are explained in

five sections: the description of participants, manipulation check, the description of

variables measured in Study 1, preliminary analyses to test the validity and reliability of

the measures, and hypotheses testing. Descriptive statistics were used to describe the

research participants and each variable including consumer emotions (pleasure and

arousal), purchase intention, and approach behaviors. A confirmatory factor analysis was

used to evaluate measurement properties. Multivariate analyses of variance were used to

test Hypotheses 1 and 2. Structural equation modeling was used to test Hypotheses 3 and

4. Multiple regression analysis was used to assess Hypothesis 5. Descriptive statistics,

multivariate analyses of variance, and univariate analyses of variance were analyzed by

using SPSS 13.0. Confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling were

analyzed by using Lisrel 8.7 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004).

4.2.1. Description of Participants

All participants were female college students. The mean age of participants

(N=136) was 21, with a range of 18 to 30. More than 80% of participants were aged

between 18 and 22. About 75% of participants were Caucasian American.

Page 116: Osu 1135134346

91

Other participants were African American (9.8%), Asian/Asian American (9.1%),

Hispanic American (3.8%), and other (3%). See Table 4.1 for demographic information.

Demographics Frequencies

Percent

Age Under 20 18 13.2% 20 – 22 96 70.6% 23 – 25 17 12.5% 26 – 30 5 3.7% Total 136* 100% Ethnic Background African American 13 9.8% Caucasian American 98 74.2% Hispanic American 5 3.8% Asian/Asian American 12 9.1% Native American 0 0% Other 4 3.0% Total 132* 100%

Note. * Different Ns are due to missing information. Table 4.1. Demographic profile of participants.

Page 117: Osu 1135134346

92

4.2.2. Manipulation Check

In Study 1, peripheral cues were manipulated with two levels (presence vs.

absence). A manipulation check was performed to determine if participants would

perceive different levels of peripheral cues manipulated in the mock websites.

Participants were randomly assigned to browse only one of two treatment conditions

(presence or absence of peripheral cues) and then asked to assess their perception of the

quality of web appearance for the manipulation check using 5 items with 5-point scales

ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Five items include 1) The

website looks attractive, 2) The website looks organized, 3) The website uses fonts

properly, 4) The website uses colors properly, and 5) The website uses multimedia

features properly.

The reliability of five items were found to be adequate (Cronbach’s α = .91). The

five items were summed to check differences in perceptions across two groups. In order

to test for significant differences in quality perceptions between presence and absence of

peripheral cues, univariate analysis of variance was performed with the levels of

peripheral cues as the independent variable and the perceived quality of web appearance

as the dependent variable. The results revealed a main effect for peripheral cues

(presence or absence) on the perceived quality of web appearance, F (1,154) = 10.028, p

< .01. This indicates that there was a difference between presence and absence of the

mock websites on the perceived quality of web appearance. Mean scores for absence and

presence of peripheral cues were 3.48 (SD = .80) and 3.89 (SD = .79), respectively. A

higher score indicates a higher perceived quality of web appearance. Based on the means,

Page 118: Osu 1135134346

93

participants who browsed the website with peripheral cues perceived the web appearance

of the mock website to be higher quality, while ones who browsed the website without

peripheral cues perceived the web appearance of the mock website to be lower quality.

This suggests that peripheral cues were successfully manipulated in Study 1 since the

website with them was rated higher than the one without them.

4.2.3. Dependent Variables

Study 1 includes four dependent variables, pleasure, arousal, purchase intention,

and approach behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Pleasure and arousal were dependent variables for Hypotheses 1 and 2 and purchase

intention and approach behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites)

were dependent variables for Hypotheses 3, 4, and 5. All four variables were latent

constructs in the proposed model for Study 1. Multiple items were used to measure the

four latent constructs. Descriptive statistics for four latent constructs are presented in this

section.

Emotional States: Pleasure and Arousal

After browsing the mock website, participants were asked to assess their feelings

using 12 7-point semantic scales (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Each pleasure and

arousal scale included six items. Pleasure was measured by happy—unhappy, pleased—

annoyed, satisfied—unsatisfied, contented—melancholic, hopeful—despairing, and

Page 119: Osu 1135134346

94

relaxed—bored and arousal was assessed by stimulated—relaxed, excited—calm,

frenzied—sluggish, jittery—dull, wide-awake—sleepy, and aroused—unaroused.

The reliability of items for pleasure and arousal was established (Cronbach’s α

= .91 and .91, respectively). Based on these results, scores from the six items tapping

pleasure (arousal) were summed to develop a single score for pleasure (arousal) and used

to test Hypotheses 1 and 2. Higher scores indicated that participants experienced more

pleasure or arousal, while lower scores indicated that participants experienced less

intensive emotions. Summed scores for pleasure and arousal were used as dependent

variables in multivariate analysis of variance for Hypotheses 1 and 2 and used as

independent variables in multiple regression analysis for Hypothesis 5. To test

Hypotheses 3 and 4 the six items for pleasure (arousal) were used as multiple indicators

for the pleasure (arousal) latent construct. Descriptive statistics for the six indicators for

each of the pleasure and arousal latent constructs are shown in Table 4.2.

Purchase Intention

Purchase intention was measured by four items used in previous research (Park et

al., 2005). The reliability of the items was calculated to check the internal consistency of

the items and was found to be reliable (Cronbach’s α = .88). The four items were used as

multiple indicators for the purchase intention latent construct in the proposed model for

Hypothesis 3. Purchase intention was used as the dependent variable in Hypothesis 3.

Summed scores for purchase intention were used as the dependent variable in Hypothesis

5. Descriptive statistics of the four indicators for purchase intention are described in

Table 4.2.

Page 120: Osu 1135134346

95

Approach Behaviors

By using four items used in previous research (Huang, 2003) approach behaviors

were measured. Cronbach’s α was calculated to assess the internal consistency of the

four items and was found to be adequate (α = .94). Approach behaviors were used as the

dependent variable in Hypothesis 4 and four items were used as multiple indicators for

approach behavior latent construct in the proposed model for Hypothesis 4. Item scores

were summed and used as a dependent variable (approach behaviors) in Hypothesis 5.

Descriptive statistics of the four items for approach behaviors are presented in Table 4.2.

Page 121: Osu 1135134346

96

Range Min. Max. Mean SD Emotional Reactions Pleasure P1 Happy – Unhappy 5 2 7 5.04 1.05 P2 Pleased – Annoyed 6 1 7 5.11 1.16 P3 Satisfied – Unsatisfied 6 1 7 5.03 1.24 P4 Contented – Melancholic 5 2 7 5.12 1.12 P5 Hopeful – Despairing 6 1 7 4.91 1.37 P6 Relaxed – Bored 6 1 7 4.87 1.31 Arousal A1 Stimulated – Relaxed 6 1 7 4.06 1.51 A2 Excited – Calm 6 1 7 3.91 1.38 A3 Frenzied – Sluggish 6 1 7 4.01 1.24 A4 Jittery – Dull 6 1 7 3.72 1.14 A5 Wide-awake – Sleepy 6 1 7 4.03 1.41 A6 Aroused – Unaroused 6 1 7 3.83 1.43 Purchase Intention PI1 How likely is it that you would buy

clothing items if you happened to see them from E-style.com?

4 1 5 2.82 1.28

PI2 How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-style.com in the next 12 months?

4 1 5 2.61 1.28

PI3 How likely is it that you will shop for apparel from E-style.com when you buy apparel in the upcoming year?

4 1 5 2.64 1.29

PI4 How likely is that you will buy apparel from E-style.com when you find something you like?

4 1 5 3.06 1.23

Approach Behaviors AB1 How much would you enjoy

exploring this site? 4 1 5 3.04 1.21

AB2 Do you like this site? 4 1 5 3.29 1.07 AB3 To what extent is this site a good

opportunity to shop? 4 1 5 3.31 1.05

AB4 Would you enjoy shopping in this site? 4 1 5 3.15 1.15

Table 4.2. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables.

Page 122: Osu 1135134346

97

4.2.4. Assessment of Measurement Properties

In theory testing and theory development research achieving unidimensionality

and construct validity2 of measurements is essential (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982;

Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Koufteros, 1999). In contemporary structural equation

model testing or developing a theory, a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) is a better

technique than an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) when assessing unidimensionality

(internal consistency and external consistency) and construct validity (convergent validity

and discriminant validity) of the measurements (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982; Anderson &

Gerbing, 1988; Anderson, Gerbing, & Hunter, 1987; Baggozi, 1981; Baggozi, Yi, &

Phillips, 1991; Baggozi, Yi, & Nassen, 1999; Gerbing & Anderson, 1984; Gerbing &

Anderson, 1988; Koufteros, 1999). Since a CFA is conducted on the entire set of

measures posited to measure each of latent constructs simultaneously, it directly tests the

unidimensionality of measures internally as well as externally and also evaluates the

construct validity of the measurement items that share each of latent variables (Anderson

& Gerbing, 1982; Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Anderson et al., 1987; Baggozi, 1981;

Baggozi et al., 1991; Baggozi et al., 1999; Gerbing & Anderson, 1984; Gerbing &

Anderson, 1988; Koufteros, 1999). On the other hand, previous research indicates that an

EFA is mainly useful in early stages of scale development particularly when a detailed

theory is missing (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Churchill, 1979; Gerbing & Anderson,

1988; Koufteros, 1999). Some critical pitfalls of the EFA technique in testing the quality

2 Construct validity measures “the psychometric accuracy of the latent variable by examining its association with other latent variables. Two of the most widely examined aspects of construct validity are convergent validity and discriminant validity” (Grefen, 2003, p.30).

Page 123: Osu 1135134346

98

of measurements are also presented in prior research. First, EFA does not provide any

explicit evaluation of unidimensionality and construct validity (Anderson & Gerbing,

1988; Churchill, 1979; Koufteros, 1999) and second, it does not account for external

consistency of unidimensionality (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Gerbing & Anderson,

1988; Koufteros, 1999).

The general structural equation model consists of two conceptually distinct

models: a measurement model and a latent variable model (or a structural model)

(Anderson & Gerbing, 1982; Bollen, 1989). The measurement model specifies the

relations between indicators (observed variables) and the latent variables (theoretical

constructs). On the other hand, the latent variable model shows the causal relations

among the latent variables (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982; Bollen, 1989; Segars & Grover,

1993). Anderson and Gerbing (1988) suggested a two-step modeling approach: the

measurement model should be assessed and respecified prior to the simultaneous

estimation of the measurement and latent variable model. The measurement model

should exhibit a satisfactory level of validity and reliability before performing the

analysis of the full model and if necessary, the measurement model should be respecified

to avoid interpretational confounding in the full model (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982;

Bagozzi, 1981; Fronell & Larker, 1981). Following the two-step approach the quality of

measurements for each latent construct (i.e., the measurement model) was evaluated and

assessed in terms of reliability, convergent validity, unidimensionality, and discriminant

validity by conducting a CFA before testing the proposed model in Study 1.

After comprehensive evaluation of the measurement model, measurement items

posited to measure each latent variable were respecified to purify measures and to reduce

Page 124: Osu 1135134346

99

the potential for interpretational confounding. Four basic methods to respecify the

measurements were suggested by Anderson and Gerbing (1988): 1) relate the

measurement to a different latent construct, 2) remove the measurement from the model,

3) relate the measurement to multiple latent constructs, or 4) correlate measurement

errors in the model. The first two methods are preferred because they preserve the

potential to have unidimensionality of the measurements. The last two methods are

acceptable only when they are based on a priori theory (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988).

Particularly, the uncritical use of correlated measurement errors for the respecification

may take advantage of chance, cause interpretational confounding, and lose theoretical

meaningfulness (Bagozzi, 1983; Fornell, 1983; Gerbing & Anderson, 1984). Based on

the assessment of the measurement model, respecifications of the measures were

performed by removing the problematic measurements from the model (the second

method suggested by Anderson and Gerbing). The following criteria were used to select

the measurement items for respecification: 1) if measurements’ path coefficients on the

posited latent construct were insignificant, 2) if item reliability (e.g., squared multiple

correlation) of the measurement was lower than the .5 standard (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991;

Bollen, 1989), 3) if the measurement had large residuals with other indicators, 4) if the

measure had highly correlated unexplainable error variances with other indicators (large

modification indices for Θδ), and 5) if the measurement shared common variance with

other indicators posited to measure other latent constructs (large modification indices for

λ). As recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), the model modification was

made based on statistical and theoretical consideration.

Page 125: Osu 1135134346

100

Table 4.3 shows the final 12 items posited to measure four latent constructs in the

model tested in Study 1. Three indicators remained for each of four latent constructs in

the final model. P5, P6, and A1 were eliminated because their squared multiple

correlations did not meet the .5 criterion (.47, .49, and .49 respectively) whereas the other

indicators exhibited squared multiple correlations (item reliability) ranging from .55

to .87, exceeding the .5 standard. Since P3, A1, A2, A4, and PI3 had highly correlated

error variances with other indicators, they were removed from the model. AB2 was

deleted because it shared common variance with other indicators from other latent

constructs such as pleasure and purchase intention. After the completion of measurement

assessments and respecifications, the adjusted measurement model was assessed in terms

of construct validity, unidimensionality, and construct reliability by conducting a CFA.

Page 126: Osu 1135134346

101

Latent Constructs Indicators Measurement Items

P1 Happy – Unhappy P2 Pleased – Annoyed P4 Contented – Melancholic

A3 Frenzied – Sluggish A5 Wide-awake – Sleepy

Pleasure Arousal

A6 Aroused – Unaroused

PI1 How likely is it that you would buy clothing items if you happened to see them from E-style.com?

PI2 How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-style.com in the next 12 months?

Purchase Intention

PI4 How likely is that you will buy apparel from E-style.com when you find something you like?

AB1 How much would you enjoy exploring this site?

AB3 To what extent is this site a good opportunity to shop?

Approach Behaviors

AB4 Would you enjoy shopping in this site?

Table 4.3. Final measurement items for each of four latent constructs.

Page 127: Osu 1135134346

102

Convergent Validity

Convergent validity is the extent of convergence seen when different attempts are

made to measure the same construct through maximally different methods. Convergent

validity examines the extent of correlation between item measures of a construct across

multiple methods of measurement (Grefen, 2003). Correlations between the different

measures of the same construct should be statistically significant and sufficiently large

(Campbell & Fiske, 1959). In a CFA, convergent validity is assessed by each

measurement’s (observed variable) path coefficient (i.e., factor loading) on its posited

latent variable (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Bagozzi et al., 1991; Koufteros, 1999). The

corresponding t-values (the ratio of path coefficient to its respective standard error)

indicate whether factor loadings are significant or not (Bagozzi et al., 1991; Bollen, 1989;

Koufteros, 1999). In addition to significant path coefficients t-values, the CFA exhibits

item reliability (i.e., squared multiple correlations). Squared multiple correlations that

exceed the .5 standard support convergent validity (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991; Bollen, 1989).

As indicated in Table 4.4, all path coefficients were significant at the p < .0001

level indicating that all measurement items are significantly related to their specified

latent constructs. In addition, squared multiple correlations of all indicators exceeded

the .5 standard (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991; Bollen, 1989). Significant t-values as well as high

squared multiple correlations indicated that convergent validity was achieved.

Page 128: Osu 1135134346

103

Latent Constructs

Indicators Unstandardized factor loading

Completely standardized

factor loading

t-values Item reliability

Pleasure P1 .92 .87 13.35*** .77 P2 .96 .83 12.31*** .69 P4 1.00 .90 13.84*** .80 Arousal A3 .94 .76 10.72*** .58 A5 1.24 .89 13.37*** .79 A6 1.20 .84 12.33*** .71 Purchase Intention

PI1 1.13 .88 13.55*** .78

PI2 .97 .76 10.90*** .58 PI4 .88 .72 10.04*** .51 Approach Behaviors

AB1 1.11 .91 14.66*** .83

AB3 .88 .84 12.93*** .71 AB4 1.07 .94 15.58*** .89

Note. *** p < .0001

Table 4.4. Factor loadings, t-values, and item reliability for convergent validity.

Page 129: Osu 1135134346

104

Unidimensionality

If each construct is measured by multiple items and each measurement item

measures only one latent construct, the set of measurement items defining each construct

is unidimensional (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982). Two criteria—internal consistency and

external consistency—are used to assess unidimensionality (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988;

Koufteros, 1999). Internal consistency is met if multiple indicators of each latent

construct converge to measure a single construct and external consistency is met if

multiple indicators of each latent construct are widely different from the indicators of

other latent constructs in the model (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Koufteros, 1999). In

CFA, both internal and external consistencies of the unidimensionality are assessed by

the evaluation of the model fit along with standardized residuals, modification indices,

and expected change.

An overall chi-square was not significant (χ2 = 53.84, df = 48, p = .2607)

indicating a good fit of the model to the data. The RMSEA was .028 and the NNFI index

was 1.00. The GFI was .95 and the AGFI was .91. All fit indices are good within

acceptable ranges indicating strong evidence of the internal and external consistency of

the unidimensionality. In addition, standardized residuals and modification indices with

expected change, which can also provide useful information in the assessment of the

measurement model and particularly unidimensionality, were inspected. No standardized

residuals were greater than 2.58 (or less than -2.58) (Grefen, 2003; Joreskog & Sorbom,

1989), no modification indices for λx and Θδ were significantly large (all less than 5)

(Grefen, 2003), and completely standardized expected change for λx and Θδ were not

significantly large (all less than .3), providing significant evidence of unidimensionality

Page 130: Osu 1135134346

105

(Koufteros, 1999). Appendix N shows standardized residuals for the 12 measurement

items.

Discriminant Validity

Discriminant validity is the extent to which a latent construct and its measures

differ from other constructs and their indicators (Churchill, 1979; Grefen, 2003).

Discriminant validity can be assessed by three methods: 1) perform chi-square difference

test for the constrained (correlation between two estimated latent constructs is set to 1)

and unconstrained model (correlation between two constructs is freely measured); if the

chi-square value for the unconstrained model is significantly lower than the value for the

constrained model, it indicates that two latent constructs are not perfectly correlated and

that discriminant validity is achieved (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Bagozzi & Phillips,

1982; Bagozzi et al., 1991; Koufteros, 1999), 2) determine whether a confidence interval

constructed by the correlation between two latent constructs plus or minus two standard

errors includes 1; if the confidence interval does not include 1, it is the evidence of

discriminant validity (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988; Koufteros, 1999), and 3) compare the

average variance extracted (AVE) with the squared correlation between constructs; if

AVE for a construct is higher than the squared correlation between the construct and

other constructs, discriminant validity is established (Fornell & Larker, 1981; Koufteros,

1999; Segars, 1997). For the first method, the chi-square test should be performed for

one pair of latent constructs at a time rather than as a simultaneous test of all pairs of

latent constructs (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988).

Page 131: Osu 1135134346

106

Based on the first method of evaluating discriminant validity, all the chi-square

differences between the unconstrained and constrained models were significant (See

Table 4.5), thus indicating discriminant validity of the measures. Evidence of

discriminant validity is also supported by the second method. As shown in Table 4.6,

none of the confidence intervals include the value of 1 supporting discriminant validity.

Additional evidence of discriminant validity is also provided by the third method. The

AVE for each latent variable was higher than the squared correlation between the

construct and all other constructs (See Table 4.6). This indicated that the measures for

each latent variable shared more common variance with their posited construct than any

variance shared with other constructs, thus discriminant validity was achieved.

Constraint

Chi-square

df

Chi-square difference (Δχ2)

df = 1 Unconstrained model 53.84 48 Pleasure and Arousal 220.62 49 166.78*** Pleasure and Purchase Intention 353.70 49 299.86*** Pleasure and Approach Behaviors 227.65 49 173.81*** Arousal and Purchase Intention 183.72 49 129.88*** Arousal and Approach Behaviors 204.81 49 150.97*** Purchase Intention and Approach Behaviors 69.44 49 15.60**

Note. *** p < .0001, ** p < .005

Table 4.5. Chi-square difference tests for discriminant validity3

3 “When a number of chi-square difference tests are performed for assessements of discriminant validity, the significance level for each test should be adjusted to maintain the “true” overall significance level for the family of the test (cf. Finn, 1974). This adjustment can be given as α0 = 1-(1-αi)t, where α0 is the overall significance level that should be used for each individual hypothesis test of discrminant validity; and t is the number of tests performed” (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988, p. 416).

Page 132: Osu 1135134346

107

Latent Constructs

Pleasure

Arousal

Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

Pleasure .75e Arousal .55a

(.07)b

(.41, .69)c .30d

.70

Purchase Intention .50

(.07) (.36, .64)

.25

.57 (.07)

(.43, .71) .33

.67

Approach Behaviors .54

(.06) (.42, .66)

.29

.62 (.06)

(.50, .74) .38

.82 (.02)

(.78, .86) .64

.81

Note. a Correlation, b Standard Error, c Confidence Interval, d Squared Correlation, e Average variance extracted.

Table 4.6. Correlations and confidence intervals for discriminant validity.

Page 133: Osu 1135134346

108

Assessment of Reliability

Reliability is the consistency of the measurement which is different from the

validity of measures (Anderson et al., 1987; Bollen, 1989). After the unidimensionality

of a set of measures is established, its reliability should be assessed because even

unidimensional measures will not be useful if they have unacceptably low reliability

(Gerbing & Anderson, 1988). A typical reliability assessment, Cronbach α coefficient, is

a commonly used index for evaluating reliability. However, since it is based on restricted

assumptions of equal importance of all indicators, reliability in structural equation

modeling is assessed by composite reliability and average variance extracted (AVE)

which are defined in terms of the factor loading of the measurement (Fornell & Larcker,

1981: Gerbing & Anderson, 1988; Koufteros, 1999: Zhang, Lim, & Cao, 2004).

Composite reliability means that a set of measures posited to measure a latent construct is

consistent in their measurement. In other words, highly reliable latent constructs are

those in which the measures are highly intercorrelated meaning that they are all

measuring the same latent construct (Fornell & Larcker, 1981). No generally acceptable

value has been established for composite reliability. A value of .80 or higher is

considered as a strong composite reliability (Grefen, 2003). Some researchers suggests

different standard value such as .60 (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988) or .70 (Lusch & Brown, 1996;

Sun & Zhang, 2004; Segars, 1997). A value of .70 is often cited as the normal threshold

(Segars, 1997). The AVE measures the variance captured by the measures relative to

measurement error, and it should be greater than .50 to justify the reliability (Bagozzi &

Yi, 1988; Fornell & Larcker, 1981; Segars, 1997). Higher variance extracted values

happen when the measures are truly representative of the latent construct. As shown in

Page 134: Osu 1135134346

109

Table 4.7, the composite reliability of all latent constructs exceeded .80, indicating the

strong composite reliability. In addition, the AVE estimates for four latent constructs

exceeded the .50 critical value indicating the further evidence of the reliability.

Latent Constructs

Composite reliability4

Average variance extracted (AVE)5

Status

Pleasure .90 .75 Accepted Arousal .87 .70 Accepted Purchase Intention .86 .67 Accepted Approach Behaviors .93 .81 Accepted

Note. Minimum standards for composite reliability and AVE are .80 and .50, respectively.

Table 4.7. Composite reliability and AVE of latent constructs.

4 Composite reliability = (Σλi)2/{(Σλi)2+Σθi} (Fornell & Larcker, 1981; Grefen, 2003; Segars, 1997) 5 Average variance extracted (AVE) = (Σλi

2)/{(Σλi2)+Σθi} (Fornell & Lalrcker, 1981; Grefen, 2003; Segars,

1997)

Page 135: Osu 1135134346

110

Model Specification

Once an acceptable measurement model is available, an evaluation of the

structural model can begin. Structural equation modeling was used to test Hypotheses 3

and 4 in Study 1. After the assessment of the measures, 12 indicators were selected for

the model specification. The structural model specified in Study 1 is presented in Figure

4.1. The proposed model consisted of four latent variables with 12 indicators (manifest

variables). Two latent constructs (pleasure and arousal) were exogenous latent variables

(ξ) and the other two latent constructs (purchase intention and approach behaviors) were

endogenous latent variables (η). Each of four latent constructs had three indicators. For

identification purposes, the variances of the two exogenous latent constructs were set to

one (set the diagonal elements of Φ matrix to one) and error variances of two endogenous

latent constructs were set to one (set the diagonal elements of Ψ matrix to one) (Boker &

McArdle, 2005).

Data Screening

The data were prepared using PRELIS program and assessed for the multivariate

normality assumption. Appendix O shows the data screening result for the observed

variables presenting mean, SD, skewness, and kurtosis. The distribution of the observed

variable was evaluated based on skewness and kurtosis. The distribution with skewness

and kurtosis equal to zero are considered as multivariate normal. Skewness equal to 2

and kurtosis equal to 7 are considered as moderately nonnormal and skewness equal to 3

and kurtosis equal to 21 are considered as severely nonnormal (Curran, West, & Finch,

1996). As shown in Appendix O, all skewness coefficients of the variables were close to

Page 136: Osu 1135134346

111

zero (ranged from .001 to .733) and all kurtosis coefficients were close to zero (ranged

from -1.184 to .778), indicating that the distribution of the observed variables were

multivariate normal. Therefore, the maximum likelihood (ML) function was used to

estimate model parameters with a covariance matrix in Study 1. Under the multivariate

normality assumptions and the proper model specification, the ML procedure provides

asymptotically unbiased, consistent, and efficient parameter estimates and standard errors

(Bollen, 1989).

Page 137: Osu 1135134346

112

Figure 4.1. Model specification for Study 1.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior η2

Purchase Intention η1

P1 P2 P4 PI1 PI2 PI4

A3 A5 A6 AB1 AB3 AB4

θδ11 θδ22 θδ33

θδ44 θδ55 θδ66

θε11 θε22 θε33

θε44 θε55 θε66

λx11 λx21 λx31

λx42 λx52 λx62

λy11 λy21 λy31

λy42 λy52 λy62

γ11

γ21

γ12

γ22

1

1

ς1

ς2

Page 138: Osu 1135134346

113

Note. Hypotheses 1 and 2 were analyzed using multivariate analyses of variance, Hypotheses 3 and 4 were tested using a structural equation modeling, Hypothesis 5 was assessed using a multiple regression analysis.

Figure 4.2. The proposed model in Study 1.

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

Product Involvement

Approach Behavior

Purchase Intention H1a

H1b

H2a and H2b

H3a

H4a

H4b

H3b

H5

Stimulus Organism Response

Page 139: Osu 1135134346

114

4.2.5. Hypothesis Testing

The design of Study 1 was a between-subjects experiment with one factor

(peripheral cues) with two levels (presence vs. absence). Hypothesis 1 examined in the

low situational involvement how the peripheral cues influence emotional states.

Hypothesis 2 investigated under the low involvement situation how the product

involvement influences the relations between peripheral cues and emotional states.

Hypotheses 3 and 4 examined the relationships between emotional states such as pleasure

and arousal and consumer behaviors such as purchase intention and approach behaviors.

Hypothesis 5 assessed the mediating effects of emotional states between peripheral cues

and response behaviors. See Figure 4.2 for the detailed information.

Hypothesis 1

Hypothesis 1. Under the low involvement situation, site atmospheric cues

(peripheral cues) will influence emotional reactions.

Hypothesis 1 was tested using a multivariate analysis of variance. The

independent variable was absence or presence of peripheral cues and the dependent

variables were pleasure and arousal. Between subjects multivariate analysis of variance

revealed a significant multivariate main effect for peripheral cues on emotional reactions

experienced during browsing the websites, F (2, 151) = 4.95, p < .01. Univariate

between subjects analysis of variance revealed significant main effects for peripheral

cues on pleasure, F (1, 152) = 7.15, p < .01 and on arousal, F (1, 152) = 8.36, p < .005.

Page 140: Osu 1135134346

115

In addition, omega squared (ω2) was calculated to measure treatment magnitude. The

index omega squared6 provides a relative measure of the treatment effect, reflecting the

proportion of variation accounted for by the treatment manipulation in an experiment

(Keppel, 1991). Omega squared ranges from 0 to 1, zero as no treatment effects.

According to Cohen’s suggested convention, an ω2 equal to .01 is considered to be a

small effect, an ω2 equal to .06 is considered to be a medium effect, and an ω2 equal

to .15 is considered to be a large effect. Regarding treatment effects, a small effect size

(ω2 = .01) represents the lower limit of a meaningful effect and a medium effect size is

considered to be meaningful and definitely worthy of the experiment. Due to effects of

error variance, the potential of a large value of ω2 is highly unlikely. A significant F

statistic implies that ω2 is also significantly greater than zero (Keppel, 1991).

Hypothesis 1a. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

pleasure.

Univariate analysis of variance revealed a significant main effect for peripheral

cues on pleasure experienced while browsing the websites, F (1, 152) = 7.15, p < .01, ω2

= .045. Mean scores for pleasure [M = 5.32, SD = .99] in the presence of peripheral cues

were higher than those for pleasure [M = 4.88, SD = 1.01] in the condition without

peripheral cues. The results indicated that participants experienced more pleasure while

browsing the website with the presence of peripheral cues as compared to participants in

6 ω2

A = σ2A / (σ2

A + σ2S/A)

Page 141: Osu 1135134346

116

the condition without peripheral cues. Effect size (ω2 = .045) was close to the medium

effect according to Cohen’s guides. Nearly 4.5% of variance in pleasure is accounted for

by the experiment treatment of the peripheral cues. Thus, Hypothesis 1a was supported.

Hypothesis 1b. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

arousal.

Univariate between subjects analysis of variance revealed a significant main

effect for peripheral cues on arousal experienced while browsing the websites, F (1, 152)

= 8.36, p<.005, ω2 = .052. Mean scores for arousal [M = 4.26, SD = 1.18] in the

presence of peripheral cues were higher than those for arousal [M = 3.66, SD = 1.12] in

the condition without peripheral cues. The results indicated that participants experienced

more arousal while browsing the website with the presence of peripheral cues as

compared to participants in the condition without peripheral cues. According to Cohen’s

convention, effect size (ω2 = .052) was close to the medium effect. Approximately 5.2%

of variance in arousal is accounted for by the experiment treatment of peripheral cues.

Therefore, Hypothesis 1b was supported.

Page 142: Osu 1135134346

117

Hypothesis 2

To test for a moderating effect of product involvement (with clothing) on the

relationship between peripheral cues and emotional states, participants were categorized

into low or high product involvement groups. To identify groups according to

participants’ product involvement with clothing, scores of the 10 items measuring

product involvement were summed (See Table 4.8). Then, the median value for the

summed product involvement items was calculated (median = 50). Six participants

received the median score. Participants with scores greater than or equal to the median

value were categorized into high product involvement group (N = 79), and subjects with

lower scores than the median value were categorized into low product involvement group

(N = 75).

Page 143: Osu 1135134346

118

Items Min. Max. Mean Median SD Unimportant-Important 1 7 5.59 6 1.42 Irrelevent-Relevent 1 7 5.60 6 1.41 Means a lot to me-Means nothing to mea

1 7 5.03 5 1.73

Valuable-Worthlessa 1 7 5.25 5 1.48 Boring-Interesting 1 7 5.21 6 1.67 Unexciting-Exciting 1 7 5.14 5 1.67 Appealing-Unappealinga

1 7 5.28 6 1.63

Mundane-Fascinating 1 7 4.97 5 1.63 Not needed-Needed 1 7 5.35 5.5 1.64 Involving-Uninvolvinga 1 7 4.88 5 1.57 Cronbach’s α = .957 Summed Product Involvement with Clothing

10 70 52.19 50.00 13.42

Note. aReverse coded

Table 4.8. Descriptive statistics for clothing involvement items.

Page 144: Osu 1135134346

119

Hypothesis 2. Under the low involvement situation, product involvement (i.e.,

personal relevance of clothing products) will moderate the relationship between

peripheral cues and emotional reactions.

Hypothesis 2 was tested using multivariate analyses of variance. A 2 (product

involvement) x 2 (peripheral cues) between subjects multivariate analysis of variance was

conducted to evaluate the product involvement by peripheral cues two-way interaction.

Product involvement and peripheral cues were the independent variables and emotional

states were the dependent variables. There was a significant multivariate two-way

interaction, F (2, 150) = 3.13, p < .05. The analysis further revealed significant main

effects of peripheral cues (F (2, 150) = 4.49, p < .05) and product involvement (F (2,

150) = 7.17, p < .001) on emotional states. Univariate analyses of variance revealed the

significant main effects for peripheral cues on pleasure (F (1, 151) = 6.66, p < .05) and on

arousal (F (1, 151) = 7.08, p < .01). Univariate analysis of variance revealed a significant

main effect for product involvement on pleasure (F (1, 151) = 13.99, p < .001). The main

effect for product involvement on arousal was not significant (F (1, 151) = 1.57, p

= .212). Further analysis showed a significant interaction effect for product involvement

by peripheral cues on pleasure (F (1, 151) = 3.98, p < .05) and arousal (F (1, 151) = 5.33,

p < .05).

Keppel (1991) suggested testing simple effects when the interaction is significant.

The interaction effect in the study was found to be significant, indicating the simple

effects of peripheral cues were not the same at each level of product involvement. To

Page 145: Osu 1135134346

120

evaluate the simple effects of peripheral cues the sample was divided into two product

involvement groups.

Multivariate analysis of variance was conducted for each of two product

involvement groups (low vs. high). The independent variable was absence or presence of

peripheral cues and the dependent variables were pleasure and arousal. Between subjects

multivariate analysis of variance for low product involvement group revealed a

significant multivariate main effect for peripheral cues on emotional reactions

experienced during browsing the websites, F (2, 73) = 6.18, p < .005. On the other hand,

the result of multivariate analysis of variance for the high product involvement group was

not significant, F (2, 75) = .093, p = .912. These results indicate that the effects of

peripheral cues on emotional states are significant only for participants with low personal

relevance of clothing products. For the low product involvement group, to ascertain

which of the dependent variables contributed to the overall significant multivariate main

effects, univariate between subjects analyses of variance were computed for pleasure and

arousal. The results revealed a significant main effect for peripheral cues on pleasure (F

(1, 74) = 8.25, p < .005) and arousal (F (1, 74) = 10.22, p < .005). Omega squared (ω2)

was also calculated to measure the magnitude of the treatment effects in the study.

Page 146: Osu 1135134346

121

Hypothesis 2a. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on pleasure for people

with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Results of multivariate analysis of variance for low product involvement group

showed significant effects for peripheral cues on emotional states experienced while

browsing the websites and univariate between subjects analysis of variance revealed a

significant effect for peripheral cues on pleasure, F (1, 74) = 8.25, p < .005, ω2 = .087.

According to Cohen’s suggested convention, the effect size (ω2) indicated a moderate to

large effect of peripheral cues on pleasure. Nearly 8.7 % of the total variance in pleasure

is accounted for by the treatment of peripheral cues. Inspection of cell means of pleasure

revealed that when product involvement was low, participants who browsed the website

with the presence of peripheral cues experienced greater pleasure [M = 5.18, SD = 1.18]

than those who browsed the website without peripheral cues [M = 4.43, SD = 1.03]. This

difference was larger when the product involvement was low rather than when the

product involvement was high (See Table 4.9 and Figure 4.3). For the high product

involvement group multivariate analysis of variance was non-significant so further

univariate analysis of variance was not conducted. Since the effect of peripheral cues on

pleasure was significant only for the group with low product involvement and not for the

group with high product involvement, peripheral cues had a stronger effect on pleasure

for participants with low product involvement than for those with high product

involvement. Thus, Hypothesis 2a was supported.

Page 147: Osu 1135134346

122

Hypothesis 2b. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on arousal for people

with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Between subjects multivariate analysis of variance for low product involvement

group revealed a significant multivariate main effect for peripheral cues on emotional

reactions and univariate analysis of variance revealed a significant main effect for

peripheral cues on arousal, F (1, 74) = 10.22, p < .005, ω2 = .108. According to Cohen’s

rule, effect size (ω2) indicated a moderate to large effect of peripheral cues on arousal.

Nearly 10.8 % of the total variance in arousal is accounted for by the treatment of

peripheral cues. Inspection of cell means of arousal revealed that when product

involvement was low, participants exposed to the website with the presence of peripheral

cues exhibited greater arousal [M = 4.30, SD = 1.22] than those exposed to the website

without peripheral cues [M = 3.37, SD = 1.17]. This difference was larger when the

product involvement was low rather than when product involvement was high (See Table

4.10 and Figure 4.4). For the high product involvement group there were no significant

multivariate effects for peripheral cues on emotional states, thus no further analysis was

performed. The results suggested that the effect of peripheral cues on arousal was

significant only for the low product involvement group; thus, peripheral cues had a

stronger effect on arousal for participants with low product involvement than those with

high product involvement. Therefore, Hypothesis 2b was supported

Page 148: Osu 1135134346

123

presenceabsence

Peripheral Cues

5.60

5.40

5.20

5.00

4.80

4.60

4.40

Plea

sure low

highproduct involvement

4.43

5.18

5.38

5.47

Figure 4.3. Effects of peripheral cues and product involvement on pleasure.

Page 149: Osu 1135134346

124

Product Involvement

Low (N = 75) High (N = 79) Mean SD Mean SD

Absence 4.43 1.18 5.38 .88 Peripheral Cues Presence 5.18 1.03 5.47 .94

Difference .75 .09

Table 4.9. Mean differences for pleasure influenced by peripheral cues and product involvement interaction.

Product Involvement

Low (N = 75) High (N = 79) Mean SD Mean SD

Absence 3.37 1.22 4.03 1.02 Peripheral Cues Presence 4.30 1.27 4.10 1.06

Difference .93 .07

Table 4.10. Mean differences for arousal influenced by peripheral cues and product involvement interaction.

Page 150: Osu 1135134346

125

presenceabsence

Peripheral Cues

4.40

4.20

4.00

3.80

3.60

3.40

3.20

Aro

usal low

highproduct involvement

3.37

4.30

4.03

4.10

Figure 4.4. Effects of peripheral cues and product involvement on arousal.

Page 151: Osu 1135134346

126

Hypotheses 3 and 4

To test Hypotheses 3 and 4 structural equation modeling using Lisrel 8.7

(Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004) was used. The focus of Hypotheses 3 and 4 was to

investigate the relationships between emotional states (pleasure and arousal) induced by

peripheral cues in the websites and consumer response behaviors (purchase intention and

approach behaviors). The proposed model (Figure 4.1) consisted of four latent variables

with 12 indicators (manifest variables). Two latent constructs (pleasure and arousal)

were exogenous latent variables (ξ) and the other two latent constructs (purchase

intention and approach behaviors) were endogenous latent variables (η). The model was

analyzed using the maximum likelihood (ML) procedure with a covariance matrix.

Model fit. To assess the fit of the model to the data, chi-square, RMSEA, GFI,

AGFI, and NNFI were computed. The chi-square statistic measures the difference

between the observed covariance matrix and the expected covariance matrix obtained

when the model is fit to the sample (Bollen, 1989). The chi-square statistic tests the null

hypothesis that there is no difference between the observed covariance matrix and the

expected matrix from the hypothesized model. A significant chi-square statistic indicates

that the null hypothesis of perfect fit is rejected while an insignificant chi-square statistic

indicates that the null hypothesis of perfect fit is not rejected and the hypothesized model

is plausible. However, due to the sensitivity of the chi-square statistic to sample sizes

and to models with large numbers of indicators, the statistically significant chi-square

value, indicating the significant departure of the observed covariance matrix from the

Page 152: Osu 1135134346

127

expected covariance matrix, is often found with large sample sizes even though the

discrepancy is small. In contrast, the insignificant chi-square statistic is often found with

very small sample sizes although the discrepancy is not small (Bagozzi & Phillips, 1982;

Bagozzi & Phillips, 1991; Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1995;

Segars & Grover, 1993). Due to the potential problems with the chi-square statistic in

evaluating the fit of the model, other model fit indices were also used to assess the model

fit such as RMSEA, NNFI, AGFI, and GFI.

The RMSEA (root mean square error of approximation), the GFI (goodness of fit

index), and the AGFI (adjusted goodness of fit index) are examples of the Absolute Fit

Indices assessing how well an a priori model reproduces the sample data (Hu & Bentler,

1999). The RMSEA is a measure of the discrepancy per degree of freedom for the model

and thus, imposes a penalty for adding more parameters to the model without

substantially improving the discrepancy between the population covariance matrix and

the fitted matrix (MacCallum, Browne, & Sugawara, 1996). Guidelines for interpretation

of the RMSEA are suggested by previous research: RMSEA less than .05 indicates a

close fit of the model, RMSEA in the range of .05 to .08 indicates a fair fit, and RMSEA

greater than .10 indicates a poor fit (Brown & Cudeck, 1992; MacCallum et al., 1996).

The GFI measures the relative amount of the variances and covariances in the

sample covariance matrix that are predicted by the model estimate of the population

covariance matrix (Bollen, 1989; MacCallum & Hong, 1997). Although the GFI has

been commonly used to test a model fit, it has a significant limitation. The GFI increases

as more parameters are introduced into a model. It favors more complex models over

simpler models. Therefore, the AGFI was suggested by previous research (Joreskog &

Page 153: Osu 1135134346

128

Sorbom, 1984). The AGFI is an adjusted GFI for the degrees of freedom of a model

relative to the number of variables (Bollen, 1989; MacCallum & Hong, 1997). The GFI

greater than or equal to .95 and the AGFI greater than .90 are widely acceptable criteria

suggesting a good fit of the model (Kim, 2005; MacCallum & Hong, 1997).

The NNFI (Non-Normed Fit Index) is one of the Incremental Fit Measures

comparing a given model under the study to two reference models: a null model

(uncorrelated indicators in the population) and an ideal model (a true model). NNFI

greater than or equal to .95 is a desirable threshold value, indicating a good fit (Bentler &

Bonett, 1980; Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1999).

The results of fitting the structural model to the data indicate that the model had a

good fit (Table 4.11). All path coefficients for the measurement model were significant,

indicating the validity of the observed variables posited to measure latent constructs. The

specified relationships between emotional states and consumer response behaviors were

supported by significant t-values. An insignificant chi-square 53.84 (df = 48, p = .26)

indicated that the null hypothesis of perfect fit was not rejected. A small RMSEA (.028)

indicated a close fit of the model according to Brown and Cudeck (1992). GFI was .95,

AGFI was .91, and NNFI was 1.00. All fit indices within acceptable ranges (See Table

4.11) suggested that the proposed model in Study 1 fits the data very well. Table 4.11

presents the results of the model fit including all path coefficients for the measurement

and structural models. Figure 4.5 and 4.6 showed all parameter estimates

(unstandardized and standardized) calculated in the proposed model.

Page 154: Osu 1135134346

129

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Structural Model Pleasure (ξ1) Purchase Intention (η1) γ11 .34 .13 2.68** Pleasure (ξ1) Approach Behaviors (η2) γ21 .39 .12 3.25** Arousal (ξ2) Purchase Intention (η1) γ12 .54 .13 3.99*** Arousal (ξ2) Approach Behaviors (η2) γ22 .62 .13 4.73*** Measurement Model Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 .92 .07 13.35*** Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 .96 .08 12.31*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.00 .07 13.84*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 .94 .09 10.72*** Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.24 .09 13.37*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.20 .10 12.33*** Purchase Intention (η1) PI1 λy11 .89 .07 12.06*** Purchase Intention (η1) PI2 λy21 .77 .08 10.15*** Purchase Intention (η1) PI4 λy31 .69 .07 9.45*** Approach Behaviors (η2) AB1 λy41 .83 .06 13.45*** Approach Behaviors (η2) AB3 λy51 .66 .05 12.08*** Approach Behaviors (η2) AB4 λy61 .80 .06 14.10*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .55 .07 8.42*** Purchase Intention (η1) Approach Behaviors (η2)

ψ21 .70 .06 11.78***

Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 53.84

df = 48 p = .26

Acceptable Criteria RMSEA .028 Less than .05 : Close fit

The range of .05 to .08: Fair fit Greater than .10 : Poor fit

(Brown & Cudeck, 1992; MacCallum et al., 1996)

GFI .95 Greater than or equal to .95: Good fit

(Kim, 2005; MacCallum & Hong, 1997)

AGFI .91 Greater than .90: Good fit (Kim, 2005; MacCallum & Hong, 1997)

NNFI 1.00 Greater than or equal to .95: Good fit

(Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1999)

Note. **p < .01, ***p< .001

Table 4.11. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit for Hypotheses 3 and 4.

Page 155: Osu 1135134346

130

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 4.5. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Hypotheses 3 and 4.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior η2

Purchase Intention η1

P1 P2 P4 PI1 PI2 PI4

A3 A5 A6 AB1 AB3 AB4

.92 .96 1.00

.94 1.24 1.20

.89 .77 .69

.83 .66 .80

.34

.39

.54

.62

1

1

.55 .70

ς1

ς2

1

1

Page 156: Osu 1135134346

131

Figure 4.6. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Hypotheses 3 and 4.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior η2

Purchase Intention η1

P1 P2 P4 PI1 PI2 PI4

A3 A5 A6 AB1 AB3 AB4

.87 .83 .90

.76 .89 .84

.88 .76 .72

.91 .84 .94

.26

.29

.43

.46

ς1

ς2

.55 .79

Page 157: Osu 1135134346

132

Hypothesis 3. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal experienced from

the apparel website will influence purchase intention.

Hypothesis 3 proposed that emotional states induced by the peripheral cues in the

websites will influence purchase intention. The results of the structural model showed

significant effects of emotional states on purchase intention (pleasure: γ11 = .34, t = 2.68,

p < .01, arousal: γ12 = .54, t = 3.99, p < .001), indicating that emotional states experienced

while browsing the apparel websites influenced purchase intention behaviors.

Hypothesis 3a. Pleasure will be positively related to purchase intention.

Hypothesis 3a proposed that pleasure induced by the peripheral cues presented in

the apparel website will be positively related to purchase intention. The significant path

coefficient showed a positive effect of pleasure on purchase intention (γ11 = .34, t = 2.68,

p < .01), indicating that participants who experienced more pleasure during browsing the

websites tended to have higher purchase intention toward the website they browsed.

Therefore, Hypothesis 3a was supported.

Hypothesis 3b. Arousal will be positively related to purchase intention.

Hypothesis 3b proposed that arousal induced by the peripheral cues while

browsing the websites will be positively related to purchase intention. The significant

path coefficient showed a positive effect of arousal on purchase intention (γ12 = .54, t =

Page 158: Osu 1135134346

133

3.99, p < .001), implying that participants who experienced more arousal while browsing

the websites tended to have higher purchase intention. Therefore, Hypothesis 3b was

supported.

Hypothesis 4. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal experienced from

the apparel website will influence approach behaviors (desire to explore or shop

and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 4 predicted that emotional states induced by the peripheral cues in the

websites will influence approach behaviors. The results of the structural model showed

significant effects of emotional states on approach behaviors (pleasure: γ21 = .39, t = 3.25,

p < .001, arousal: γ22 = .62, t = 4.73, p < .001), indicating that emotional states

experienced while browsing the apparel websites had an effect on approach behaviors.

H4a. Pleasure will be positively related to approach behaviors (desire to explore

or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 4a proposed that pleasure induced by the peripheral cues presented in

the apparel websites will be positively related to approach behaviors. The significant

path coefficient showed a positive effect of pleasure on approach behaviors (γ21 = .39, t =

3.25, p < .001), implying that participants who experienced more pleasure while

browsing the apparel websites tended to have more positive approach behaviors.

Therefore, Hypothesis 4a was supported.

Page 159: Osu 1135134346

134

H4b. Arousal will be positively related to approach behaviors (desire to explore

or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 4b predicted that arousal induced by the peripheral cues in the apparel

websites will be positively related to approach behaviors. The significant path coefficient

showed a positive effect of arousal on approach behaviors (γ22 = .62, t = 4.73, p < .001),

indicating that participants who experienced more arousal during browsing the apparel

websites tended to have more positive approach behaviors. Therefore, Hypothesis 4b

was supported.

Hypothesis 5

Hypothesis 5. Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the

relationship between peripheral cues and consumers’ response behaviors

(purchase intention and approach behaviors).

According to Baron and Kenny (1986), the basic causal relationships involved in

mediation are the direct effect of the independent variable (peripheral cues) and the effect

of the mediators (emotional states) on the dependent variables (response behaviors) and

the direct impact of the independent variable (peripheral cues) on the mediators

(emotional states). If the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variables

decreases or disappears when the proposed mediators (emotional states) are entered into

the model, it will be a strong demonstration of mediating effects of the mediators (Baron

& Kenny, 1986). Baron and Kenny suggested three steps to test the mediating effects: 1)

Page 160: Osu 1135134346

135

regress the mediator on the independent variable, 2) regress the dependent variables on

the independent variable, and 3) regress the dependent variables on both the independent

variable and on the mediators. Hypotheses 1 and 2 supported the direct effects of

peripheral cues (independent variable) on emotional states (mediators), and Hypotheses 3

and 4 suggested the direct effects of emotional states (mediators) on response behaviors

(dependent variables). To test the mediating effects of pleasure and arousal on the

relationship between peripheral cues and response behaviors Hypothesis 5 1) tested the

direct effect of independent variable (peripheral cues) on the dependent variables

(response behaviors) using multivariate analysis of variance and 2) assessed the change in

the magnitude of the influence of the independent variable (peripheral cues) on the

dependent variable (response behaviors) when the mediators were added to the analysis.

The direct effects of peripheral cues on response behaviors. The independent

variable was absence or presence of peripheral cues and the dependent variables were

purchase intention and approach behaviors. Between subjects multivariate analysis of

variance revealed a significant multivariate main effect for peripheral cues on response

behaviors, F (2, 147) = 6.01, p < .005. Univariate between subjects analysis of variance

further revealed a significant main effect for peripheral cues on approach behaviors, F (1,

148) = 4.75, p < .05, ω2 = .029 but not on purchase intention, F (1, 148) = .077, p = .78,

ω2 = .001. Mean scores for approach behaviors [M = 3.47, SD = .94] in the presence of

peripheral cues were higher than those for approach behaviors [M = 3.09, SD = 1.06] in

the condition without peripheral cues. The results indicated that participants had higher

approach behaviors toward the website with the presence of peripheral cues as compared

Page 161: Osu 1135134346

136

to participants in the condition without peripheral cues. Effect size (ω2 = .029) was a

small to the medium effect according to Cohen’s guides. Nearly 2.9% of the variance in

approach behaviors is accounted for by the experimental treatment of the peripheral cues.

The mediating effects of emotional states. Multivariate analysis of variance had

revealed a significant multivariate main effect for peripheral cues on response behaviors;

in the next step, the change in the magnitude of the influence of the independent variable

(peripheral cues) on the dependent variable (response behaviors) when the mediators

were added to the model was assessed using multiple regression analyses. To test the

mediating effects of pleasure and arousal on the relationship between peripheral cues and

response behaviors, the mediators (pleasure and arousal) and the independent variable

together were entered into a regression equation predicting response behaviors. The

overall multiple regression analyses showed that approximately 28% and 41% of total

variance in purchase intention and approach behaviors respectively were accounted for

by a linear combination of the two mediators and the independent variable (See Tables

4.12 and 4.13). Results of the multiple regression analyses revealed that when the two

emotion variables were added to the model, the effects of peripheral cues on the

dependent variables were found to be not significant (purchase intention: t = -1.721, p

= .09, approach behaviors: t = -.052, p = .96). On the other hand, two emotional

variables (pleasure and arousal) were significantly related to purchase intention (pleasure:

t = 2.876, p < .005, arousal: t = 4.935, p < .001) and approach behaviors (pleasure: t =

4.693, p < .001, arousal: t = 5.485, p < .001). In sum, results indicated that there was no

significant effect of peripheral cues on consumers’ response behaviors when emotion

Page 162: Osu 1135134346

137

variables were added. In other word, the relationship between peripheral cues and

response behaviors (purchase intention, and approach behaviors) were mediated by

emotional states (pleasure and arousal). Therefore, Hypothesis 5 was supported.

Variable SS df MS F Sig. Regression 52.038 3 17.346 20.762 .000*** Residual 122.810 147 .835 Total 174.848 150

Note. R2 = .298, Adjusted R2 = .283, ***p < .001

Table 4.12. Multiple regression analysis for purchase intention in Hypothesis 5.

Variable SS df MS F Sig. Regression 65.864 3 21.955 35.382 .000*** Residual 89.974 145 .621 Total 155.839 148

Note. R2 = .423, Adjusted R2 = .411, ***p < .001

Table 4.13. Multiple regression analysis for approach behaviors in Hypothesis 5.

Page 163: Osu 1135134346

138

CHAPTER 5

MAIN STUDY 2

This research consists of two main studies. This chapter describes the method,

analyses, and results of Study 2. Based on the proposed model, Study 2 includes four

main parts: 1) Part 1 examined the effects of type of cue (central cues and peripheral

cues) on emotional reactions (pleasure and arousal), 2) Part 2 assessed the effects of

emotions on consumers’ response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and

approach behaviors), 3) Part 3 examined the effects of situational involvement as a

moderator between S-O, and 4) Part 4 investigated the mediating effects of emotions on

the relationship between type of cue and consumers’ response behaviors. The method,

research design, experimental manipulations, data collection procedure, and instrument

development for main Study 2 are presented. Analyses and results describe

demographics of participants, dependent variables, manipulation checks, preliminary

analyses (assessment of measurement properties and testing invariance of measurement

model over groups), and hypotheses testing for Study 2. This research was exempted

from IRB review (Protocol number # 2005E0018, see Appendix S).

Page 164: Osu 1135134346

139

5.1. Method

5.1.1. Research Design and Experimental Manipulations

A 2 x 2 x 2 between subjects’ factorial design was employed in Study 2:

situational involvement (high vs. low) x central cues (medium amount vs. high amount) x

peripheral cues (presence vs. absence). One of two different scenarios was randomly

given to participants to manipulate situational involvement in main study 2 (See

Appendix I). Participants in the high involvement situation were given the following

scenario: “Imagine that you have been given a $100 gift certificate to purchase clothing

from an online apparel store, E-style.com. Please browse for five pairs of pants on the

website for a while and select one item that you would like to purchase. Then, finish the

survey after shopping the site”. Participants in the low involvement situation were given

the following scenario: “Imagine that today you find the online apparel store, E-style.com.

Browse the website for a while and finish the survey after browsing the site”. These

scenarios were developed based on Eroglu et al.’s (2003) study and Pilot Study 1 and

then were edited for Study 2.

Petty et al. (1983) used free gifts to foster situational involvement in their

experiment. Their participants under the high involvement situation were allowed to

choose a free gift (Edge) from the products examined in the study, while those under the

low involvement situation were allowed to select a free gift from other products

(toothpastes) not related to the study (Petty et al., 1983). Analogously to Petty et al. and

to boost the reality of the purchasing situation (high involvement situation) in Study 2,

Page 165: Osu 1135134346

140

participants in the high involvement situation were informed that randomly selected

participants would receive the apparel item selected during the experiment or a cash

award, while participants in the low involvement situation were informed that randomly

selected participants would receive a cash award only.

Four different apparel websites were created to manipulate central cues and

peripheral cues (See Appendices H through L): 1) medium amount of central cues and

absence of peripheral cues, 2) medium amount of central cues and presence of peripheral

cues, 3) high amount of central cues and absence of peripheral cues, and 4) high amount

of central cues and presence of peripheral cues. Based on results of the content analysis

(See Section 3.4), the websites with medium amount of central cues contained 10 product

related web cues (e.g., front larger view, standard verbal information related to product,

and one item for mix and match suggestion) and the websites with high amount central

cues contained 18 product related web cues (e.g., larger views for front, back, side, and

details, more specific verbal information, and three items for mix and match suggestion).

See Table 5.1 and Appendix K for more information.

Peripheral cues were manipulated by presence or absence of colorful icons and

background colors with a brand logo pattern. In the same manner as Study 1, pink was

used as a main color in the mock websites with the presence of the peripheral cues (See

Section 4.1.1 for more information). The website with peripheral cues consisted of

colorful icons, a flashing brand logo image with color, and pink background color with a

brand logo pattern. The website without peripheral cues contained text icons without

colorful images, a static brand logo image (non-flashing) in black, and white background

without any pattern. See Appendices H through L for manipulations of peripheral cues

Page 166: Osu 1135134346

141

used in main study 2. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two levels of

situational involvement in one of four different treatment combinations of central cues

and peripheral cues for a total of eight treatments. Table 5.1 shows the manipulations of

each of the eight treatments.

Page 167: Osu 1135134346

142

High Involvement Situation

Low Involvement Situation

Central Cues (Medium Amount)

Central Cues (High Amount)

Central Cues (Medium Amount)

Central Cues (High Amount)

Peripheral Cues (Presence: colorful icons with a roll-over image, a flashing brand logo image with color, colorful menu bars, background colors with a brand logo pattern, and colorful texts and images)

(1) Medium amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, and inseam measurement) One mix & match suggestion Only front larger view

(2) High amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, inseam measurement, fit information, waist information, design details (pockets, belt, and/or stitching), and item care) Three mix & match suggestions Front, back, side, and detail views

(3) Medium amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, and inseam measurement) One mix & match suggestion Only front larger view

(4) High amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, inseam measurement, fit information, waist information, design details (pockets, belt, and/or stitching), and item care) Three mix & match suggestions Front, back, side, and detail views

Peripheral Cues (Absence: text icons without colorful images, a static brand logo image with black color, grey menu bars, white background without any pattern, achromatic text colors: black and grey except ‘sale’ menu)

(5) Medium amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, and inseam measurement) One mix & match suggestion Only front larger view

(6) High amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, inseam measurement, fit information, waist information, design details (pockets, belt, and/or stitching), and item care) Three mix & match suggestions Front, back, side, and detail views

(7) Medium amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, and inseam measurement) One mix & match suggestion Only front larger view

(8) High amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, inseam measurement, fit information, waist information, design details (pockets, belt, and/or stitching), and item care) Three mix & match suggestions Front, back, side, and detail views

Table 5.1. The eight treatments in Study 2.

Page 168: Osu 1135134346

143

5.1.2. Instrument Development

The dependent measures in Study 2 contained 5 major parts. Part 1 measured

participants’ emotional states (pleasure and arousal) after shopping or browsing the

website. Part 2 assessed participants’ response behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase

intentions, and approach behaviors. In Part 3 perceived amount of information and user-

perceived quality of web appearance were assessed to check the manipulations. The

effectiveness of situational involvement was assessed in Part 4. In Part 5 participants

provided demographic information and the extent of their prior experiences with the

Internet and Internet shopping. See Appendix M for all items used in Study 2.

Emotional States

Pleasure and arousal were measured by using 12 7 -point semantic differential

scales (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). The measures included six pleasure items (happy—

unhappy, pleased—annoyed, satisfied—unsatisfied, contented—melancholic, hopeful—

despairing, and relaxed—bored) and six arousal items (stimulated—relaxed, excited—

calm, frenzied—sluggish, jittery—dull, wide-awake—sleepy, and aroused—unaroused).

These measures have been used extensively to measure emotional responses toward

physical retail environments (Baker et al., 1992; Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Dovovan et

al., 1994; Fiore & Kimle, 1997) and website environments (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore, Jin,

& Kim, 2005; Menon & Kahn, 2002). The reliabilities of pleasure and arousal

(Cronbach’s α = .93 and .90, respectively) were found to be adequate in previous research

(Fiore et al., 2005).

Page 169: Osu 1135134346

144

Satisfaction

Satisfaction with browsing or shopping experiences at E-style.com was measured

by 4 items used in Eroglu et al. (2003): 1) I enjoyed visiting E-style.com, 2) I was

satisfied with my shopping experience at E-style.com, 3) Given a choice, I would

probably not go back to E-style.com (reverse coded), and 4) I would recommend E-

style.com to other people. These items were measured using 5-point Likert scales

ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The reliability of the four items

was found to be adequate (Cronbach’s α = .88) in prior research (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Purchase Intention

Purchase intention in Internet apparel shopping was measured by adapting

questions used in Park et al. (2005) using 5-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1

(unlikely) to 5 (likely). The measures included 1) How likely is it that you would buy

clothing items if you happened to see them from E-style.com?, 2) How likely is it that

you will buy the apparel item from E-style.com in the next 12 months?, 3) How likely is

it that you will shop for apparel from E-style.com when you buy apparel in the upcoming

year?, and 4) How likely is that you will buy apparel from E-style.com when you find

something you like?. The reliability of these measures (Cronbach’s α = .89) was

established in previous research (Park et al., 2005).

Approach Behavior

Approach behaviors were measured by four questions used in Huang’s (2003)

study (Cronbach’s α = .82). All four items were measured using 5-point Likert-type

Page 170: Osu 1135134346

145

scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much so). Items included 1) How much

would you enjoy exploring this site?, 2) Do you like this site?, 3) To what extent is this

site a good opportunity to shop?, and 4) Would you enjoy shopping in this site?.

Perceived Amount of Information.

By using 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly

agree), perceived amount of information — central cues — on the site was measured.

The items included 1) The website you browsed today contained very much information,

2) From browsing the website, I learned a great deal about the product, 3) The website

was very informative, 4) After browsing the website, I know enough to make an informed

purchase decision, and 5) I can fully trust information given by the website. These

questions are from Kim and Lennon (2000) and revised for Internet shopping. The

reliability of the five items were found to be reliable (Cronbach’s α = .94) in Kim and

Lennon’s (2000) study.

Perceived Quality of Web Appearance

Perceived quality of web appearance — peripheral cues — was measured using

five items used in Aladwani and Palvia’s (2002) study. The intent was to use perceived

quality of web appearance as a manipulation check of the peripheral cues because the

mock websites with two different levels of peripheral cues were manipulated by presence

or absence of the peripheral web cues (e.g., colorful background, flashing icon). If

differences were found across the two levels of peripheral cues such that the website

having the peripheral cues received higher ratings of web appearance, this would be

Page 171: Osu 1135134346

146

evidence that participants were affected by those cues. Since the website with the

peripheral cues used a more appealing color to the target participants along with various

multimedia features (hyperlinks with roll over image and a flashing image), the website

with the the peripheral cues could be perceived as higher quality than the website without

peripheral cues. All five items were assessed using 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1

(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The measures included 1) The website looks

attractive, 2) The website looks organized, 3) The website uses fonts properly, 4) The

website uses colors properly, and 5) the website uses multimedia features properly. The

reliability of these five items (Cronbach’s α = .87) were established in previous research

(Aladwani & Palvia, 2002).

Situational Involvement

Participants’ level of involvement was also measured to assess the effectiveness

of the situational involvement manipulations using ten 7-point semantic differential

scales developed by Zaichkowsky (1994): important—unimportant, irrelevant—relevant,

means a lot to me—means nothing to me, valuable—worthless, boring—interesting,

unexciting—exciting, appealing—unappealing, mundane—fascinating, not needed—

needed, and involving—uninvolving. Cronbach’s α of the 10 items ranged from .91

to .95 in prior research (Zaichkowsky, 1994). These items were originally developed to

measure personal involvement with a certain product (Zaichkowsky, 1985). However,

research (Garlin & McGuiggan, 2002; Shao et al., 2004; Stafford & Stern, 2002;

Zaichkowsky, 1986; Zaichkowsky, 1994) also demonstrated the sensitivity of the

measure toward different situational involvement (e.g., purchase or no purchase).

Page 172: Osu 1135134346

147

Therefore, same scale items were used to measure personal involvement with clothing

(Study 1) and involvement with clothing shopping or clothing browsing situations (Study

2).

Demographic Information and Prior Experiences

Participants’ demographic information such as age, ethnicity, and academic

standing were gathered for background information. Participants were asked to fill in the

blank for age (open-ended) and check the response (closed-ended) corresponding to their

ethnicity and academic standing. Prior experiences with the Internet and Internet

shopping were also assessed using five questions 1) How often do you use the Internet?,

2) How often do you browse online for information search?, 3) How often do you

purchase online?, 4) How often do you browse online for apparel information search?,

and 5) How often do you purchase apparel online? These items were adopted from Ha

and Stoel’s (2004) research and edited for the study. All five items were measured based

on a 6-point scale ranging from 0 (Never) to 5 (Very often).

5.1.3. Website Development

For the low involvement situation, the mock apparel website consisted of an

instruction page, a scenario page for the situational involvement manipulations, the main

page showing all five items together, a product page for each of five apparel items, and

the survey page. For the high involvement situation, the mock website contained all

pages included in the low involvement situation plus a purchasing page to select one item

Page 173: Osu 1135134346

148

that participants would like to buy from the website. In the high involvement conditions,

each product page had links to the front and back larger views, alternative views (side

view, three close-up views), and the size chart (See Appendices H through L).

Apparel Stimuli Preparations for the Websites

Five final apparel stimuli selected in Pilot Study 2 were edited for the mock

websites used in Study 2. Front views of all items were prepared in a small size (150 x

190 pixels) and a medium size (225 x 300 pixels). Items in the small size were displayed

in the main page (Appendix J) and those in the medium size were displayed on each

product page (Appendix K). The resolution of front and back larger views was retained

to be 450 x 450 pixels for all five apparel items. Four different alternative larger views

including a side view and close-up views from three different angles (side, front, and

back) retained the resolution of 450 x 450 pixels (Appendix K).

Mock Website Development for the Main Study 2

Mock apparel websites with different manipulations were developed using a web-

design program, Macromedia Dreamweaver MX 2004, to simulate online apparel stores

as closely as possible. A brand name ‘E-Style.com’ was created and used for the mock

websites to avoid the influence of existing brand names on the results of the study. The

brand logo was edited using Adobe Photoshop 7 and Imageready 7 and used to

manipulate different types of peripheral cues in the mock websites (e.g., a flashing brand

logo, a background with a brand logo). The mock websites had the appearance and the

functions of a real apparel online store, even though several functions (e.g., shopping bag,

Page 174: Osu 1135134346

149

order status, customer service), which were unrelated to the study, were disabled.

Comments on the websites were continuously sought from graduate students in Textiles

and Clothing major to increase the reality of the websites.

The websites were designed to allow participants to browse among five pairs of

pants selected in Pilot Study 2. After reading instructions and a given scenario (high or

low involvement manipulations), participants started browsing the main page displaying

the five pairs of pants. Participants could click images or titles of five pairs of pants to

search for information about each pair of pants. Each product page consisted of different

levels of central cues and peripheral cues depending on the treatment combination to

which they were assigned. In each product page, participants were able to find

information for each item and could click for different larger views and a size chart.

After browsing each product page, participants were asked to go back to the main

page to browse for other items. After they finished browsing for all five items,

participants under the high involvement situation were asked to fill out an order form for

one item that they would like to buy from the website and then moved to the next page to

finish the survey. Participants in the low involvement situation went directly to the

survey page upon the completion of browsing. All pages in the mock website excluding

the five product pages were manipulated only by peripheral cues (presence vs. absence).

See Appendices H to L for more information.

Page 175: Osu 1135134346

150

5.1.4. Recruitment of Participants

A random sample was drawn from students at the Ohio State University. Eight-

thousand email addresses of female undergraduate students at the Ohio State University

were randomly selected by the University Registrar. Female undergraduate students were

recruited for the study because women are more likely than men to be Internet apparel

shoppers. Women make up more than 52% of the U.S. Internet users (Rush, 2004) and

clothing and shoes are the most popular products for female Internet shoppers (Greenspan,

2003). According to previous research, young women are significant Internet apparel

purchasers and browsers (Lee & Johnson, 2002). Thus, apparel stimuli and the mock

websites were developed to target young female undergraduate students.

Before sending the first invitation emails to potential participants, 8,000 email

addresses were randomly categorized into eight groups using an Excel program. Each

group with one thousand email addresses was randomly assigned to one of the eight

treatment conditions. Therefore, potential participants were randomly assigned to one of

eight treatment groups. Participants were recruited via email for Study 2. Because of

dissimilar incentives for the two levels of situational involvement (See Section 5.1.1),

two types of invitation letters were created (See Appendix G). The first invitation emails

were sent to recruit research participants for the study. Potential respondents were able to

participate by clicking the URL provided in the invitation email. Two reminder emails

were sent. The first reminder was emailed to non-responders six days after the invitation

email; people who did not respond to the first or second invitation were sent another

reminder email six days after the first reminder email.

Page 176: Osu 1135134346

151

5.1.5. Experiment Procedure for Study 2

Study 2 employed a 2 (situational involvement: high or low) x 2 (central cues:

medium or high amount) x 2 (peripheral cues: absence or presence) between-subjects

factorial design to examine how consumers respond to different amounts of central cues

and peripheral cues under different situational involvement.

The study was conducted as an online experiment. When participants went to the

website by clicking the URL provided in the invitation email, they were asked to read the

instruction page describing the purpose of the research and providing brief instructions

for the experiment. After reading the instruction page, participants were asked to go to

the next page to start the experiment (See Appendix H).

In the next page, all participants were asked to read a given scenario developed to

manipulate situational involvement (high or low) (See Appendix I). After reading the

given scenario, participants moved to the main page to start browsing the website.

Participants were able to click the title or the image of each product to go to the

individual product page to gather more information about each item (See Appendices J

and K).

After browsing for all five items, participants under the high involvement

situation went to the purchase page to complete an order form for one product that they

would like to buy from the website. In the purchase page (See Appendix L), participants

were asked to choose one item among five pairs of pants and select their size and inseam

for the product. For the shipping information, participants were asked to provide their

full name and email address. This process was used to foster high involvement and to

Page 177: Osu 1135134346

152

enhance the reality of the online apparel purchasing process. Upon the completion of the

purchasing process, participants went to the survey page to complete the dependent

measures. In the case of the low involvement situation, participants moved to the survey

page right after browsing the website without the purchasing step.

Page 178: Osu 1135134346

153

5.2. Analysis and Results

Study 2 investigated how the central and peripheral cues affect consumer

emotions in different situational involvements that in turn influence consumer behaviors.

Analyses and results of Study 2 are presented in four sections: the description of

participants, manipulation checks, the description of variables measured in Study 2,

preliminary analyses to check the reliability and validity of the measures and to test

invariance of measurement model across groups, and hypotheses testing for the four parts

of the proposed model in Study 2. Descriptive statistics were used to describe the

research participants and each variable including consumer emotions (pleasure and

arousal), satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors. Multivariate analyses

of variance were used for manipulation checks. Confirmatory factor analyses were used

to evaluate measurement properties, and structural equation modeling was used to test

hypotheses. Single group structural equation modeling and multi-group structural

equation modeling were used for hypotheses testing of the proposed model in Study 2.

Descriptive statistics, multivariate analyses of variance, and univariate analyses of

variance were analyzed using SPSS 13.0. Confirmatory factor analyses and structural

equation modeling were analyzed using Lisrel 8.7 (Jöreskog & Sörbom, 2004).

5.2.1. Description of Participants

Invitation emails were sent to 8,000 randomly selected female undergraduate

students at the Ohio State University. All participants were asked to provide their name

Page 179: Osu 1135134346

154

and a valid university email address to complete the survey. This information was used

to purify the data with multiple responses. After cleaning the data for multiple

submissions and other errors, 1694 responses remained. In addition to cleaning the data

with multiple responses, the responses having more than 30% missing values were

removed. Upon the completion of the data cleaning regarding missing information, 1634

usable responses were obtained. The overall response rate for usable data was 20.4%.

All participants were female college students. The mean age of participants was

21, with a range of 18 to 62. About 90% of participants were aged between 18 and 23.

About 80% of participants were Caucasian American. Other participants were African

American (7.4%), Asian/Asian American (6.4%), Hispanic American (2.6%), Native

American (.2%), and other (3.9%). The academic standing of the participants excluding

seniors was evenly spread out. Seniors were the single largest group accounting for

about 36 % of all participants. See Table 5.2 for demographic information.

Information about participants’ prior Internet usage and previous online browsing

and purchasing experiences was also assessed (See Table 5.3). A majority of participants

answered that they use the Internet and browse online for information search very often.

Approximately, 81% of participants use the Internet very often. More than 90% of

participants browse online for information search often or very often, 22% and 69%,

respectively. About one fourth of participants answered that they purchase products

online often or very often. Although online apparel browsing and purchasing are not yet

prevalent compared to general online browsing and purchasing activities, participants

tend to browse and purchase apparel products online quite often. Over 95% of

participants had browsed for apparel products online and over 85% of participants had

Page 180: Osu 1135134346

155

purchased apparel online. Among these participants, nearly 45% responded that they

browse online for apparel products often or very often and about 23% answered that they

purchase apparel products online often or very often. Only about 14% of participants had

not purchased apparel products online. See Table 5.3 for more detailed information.

Page 181: Osu 1135134346

156

Demographics Frequencies

Percent

Age Under 20 442 28.8% 20 – 22 857 55.9% 23 – 25 152 9.9% 26 – 30 54 3.5% 31and Over 29 1.9% Total 1534* 100% Ethnic Background African American 113 7.4% Caucasian American 1209 79.5% Hispanic American 39 2.6% Asian/Asian American 97 6.4% Native American 3 .2% Other 60 3.9% Total 1521* 100%

Academic Standing

Freshmen 252 16.6% Sophomore 323 21.3% Junior 330 21.7% Senior 539 35.5% Other 74 4.9% Total 1518* 100%

Note. *Different Ns are due to missing information.

Table 5.2. Demographic descriptions of participants.

Page 182: Osu 1135134346

157

General Internet Usage

Online Browsing

Online Purchasing

Online Apparel

Browsing

Online Apparel

Purchasing f % f % f % f % f % Never 1 .1 0 0 54 3.6 63 4.2 211 13.9 Very not often

13 .9 10 .7 317 20.9 221 14.6 431 28.4

Not often 10 .7 15 1.0 247 16.3 197 13.0 242 15.9 Sometimes 100 6.6 109 87.2 515 33.9 363 23.9 341 22.4 Often 172 11.3 333 22.0 221 14.6 365 24.1 168 11.1 Very often 1226 80.6 1045 69.1 164 10.8 308 20.3 127 8.4

Table 5.3. Participants’ prior Internet usage and online browsing/purchasing experiences.

Page 183: Osu 1135134346

158

5.2.2. Manipulation Check

Situational Involvement

In Study 2, situational involvement was manipulated with two levels (low vs.

high) so a manipulation check was performed to determine if participants’ involvement

differed as a function of the two levels of situational involvement manipulated in the

mock websites. Participants were randomly assigned to only one of two involvement

treatment conditions (low or high situational scenario). Upon the completion of browsing

(low involvement) or purchasing (high involvement), participants were asked to rate 10

items using 7-point semantic differential scales measuring their levels of involvement in

situations (See Table 5.4). Of the 10 items, four items were reverse coded.

The reliability of 10 items was established (Cronbach’s α = .92). The 10 items

were summed to test participants’ level of involvement in different situations. In order

to test for significant differences between two treatment conditions, univariate analysis of

variance was performed: situational involvement was the independent variable and

participants’ level of involvement was the dependent variable. The results showed a

significant main effect for involvement manipulation on participants’ level of

involvement, F (1, 1564) = 9.783, p < .01. Mean scores for low and high involvement

situations were 39.20 (SD = 11.90) and 41.00 (SD = 10.87), respectively, indicating a

higher score as higher involvement. Inspection of mean scores revealed that participants

in the high involvement situation were more likely to be involved than those in the low

involvement situation. This indicates that participants’ level of involvement differed in

Page 184: Osu 1135134346

159

two situational involvement conditions. Therefore, situational involvement was

successfully manipulated in Study 2.

Central Cues

Central cues were manipulated with two levels (medium amount vs. high amount)

in Study 2 and participants were randomly assigned to browse one of two treatment

conditions (medium or high amount of central cues). To verify if participants would

perceive a different amount of central cues manipulated in the mock websites, a

manipulation check was performed. Since central cues were manipulated by different

amounts of product related information in Study 2, participants were asked to assess their

perception of the amount of information presented in the websites for the manipulation

check using 5 items with 5-point scales. Five items included 1) the website you browsed

today contained very much information, 2) from browsing the website, I learned a great

deal about the product, 3) the website was very informative, 4) after browsing the website,

I know enough to make an informed purchase decision, and 5) I can fully trust

information given by the website. See Table 5.4 for descriptive statistics.

The reliability of the five items was found to be satisfactory (Cronbach’s α = .92).

The five items were summed to test for differences between the two treatment groups

(medium vs. high amount central cues) and entered into a univariate analysis of variance.

This procedure was conducted to verify a significant difference between two treatment

conditions of central cues: the levels of central cues as the independent variable and the

perceived amount of information as the dependent variable. The results revealed a

significant main effect for central cues on perceived amount of information, F (1, 1601) =

Page 185: Osu 1135134346

160

47.217, p < .001, implying that perceived amount of information was significantly higher

for participants who browsed the websites with the high amount of central cues than for

those who browsed the sites with the medium amount. Mean scores for medium and high

amount of central cues were 14.97 (SD = 4.51) and 16.51 (SD = 4.47), respectively. A

higher score indicates that participants perceived a higher amount of information.

Inspection of cell means showed that participants who browsed the website with the high

amount of central cues perceived a greater amount of information in the mock website

than those who browsed the website with the medium amount of central cues. Thus, the

websites with different amounts of central cues were perceived to display different

amounts of information and the manipulation was successfully operationalized in Study 2.

Peripheral Cues

In Study 2, peripheral cues were manipulated with two conditions (presence vs.

absence). Participants were randomly assigned to browse one of two treatment

conditions. In order to determine whether participants would perceive different levels of

peripheral cues manipulated in the websites, a manipulation check was performed.

Because websites with two different levels of peripheral cues may have presented

different quality of web appearance aspects in terms of color, fonts, and multimedia

features, participants were asked to evaluate the perceived quality of web appearance for

the manipulation check using 5 items with 5-point scales. The five items include 1) The

website looks attractive, 2) The website looks organized, 3) The website uses fonts

properly, 4) The website uses colors properly, and 5) The website uses multimedia

features properly. See Table 5.4 for detailed information.

Page 186: Osu 1135134346

161

Cronbach’s α was .92, indicating the adequate reliability of five items. The five

items were summed and entered into a univariate analysis of variance for a manipulation

check. This procedure was performed to verify significant differences between two

treatment groups in terms of the perceived quality of web appearance. Presence or

absence of peripheral cues was the independent variable and the perceived quality of web

appearance was the dependent variable. The results revealed a significant main effect for

presence or absence of peripheral cues on participants’ perceptions of web appearance, F

(1, 1583) = 15.322, p < .001. Mean scores for absence and presence of peripheral cues

were 17.91 (SD = 4.08) and 18.73 (SD = 4.12), respectively. A higher score indicates a

higher perceived quality of web appearance. Inspection of cell means showed that

participants who browsed the website with the presence of peripheral cues tended to

perceive the web appearance to be higher quality than those who browsed the website

without peripheral cues. This implies that the perceived quality of web appearance was

significantly higher for participants who browsed the websites with the presence of

peripheral cues. Thus, the peripheral cue manipulation affected perceptions of the mock

websites in Study 2.

Page 187: Osu 1135134346

162

Min. Max. Mean SD Situation Involvement Unimportant-Important 1 7 3.61 1.51 Irrelevent-Relevent 1 7 3.71 1.59 Means a lot to me-Means nothing to

mea 1 7 3.60 1.56

Valuable-Worthlessa 1 7 4.21 1.41 Boring-Interesting 1 7 4.35 1.54 Unexciting-Exciting 1 7 4.06 1.45 Appealing-Unappealinga 1 7 4.67 1.48 Mundane-Fascinating 1 7 3.90 1.36 Not needed-Needed 1 7 3.70 1.46 Involving-Uninvolvinga 1 7 4.35 1.47 Sum of situation involvement 10 70 40.16 11.39 Reliability .92 Central Cues The website you browsed today

contained very much information 1 5 3.21 1.05

From browsing the website, I learned a great deal about the product

1 5 3.21 1.07

The website was very informative 1 5 3.29 1.05 After browsing the website, I know

enough to make an informed purchase decision

1 5 3.17 1.10

I can fully trust information given by the website

1 5 2.88 1.01

Sum of central cues 5 25 15.75 4.55 Reliability .92 Peripheral Cues The website looks attractive 1 5 3.46 1.04 The website looks organized 1 5 3.88 .84 The website uses fonts properly 1 5 3.79 .87 The website uses colors properly 1 5 3.61 1.01 The website uses multimedia features

properly 1 5 3.58 .98

Sum of peripheral cues 5 25 18.32 4.12 Reliability .92

Note. a Items reverse coded.

Table 5.4. Descriptive statistics for manipulation check items in Study 2.

Page 188: Osu 1135134346

163

5.2.3. Dependent Variables

Study 2 includes five dependent variables, pleasure, arousal, satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors. Pleasure and arousal were dependent

variables for Part 1 and Part 3. Satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors

were dependent variables in Part 2 and Part 4. All five variables were latent constructs in

the proposed model in Part 2 and Part 4. In order to measure each of five latent

constructs multiple items were used. Descriptive statistics for five latent variables are

presented in this section.

Emotional States: Pleasure and Arousal

After the completion of browsing for five pairs of pants in the mock websites,

participants were asked to rate their current feelings using 12 7-point semantic scales

(Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Each of pleasure and arousal was measured by 6 items

(See Table 5.5). The reliabilities for pleasure and arousal were found to be adequate

(Cronbach’s α = .91 and .83, respectively). Higher scores indicate that participants

experienced more pleasure or arousal, while lower scores indicate that participants

experienced less intensive emotions. Six items for each of pleasure and arousal were

used as multiple indicators for pleasure and arousal latent constructs in the proposed

model for Study 2. Descriptive statistics for the six indicators for each of pleasure and

arousal latent constructs are shown in Table 5.5.

Page 189: Osu 1135134346

164

Satisfaction

Four items used in Eroglu et al. (2003) were used to measure satisfaction with

browsing or shopping experiences at E-style.com. The reliability of the four items was

found to be adequate (Cronbach’s α = .82). Satisfaction was used as the dependent

variable in Part 2 and Part 4 of Study 2. Four items were used as multiple indicators for

satisfaction latent construct in the proposed model. Table 5.5 illustrates descriptive

statistics of the four items measuring satisfaction.

Purchase Intention

Purchase intention was measured by the four items used in Park et al.’s (2005)

study. The reliability of the four items was estimated and was found to be adequate

(Cronbach’s α = .92). The four items were used as multiple indicators for the purchase

intention latent construct in the proposed model for Study 2. Purchase intention was used

as the dependent variable in Part 2 and Part 4. Descriptive statistics of the four indicators

for purchase intention are described in Table 5.5.

Approach Behaviors

By using four items from previous research (Huang, 2003) approach behaviors

were measured. Cronbach’s α was calculated and was found to be reliable (α = .91).

Approach behaviors were used as the dependent variable in Part 2 and Part 4. The four

items were used as multiple indicators for approach behavior latent construct in the

proposed model for Study 2. Descriptive statistics of the four items for approach

behaviors are presented in Table 5.5.

Page 190: Osu 1135134346

165

Min. Max. Mean SD Emotional Reactions Pleasure P1 Happy – Unhappy 1 7 5.10 1.15 P2 Pleased – Annoyed 1 7 4.97 1.39 P3 Satisfied – Unsatisfied 1 7 4.93 1.42 P4 Contented – Melancholic 1 7 5.02 1.28 P5 Hopeful – Despairing 1 7 4.87 1.31 P6 Relaxed – Bored 1 7 4.78 1.57 Arousal A1 Stimulated – Relaxed 1 7 4.06 1.50 A2 Excited – Calm 1 7 3.90 1.54 A3 Frenzied – Sluggish 1 7 3.61 1.08 A4 Jittery – Dull 1 7 3.57 1.07 A5 Wide-awake – Sleepy 1 7 3.91 1.47 A6 Aroused – Unaroused 1 7 3.74 1.38 Satisfaction SA1 I enjoyed visiting E-style.com 1 5 3.55 1.02 SA2 I was satisfied with my shopping experience

at E-style.com 1 5 3.44 1.05

aSA3 Given a choice, I would probably not go back to E-style.com

1 5 3.28 1.23

SA4 I would recommend E-style.com to other people

1 5 3.20 1.11

Purchase Intention PI1 How likely is it that you would buy clothing

items if you happened to see them from E-style.com?

1 5 2.90 1.20

PI2 How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-style.com in the next 12 months? 1 5 2.55 1.21

PI3 How likely is it that you will shop for apparel from E-style.com when you buy apparel in the upcoming year?

1 5 2.62 1.21

PI4 How likely is that you will buy apparel from E-style.com when you find something you like?

1 5 3.28 1.20

Approach Behaviors AB1 How much would you enjoy exploring this

site? 1 5 3.31 1.10

AB2 Do you like this site? 1 5 3.42 .95 AB3 To what extent is this site a good opportunity

to shop? 1 5 3.36 .96

AB4 Would you enjoy shopping in this site? 1 5 3.31 1.04

Note. a Item reverse coded.

Table 5.5. Descriptive statistics of dependent variables.

Page 191: Osu 1135134346

166

5.2.4. Assessment of Measurement Properties

As indicated in Study 1 (See Section 4.1.4 for more information), the

measurement properties were assessed in terms of unidimensionality (internal and

external consistency) and construct validity (convergent validity and discriminant

validity) using a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The general structural equation

model consists of two conceptually distinct models: a measurement model and a latent

construct model (Anderson & Gerbing, 1982; Bollen, 1989). Following the two-step

approach according to Anderson and Gerbing (1988) the measurement model specifying

the relations between indicators and the latent constructs was evaluated and respecified

before conducting the analysis of the full model.

After comprehensive assessment of the measurements, the measurement model

was respecified to purify measures and consequently, to reduce the possibility of

interpretational confounding in the full model. As suggested by Anderson and Gerbing

(1988), the measurement model was respecified by deleting the problematical

measurements from the model. The following principles were used to respecify the

measures: 1) if a measurement’s path coefficients on its posited latent construct were

insignificant, 2) if item reliability (e.g., squared multiple correlation) of the measurement

was lower than the .5 standard (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991; Bollen, 1989), 3) if the

measurement had large residuals with other indicators, 4) if the measure had highly

correlated unexplainable error variances with other indicators (large modification indices

for Θδ), 5) if the measurement shared common variance with other indicators posited to

measure other latent constructs (large modification indices for λ). The model

Page 192: Osu 1135134346

167

modification was made based on statistical and theoretical consideration (Anderson &

Gerbing, 1988; Bollen, 1989).

After the respecification of the measurement model, 15 final items posited to

measure the five latent constructs remained. Each of the five latent variables had three

indicators in the final model. A1, A2, and SA3 were removed due to their small squared

multiple correlations (.41, .45, and .20, respectively) that did not meet the .5 standard

(Bagozzi & Yi, 1991; Bollen, 1989). P6 and AB2 were deleted because these shared

common variance with other indicators posited to measure other latent variables. Since

highly correlated error variance with other indicators was present, P3, P5, A4, and PI3

were also eliminated. Table 5.6 shows the final 15 items for the model. After the

assessment and respecification of the measurement model, the respecified model was

evaluated in terms of construct validity, unidimensionality, and construct reliability by

performing a CFA.

Page 193: Osu 1135134346

168

Latent Constructs Indicators Measurement Items Pleasure P1 Happy – Unhappy P2 Pleased – Annoyed P4 Contented – Melancholic Arousal A3 Frenzied – Sluggish A5 Wide-awake – Sleepy A6 Aroused – Unaroused Satisfaction SA1 I enjoyed visiting E-style.com SA2 I was satisfied with my shopping experience at E-

style.com SA4 I would recommend E-style.com to other people Purchase Intention PI1 How likely is it that you would buy clothing items if you

happened to see them from E-style.com? PI2 How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-

style.com in the next 12 months? PI4 How likely is that you will buy apparel from E-style.com

when you find something you like? Approach Behaviors AB1 How much would you enjoy exploring this site? AB3 To what extent is this site a good opportunity to shop? AB4 Would you enjoy shopping in this site?

Table 5.6. Final measurement items for each of five latent constructs.

Page 194: Osu 1135134346

169

Convergent Validity

Convergent validity was assessed by examining each measurement’s path

coefficient (factor loading) with a significant t-value on its posited latent construct and

each observed variable’s squared multiple correlation. As shown in Table 5.7, all path

coefficients were significant at the p < .0001 level indicating that all measurement items

are significantly related to their specified latent constructs. In addition, squared multiple

correlations of all indicators exceeded the .5 standard (Bagozzi & Yi, 1991; Bollen, 1989)

supporting the achievement of convergent validity of the model. Significant factor

loadings and high squared multiple correlations indicated that convergent validity of the

measurement model was achieved.

Page 195: Osu 1135134346

170

Latent Constructs

Indicators Unstandardized factor loading

Completely standardized

factor loading

t-values Item reliability

Pleasure P1 .93 .81 37.33*** .65 P2 1.17 .84 39.49*** .71 P4 1.02 .80 36.73*** .64 Arousal A3 .70 .65 26.03*** .50 A5 1.12 .76 30.80*** .58 A6 .98 .71 28.61*** .51 Satisfaction SA1 .92 .91 46.07*** .82 SA2 .93 .89 44.48*** .78 SA4 .84 .76 35.30*** .58 Purchase Intention

PI1 1.06 .89 43.99*** .79

PI2 1.04 .86 41.70*** .73 PI4 .90 .76 34.68*** .57 Approach Behaviors

AB1 .86 .79 37.28*** .62

AB3 .80 .83 40.23*** .69 AB4 .95 .91 47.00*** .84

Note. ***p < .0001

Table 5.7. Factor loading, t-values, and item reliability for convergent validity.

Page 196: Osu 1135134346

171

Unidimensionality

Unidimensionality of the measurement model was assessed by examining internal

consistency and external consistency. Both internal and external consistencies were

evaluated by assessing the model fit along with standardized residuals, modification

indices, and expected change.

An overall chi-square was significant (χ2 = 354.15, p < .0001). Since the chi-

square statistic is sensitive to large sample sizes and models with large numbers of

indicators, the significant chi-square is not surprising (Bagozzi & Phillips, 1982; Bagozzi

& Phillips, 1991; Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1995; Segars &

Grover, 1993). Due to the potential problems with the chi-square statistic in evaluating

the fit of the model, other model fit indices were also used to assess the model fit such as

RMSEA, NNFI, AGFI, and GFI. The RMSEA was .046 and NNFI index was .99. The

AGFI was .96 and the GFI was .97. All other model fit indices are good within

acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11) supporting the strong internal and external

consistencies of the unidimensionality. In addition to the overall model fit, standardized

residuals and modification indices with expected change provided useful information

with respect to the unidimensionality of the model. No standardized residuals were

greater than 2.58 (or less than -2.58) (Grefen, 2003; Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989).

Modification indices for λx and Θδ were all less than 5 (Grefen, 2003) and completely

standardized expected change in chi-square statistics was all less than .3 (Koufteros,

1999), supporting significant evidence of the unidimensionality of the measurement

model. Appendix P shows standardized residuals for the 15 measurement items used in

Study 2.

Page 197: Osu 1135134346

172

Discriminant Validity

Discriminant validity was assessed by three methods : 1) perform chi-square

difference test for the constrained (correlation between two estimated latent constructs is

set to 1) and unconstrained model (correlation between two constructs is freely

measured); if the chi-square value for the unconstrained model is significantly lower than

the value for the constrained model, it indicates that two latent constructs are not

perfectly correlated and that discriminant validity is achieved (Anderson & Gerbing,

1988; Bagozzi & Phillips, 1982; Bagozzi et al., 1991; Koufteros, 1999), 2) determine

whether a confidence interval constructed by the correlation between two latent

constructs plus or minus two standard errors includes 1; if the confidence interval does

not include 1, it is the evidence of discriminant validity (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988;

Koufteros, 1999), and 3) compare the average variance extracted (AVE) with the squared

correlation between constructs; if AVE for a construct is higher than the squared

correlation between the construct and other constructs, discriminant validity is

established (Fornell & Larker, 1981; Koufteros, 1999).

Based on the results of the first method, all the chi-square differences between the

unconstrained and constrained models were significant (See Table 5.8), indicating

discriminant validity of the measures. The second method also supported discriminant

validity of the model. As indicated in Table 5.9, all confidence intervals do not include

the value of 1 providing the evidence of discriminant validity. The results of the third

one provided an additional evidence of discriminant validity of the measures. The AVE

for each latent construct was higher than the squared correlation between the construct

and all other constructs (See Table 5.9), indicating that the measures for each latent

Page 198: Osu 1135134346

173

construct shared more common variance with their underlying construct than any

variance shared with other constructs, thus discriminant validity was achieved.

Constraint

Chi-square

df

Chi-square difference (Δχ2)

df = 1 Unconstrained model 351.61 80 Pleasure and Arousal 1238.43 81 886.82*** Pleasure and Satisfaction 1642.02 81 1290.41*** Pleasure and Purchase Intention 2299.60 81 1947.99*** Pleasure and Approach Behaviors 2035.02 81 1683.41*** Arousal and Satisfaction 1308.66 81 957.05*** Arousal and Purchase Intention 1370.89 81 1019.28*** Arousal and Approach Behaviors 1327.99 81 976.38*** Satisfaction and Purchase Intention 2122.21 81 1770.60*** Satisfaction and Approach Behaviors 1112.15 81 760.54*** Purchase Intention and Approach Behaviors 1037.12 81 685.51***

Note. *** p < .0001

Table 5.8. Chi-square difference tests for discriminant validity7.

7 “When a number of chi-square difference tests are performed for assessments of discriminant validity, the significance level for each test should be adjusted to maintain the “true” overall significance level for the family of the test (cf. Finn, 1974). This adjustment can be given as α0 = 1-(1-αi)t, where α0 is the overall significance level that should be used for each individual hypothesis test of discriminant validity; and t is the number of tests performed” (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988, p. 416).

Page 199: Osu 1135134346

174

Latent Constructs

Pleasure

Arousal

Satisfaction

Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

Pleasure .67e Arousal .52a

(.02)b

(.48, .56)c .27d

.51

Satisfaction .68

(.02) (.64, .72)

.46

.47 (.02)

(.43, .51) .22

.73

Purchase Intention

.43 (.02)

(.39, .47) .18

.41 (.03)

(.35, .47) .17

.65 (.02)

(.61, .69) .42

.70

Approach Behaviors

.57 (.02)

(.53, .61) .33

.45 (.03)

(.39, .51) .20

.80 (.01)

(.78, .82) .64

.82 (.01)

(.80, .84) .67

.72

Note. a Correlation, b Standard Error, c Confidence Interval, d Squared Correlation, e Average variance extracted.

Table 5.9. Correlations and confidence intervals for discriminant validity.

Page 200: Osu 1135134346

175

Assessment of Reliability

The reliability in structural equation modeling is evaluated by composite

reliability and average variance extracted (AVE) which are defined in terms of the factor

loading of measurement (Fornell & Larcker, 1981: Gerbing & Anderson, 1988; Grefen,

2003; Koufteros, 1999: Zhang, Lim, & Cao, 2004). As indicated in Table 5.10, the

composite reliability of all latent constructs except ‘arousal’ exceeded .80, supporting

strong composite reliability. Although it was not as strong as the .80 standard, the

reliability of the ‘arousal’ latent construct was also higher than the normal acceptable

level (.70) suggested by previous research (Lusch & Brown, 1996; Sun & Zhang, 2004).

The AVE estimates for five latent constructs were also good within the acceptable range

(over .50) (Bagozzi & Yi, 1988; Fornell & Larcker, 1981), providing further evidence of

strong reliability.

Page 201: Osu 1135134346

176

Latent Constructs

Composite reliability8

Average variance extracted (AVE)9

Status

Pleasure .86 .67 Accepted Arousal .75 .51 Accepted Satisfaction .89 .73 Accepted Purchase Intention .87 .70 Accepted Approach Behaviors .88 .72 Accepted

Note. Minimum standards for composite reliability and AVE are .70 and .50, respectively.

Table 5.10. Composite reliability and AVE of latent constructs.

8 Composite reliability = (Σλi)2/{(Σλi)2+Σθi} (Fornell & Larcker, 1981; Grefen, 2003; Segars, 1997) 9 Average variance extracted (AVE) = (Σλi

2)/{(Σλi2)+Σθi} (Fornell & Lalrcker, 1981; Grefen, 2003; Segars,

1997)

Page 202: Osu 1135134346

177

Testing Invariance of Measurement Model over Groups

In Part 1, Part 3, and Part 4 of Study 2, causal relations between latent constructs

and means of latent constructs in the proposed model were compared by different

treatment groups (e.g., medium amount of central cues vs. high amount of central cues).

It is assumed that at least the invariance of the model form (models in different treatment

groups should have the same form) and the invariance of factor loadings should hold

before testing the equality of regression parameters and estimating mean differences of

latent variables across groups (Bollen, 1989; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). The invariance

of the model form across groups is achieved if the model for each group has the same

indicators (i.e., the same measurements) that load on the same latent constructs and that

have the same patterns of fixed, free, and constrained parameters (Bollen, 1989). Bollen

suggested a testing hierarchy of the invariance across groups applicable to measurement

models: 1) first, test if the form of the model is the same in different groups (Hform) and if

this holds, then 2) assess the equality of factor loadings (i.e., the coefficients linking the

latent to the observed variables) across groups (HΛx) and if this holds, then 3) add the

equality of measurement error variances to the test (HΛxΘδ) and if this holds, then 4) add

the equality of the covariance matrices in different groups to the assessment (HΛxΘδΦ).

The third and forth ones are interchangeable in the hierarchy according to the research

interests. To go further to assess the invariance of the general structural equation model

with latent variables (i.e., testing the equality of regression coefficients between latent

variables or estimating mean differences of latent constructs across groups) at least the

first two assumptions should hold (Bollen, 1989; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). Following

Page 203: Osu 1135134346

178

the steps suggested by Bollen, the invariance of the measurement model for different

groups was assessed before testing the proposed hypotheses in Study 2.

Table 5.11 shows the results of testing the invariance hypotheses. The Hform

hypothesis showed a good match to the data with the NNFI equal to .99 and the RMSEA

equal to .051. In group comparisons, the GFI fit index is useful because it is calculable

for each group (Bollen, 1989). However, the AGFI is not measurable in group

comparisons. The GFIs for different groups ranged from .91 to .94. All the fit indices

indicated that all groups had the same model form. The next hypothesis HΛx also showed

a good fit to the data with the NNFI equal to .99 and the RMSEA equal to .048. The

RMSEA decreased for HΛx. The GFIs were greater than .90 for all treatment groups.

Since the hierarchies of invariances contain nested models (e.g., HΛxΘδ is nested in HΛx),

chi-square difference tests were performed to assess relative fit (See Table 5.11). The

chi-square difference between Hform and HΛx was 73.34 (Δdf = 70) which is not

statistically significant, indicating that all factor loadings appeared to be equal for all

treatment groups (i.e., more constrained model, HΛx is better than Hform). Thus, the next

hypothesis HΛxΘδ added the constraint that the measurement error variances are equal for

all groups. The chi-square difference of HΛx and HΛxΘδ was 257.53 with Δdf = 105,

which is statistically significant (p < .001), suggesting that the equality of the

measurement error variances across groups is not tenable. Thus, it was unnecessary to

assess the next hypothesis HΛxΘδΦ . Overall, the results of testing the invariance of the

measurement model support the hypotheses that all treatment groups have the same

model form and that all factor loadings are the same across groups. Accordingly, the

Page 204: Osu 1135134346

179

model form and factor loadings for different groups were set equal in the proposed model

tested in Part 1, Part 3, and Part 4 for Study 2.

Hypotheses

RMSEA

Chi-

square

df

Chi-square difference (Δχ2)

Status

Hform (Same model form across groups)

.051

973.12

640

Hold

HΛx (Same Λx across groups)

.048

1046.46

710

HΛx - Hform= 73.34

(Δdf = 710 - 640 =70)

Hold

HΛxΘδ (Same Λx and Θδ across groups)

.054

1303.99

815

HΛxΘδ - HΛx= 257.53** (Δdf = 815 – 710 =105)

Not Hold

Note. **p < .001

Table 5.11. The results of testing the invariance of the measurement model.

Page 205: Osu 1135134346

180

Model Specification

Once the measurement model is assessed and found to be acceptable, an

evaluation of the structural model can begin. Structural equation modeling was used to

test hypotheses in Study 2. After the assessment of the measures, 15 indicators were

selected for the model specification. The structural model specified in Study 2 is

presented in Figure 5.1 for Part 1 and Part 3 and in Figure 5.2 for Part 2 and Part 4. The

proposed model in Parts 1 and 3 included two exogenous (ξ) latent constructs (pleasure

and arousal) with six manifest variables (Figure 5.1). The proposed model in Part 2 and

Part 4 consisted of five latent variables with 15 indicators (manifest variables) (Figure

5.2). Two latent constructs (pleasure and arousal) were exogenous latent variables (ξ)

and the other three latent constructs (satisfaction, purchase intention, approach behaviors)

were endogenous latent variables (η). Each of the five latent constructs had three

indicators.

For identification purposes, the variances of the two exogenous latent constructs

were set to one (set the diagonal elements of Φ matrix to one) and the error variances of

the three endogenous latent constructs were set to one (set the diagonal elements of Ψ

matrix to one) in Part 2 and Part 4 (See Figure 5.2) (Boker & McArdle, 2005). In a single

population study an origin of latent construct is fixed by assuming that all observed

indicators are measured in deviations from their means and that the means of all latent

constructs are zero so that generally the unit of measurement of each latent construct is

fixed either by setting a variance of each latent construct to 1 or by fixing one factor

loading for each latent construct to a reference variable (Bollen, 1989; Jöreskog &

Sörbom, 1996). In multi-group mean comparison analyses, however, these restrictions

Page 206: Osu 1135134346

181

can be somewhat relaxed by assuming that the latent variables are on the same scale in all

groups. Since means of latent constructs in the proposed model were compared by

groups in Part 1 and Part3 the new restrictions were required to identify the model

(Bollen, 1989; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). The way to do this is to set the means of the

latent variables equal to zero in one group and to scale each latent variable to one of its

observed variables (e.g., λ11 = 1) (Bollen, 1989; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1996). Under these

assumptions, it is possible to estimate and compare the means of the latent variables

across groups. Consequently, λs for P1 and A3 were scaled to 1 to its posited latent

construct (pleasure and arousal) in Parts 1 and 3 (See Figure 5.1).

Page 207: Osu 1135134346

182

Figure 5.1. Model specification for Parts 1 and 3 in Study 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

θδ11 θδ22 θδ33

θδ44 θδ55 θδ66

1 λx21 λx31

1 λx52 λx62

φ22

φ11

Page 208: Osu 1135134346

183

Figure 5.2. Model specification for Part 2 and Part 4 in Study 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

θδ11 θδ22 θδ33

θδ44 θδ55 θδ66

θε11 θε22 θε33

θε77 θε88 θε99

λx11 λx21 λx31

λx42 λx52 λx62

λy11 λy21 λy31

λy73 λy83 λy93

γ11 γ21

γ12

γ22

1

1

Purchase Intention η2

γ32

γ31 PI1

PI2

PI4

θε44

θε55

θε66

λy42

λy52

λy62

ς1

ς2

ς3

1

1

1

Page 209: Osu 1135134346

184

Data Screening

The data used in Study 2 were prepared using PRELIS program and assessed for

the multivariate normality assumption before testing hypotheses. Appendix Q shows the

data screening result for the 15 observed variables analyzed in the proposed model for

Study 2, presenting mean, SD, skewness, and kurtosis. The distribution of the observed

variables was evaluated based on skewness and kurtosis. The distribution with skewness

and kurtosis equal to zero are considered as multivariate normal (Curran et al., 1996).

Skewness and kurtosis equal to 2 and 7 respectively are considered as moderately

nonnormal. Skewness equal to 3 and kurtosis equal to 21 are considered as severely

nonnormal (Curran et al., 1996). As shown in Appendix Q, all skewness coefficients of

the variables were close to zero (ranged from -.739 to .219) and all kurtosis coefficients

were close to zero (ranged from -1.006 to .690), indicating that the distribution of the

observed variables were approximately multivariate normal. Under the multivariate

normality assumptions and the proper model specification, the maximum likelihood (ML)

procedure provides asymptotically unbiased, consistent, and efficient parameter estimates

and standard errors (Bollen, 1989). Therefore, the ML function was used to estimate

model parameters with a covariance matrix in Study 2.

Page 210: Osu 1135134346

185

5.2.5. Hypotheses Testing

The design of Study 2 was a 2 x 2 x 2 between-subjects experiment with three

factors: situational involvement (high vs. low) x central cues (medium amount vs. high

amount) x peripheral cues (presence vs. absence). Study 2 consisted of four main parts.

Part 1 (Hypotheses 1 and 2) of the proposed model examined the influence of type of cue

(central cues and peripheral cues) on emotional states (pleasure and arousal) and mean

differences in emotional states over different treatment groups were estimated and

compared using multi-group structural equation modeling. Part 2 (Hypotheses 3)

investigating the effects of emotions on consumers’ response behaviors (satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors) was analyzed using a single group structural

equation model. Part 3 (Hypothesis 4) of the proposed model assessing the effects of

situational involvement as a moderator between S-O was tested using multi-group

structural equation modeling. Part 4 (Hypothesis 5) investigated the mediating effects of

emotions on the relationship between type of cue and consumers’ response behaviors and

the model was assessed using multi-group structural equation modeling. See Figure 5.3

for the proposed model in Study 2.

Page 211: Osu 1135134346

186

Figure 5.3. The proposed model in Study 2.

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Situational Involvement

Approach Behavior

Satisfaction H1a

H1b

H2a

H2b

H4a and H4b

H3a

H3b

H3d

H3c

H3e

H3f

Part Four

Part One Part Two

Part Three

H5a and H5b

Stimulus Organism Response

Page 212: Osu 1135134346

187

Part One (Hypotheses 1 and 2)

The first part of the proposed model investigated the effects of central cues and

peripheral cues on consumers’ emotional states such as pleasure and arousal. Mean

differences in emotional states over different treatment groups were estimated and

compared using multi-group structural equation modeling. Means of two exogenous

latent variables in the model (Figure 5.1) were compared over groups with different

levels of central cues (Hypothesis 1) or peripheral cues (Hypothesis 2).

Hypothesis 1. Central cues (e.g., number of different product views-front, back,

sides, details, amount of verbal information, and amount of mix and match

suggestions) will influence consumers’ emotional reactions experienced from an

apparel website.

Hypothesis 1 was tested using multi-group structural equation modeling. The

purpose of Hypothesis 1 was to examine the influence of different amounts of central

cues on emotional states (pleasure and arousal). The model included two latent variables

(pleasure and arousal) with 6 manifest variables (Figure 5.1). Means of pleasure and

arousal were compared by two groups with the high amount and the medium amount of

central cues. Means of the latent variables were set to zero in the group with the high

amount of central cues (group 1) but estimated in the group with the medium amount of

central cues (group 2). Factor loadings for P1 and A3 posited to measure the latent

Page 213: Osu 1135134346

188

variables were set to 1 for the identification purpose (Figure 5.1). The model was

analyzed using the maximum likelihood (ML) function with a covariance matrix.

Model fit. Since the AGFI is not measurable in a multi-group comparison, the

model fit was assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFIs, and NNFI. An overall chi-square

(df=24) was not significant (χ2 = 30.28, p = .17), indicating that the null hypothesis of

perfect fit was not rejected. The GFIs for each of the two groups (high amount vs.

medium amount of central cues) were calculated (.99 and 1.00, respectively). The NNFI

was 1.00 and the RMSEA value was .018. All fit indices were within acceptable ranges

(See Table 4.11) and indicated a good fit of the proposed model in Hypothesis 1 to the

data. All path coefficients for the model were significant, supported by significant t-

values. See Table 5.12 for the summary of the model fit. Figure 5.4 (unstandardized

solution) and Figure 5.5 (completely standardized solution) show all parameter estimates

calculated in the proposed model.

Page 214: Osu 1135134346

189

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.30 .04 33.81*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.13 .03 32.71*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.58 .07 21.72*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.35 .06 21.25*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .34 .03 10.60*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 30.38

df = 24 p = .17

RMSEA .018 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=1.00

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.12. Summary of the model fit for the proposed model in Hypothesis 1.

Page 215: Osu 1135134346

190

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 5.4. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Hypothesis 1.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

1 1.30 1.13

1 1.58 1.35

.34

Page 216: Osu 1135134346

191

Figure 5.5. Completely standardized parameter estimates for Hypothesis 1.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

.79 .85 .80

.67 .77 .70

.53

Page 217: Osu 1135134346

192

Hypothesis 1a. As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium

amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of

central cues will experience more pleasure.

Hypothesis 1a proposed that participants exposed to the website with the high

amount of central cues (group 1) will experience more pleasure than those exposed to the

medium amount of central cues (group 2). The estimated group means of the latent

variables are shown in Table 5.13. The mean of group 2 is interpreted as the mean

difference in pleasure between two groups with different amount of central cues. The

mean of pleasure for group 2 was -.14 with a significant t-value, indicating that

participants in group 1 (high amount of central cues) experienced significantly more

pleasure than those in group 2 (medium amount of central cues). Thus, Hypothesis 1a

was supported.

Hypothesis 1b. As compared to those exposed to the website with a medium

amount of central cues, consumers exposed to the website with a high amount of

central cues will experience more arousal.

Hypothesis 1a proposed that participants exposed to the website with the high

amount of central cues (group 1) will experience more arousal than those exposed to the

medium amount of central cues (group 2). The estimated group means of the latent

variables are presented in Table 5.13. The mean of group 2 is interpreted as the mean

difference in arousal between two groups with different amount of central cues. The

Page 218: Osu 1135134346

193

mean of pleasure for group 2 was -.05, indicating that participants in group 1 experienced

more arousal than those in group 2. However, the mean difference (t = -1.27) was not

statistically significant. Thus, Hypothesis 1b was not supported.

Pleasure Arousal High amount of central cues (Group 1, N=830)

0 0

Medium amount of central cues (Group 2, N = 804)

-.14 (.05)a

-2.96b**

-.05 (.04)a

-1.27b

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; ** p < .005

Table 5.13. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 1.

Page 219: Osu 1135134346

194

Hypothesis 2. Peripheral cues (e.g., pictorial icons and background colors) will

influence consumers’ emotional reactions experienced from the apparel website.

Hypothesis 2 was tested using multi-group structural equation modeling. The

focus of Hypothesis 2 was to assess the effects of presence or absence of peripheral cues

on emotional states (pleasure and arousal). Two exogenous latent variables (pleasure and

arousal) with 6 manifest variables were included in the proposed model (Figure 5.1).

Means of pleasure and arousal were estimated and compared across two treatment groups

manipulated by the presence or absence of peripheral cues. Means of the latent variables

were set to zero in the group with the presence of peripheral cues (group 1) but estimated

in the group with the absence of peripheral cues (group 2). Factor loadings for P1 and A3

posited to measure the latent variables were set to 1 for identification purposes (Figure

5.1). The model was tested using the maximum likelihood (ML) procedure with a

covariance matrix.

Model fit. The model fit was assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFIs, and NNFI.

An overall chi-square (df=24) was not significant (χ2 = 33.05, p = .10), indicating that the

null hypothesis of perfect fit was not rejected. The GFI for both groups (presence vs.

absence of peripheral cues) was .99. The NNFI was 1.00 and the RMSEA value was .021.

All fit indices were within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11), supporting a good fit of

the proposed model in Hypothesis 2 to the data. All path coefficients for the model were

significant, supported by significant t-values. Table 5.14 presents the summary of the

Page 220: Osu 1135134346

195

model fit. Figure 5.6 (unstandardized solution) and Figure 5.7 (completely standardized

solution) show all parameter estimates estimated in the proposed model.

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.31 .04 33.79*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.14 .03 32.64*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.60 .07 21.58*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.37 .06 21.14*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .31 .03 10.12*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 33.05

df = 24 p = .10

RMSEA .021 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=.99

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.14. Summary of the model fit for the proposed model in Hypothesis 2.

Page 221: Osu 1135134346

196

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 5.6. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Hypothesis 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

1 1.31 1.14

1 1.60 1.37

.31

Page 222: Osu 1135134346

197

Figure 5.7. Completely standardized parameter estimates for Hypothesis 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

.79 .85 .80

.66 .77 .70

.51

Page 223: Osu 1135134346

198

Hypothesis 2a. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, consumers exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

pleasure.

Hypothesis 2a suggested that participants who browsed the website with the

peripheral cues present (group 1) will experience more pleasure than those who browsed

the website without peripheral cues (group 2). The group means of the latent variables

are shown in Table 5.15. The mean of group 2 is interpreted as the mean difference in

pleasure between two groups manipulated by the presence or absence of peripheral cues.

The mean of pleasure for group 2 was -.09, suggesting that participants in group 2

(absence of peripheral cues) experienced less pleasure than those in group 1 (presence of

peripheral cues). However, the mean difference (t = -1.91) was not statistically

significant. Therefore, Hypothesis 2a was not supported.

Hypothesis 2b. As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral

cues, consumers exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more

arousal.

Hypothesis 2b suggested that participants exposed to the website with the

peripheral cues present (group 1) will experience more arousal than those exposed to the

website without peripheral cues (group 2). The mean of group 2 is interpreted as the

mean difference in arousal between two groups manipulated by the presence or absence

of peripheral cues. The group means of the latent variables are presented in Table 5.15.

Page 224: Osu 1135134346

199

The mean of arousal for group 2 was -.13 with a significant t-value, supporting that

participants in group 2 (absence of peripheral cues) experienced significantly less arousal

than those in group 1 (presence of peripheral cues). Thus, Hypothesis 2b was supported.

Pleasure Arousal Presence of peripheral cues (Group 1, N=807)

0 0

Absence of peripheral cues (Group 2, N = 827)

-.09 (.05)a -1.91b

-.13 (.04)a

-3.14b**

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; ** p < .005

Table 5.15. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 2.

Page 225: Osu 1135134346

200

Part Two (Hypothesis 3)

The second part of the proposed model investigated the effects of emotional states

(pleasure and arousal) induced by central cues and peripheral cues presented in the

websites on consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors). The proposed model in Part 2 (Figure 5.8) consisted of five latent variables

with 15 indicators (manifest variables). Two latent constructs (pleasure and arousal)

were exogenous latent variables (ξ) and the other three latent constructs (satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behaviors) were endogenous latent variables (η). The

model was analyzed using the maximum likelihood (ML) function with a covariance

matrix.

Hypothesis 3. Emotional states will be positively related to consumers’ response

behaviors.

Model fit. Hypothesis 3 was tested using single group structural equation

modeling. To assess the fit of the model to the data, chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, AGFI,

and NNFI were computed. The results of fitting the structural model to the data indicated

that the model had a good fit (Table 5.16). All path coefficients for the measurement

model were significant, indicating the validity of the observed variables posited to

measure latent constructs. The specified relationships between emotional states and

consumer response behaviors were statistically significant, as supported by significant t-

values. An overall chi-square was 351.61 (df = 80, p < .0001). Since the chi-square

Page 226: Osu 1135134346

201

statistic is sensitive to large sample sizes, the significant chi-square is not surprising. All

other fit indices were within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11), suggesting that the

proposed model in Part 2 fits the data very well. The RMSEA (.046) indicated a close fit

of the model according to Brown and Cudeck (1992). The GFI was .97, the AGFI

was .96, and the NNFI was 1.00. Table 5.16 presents the results of the model fit

including all path coefficients for the measurement and structural models. Figure 5.8 and

5.9 show all parameter estimates (unstandardized and standardized) calculated in the

proposed model.

Page 227: Osu 1135134346

202

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Structural Model Pleasure (ξ1) Satisfaction (η1) γ11 .83 .05 17.63*** Pleasure (ξ1) Purchase Intention (η2) γ21 .33 .04 8.79*** Pleasure (ξ1) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .57 .04 13.80*** Arousal (ξ2) Satisfaction (η1) γ12 .23 .04 5.46*** Arousal (ξ2) Purchase Intention (η2) γ22 .30 .04 7.40*** Arousal (ξ2) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .27 .04 6.70*** Measurement Model Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 .93 .02 37.28*** Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.17 .03 39.61*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.02 .03 36.85*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 .70 .03 26.02*** Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.12 .04 30.79*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 .98 .03 28.60*** Satisfaction (η1) SA1 λy11 .66 .02 39.11*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 2 λy21 .67 .02 38.29*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 4 λy31 .60 .02 31.98*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI1 λy41 .93 .02 41.41*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI2 λy51 .91 .02 39.63*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI4 λy61 .79 .02 33.43*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB1 λy71 .69 .02 34.89*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB3 λy81 .64 .02 37.26*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB4 λy91 .76 .02 42.09*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .52 .02 21.45*** Satisfaction (η1) Purchase Intention (η2) ψ21 .51 .02 21.08*** Satisfaction (η1) Approach Behaviors(η3) ψ31 .68 .02 35.18*** Purchase Intention (η2) Approach Behaviors (η3)

ψ32 .76 .02 48.72***

Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 351.61

df = 80 p < .0001

RMSEA .046 C.I. (.041; .051) GFI .97 AGFI .96 NNFI 1.00

Note. ***p< .001

Table 5.16. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in Part 2.

Page 228: Osu 1135134346

203

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 5.8. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Part 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.93 1.17 1.02

.70 1.12 .98

.66 .67 .60

.69 .64 .76

.83

.33

.23

.30

1

1

Purchase Intention

η2

.27

.57 PI1

PI2

PI4

.93

.91

.79

.52

ς1

ς2

ς3

.51

.76

.68

1

1

1

Page 229: Osu 1135134346

204

Figure 5.9. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the proposed model for Part 2.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.81 .84 .80

.65 .76 .71

.91 .89 .76

.79 .83 .91

.60

.29

.16

.26

Purchase Intention

η2

.22

.45 PI1

PI2

PI4

.89

.86

.75

ς1

ς2

ς3

.52

.80

.65

.82

Page 230: Osu 1135134346

205

Hypothesis 3a. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

Hypothesis 3a proposed that pleasure induced by the web cues presented in the

apparel website will be positively related to satisfaction. The significant path coefficient

showed a positive effect for pleasure on satisfaction (γ11 = .83, t = 17.63, p < .001),

indicating that participants who experienced more pleasure while browsing the websites

tended to have higher satisfaction with the website they browsed. Therefore, Hypothesis

3a was supported.

Hypothesis 3b. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ purchase

intention.

Hypothesis 3b predicted that pleasure induced by the web cues shown in the

apparel website will be positively related to purchase intention. The significant path

coefficient indicated a positive effect for pleasure on purchase intention (γ12 = .33, t =

8.79, p < .001), suggesting that participants who experienced more pleasure while

browsing the websites were likely to have higher purchase intention toward the website

they browsed. Thus, Hypothesis 3b was supported.

Page 231: Osu 1135134346

206

Hypothesis 3c. Pleasure will be positively related to consumers’ approach

behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 3c suggested that pleasure induced by the web cues offered on the

apparel website will be positively related to approach behaviors. The significant path

coefficient showed a positive effect of pleasure on approach behaviors (γ13 = .57, t =

13.80, p < .001), supporting that participants who experienced more pleasure when

browsing the apparel websites were likely to have more positive approach behaviors.

Consequently, Hypothesis 3c was supported.

Hypothesis 3d. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ satisfaction.

Hypothesis 3d proposed that arousal induced by the web cues presented in the

apparel websites will be positively related to consumer satisfaction. The significant path

coefficient indicated a positive effect of arousal on satisfaction (γ12 = .23, t = 5.46, p

< .001), implying that participants who experienced more arousal while browsing the

apparel websites were more likely to be satisfied with their shopping experience at the

mock websites. Therefore, Hypothesis 3d was supported.

Page 232: Osu 1135134346

207

Hypothesis 3e. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ purchase

intention.

Hypothesis 3e predicted that arousal induced by the web cues shown in the

apparel websites will be positively related to consumer purchase intention. The

significant path coefficient showed a positive effect for arousal on purchase intention (γ22

= .30, t = 7.40, p < .001), indicating that participants who experienced more arousal while

browsing the websites tended to have higher purchase intention toward the website they

browsed. Thus, Hypothesis 3e was supported.

Hypothesis 3f. Arousal will be positively related to consumers’ approach

behaviors (desire to explore or shop and likability of the websites).

Hypothesis 3f suggested that arousal induced by the web cues offered on the

apparel website will be positively related to approach behaviors. The significant path

coefficient indicated a positive effect for arousal on approach behaviors (γ33 = .27, t =

6.70, p < .001), indicating that participants who experienced more arousal when browsing

the apparel websites were likely to exhibit more approach behaviors. Therefore,

Hypothesis 3f was supported.

Page 233: Osu 1135134346

208

Part Three (Hypotheses 4)

The third part of the proposed model investigated if situational involvement

moderates the relationship between web cues (central cues and peripheral cues) and

consumers’ emotional states (pleasure and arousal). To test Hypothesis 4 mean

differences in emotional states over different treatment groups were estimated and

compared according to two different situational involvements using multi-group

structural equation modeling.

Hypothesis 4. Situational involvement will moderate the relationship between

web cues and emotional reactions.

Hypothesis 4 was assessed using multi-group structural equation modeling. The

focus of Hypothesis 4 was to assess the moderating effects of the situational involvement

on the relationship between web cues and emotional states (pleasure and arousal). Two

exogenous latent variables (pleasure and arousal) with 6 manifest variables (indicators)

were included in the proposed model (See Figure 5.1). Means of pleasure and arousal

induced by different web cues were estimated and compared across the two levels of

situational involvement. Means of pleasure and arousal were scaled to zero in the group

with the high amount of central cues in Hypothesis 4a and in the group with the presence

of peripheral cues in Hypothesis 4b. Means of the latent variables were estimated in the

group with the medium amount of central cues in Hypothesis 4a and with absence of

peripheral cues in Hypothesis 4b. Factor loadings for P1 and A3 posited to measure the

Page 234: Osu 1135134346

209

latent variables were set to 1 for identification purposes (Figure 5.10). The model was

tested using the maximum likelihood (ML) procedure with a covariance matrix.

Hypothesis 4a. Central cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions

under a high involvement situation than under a low involvement situation.

Hypothesis 4a proposed that the relationship between central cues and emotional

states (pleasure and arousal) will be moderated by situational involvement. The sample

was split into high and low involvement. The multi-group structural equation modeling

was conducted for each group (high vs. low involvement). Mean differences of pleasure

and arousal induced by different amount of central cues were estimated and compared

across the two different levels of involvement to test Hypothesis 4a.

Fit of the model in the high involvement condition (Model 1). To assess the fit of

the model to the data, chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI were computed. An overall

chi-square was 29.96 (df = 24, p = .186), indicating the null hypothesis of perfect fit was

not rejected. All other fit indices were within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11),

suggesting that the model fits the data very well. The RMSEA (.024) indicated a close fit

of the model according to Brown and Cudeck (1992). The GFI for each of the two

treatment groups (high amount (group 1) vs. medium amount of central cues (group 2))

was .99. The NNFI was 1.00. Table 5.17 presents the summary of the model fit

including all path coefficients for the measurement model. Figure 5.10 and 5.11 show all

parameter estimates (unstandardized and standardized) calculated in the model.

Page 235: Osu 1135134346

210

Fit of the model in the low involvement condition (Model 2). The model fit was

assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI. The non-significant chi-square

indicated that the null hypothesis of perfect fit was not rejected (χ2 = 22.56, df = 24, p

= .546). The GFI for each of the two treatment groups [high amount (group 1) vs.

medium amount of central cues (group 2)] was .99. The NNFI was 1 and the RMSEA

value was .010. All fit indices within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11) and provided

strong evidence of a good model fit. All path coefficients for the model were significant,

supported by significant t-values. See Table 5.18 for the results of the model fit. Figure

5.10 and 5.11 show all parameter estimates (unstandardized and standardized) calculated

in the model.

Page 236: Osu 1135134346

211

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.23 .05 24.17*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.12 .05 24.01*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.70 .11 14.88*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.38 .09 14.58*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .30 .04 7.38*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 29.96

df = 24 p = .186

RMSEA .024 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=.99

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.17. Summary of the model fit for Model 1 (high involvement) in Hypothesis 4a.

Page 237: Osu 1135134346

212

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.39 .06 23.08*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.15 .05 21.78*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.48 .09 15.77*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.32 .09 15.39*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .35 .05 7.25*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 22.56

df = 24 p = .546

RMSEA .010 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=.99

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.18. Summary of the model fit for Model 2 (low involvement) in Hypothesis 4a.

Page 238: Osu 1135134346

213

Note. All path coefficients (high involvement/low involvement) were significant.

Figure 5.10. Unstandardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4a.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

1 1.23/ 1.39 1.12/

1.15

1 1.70/1.48

1.38/1.32

.30/.35

Page 239: Osu 1135134346

214

Note. Path coefficients: high involvement/low involvement

Figure 5.11. Completely standardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4a.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

.80/.78 .82/.88 .82/.79

.63/.69 .77/.77 .68/.71

.47/.52

Page 240: Osu 1135134346

215

Mean comparisons. Hypothesis 4a proposed that central cues will have a stronger

effect on emotional states (pleasure and arousal) under the high involvement situation

(Model 1) than under the low involvement situation (Model 2). The estimated group

means of the latent variables (pleasure and arousal) in Model 1 and Model 2 are shown in

Table 5.19. The means of group 2 (medium amount of central cues) are interpreted as the

mean differences in pleasure and arousal between the two groups with different amounts

of central cues. As shown in Table 5.19, the effects of different amounts of central cues

on emotional states were significant only in the high involvement condition, supporting a

moderating effect of situational involvement between central cues and emotional states.

In Model 1, the estimated means of pleasure and arousal for group 2 were -.24 and -.21,

respectively with significant t-values, indicating that participants in group 2 (medium

amount of central cues) experienced significantly less pleasure and less arousal than those

in group 1 (high amount of central cues) under the high involvement situation. Whereas

in Model 2, the estimated means of two emotional states were not significantly different,

suggesting that emotional states were not significantly influenced by different amounts of

central cues in the low involvement situation. Thus, Hypothesis 4a was supported.

Page 241: Osu 1135134346

216

Model 1 (High involvement) Model 2 (Low involvement) Pleasure Arousal Pleasure Arousal High amount of central cues (Group 1, N= 440)

0 0 0 0

Medium amount of central cues (Group 2, N = 427)

-.24 (.07)a

-3.29b***

-.21 (.07)

-3.00**

-.07 (.08) -.87

.10 (.08) 1.16

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; ** p < .005, *** p < .001

Table 5.19. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 4a.

Hypothesis 4b. Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on emotional reactions

under a low involvement situation than under a high involvement situation.

Hypothesis 4b predicted that the relationship between peripheral cues and

emotional states (pleasure and arousal) will be moderated by different levels of

situational involvement. The sample was divided into high and low involvement as a

function of the verbal instructions. The multi-group structural equation modeling was

conducted for each group (high vs. low involvement). Mean differences of pleasure and

arousal induced by the presence or absence of peripheral cues were estimated and

compared by two different involvement groups to test Hypothesis 4b.

Page 242: Osu 1135134346

217

Fit of the model in the high involvement condition (Model 1). To test the fit of the

model to the data, chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI were calculated. An overall chi-

square was 28.23 (df = 24, p = .251), indicating that the null hypothesis of perfect fit was

not rejected. All other fit indices within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11) also

supported model fit. The small RMSEA (.020) indicated a close fit of the model. The

GFI for each of the two treatment groups [presence (group 1) vs. absence of peripheral

cues (group 2)] was .99. The NNFI was 1.00. Table 5.20 shows the summary of the

model fit including all path coefficients for the measurement model. Figure 5.12 and

5.13 illustrate all parameter estimates (unstandardized and standardized) measured in the

model.

Fit of the model in the low involvement condition (Model 2). The model fit was

assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI. The non-significant chi-square

indicated that the null hypothesis of perfect fit was not rejected (χ2 = 29.04, df = 24, p

= .219). The GFI for each of the two treatment groups [presence (group 1) vs. absence of

peripheral cues (group 2)] was .99. The NNFI was 1. The RMSEA value was .023,

indicating a close fit of the model. All fit indices within acceptable ranges (See Table

4.11) provided strong evidence of a good fit. All path coefficients for the model were

significant, supported by significant t-values. See Table 5.21 for the results of the model

fit. Figure 5.12 and 5.13 show all parameter estimates (unstandardized and standardized)

computed in the model.

Page 243: Osu 1135134346

218

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.25 .05 24.21*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.13 .05 24.02*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.73 .12 14.59*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.42 .10 14.39*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .29 .04 7.32*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 28.23

df = 24 p = .251

RMSEA .020 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=.99

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.20. Summary of the model fit for Model 1 (high involvement) in Hypothesis 4b.

Page 244: Osu 1135134346

219

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.40 .06 23.11*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.15 .05 21.77*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 1.00 n/aa n/aa

Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.49 .10 15.63*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 1.33 .09 15.25*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .33 .05 6.89*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 29.04

df = 24 p = .219

RMSEA .023 GFI Group 1=.99

Group 2=.99

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.21. Summary of the model fit for Model 2 (low involvement) in Hypothesis 4b.

Page 245: Osu 1135134346

220

Note. All path coefficients (high involvement/low involvement) were significant.

Figure 5.12. Unstandardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4b.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

1 1.25/ 1.40 1.13/

1.15

1 1.73/1.49

1.42/1.33

.29/.33

Page 246: Osu 1135134346

221

Note. Path coefficients: high involvement/low involvement

Figure 5.13. Completely standardized parameter estimates in Models 1 and 2 for Hypothesis 4b.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

P1 P2 P4

A3 A5 A6

.80/.77 .83/.88 .82/.78

.62/.69 .77/.79 .69/.70

.51/.51

Page 247: Osu 1135134346

222

Mean comparisons. Hypothesis 4b predicted that peripheral cues will have a

stronger effect on emotional states (pleasure and arousal) under the high involvement

situation (Model 1) than under the low involvement situation (Model 2). The estimated

group means of the latent variables (pleasure and arousal) in Model 1 and Model 2 are

shown in Table 5.22. The means of group 2 (absence of peripheral cues) are interpreted

as the mean differences in emotional states between groups with different levels of

peripheral cues. As presented in Table 5.22, the effect of peripheral cues on pleasure and

arousal was significant only in the low involvement situation, supporting a moderating

effect for situational involvement between peripheral cues (stimuli) and emotional states

(organism). Under the low involvement situation the estimated means of pleasure and

arousal for group 2 were -.17 and -.25, respectively with significant t-values, indicating

that in the low involvement condition participants exposed to the websites without

peripheral cues (group 2) experienced significantly less pleasure and arousal than those

exposed to the websites with the presence of peripheral cues (group 1). Whereas under

the high involvement situation the estimated means of two emotional states were not

significant, suggesting that emotional states were not significantly influenced by the

presence or absence of peripheral cues in the high involvement condition. Therefore,

Hypothesis 4b was supported.

Page 248: Osu 1135134346

223

Model 1 (High involvement) Model 2 (Low involvement) Pleasure Arousal Pleasure Arousal Presence of peripheral cues (Group 1, N=405)

0 0 0 0

Absence of peripheral cues (Group 2, N = 425)

-.03 (.07)a -.41b

-.10 (.07)

-1.32

-.17 (.08)

-2.14*

-.25 (.08)

-2.95**

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; * p < .05, ** p < .005

Table 5.22. Estimated means of pleasure and arousal in Hypothesis 4b.

Page 249: Osu 1135134346

224

Part Four (Hypothesis 5)

The results of Part 1 and Part 3 revealed the significant effects of different web

cues on emotional states when browsing the apparel websites. In addition, the result of

Part 2 supported the significant effects of emotional states on consumers’ response

behaviors. By taking the whole model into account, the fourth part of the proposed

model in Study 2 investigated the mediating effects of emotional states between web cues

and consumers’ response behaviors. In Part 4, to assess the mediating effects of

emotional states Hypothesis 5 tested to determine 1) the direct effect of web cues on

consumers’ response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors)

and 2) the change in the magnitude of the effect of web cues on consumers’ response

behaviors when the mediators (pleasure and arousal) were added to the model. The

models were tested using the maximum likelihood (ML) procedure with a covariance

matrix.

Hypothesis 5. Emotional states will mediate the relationship between type of cue

and consumers’ response behaviors.

The significant effects of different web cues on emotional states were found in

Part 1 and Part 3 and the significant effects of emotional states on consumers’ response

behaviors were supported in Part 2. Hypothesis 5 assessed the mediating effects of

emotional states on the relationship between web cues and consumers’ response

behaviors by testing the change in the magnitude of the effects of web cues on

Page 250: Osu 1135134346

225

consumers’ response behaviors when mediating variables (pleasure and arousal) were

added to the model that was used to test the direct effects of web cues on response

behaviors. If the effect of web cues on consumers’ response behaviors decreases when

emotional states are added to the model, it indicates the significant mediating effect of

emotional states between web cues and response behaviors. The proposed model in

Hypothesis 5 (Figure 5.16) consisted of five latent variables with 15 indicators (manifest

variables). Two latent constructs (pleasure and arousal) were exogenous latent variables

(ξ) and the other three latent constructs (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors) were endogenous latent variables (η). Multi-group structural equation

modeling was used to test Hypotheses 5a and 5b. The sample was split by two levels of

central cues for Hypothesis 5a and two levels of peripheral cues for Hypothesis 5b. The

models were tested using the maximum likelihood (ML) procedure with a covariance

matrix.

Before assessing the mediating effects of emotional states on the relationship

between web cues and response behaviors, the direct effects of web cues on response

behaviors should be tested (Baron & Kenny, 1986). Thus, multi-group structural

equation modeling was conducted to test the direct effects of web cues on consumers’

response behaviors. Mean differences of three latent variables were estimated and

compared by different levels of web cues presented on the websites. The proposed model

(Figure 5.14) consisted of three exogenous latent variables (ξ) with 9 indicators (manifest

variables). Factor loadings for SA1, PI1 and AB1 posited to measure the latent variables

were set to 1 for the identification purposes.

Page 251: Osu 1135134346

226

Hypothesis 5a. The relationships between central cues and response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) will be mediated by

emotional states.

Hypothesis 5a predicted the mediating effects of emotional states between central

cues and response behaviors in the proposed model. The sample was split by two

different levels of central cues (high amount vs. medium amount). To test for the

mediating effects, first the direct effect of central cues on consumers’ response behaviors

was assessed. Second, the change in the extent of the effects of central cues on consumer

response behaviors when emotional states were added to the model was tested.

The direct effects of central cues on response behaviors. The sample was split by

the two levels of central cues to compare the means of response behaviors across groups

exposed to different amounts of central cues. Means of three latent constructs were

scaled to zero in the group with the high amount of central cues. Means of the latent

variables were estimated in the group with the medium amount of central cues.

It was predicted that participants exposed to the website with the high amount of

central cues (group 1) would have greater satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors than those exposed to the medium amount of central cues (group 2). The

model fit was assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFIs, and NNFI. An overall chi-square

(df=69) was significant (χ2 = 290.57, p <.0001) which is not surprising with the large

sample size. The GFIs for both groups (high amount vs. medium amount of central cues)

were .95 and .97, respectively. The NNFI was .99. The RMSEA value was .063,

Page 252: Osu 1135134346

227

indicating a fair fit (Brown & Cudeck, 1992; MacCallum et al., 1996). All fit indices

were within acceptable ranges (See Table 4.11), supporting a moderate fit of the model to

the data. All path coefficients for the model were significant, supported by significant t-

values. Table 5.23 presents the summary of the model fit and parameter estimates

measured in the model.

The estimated group means of the latent variables are shown in Table 5.24. The

mean of group 2 is interpreted as the mean difference in consumers’ response behaviors

between two groups exposed to different amount of central cues. The means of

satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors were -.12, -.05, and -.09,

respectively, supporting that participants in group 1 (high amount of central cues) had

higher satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors than those in group 2

(medium amount of central cues). The mean difference for satisfaction and approach

behaviors were supported by significant t-values (See Table 5.24). However, the effect

on purchase intention was not statistically significant. Thus, the prediction of direct

effects for central cues on response behaviors was partially supported.

Page 253: Osu 1135134346

228

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Satisfaction (η1) SA1 λy11 1 n/aa n/aa Satisfaction (η1) SA 2 λy21 1.01 .02 49.27*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 4 λy31 .91 .02 38.17*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI1 λy41 1 n/aa n/aa Purchase Intention (η2) PI2 λy51 .98 .02 43.80*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI4 λy61 .85 .02 36.37*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB1 λy71 1 n/aa n/aa Approach Behaviors (η3) AB3 λy81 .93 .03 36.88*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB4 λy91 1.11 .03 41.55*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 290.57

df = 69 p < .0001

RMSEA .063 C.I. (.055; .070) GFI Group 1 = .95

Group 2 = .97

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.23. Summary of the model fit for the model tested the direct effects of central cues on response behaviors.

Page 254: Osu 1135134346

229

Satisfaction Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

High amount of central cues (Group 1, N=830)

0 0 0

Medium amount of central cues (Group 2, N = 804)

-.12 (.05)a

-2.48b*

-.05 (.06) -.84

-.09 (.04)

-1.97*

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; * p < .05

Table 5.24. Estimated means of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors.

Mediating effects of emotional states. In the next step the change in the

magnitude of the influence of central cues on response behaviors when the mediators

(pleasure and arousal) were added to the model was assessed using multi-group structural

equation modeling. The intercepts of endogenous latent variables (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behavior) were computed to test the group difference. The

intercepts were set to zero in the group with the high amount of central cues (group 1) but

estimated in the group with the medium amount of central cues (group 2). The intercepts

of group 2 are interpreted as a measure of the effect of central cues on consumers’

response behaviors in the model.

The model fit was assessed by chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI. An overall

chi-square was 460.12 (df = 191, p < .0001). Although the chi-square statistic was

Page 255: Osu 1135134346

230

significant, it was not surprising with the large sample size and the large number of

indicators. All other fit indices indicated a good fit of the model to the data (See Table

4.11 for acceptable ranges). The GFI for each of two treatment groups [high amount

(group 1) vs. medium amount of central cues (group 2)] was .96 and .97, respectively.

The NNFI was .99. The RMSEA value was .042, indicating a close fit of the model. All

path coefficients for the measurement and structural model were significant, supported by

significant t-values. See Table 5.25 for the results of the model fit. Figure 5.14

(unstandardized solution) and Figure 5.15 (completely standardized solution) show all

parameter estimates estimated in the proposed model.

The intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors were

presented in Table 5.26. No intercepts in group 2 were significantly different from those

in group 1, indicating that there was no significant effect for central cues on consumers’

response behaviors when emotional state variables were added. However, significant

structural coefficients revealed the significant effects of pleasure and arousal induced by

different amounts of central cues on consumer response behaviors (See Table 5.25). In

sum, the results suggested that the relationship between central cues and response

behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) were not direct and

tended to be mediated by emotional states (pleasure and arousal). Therefore, Hypothesis

5a was supported.

Page 256: Osu 1135134346

231

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Structural Model Pleasure (ξ1) Satisfaction (η1) γ11 .82 .05 17.57*** Pleasure (ξ1) Purchase Intention (η2) γ21 .34 .04 8.81*** Pleasure (ξ1) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .57 .04 13.78*** Arousal (ξ2) Satisfaction (η1) γ12 .22 .04 5.44*** Arousal (ξ2) Purchase Intention (η2) γ22 .29 .04 7.34*** Arousal (ξ2) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .26 .04 6.57*** Measurement Model Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 .93 .02 37.45*** Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.16 .03 39.44*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.02 .03 36.83*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 .71 .03 26.10*** Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.12 .04 30.96*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 .98 .03 28.60*** Satisfaction (η1) SA1 λy11 .66 .02 39.20*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 2 λy21 .67 .02 38.36*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 4 λy31 .61 .02 32.24*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI1 λy41 .93 .02 41.44*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI2 λy51 .91 .02 39.80*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI4 λy61 .79 .02 33.54*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB1 λy71 .69 .02 34.94*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB3 λy81 .64 .02 37.34*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB4 λy91 .76 .02 42.18*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .50 .03 14.30*** Satisfaction (η1) Purchase Intention (η2) ψ21 .54 .03 17.08*** Satisfaction (η1) Approach Behaviors(η3) ψ31 .70 .03 27.85*** Purchase Intention (η2) Approach Behaviors (η3)

ψ32 .77 .02 36.25***

Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 460.12

df = 191 p < .0001

RMSEA .042 C.I. (.037; .046) GFI Group 1a = .96

Group 2b = .97

NNFI .99

Note. a Group 1 (high amount of central cues), b Group 2 (medium amount of central cues); ***p< .001

Table 5.25. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in Hypothesis 5a.

Page 257: Osu 1135134346

232

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 5.14. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5a.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.93 1.16 1.02

.71 1.12 .98

.66 .67 .61

.69 .64 .76

.82

.34

.22

.29

1

1

Purchase Intention

η2

.26

.57 PI1

PI2

PI4

.93

.91

.79

ς1

ς2

ς3

1

1

1

.50

.70

.54

.77

Page 258: Osu 1135134346

233

Figure 5.15. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5a.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.81 .84 .80

.66 .76 .71

.91 .89 .76

.79 .83 .91

.59

.29

.16

.26

Purchase Intention

η2

.21

.46 PI1

PI2

PI4

.89

.86

.76

ς1

ς2

ς3

.79

.50

.82

.63

Page 259: Osu 1135134346

234

Satisfaction Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

High amount of central cues (Group 1, N=830)

0 0 0

Medium amount of central cues (Group 2, N = 804)

-.03 (.06)a -.52b

.02 (.05) .45

-.02 (.06)

-.35

Note. a Standard error, b t-values

Table 5.26. Estimated intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors in Hypothesis 5a.

Hypothesis 5b. The relationships between peripheral cues and response behaviors

(satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) will be mediated by

emotional states.

Hypothesis 5b proposed the mediating effects of emotional states between

peripheral cues and response behaviors in the proposed model. The sample was divided

into the presence or absence of peripheral cues. To test for the mediating effects, first the

direct effects of peripheral cues on consumers’ response behaviors were assessed.

Second, the change in the extent of the effect for peripheral cues on consumers’ response

behaviors when emotional states were added to the model was tested.

Page 260: Osu 1135134346

235

The direct effects of peripheral cues on response behaviors. The sample was split

by two levels of peripheral cues to compare the means of response behaviors across

groups exposed to the presence or absence of peripheral cues. Means of three latent

constructs were scaled to zero in the group with the presence of peripheral cues. Means

of the latent variables were estimated in the group with no peripheral cues.

It was proposed that participants exposed to the website with the presence of

peripheral cues (group 1) would have higher satisfaction, purchase intention, and

approach behaviors than those exposed to the absence of peripheral cues (group 2). A

chi-square, RMSEA, GFIs, and NNFI were computed to assess the model fit. An overall

chi-square was 310.78 (df = 69, p <.0001). Because the chi-square statistic is sensitive to

the large sample size, the significant chi-square is not surprising. The GFIs for both

groups (presence vs. absence of peripheral cues) were .95 and .97, respectively. The

NNFI was .99. The RMSEA value was .066, indicating a fair fit (Brown & Cudeck,

1992; MacCallum et al., 1996). All fit indices were within acceptable ranges (See Table

4.11), suggesting a moderate fit of the model to the data. All path coefficients for the

model were significant. Table 5.27 presents the summary of the model fit and parameter

estimates measured in the model.

The mean of group 2 is interpreted as the mean difference in consumers’ response

behaviors between two groups manipulated by the presence or absence of peripheral cues.

The estimated group means of the latent variables are presented in Table 5.28. The group

means of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors were -.08, -.08, and -

.12, respectively, suggesting that participants in group 1 (presence of peripheral cues) had

higher satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors than those in group 2

Page 261: Osu 1135134346

236

(absence of peripheral cues). However, the mean difference was statistically significant

for approach behaviors only. The mean differences in satisfaction and purchase intention

were not statistically significant. Therefore, the direct effect of peripheral cues on

response behaviors was partially supported.

Page 262: Osu 1135134346

237

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Model Paths Satisfaction (η1) SA1 λy11 1 n/aa n/aa Satisfaction (η1) SA 2 λy21 1.01 .02 49.23*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 4 λy31 .91 .02 38.21*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI1 λy41 1 n/aa n/aa Purchase Intention (η2) PI2 λy51 .98 .02 43.78*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI4 λy61 .85 .02 36.39*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB1 λy71 1 n/aa n/aa Approach Behaviors (η3) AB3 λy81 .92 .03 36.92*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB4 λy91 1.11 .03 41.60*** Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 310.78

df = 69 p < .0001

RMSEA .066 C.I. (.058; .073) GFI Group 1 = .95

Group 2 = .97

NNFI 1.00

Note. a The values are not available because the path coefficients were set to 1 for the identification purpose; ***p< .001

Table 5.27. Summary of the model fit for the model tested the direct effects of peripheral cues on response behaviors.

Page 263: Osu 1135134346

238

Satisfaction Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

Presence of peripheral cues (Group 1, N=807)

0 0 0

Absence of peripheral cues (Group 2, N = 827)

-.08 (.05)a -1.58b

-.08 (.06) -1.49

-.12 (.04)

-2.71*

Note. a Standard error, b t-values; * p < .05

Table 5.28. Estimated means of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors.

Mediating effects of emotional states. The change in the magnitude of the

influence of peripheral cues on response behaviors when the mediators (pleasure and

arousal) were entered to the model was assessed using multi-group structural equation

modeling in the next step. The intercepts of endogenous latent variables (satisfaction,

purchase intention, and approach behavior) were calculated to test the group difference.

The intercepts were set to zero in the group with the presence of peripheral cues (group 1)

but estimated in the group with the absence of peripheral cues (group 2). The intercepts

of group 2 are interpreted as a measure of the effect of peripheral cues on consumers’

response behaviors in the model.

To assess the fit of the model to the data, chi-square, RMSEA, GFI, and NNFI

were computed. An overall chi-square was 509.25 (df = 191, p < .0001). A significant

chi-square was not surprising with the large sample size and the large number of

Page 264: Osu 1135134346

239

indicators. All other fit indices indicated a good fit of the model to the data (See Table

4.11 for acceptable ranges). The GFI for each of two treatment groups (presence (group

1) vs. absence of peripheral cues (group 2)) was .95 and .97, respectively. The NNFI

was .99. The RMSEA value was .045, indicating a close fit of the model. All path

coefficients for the measurement and structural model were significant, supported by

significant t-values. See Table 5.29 for the results of the model fit. Figure 5.16

(unstandardized solution) and Figure 5.17 (completely standardized solution) show all

parameter estimates estimated in the proposed model.

The intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors were

presented in Table 5.30. No intercepts in group 2 were significantly different from those

in group 1, indicating that there was no significant effect for peripheral cues on

consumers’ response behaviors when emotional variables were added. On the other hand,

significant structural coefficients indicated the significant effects of pleasure and arousal

induced by peripheral cues on consumer response behaviors (See Table 5.29). In other

words, the results demonstrated that the relationship between peripheral cues and

response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) were not

direct and were mediated by emotional states (pleasure and arousal). Therefore,

Hypothesis 5b was supported.

Page 265: Osu 1135134346

240

Parameters ML estimates

Standard errors

t-values

Structural Model Pleasure (ξ1) Satisfaction (η1) γ11 .82 .05 17.59*** Pleasure (ξ1) Purchase Intention (η2) γ21 .33 .04 8.74*** Pleasure (ξ1) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .56 .04 13.75*** Arousal (ξ2) Satisfaction (η1) γ12 .23 .04 5.57*** Arousal (ξ2) Purchase Intention (η2) γ22 .30 .04 7.48*** Arousal (ξ2) Approach Behaviors (η3) γ31 .27 .04 6.62*** Measurement Model Pleasure (ξ1) P1 λx11 .92 .02 37.20*** Pleasure (ξ1) P2 λx21 1.16 .03 39.75*** Pleasure (ξ1) P4 λx31 1.02 .03 36.90*** Arousal (ξ2) A3 λx41 .69 .03 25.83*** Arousal (ξ2) A5 λx51 1.12 .04 30.90*** Arousal (ξ2) A6 λx61 .98 .03 28.61*** Satisfaction (η1) SA1 λy11 .66 .02 39.15*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 2 λy21 .67 .02 38.30*** Satisfaction (η1) SA 4 λy31 .60 .02 31.98*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI1 λy41 .93 .02 41.38*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI2 λy51 .91 .02 39.58*** Purchase Intention (η2) PI4 λy61 .79 .02 33.47*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB1 λy71 .69 .02 34.96*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB3 λy81 .64 .02 37.35*** Approach Behaviors (η3) AB4 λy91 .76 .02 42.13*** Non-causal Relationship Pleasure (ξ1) Arousal (ξ2) φ21 .52 .03 15.04*** Satisfaction (η1) Purchase Intention (η2) ψ21 .53 .03 16.59*** Satisfaction (η1) Approach Behaviors(η3) ψ31 .71 .03 28.27*** Purchase Intention (η2) Approach Behaviors (η3)

ψ32 .75 .02 33.86***

Model fit Chi-square (χ2) 509.25

df = 191 p < .0001

RMSEA .045 C.I. (.040; .050) GFI Group 1a = .95

Group 2b = .97

NNFI .99

Note. a Group 1 (presence of peripheral cues), b Group 2 (absence of peripheral cues); ***p< .001

Table 5.29. Summary of measurement and structural models and model fit in Hypothesis 5b.

Page 266: Osu 1135134346

241

Note. All path coefficients were significant.

Figure 5.16. Unstandardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5b.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.92 1.17 1.02

.69 1.12 .98

.66 .67 .60

.69 .64 .76

.82

.33

.23

.30

1

1

Purchase Intention

η2

.27

.56 PI1

PI2

PI4

.93

.91

.79

ς1

ς2

ς3

.52

.71

.75

.53

1

1

1

Page 267: Osu 1135134346

242

Figure 5.17. Completely standardized parameter estimates in the model for Hypothesis 5b.

Arousal ξ2

Pleasure ξ1

Approach Behavior

η3

Satisfaction η1

P1 P2 P4

SA1 SA2 SA4

A3 A5 A6

AB1 AB3 AB4

.80 .84 .80

.65 .76 .71

.91 .88 .75

.79 .83 .91

.59

.29

.17

.26

Purchase Intention

η2

.21

.45 PI1

PI2

PI4

.89

.86

.75

ς1

ς2

ς3

.52

.78

.63

.81

Page 268: Osu 1135134346

243

Satisfaction Purchase Intention

Approach Behaviors

Presence of peripheral cues (Group 1, N=807)

0 0 0

Absence of peripheral cues (Group 2, N = 827)

.01 (.06)a .17b

.00 (.05) .06

-.07 (.06)

-1.26

Note. a Standard error, b t-values

Table 5.30. Estimated intercepts of satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors in Hypothesis 5b.

Page 269: Osu 1135134346

244

CHAPTER 6

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The purpose of the research was to examine the impact of web cues, product

involvement, and situational involvement on consumers’ emotions that in turn, influence

their response behaviors based on the Elaboration Likelihood Model and the Stimuli-

Organism-Response paradigm. This research consists of two main studies. Study 1

investigated: 1) the effects of peripheral cues on emotions (pleasure and arousal) under a

low involvement situation (Hypothesis 1), 2) the effect of product involvement as a

moderator between peripheral cues and consumer emotions (Hypothesis 2), 3) the effects

of emotions on consumer response behaviors (purchase intention and approach

behaviors) (Hypotheses 3 and 4), and 4) the mediating effect of emotions between

peripheral cues and response behaviors (Hypothesis 5). Table 6.1 presents the results of

Hypotheses testing in Study 1. Study 2 explored: 1) the effects of type of cue (central

cues and peripheral cues) on emotions (pleasure and arousal) (Hypotheses 1 and 2), 2) the

effects of emotions on consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and

approach behaviors) (Hypothesis 3), 3) the effects of situational involvement as a

moderator between web cues and emotions (Hypotheses 4), and 4) the mediating effects

Page 270: Osu 1135134346

245

of emotions on the relationship between type of cue and consumer response behaviors

(Hypotheses 5). Table 6.3 shows the results of Hypotheses testing in Study 2. This

chapter 1) summarizes the findings of Study 1 and Study 2, 2) discusses research

implications for Study 1 and Study 2, 3) addresses limitations, and 4) suggests future

research.

6.1. Discussion

6.1.1. Findings from Study 1

The Effects of Peripheral Cues on Emotional States

In Study 1 all participants were exposed to low situational involvement

manipulated by giving them instructions to browse the website. To induce a low

involvement situation, all participants were asked to read the same scenario: “Now, you

are going to visit one clothing website. Browse and look around the site for a while.”

The effects of peripheral cues (atmospheric web cues: fonts, icons, and background color)

on emotions were tested using between-subjects multivariate analyses of variance.

Hypotheses (1a and 1b) were supported by the results of the analysis (Table 6.1 and

Figure 6.1). The results revealed that the peripheral cues (presence or absence of)

significantly influenced consumers’ emotions (pleasure and arousal). People exposed to

the websites with the presence of peripheral cues (a pink background with a logo pattern,

a flashing pink brand logo, and icons with roll-over images) exhibited more pleasure and

arousal than people exposed to the websites without those peripheral cues (white

Page 271: Osu 1135134346

246

background, a static brand logo with black color, and text icons). This suggests that

when peripheral cues are presented on apparel websites, people are more likely to feel

pleased and aroused than when peripheral cues are not presented on the apparel websites.

As expected, in the low involvement situation the presence of peripheral cues had a great

effect on consumer emotions. This result is consistent with the S-O-R paradigm

(Mehrabian & Russell, 1974) and the findings of prior research (Babin et al., 2003; Baker

et al., 1992; Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al., 1994; Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon

& Kahn, 2002) that examined the effects of atmospheric cues on consumers’ pleasure and

arousal in either in-store or online settings.

Page 272: Osu 1135134346

247

Note. * p < .01, ** p < .005

Figure 6.1. The results of Hypotheses 1a and 1b.

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

F (1, 152) = 7.15*

F (1, 152) = 8.36**

Stimulus Organism

MANOVA: F (2, 151) = 4.95*

Page 273: Osu 1135134346

248

The Moderating Effect of Product Involvement between S-O

The effect of product involvement (with clothing products) on the relationship

between peripheral cues and emotions was tested using a 2 (high vs. low product

involvement) x 2 (presence vs. absence of peripheral cues) between subjects multivariate

analysis of variance. Product involvement with clothing products was measured using 10

items (Zaichkowsky, 1994). To test for a moderating effect of product involvement (with

clothing) on the relationship between peripheral cues and emotions, participants were

split into low or high product involvement groups based on the median value. The two

Hypotheses (2a and 2b) were supported by the results (Table 6.1). The results showed

that clothing product involvement had a significant impact on the relationship between

peripheral cues and consumers’ emotions (pleasure and arousal). The effects of

peripheral cues on emotions were only significant for people for whom clothing products

had low personal relevance (i.e., low level of clothing involvement) (See Figure 6.2). No

effects of peripheral cues on emotions were found for people for whom clothing products

had high personal relevance (i.e., high level of clothing involvement). Emotions

experienced by people with high product involvement were not significantly influenced

by peripheral cues presented on the websites. This indicates that peripheral cues may

have a stronger effect on pleasure or arousal for consumers with a low level of product

involvement than for those with a high level of product involvement. In other words,

when situational involvement is controlled and low, different levels of product

involvement (personal relevance of the clothing products) moderate the relationship

between peripheral cues and the emotions experienced by consumers.

Page 274: Osu 1135134346

249

These results support the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996) that explains the

influence of peripheral cues on various consumer behaviors under a low involvement

condition. According to the ELM, in comparison with the highly involved consumers,

consumers in the low involvement condition tend to pay more attention to peripheral cues

(atmospheric cues). Therefore, peripheral cues affect pleasure and arousal for consumers

with low levels of product involvement as compared to those with high levels of product

involvement.

Page 275: Osu 1135134346

250

Note. a The effects of peripheral cues on emotional states are significant only for low product involvement group; ** p < .005

Figure 6.2. The results of Hypotheses 2a and 2b.

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

F (1, 74) = 8.25a**

F (1, 74) = 10.22a**

Stimulus Organism

MANOVA for low product involvement: F (2, 73) = 6.18** MANOVA for high product involvement: F (2, 75) = .093

Page 276: Osu 1135134346

251

The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors

The focus of Hypotheses 3 and 4 in Study 1 was to assess the effects of emotions

(pleasure and arousal) induced by peripheral cues on consumer response behaviors

(purchase intention and approach behaviors) using single group structural equation

modeling (See Figure 6.3 for the results).

The results showed that pleasure had significant effects on purchase intention and

approach behaviors. People who experienced more pleasure while browsing the apparel

website were likely to have higher purchase intention for the website they browsed than

those who experienced less pleasure. In addition, pleasure had a great impact on

consumer approach behaviors. Pleasure was positively related to likelihood of exploring

in or shopping from the website. Pleasure experienced by consumers while browsing the

website may significantly influence their purchase intention and approach behaviors

(likely to explore or shop).

Arousal also had significant effects on consumer response behaviors such as

purchase intention and approach behaviors. People who experienced more arousal while

browsing the website tended to have greater purchase intention than those who

experienced less arousal. Moreover, approach behaviors (likelihood of exploring or

shopping) were influenced by arousal experienced by people while browsing the apparel

website. Arousal was positively related to likelihood of exploring in or shopping from

the website. These results indicate that arousal experienced by consumers while browsing

may lead to more positive response behaviors toward the apparel online retailers.

These results are consistent with the S-O-R paradigm suggesting the significant

effects of the emotions on consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). In

Page 277: Osu 1135134346

252

addition, the findings support previous studies that investigated the effects of emotions on

purchase intention (Baker et al., 1992; Fiore & Kimle, 1997; Fiore et al., 2005; Park et al.,

2005) and approach behaviors (Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et

al., 2005; Huang, 2003; Hui, Dube, & Chebat, 1997; Menon & Kahn, 2002; Sherman et

al., 1997).

Page 278: Osu 1135134346

253

Note. ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Figure 6.3. The results of Hypotheses 3 and 4.

Arousal

Pleasure

Approach Behavior

Patronage Intention

t = 2.68**

t = 3.25**

t = 4.73***

t = 3.99***

Organism Response

Chi-square (χ2) RMSEA GFI AGFI NNFI 53.84, p=.26 .028 .95 .91 1.00

Page 279: Osu 1135134346

254

The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R

Hypothesis 5 in Study 1 predicted consumer emotions as mediators between

peripheral cues and response behaviors. To test if emotions were mediating the

relationship between peripheral cues and response behaviors, first the direct effects of

peripheral cues on response behaviors were assessed (Baron & Kenny, 1986). The

effects of peripheral cues presented in the apparel websites on response behaviors such as

purchase intention and approach behaviors were assessed using multivariate analysis of

variance. The results revealed the critical impact of peripheral cues on approach

behaviors but not on purchase intention. People exposed to the website with the presence

of peripheral cues (a pink background with a logo pattern, a flashing pink brand logo, and

icons with roll-over images) were more likely to explore or shop the website and tended

to like the website more than people exposed to the website without the peripheral cues.

However, there was no direct effect for peripheral cues on purchase intention. Purchase

intention did not significantly differ as a function of exposure to the website. .

In the next step, emotions (pleasure and arousal) were added to the analysis.

Multiple regression analysis was conducted to test the mediating effects of emotions

(Baron & Kenny, 1986). The results showed that there were no significant effects for

peripheral cues on consumer response behaviors when emotions were added to the

analysis (See Figure 6.4). However, pleasure and arousal as mediators had significant

impacts on approach behaviors and purchase intention (See Figure 6.4). The findings

suggest that pleasure and arousal induced by peripheral cues while browsing the apparel

websites may affect consumers’ purchase intentions and approach behaviors. In other

words, the effects of peripheral cues (background color and colorful icons) on purchase

Page 280: Osu 1135134346

255

intentions and approach behaviors are not direct and appear to be mediated by people’s

emotions experienced while browsing the apparel website. The results are consistent

with the S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974) that proposes that emotions are

mediating variables in determining a variety of consumer response behaviors such as

purchase intentions and approach behaviors (e.g., likely to explore and shop). Results are

also congruent with findings from previous research (Eroglu et al., 2003) that suggest that

emotions experienced by shoppers mediate the effects of online atmospherics on

shopping outcomes (satisfaction and approach behaviors).

Page 281: Osu 1135134346

256

Note. **p < .01, ***p< .001

Figure 6.4. The results of Hypothesis 5.

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure

Approach Behavior

Patronage Intention H1a and H2a

H1b and H2b

t = 2.876**

t = 4.935***

t = 5.485***

t = 4.693***

Stimulus Organism Response

t = -1.721

t = -.052

Page 282: Osu 1135134346

257

Hypotheses

Status

H1a As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more pleasure.

Supported

H1b As compared to those exposed to the website without peripheral cues, those exposed to the website with peripheral cues will experience more arousal.

Supported

H2a Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on pleasure for people with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Supported

H2b Peripheral cues will have a stronger effect on arousal for people with low product involvement than those with high product involvement.

Supported

H3a Pleasure will be positively related to purchase intention.

Supported

H3b Arousal will be positively related to purchase intention.

Supported

H4a Pleasure will be positively related to approach behaviors.

Supported

H4b Arousal will be positively related to approach behaviors.

Supported

H5 Emotional states such as pleasure and arousal will mediate the relationship between peripheral cues and consumers’ response behaviors.

Supported

Note. All hypotheses were tested under low situational involvement.

Table 6.1. Summary of hypotheses testing results in Study 1.

Page 283: Osu 1135134346

258

6.1.2. Findings from Study 2

Based on the proposed model, Study 2 includes four main parts: 1) Part 1

examined the effects of type of cue (central cues and peripheral cues) on emotions

(pleasure and arousal), 2) Part 2 assessed the effects of emotions on consumer response

behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors), 3) Part 3 examined

the effects of situational involvement as a moderator between S-O, and 4) Part 4

investigated the mediating effects of emotions on the relationship between type of cue

and consumer response behaviors. A 2 x 2 x 2 between subjects’ factorial design was

employed in Study 2: situational involvement (high vs. low) x central cues (medium

amount vs. high amount) x peripheral cues (presence vs. absence). Table 6.2 shows

experimental manipulations used in Study 2. In this section, the findings from the four

parts of Study 2 are presented.

Page 284: Osu 1135134346

259

Manipulations Manipulation Check

Situational Involvement

F (1, 1564) = 9.78**

High involvement (Purchasing situation)

1. Rewards: Apparel items selected during browsing or cash award 2. Given scenario: “Imagine that you have been given a $100 gift certificate to purchase clothing from an online apparel store, E-style.com. Please browse for five pairs of pants on the website for a while and select one item that you would like to purchase. Then, finish the survey after shopping the site” 3. Order form: Complete the order form for an item

M = 41.00 SD = 10.87

Low involvement (Browsing situation without purchase)

1. Rewards: Cash award only 2. Given scenario: “Imagine that today you find an online apparel store, E-style.com. Browse the website for a while and finish the survey after browsing the site” 3. No order form

M = 39.20 SD = 11.90

Central Cues F (1, 1601) = 47.22*** High amount 1. High amount of verbal information

(Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, inseam measurement, fit information, waist information, design details (pockets, belt, and/or stitching), and item care) 2. Three mix & match suggestions 3. Front, back, side, and detail views

M = 16.51 SD = 4.47

Medium amount 1. Medium amount of verbal information (Color, price, size, fabric, enclosure, and style information, country of origin, and inseam measurement) 2. One mix & match suggestion 3. Only front larger view

M = 14.97 SD = 4.51

Peripheral Cues F (1, 1583) = 15.32*** Presence colorful icons with a roll-over image, a flashing brand

logo image with pink color, colorful menu bars, pink background with a brand logo pattern, and colorful texts (blue) and images

M = 18.73 SD = 4.12

Absence text icons without colorful images, a static brand logo image with black color, grey menu bars, white background without any pattern, achromatic text colors: black and grey except ‘sale’ menu

M =17.91 SD = 4.08

Note. ** p < .01, *** p < .001 Table 6.2. Summary of manipulations used in Study 2.

Page 285: Osu 1135134346

260

The Effects of Web Cues on Emotional States

The proposed model for Part 1 in Study 2 investigated the effects of web cues

(central cues and peripheral cues) on consumers’ emotions (pleasure and arousal) while

browsing apparel websites. Mean differences in emotions across different treatment

groups were calculated and compared using multi-group structural equation modeling.

The results of Hypotheses 1 and 2 are presented in Figure 6.5.

The results of Hypothesis 1 showed that the means of pleasure and arousal were

higher for people exposed to the website with the high amount of central cues (product

related web cues) than for those exposed to the website with the medium amount of

central cues. People who were exposed to the websites with the high amount of product

related web cues (high amount of verbal information, three mix and match suggestions,

and front, back, side, and detail larger views) tended to experience significantly more

pleasure than those who exposed to the websites with the medium amount of product

related web cues (medium amount of verbal information, one mix and match suggestion,

and front larger view only). Although the means of arousal experienced by shoppers

were higher in the group exposed to the high amount of central cues, the difference in

arousal across two treatment groups was not statistically significant. In this research,

arousal experienced by shoppers while browsing the apparel websites did not

significantly differ as a function of the amount of central cues presented in the website.

In the case of peripheral cues, the results were reversed. The results of

Hypothesis 2 showed that the means of pleasure and arousal were higher for people

exposed to the website with the presence of peripheral cues (pink background with the

brand logo pattern and colorful icons with roll over images) than for those exposed to the

Page 286: Osu 1135134346

261

website without peripheral cues (white background color and links with texts). People

who were exposed to the websites with the presence of peripheral cues tended to

experience significantly more arousal than those who exposed to the websites with the

absence of peripheral cues. Although the pleasure experienced by shoppers was higher in

the group exposed to the presence of peripheral cues, the difference in pleasure across

two treatment groups was not statistically significant. Pleasure experienced by shoppers

during browsing the website did not significantly differ by the presence or absence of

peripheral cues shown in the apparel website.

In sum, before taking the effects of the situational involvement into account,

central cues tended to affect pleasure (and not arousal) while peripheral cues tended to

affect arousal (and not pleasure).

Page 287: Osu 1135134346

262

Note. a high amount of central cues, b medium amount of central cues, c presence of peripheral cues, d absence of peripheral cues; Negative t-values indicate that groups with the medium amount of central cues and with the absence of peripheral cues have lower pleasure and arousal; ** p < .005

Figure 6.5. The results of Hypotheses 1 and 2 in Part 1.

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues Arousal

Pleasure t = -2.96**

t = -1.27

t = -1.91

t = -3.14**

Stimulus Organism

Chi-square (χ2) RMSEA GFI NNFI Central cues: 30.38, p=.17 .018 .99a/1.00b 1.00 Peripheral cues: 33.05, p=.10 .021 .99c/.99d 1.00

Page 288: Osu 1135134346

263

The Effects of Emotional States on Response Behaviors

Part 2 in Study 2 assessed the effects of consumers’ emotions (pleasure and

arousal) induced by web cues while browsing the websites on consumer response

behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors) using single group

structural equation modeling. As shown in Table 6.3, all six Hypotheses (3a, 3b, 3c, 3d,

3e, and 3f) predicting the effects of the emotions on consumer response behaviors were

supported. Also see Figure 6.6 for the results of Hypothesis 3.

Pleasure had significant effects on satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors. People who experienced more pleasure while browsing the apparel website

were more likely to be satisfied with the website they browsed than those who

experienced less pleasure. Pleasure also influenced consumers’ purchase intentions.

People who experienced more pleasure tended to have higher purchase intentions than

those who experienced less pleasure. In addition, pleasure had a great impact on

consumers’ approach behaviors. Those who experienced more pleasure were more likely

to explore or shop in the website than those who experienced less pleasure. Pleasure

experienced by consumers while browsing the website may significantly influence their

satisfaction with the website, purchase intentions, and approach behaviors (likely to

explore or shop).

Arousal also had significant effects on consumer response behaviors such as

satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors. People who experienced more

arousal while browsing the website tended to have higher satisfaction than those who

experienced less arousal. The positive effect of arousal on purchase intention was also

found. More aroused people were likely to have greater purchase intentions than less

Page 289: Osu 1135134346

264

aroused people. Approach behaviors (likely to explore or shop) were also influenced by

the arousal experienced while browsing the apparel website. More aroused people were

more likely to explore or shop more in the apparel website than less aroused people.

These results indicate that high arousal experienced by consumers may result in more

positive response behaviors toward the apparel online retailers.

The results are congruent with the findings from Study 1. Both Study 1 and Study

2 found significant effects for emotions on consumer response behaviors. These findings

support the concept of the S-O-R paradigm predicting significant effects for emotions on

consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). The results also support

previous research that found the effects for emotions on various consumer response

behaviors such as satisfaction (Eroglu et al., 2003), purchase intentions (Baker et al.,

1992; Fiore & Kimle, 1997; Fiore et al., 2005; Park et al., 2005), and approach behaviors

(Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et al., 2005; Huang, 2003; Hui et

al., 1997; Menon & Kahn, 2002; Sherman et al., 1997).

Page 290: Osu 1135134346

265

Note. ***p< .001

Figure 6.6. The results of Hypothesis 3 in Part 2.

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Approach behavior

Satisfaction t = 17.63***

t = 8.79***

t = 5.46***

t = 13.80***

t = 7.40***

t = 6.70***

Organism Response

Chi-square (χ2) RMSEA GFI AGFI NNFI 351.61, p < .0001 .046 .97 .96 1.00

Page 291: Osu 1135134346

266

The Moderating Effect of Situational Involvement between S-O

The results of Part 3 revealed significant effects for situational involvement on the

relationship between web cues and consumers’ emotions. As presented in Table 6.3,

Hypotheses 4a and 4b were supported. Hypotheses 4a and 4b were tested using multi-

group mean comparison structural equation modeling. The results are shown in Figure

6.7.

The result of Hypothesis 4a showed that central cues affected emotions (pleasure

and arousal) under the high involvement situation, but had no effect on emotions under

the low involvement situation (See Figure 6.7). Under high situational involvement (e.g.,

purchasing situation) people exposed to the apparel website with the high amount of

central cues were likely to experience significantly more pleasure and arousal than those

exposed to the medium amount of central cues. Whereas under the low involvement

situation the emotions (pleasure and arousal) experienced by people did not significantly

differ by the different amount of central cues provided in the website.

The result of Hypothesis 4b revealed that peripheral cues affected emotions

(pleasure and arousal) under the low involvement situation (e.g., browsing situation

without a purchasing goal) (See Figure 6.7). However, under the high involvement

situation (e.g., purchasing situation) peripheral cues had no impact on emotions. People

in the low involvement condition experienced more pleasure and arousal when they were

exposed to the website in which peripheral cues were present, as compared to when the

peripheral cues were absent. However, in the high involvement condition people’s

pleasure and arousal were not significantly influenced by the presence or absence of

peripheral cues. This result is consistent with the ELM and prior research that shows the

Page 292: Osu 1135134346

267

significant effect of online atmospheric cues (peripheral cues) on pleasure under the low

involvement situation but not under the high involvement situation (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Overall, the results indicate that different types of web cues may influence the

level of pleasure and arousal felt by consumers while browsing the apparel websites.

Consistent with the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), the effects of different types of web

cues on consumers’ emotions are moderated by situational involvement (high vs. low).

Under the high involvement situation central cues (product related web cues) rather than

peripheral cues (atmospheric web cues: background color and colorful icons) may have

greater effect on the level of pleasure and arousal experienced by consumers, while under

the low involvement situation peripheral cues rather than central cues may have more

impact on the emotion felt by consumers.

Page 293: Osu 1135134346

268

Note. a high amount of central cues, b medium amount of central cues, c presence of peripheral cues, d absence of peripheral cues; Negative t-values indicate that groups with the medium amount of central cues and with the absence of peripheral cues have lower pleasure and arousal; * p < .05, ** p < .005, *** p < .001

Figure 6.7. The results of Hypothesis 4 in Part 3.

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Arousal

Pleasure t = -3.29***/-.87

t = -3.00***/1.16

t = -.41/-2.14*

t = -1.32/-2.95**

Stimulus Organism

Chi-square (χ2) RMSEA GFI NNFI Central cues (high inv/low inv): 29.96/22.56, p=.186/.546 .024/.010 .99a,.99b/.99,.99 1.00 Peripheral cues (high inv/low inv): 28.23/29.04, p=.251/.219 .020/.023 .99c,.99d/.99,.99 1.00

Page 294: Osu 1135134346

269

The Mediating Effects of Emotional States between S-R

In Part 4, the study investigated the mediating effects of emotions between web

cues and consumer response behaviors. To test if emotions are mediating the relationship

between web cues and response behaviors, first the direct effects of web cues on response

behaviors were tested (Baron & Kenny, 1986) using multi-group mean comparison

structural equation modeling and then the change in the extent of the effects of web cues

on response behaviors was assessed using multi-group structural equation modeling. As

presented in Table 6.3 and Figure 6.8, Hypotheses 5a and 5b were supported.

Hypothesis 5a revealed significant mediating effects of emotions between amount

of central cues presented on the websites and response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors). The direct effect of amount of central cues on

response behaviors was significant for satisfaction and approach behaviors. Although

purchase intentions were not statistically affected, people who were exposed to the high

amount of central cues had higher satisfaction and approach behaviors than those exposed

to the medium amount of central cues. When emotions was added to the model, the

effects of amount of central cues on consumer response behaviors disappeared (See

Figure 6.8). All significant regression coefficients (the effects of emotions on response

behaviors) indicated the strong effects of pleasure and arousal on consumer response

behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors). The results

revealed that the effects of different amounts of central cues presented in the apparel

websites on consumer response behaviors were mediated by pleasure and arousal

experienced by people while browsing the website.

Page 295: Osu 1135134346

270

The significant mediating effects of pleasure and arousal between presence or

absence of peripheral cues and consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors) were also found from Hypothesis 5b. The direct

effects of presence or absence of peripheral cues on response behaviors were significant

for approach behaviors. Although satisfaction and purchase intention were not

statistically affected, people exposed to the website with peripheral cues had higher

approach behaviors than those exposed to the website with no peripheral cues. When

emotions were added to the model, the effects of peripheral cues on consumer response

behaviors disappeared (See Figure 6.8). All significant regression coefficients (the

effects of emotions on response behaviors) indicated the strong effects of pleasure and

arousal on consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors). The results revealed that pleasure and arousal induced by the presence or

absence of peripheral cues shown in the apparel websites mediated the relationships

between peripheral cues and various response behaviors. This result is congruent with

the finding from Study 1, supporting the mediating effects of emotions between

peripheral cues and consumer response behaviors.

As a whole, the results of Hypotheses 5a and 5b suggest that consumers’ emotions

(pleasure and arousal) induced by different levels of web cues while browsing or

shopping the apparel websites may have significant effects on consumer response

behaviors. In other words, the effects of web cues on satisfaction, purchase intention, and

approach behaviors are not direct and tended to be mediated by the emotion felt by

consumers while browsing or shopping the apparel websites with various types of web

cues. The findings strongly support the S-O-R paradigm that predicts the emotions as

Page 296: Osu 1135134346

271

mediators in the relationship between various stimuli and consumer response behaviors

(Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). The results are consistent with earlier studies that found

the mediating effects of pleasure and arousal between the site or store atmospheric cues

and consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors) (Baker et al., 1992; Donovan & Rossiter, 1982; Donovan et al., 1994; Fiore &

Kimle, 1997; Eroglu et al., 2003; Menon & Kahn, 2002).

Page 297: Osu 1135134346

272

Note. The direct effects of central cues a and peripheral cues b on response behaviors after adding the emotions to the model; c high amount of central cues, d medium amount of central cues, e presence of peripheral cues, f absence of peripheral cues; *** p < .001

Figure 6.8. The results of Hypothesis 5 in Part 4.

Central Cues

Peripheral Cues

Purchase Intention

Arousal

Pleasure

Approach behavior

Satisfaction

H1a and H4a

H1b and H4a

H2a and H4b

H2b and H4b

Stimulus Organism Response

t = -.52a/.17b

t = .45/.06

t = -.35/-1.26

Chi-square (χ2) RMSEA GFI NNFI Central cues: 460.12, p < .0001 .046 .96c/.97d .99 Peripheral cues: 509.25, p < .0001 .045 .95e/.97f .99

t = 17.57***

t = 8.81***

t = 13.78***

t = 5.44***

t = 7.34***

t = 6.57***

Page 298: Osu 1135134346

273

Hypotheses

Status

Part 1

H1a Central cues Pleasure

Supported

H1b Central cues Arousal

Not Supported

H2a Peripheral cues Pleasure

Not Supported

H2b Peripheral cues Arousal

Supported

Part 2

H3a Pleasure Satisfaction

Supported

H3b Pleasure Purchase intention

Supported

H3c Pleasure Approach behaviors

Supported

H3d Arousal Satisfaction

Supported

H3e Arousal Purchase intention

Supported

H3f Arousal Approach behaviors

Supported

Part 3

H4a Situational involvement The relation between central cues and emotional states (pleasure and arousal)

Supported

H4b Situational involvement The relation between peripheral cues and emotional states (pleasure and arousal)

Supported

Part 4

H5a Mediating effect of emotional states Between central cues and response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors)

Supported

H5b Mediating effect of emotional states Between peripheral cues and response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors)

Supported

Table 6.3. Summary of hypotheses testing results in Study 2.

Page 299: Osu 1135134346

274

6.2. Implications

6.2.1. Managerial Implications for Study 1

The findings from Study 1 provide helpful information for apparel online retailers

developing effective online store designs that may attract online browsers. While

browsing apparel websites without a particular goal in mind (i.e., a purchasing goal),

people have a low level of situational involvement so that peripheral cues presented in the

websites influence browsers’ feelings of pleasure and arousal (Eroglu et al., 2003).

Consistent with Eroglu et al.’s research (2003), Study 1 found that while browsing

apparel websites without a purchasing goal (low situational involvement), people who

were exposed to the websites with peripheral cues (background color with a logo pattern,

blue text color, pink brand logo with flashing image, and colorful icons with roll over

images) exhibited greater pleasure and arousal than those exposed to the websites without

peripheral cues (white background without a log pattern, grey text color, static brand logo

with black color, and text icons). This indicates that peripheral cues such as background

color, text color, flashing image, and colorful icons in apparel websites may play a

significant role in enhancing the feelings of pleasure and arousal experienced by online

browsers. Accordingly, online apparel retailers may wish to use various peripheral cues

such as colors for a background or for icons and flashing images rather than static images

to build more pleasant and arousing apparel websites that attract browsers’ attention and

consequently influence their emotions while browsing the websites. Although online

browsers may not make purchases on their first visit, browsers who are more attracted to

Page 300: Osu 1135134346

275

apparel websites and who experience more pleasure and arousal while browsing may like

the websites better (Sherman et al., 1997) and have higher tendency to revisit the

websites later to buy products (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et al., 2005).

Study 1 revealed a significant effect for peripheral cues on pleasure and arousal

felt by online browsers who do not have a purchasing goal (low situational involvement).

However, even under low situational involvement, people vary in their level of enduring

product involvement with clothing (Zaichkowsky, 1986) that possibly moderates the

relationship between peripheral cues and emotions. The findings of Study 1 revealed a

significant moderating effect for clothing product involvement on the relationship

between peripheral cues and emotions (pleasure and arousal). The effects of peripheral

cues on emotions were only significant for people with a low level of clothing product

involvement. Pleasure and arousal experienced by people with high product involvement

were not significantly affected by the peripheral cues presented in apparel websites. This

indicates that peripheral cues induce more pleasure and arousal for online browsers with

the low level of clothing product involvement than for those with a high level of clothing

product involvement. This further emphasizes the important role of peripheral cues

(background color, text color, flashing image, and colorful icons) in apparel websites for

attracting online browsers who have a low level of clothing product involvement. People

with low clothing involvement may have less interest in, place less importance on, and

value clothing products less than those with high clothing involvement (Zaichkowsky,

1986). As such, those with low clothing involvement are likely not the hard-core clothing

shoppers. The low involved consumers are more likely to attend to and to be persuaded

by peripheral cues such as icons with dynamic animation, large images, and background

Page 301: Osu 1135134346

276

color (Cho, 1999; Petty, et al., 1983; Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). To attract and persuade

online browsers with low clothing involvement, the presence of peripheral cues in

apparel websites may be essential. Therefore, apparel online retailers are recommended

to use peripheral cues such as colorful images and icons to enhance pleasure and arousal

experienced by low involvement online browsers and consequently to persuade them to

stay and browse longer in the websites and to return to the websites to purchase products.

The findings in Study 1 revealed significant effects for emotions on response

behaviors such as purchase intention and approach behaviors. People who experienced

more pleasure and arousal while browsing apparel websites were likely to have greater

purchase intention and approach behaviors (e.g., desire to explore or shop). This

indicates that online browsers who feel pleased, happy, and contented are more likely to

have greater purchase intention and to enjoy exploring and shopping in the websites. In

addition, online browsers more stimulated and aroused by apparel websites tend to have

greater intention to purchase products from the websites in the future and tend to enjoy

shopping in and exploring the websites. In the online apparel shopping context,

browsers’ emotions tend to affect their response behaviors. Hence, it is important for

online apparel retailers to build a pleasant and arousing online browsing environment to

enhance consumers’ purchase intentions and approach behaviors. As suggested before,

using peripheral cues such as background color, colorful icons, and flashing images may

help online retailers develop more pleasant and arousing apparel websites that

accordingly generate more positive consumer response behaviors under the low

involvement condition.

Page 302: Osu 1135134346

277

In addition, Study 1 revealed that emotions mediated the relationship between

peripheral cues and various consumer response behaviors. The effects of environmental

cues on consumers’ response behaviors tended to be indirect via emotions rather than

direct (Eroglu et al., 2003; Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Pleasure and arousal felt by

online browsers may function as mediators between peripheral cues presented in apparel

websites and browsers’ subsequent behaviors. Since emotions induced by various

peripheral cues presented in apparel websites have strong effects on consumer response

behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003), online apparel retailers should consider consumer

emotions that could be stimulated by apparel websites when they select a background

color, icons, and colorful images for their commercial websites.

6.2.2. Managerial Implications for Study 2

Considering both central and peripheral cues the findings from Study 2 provide

valuable information that apparel online retailers can utilize to develop a successful

apparel website using various web cues. Without considering situational involvement,

Study 2 revealed significant effects for peripheral cues on arousal and central cues on

pleasure. Peripheral cues tended to increase consumers’ arousal rather than pleasure,

while central cues tended to enhance consumers’ pleasure rather than arousal. People

exposed to the websites with peripheral cues (background color with a logo pattern, blue

text color, pink brand logo with flashing image, and colorful icons with roll over images)

experienced greater arousal than those exposed to the websites without peripheral cues

(white background without a log pattern, grey text color, static brand logo with black

Page 303: Osu 1135134346

278

color, and text icons). In addition, people who were exposed to the websites with the

high amount of central cues (high amount of verbal information, three mix and match

suggestions, and front, back, side, and detail larger views) exhibited greater pleasure than

those who were exposed to the websites with the medium amount of central cues

(medium amount of verbal information, one mix and match suggestion, and front larger

view only). According to the results, a function of peripheral cues presented in apparel

websites may be to enhance consumers’ arousal while browsing the websites. On the

other hand, central cues may play a significant role in escalating pleasure experienced by

consumers during browsing. The high amount of central cues and the presence of

peripheral cues may enhance the feeling of pleasure and arousal felt by consumers while

browsing apparel websites. People exposed to the website with the high amount of

central cues experienced more pleasure than those exposed to the website with the

medium amount of central cues, while those exposed to peripheral cues tended to

experience more arousal than those exposed to the website without the peripheral cues.

Given these mixed results, online apparel retailers should consider both types of cues to

be important to provide.

Taking involvement into account, the findings of Study 2 revealed a significant

moderating effect for situational involvement on the relationship between web cues and

consumer emotions (pleasure and arousal). According to the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo,

1996), under low situational involvement (i.e., browsing websites without a purchasing

goal) peripheral cues may affect persuasion, while under high situational involvement

(i.e., browsing websites with a purchasing goal) central cues may influence persuasion.

As expected, while browsing apparel websites without a purchasing goal (low situational

Page 304: Osu 1135134346

279

involvement), people exposed to the websites with peripheral cues (background color

with a logo pattern, blue text color, pink brand logo with flashing image, and colorful

icons with roll over images) exhibited greater pleasure and arousal than those exposed to

the websites without peripheral cues (white background without a logo pattern, grey text

color, static brand logo with black color, and text icons) and the results are consistent

with the findings in Study 1. The effects of peripheral cues on emotions were not

significant under the high situational involvement (i.e., browsing websites with a

purchasing goal). These results both support the ELM and indicate the important role of

peripheral cues in persuasion under the low situational involvement. Peripheral cues

presented in apparel websites such as background color, text color, flashing images, and

colorful icons increased pleasure and arousal for online browsers. Online apparel

retailers may desire to use colors for a background or icons and to use flashing images

rather than static images to create more pleasant and arousing apparel websites that in

turn arouse and provide pleasure to online browsers while browsing apparel websites.

Although a purchase may not be made on their first visit, online browsers who experience

greater pleasure or arousal while browsing the apparel website may be more satisfied

with the website (Eroglu et al., 2003) and return to the website later to purchase products

(Fiore et al., 2005). To appeal to frequent online browsers and consequently, to persuade

them to buy products in the future, online retailers may also need to regularly update

websites with various peripheral cues (e.g., background color, images other than

products) that in turn build a positive image for the websites. Innovative site design that

differentiates the apparel retail website from other online stores may help online retailers

Page 305: Osu 1135134346

280

attract consumers and survive in the competitive online market place (“Design Web Sites

to Merchandise and Communicate with Consumers, Experts Say,” 2005).

Supporting the ELM, the findings of Study 2 also revealed that central cues (the

amount of verbal information, larger views from various angles, and mix and match

suggestions) influenced pleasure and arousal for consumers with a particular purchasing

goal (high situational involvement) rather than those without a purchasing goal (low

situational involvement). Since it is impossible to closely inspect apparel products using

the tactile sense in online contexts, consumers planning to buy apparel products from

online retailers may rely on verbal or pictorial cues (central cues) that illustrate an apparel

product (Then & Delong, 1999). Showing clear pictures of the products may be critical

because people are not able to touch the product online (Emerson, 2000). More

descriptive central cues (high amount of verbal information, larger views with various

angles, and complete mix and match suggestions) may help consumers more fully

evaluate apparel products before making an online purchase decision (Then & Delong,

1999). This effect may be stronger for consumers in high situational involvement (e.g.,

purchasing situation) than in low situational involvement (e.g., browsing situation) (Petty

& Cacioppo, 1996). As predicted, the effects of central cues on emotions were

significant under high situational involvement (i.e., browsing websites with a purchasing

goal) but not significant under low situational involvement (i.e., browsing websites

without a purchasing goal). While browsing apparel websites with a purchasing goal

(high situational involvement), people exposed to the websites with the high amount of

central cues (high amount of verbal information, larger views with various angles, and

complete mix and match suggestions) experienced greater pleasure and arousal than those

Page 306: Osu 1135134346

281

exposed to the websites with the medium amount of central cues (medium amount of

verbal information, only front larger view, and one mix and match suggestion). This

underscores the significant role of central cues in persuasion under the high situational

involvement. The high amount of central cues provided in apparel websites may

augment pleasure and arousal felt by online shoppers with a purchasing goal because the

cues describing apparel products may substitute for product examination since the tactile

sense cannot be used. Hence, online apparel retailers may wish to provide a high amount

of central cues rather than the medium amount of central cues to evoke greater pleasure

and arousal experienced by online shoppers while browsing the website with a

purchasing goal. Because online apparel shoppers browsing the websites with a

purchasing goal may make an immediate purchase when they find a product they like,

online retailers should provide a high amount of central cues to aid in product evaluation

and accelerate purchase decisions. To attract potential online purchasers and to facilitate

product purchasing from the websites, online apparel retailers should provide more than a

minimum amount of central cues describing each product in detail. These cues are

unlikely to appeal to low involved consumers, who will be affected by peripheral cues.

Therefore, both central and peripheral cues in apparel websites may play an important

role in persuading consumers under different levels of situational involvement.

As shown in Study 1, the findings in Study 2 also revealed significant effects for

emotions (pleasure and arousal) on consumer response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors). People who experienced greater pleasure and arousal

while browsing apparel websites tended to be satisfied with the website they browsed and

to have greater purchase intention and approach behaviors (e.g., desire to explore or

Page 307: Osu 1135134346

282

shop) than those who experienced less pleasure and arousal. Higher levels of pleasure

and arousal felt by online browsers and shoppers may enhance satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors (Eroglu et al., 2003; Fiore et al., 2005; Menon & Kahn,

2002). Consumers who feel pleased, happy, and contented tended to have greater

satisfaction with the website (e.g., enjoy visiting the website, satisfied with shopping

experience at the website, recommend the website to other people). In addition, those

who experienced greater pleasure and arousal while browsing the website were more

likely to have higher purchase intention and to enjoy exploring and shopping in the

websites. Consumers more stimulated and aroused by apparel websites tended to have

greater satisfaction with the websites, to have greater intention to purchase products from

the websites in the future, and to enjoy shopping in and exploring the websites. In online

apparel shopping contexts, consumers’ emotions are likely to perform important roles

that affect consumers’ response behaviors. Hence, it is important for online apparel

retailers to build pleasant and arousing apparel websites to increase consumers’

satisfaction, purchase intentions, and approach behaviors. Various web cues may help

online apparel retailers develop more pleasant and arousing apparel websites that in turn

generate more positive consumer response behaviors. As previously suggested, both

central and peripheral cues in apparel websites may play significant roles in increasing

pleasure and arousal felt by consumers as a function of level of involvement that

accordingly affects consumer response behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intentions,

and approach behaviors.

Consistent with Study 1, the findings of Study 2 strongly supported the S-O-R

paradigm predicting the emotions as mediators in the relationships between various types

Page 308: Osu 1135134346

283

of web cues and consumer response behaviors (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974). Pleasure

and arousal induced by central and peripheral cues presented in apparel websites tended

to influence consumers’ subsequent behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intention,

and approach behaviors. In other words, the effects of central and peripheral cues on

consumers’ response behaviors were more likely to be indirect through their emotions

rather than direct. As supported before, central and peripheral cues have strong effects on

consumer emotions that in turn influence consumer response behaviors. The greater

pleasure and arousal experienced by consumers while browsing the websites, the higher

the satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach behaviors that were exhibited. Pleasure

and arousal induced by apparel websites may enhance consumer satisfaction with the

websites, intention to purchase products from the websites, and the tendency to explore

or shop in the websites.

Overall, Study 2 provides valuable information to online apparel retailers in

developing new strategies for visual merchandising of their websites to attract both online

browsers (without a purchasing goal) and shoppers (with a purchasing goal). Peripheral

cues may be useful for online apparel retailers to create the websites that catch the

attention of new customers or online browsers. To attract online browsers without

intention to purchase, online apparel retailers may need to differentiate their websites

from other online retailers in terms of appearance. More colorful icons and background

and flashing images may influence online browsers’ pleasure and arousal that in turn

affect their subsequent behaviors such as satisfaction, purchase intention, and approach

behaviors. On the other hand, central cues may be helpful to build the websites that

influence consumers with a purchasing goal rather than those without a purchasing goal.

Page 309: Osu 1135134346

284

Since they cannot try on apparel items online, consumers planning to buy an apparel

product via the Internet may be attracted to more detailed verbal information and product

pictures (i.e., cues directly related to their goal) rather than colorful images or icons not

directly related to their goal (i.e., peripheral cues). Providing informative merchandise

displays may help consumers decide what to purchase and build great trust toward the

websites (“Design Web Sites to Merchandise and Communicate with Consumers, Experts

Say,” 2005). Product pages with more central cues (product related web cues) such as

product related verbal information, mix and match suggestions, color swatches, product

views in different angles, and a virtual model may increase apparel online sales. Fit

information and mix and match suggestions could make shoppers more satisfied and

consequently reduce returns (“Retail Web Sites Need to Improve Usability,” 2004).

Therefore, highly involved potential online purchasers may experience great pleasure and

arousal when they are exposed to the website with extensive product related web cues

and accordingly more pleased or aroused people may make an instant purchase when

they found a product they like.

Although this study found a significant role for peripheral cues in persuading

online apparel browsers, in a real situation as more colorful and/or moving images are

added to the website, downloading time gets longer. Particularly, a background image or

color needs a longer time to download than a white background and consequently it may

have negative effects on consumer emotions and response behaviors. Thus, considering

downloading time it is very important for online retailers to balance the amount of

peripheral cues or the file size of every image in each page when designing their websites.

As found in this study, online browsers without a purchasing goal tend to be affected by

Page 310: Osu 1135134346

285

peripheral cues while online shoppers with a purchasing goal are more likely to be

influenced by central cues (product related information). Online retailers may need to

use more peripheral cues in the homepage (i.e., the opening page of a website) because it

is the first page of the website that gains more attention from online browsers. Whereas

online retailers may wish to use fewer peripheral cues in the product page (i.e., the page

in which each product is presented) because central cues, that attract online shoppers

rather than online browsers, are more important for the product page. For example, the

opening page of apparel websites may contain a colorful background with a brand logo, a

flashing brand logo, and colorful icons with a roll-over image while to reduce

downloading time the product page may include a flashing brand logo and colorful icons

with a white background rather than a colorful background image.

6.2.3. Theoretical Implications

One of the significant limitations in online visual merchandising research is the

lack of theoretical approach to explain diverse consumer reactions to various web cues

presented in apparel websites. Besides, although the effects of website visual

merchandising have gained attention from previous researchers (Allen, 1999; Fiore et al.,

2005; Menon & Kahn, 2002; Szymanski & Hise, 2000; Then & Delong, 1999), it is

surprising that so little empirical research has been conducted to examine consumer

responses to visual merchandising in apparel websites. Applying the ELM (Petty &

Cacioppo, 1996) and the S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell, 1974) in Internet

shopping context, Eroglu et al., (2003) empirically tested the effects for peripheral cues

Page 311: Osu 1135134346

286

on consumer emotions that in turn influence consumer response behaviors and found that

peripheral cues (cues not directly related to a purchasing goal) presented in apparel

websites influence pleasure and arousal that in turn affect consumer satisfaction and

approach behaviors under low situational involvement (browsing situation). However,

the effects of central cues (cues directly related to a purchasing goal) available on apparel

websites on consumers’ emotions and behavioral responses have not been investigated.

Therefore, to empirically support the applicability of two theoretical approaches in online

visual merchandising research, the current study investigated the effects for both central

cues and peripheral cues on consumer emotions and also examined how the effects are

changed by the levels of situational or product involvement.

Blending the S-O-R paradigm and the ELM, Study 1 and Study 2 empirically

tested the effects for diverse web cues on consumer emotions and behaviors and

examined the moderating effects of different involvement conditions (product

involvement in Study 1 and situational involvement in Study 2). The findings of both

studies contribute to the theoretical development in studying the role of visual

merchandising in apparel websites. Two studies strongly support the applicability of the

S-O-R paradigm and the ELM together in understanding consumer responses to online

visual merchandising under different involvement conditions.

Supporting the S-O-R paradigm, Study 1 and Study 2 provide a comprehensive

model that describes the relationship among various web cues (peripheral and central

cues) presented on apparel websites, emotional states (pleasure and arousal) experienced

while browsing the websites, and consumers’ response behaviors (satisfaction, purchase

intention, and approach behaviors). The model also sustains the ELM describing how

Page 312: Osu 1135134346

287

consumers in different situations react to various web cues and how product involvement

influences the relationship between peripheral cues and consumer emotions.

The findings of Study 1 support the S-O-R paradigm (Mehrabian & Russell,

1974) describing the effects of atmospheric cues (colorful icons, background image, and

flashing brand logo) presented in apparel websites on consumers’ emotions that in turn

affect their subsequent behaviors such as purchase intention and approach behaviors

while browsing the websites without a purchasing goal (low situational involvement). In

addition, significant moderating effects for clothing product involvement on the

relationship between peripheral cues and consumer pleasure and arousal strongly support

the ELM (Petty & Cacioppo, 1996). In sum, blending the S-O-R paradigm and the ELM,

Study 1 provides valuable theoretical insight in understanding how peripheral cues (site

atmospheric cues) influence online apparel browsers’ emotions and subsequent behaviors

and how the effects for peripheral cues on emotions differ by the levels of clothing

product involvement.

Taking both central and peripheral cues into consideration the findings in Study 2

suggest a comprehensive model supporting the S-O-R paradigm and the ELM. However,

Part 1 in Study 2 revealed that without considering situational involvement the effects for

central and peripheral cues on pleasure and arousal were only partially supported. This

demonstrates that the S-O-R paradigm alone may not provide useful theoretical insight in

understanding consumer responses to various web cues regarding online visual

merchandising. Considering situational involvement Study 2 revealed the significant

effects for peripheral cues on consumer emotions only under low situational involvement

(browsing without a purchasing goal) and the significant effects for central cues on

Page 313: Osu 1135134346

288

consumer emotions only under high situational involvement (browsing with a purchasing

goal). According to the results, the ELM rather than the S-O-R paradigm more clearly

explains the relationship between various web cues (visual merchandising in apparel

websites) and consumer emotions. Supporting the S-O-R paradigm Study 2 also found

that the effects for various web cues on consumer response behaviors were indirect via

consumer emotions rather than direct. Overall, consistent with Study 1, Study 2 confirms

the applicability of blending the two theoretical approaches, the S-O-R paradigm and the

ELM, in studying the relationship between online visual merchandising and consumer

responses with respect to the effects of different situational involvement.

6.3. Limitations

Although this study provides valuable information regarding visual

merchandising in online apparel stores, there are several limitations recognized in the

current study: 1) the study used a homogenous sample, 2) situational involvement was

manipulated, and 3) product category was limited to one product category, apparel.

6.3.1. Homogeneity of the Sample

The sample used in the study was relatively homogeneous in terms of age and

gender. Since the mock websites used in Study 1 and Study 2 were designed to target

young female consumers, all participants in the study were female and the majority of

participants were aged between 18 and 23. Therefore, the implications of Study 1 and

Page 314: Osu 1135134346

289

Study 2 may be only applicable to online apparel retailers who target young female

consumers. In addition, the ethnicity of the sample population in the study was not

evenly distributed. Since participants were mostly Caucasian American, the results may

not hold for different ethnic groups. In sum, the findings of the study may not be

generalizable to different age, gender, or other demographic consumer groups.

6.3.2. Manipulated Situational Involvement

Although a large amount of effort was made to simulate a real time online apparel

browsing or purchasing situation in the experiment, several factors related to the

experimental procedure may reduce the reality of the study. First, participants may not

have perceived the experimental task as outlined in the scenario as real browsing or

purchasing situations. Particularly under high situational involvement because

participants are not really making a purchase online, they may feel less involved in

comparison to a real purchasing situation. In addition, several disabled functions (e.g.,

customer service, shipping information) and limited product assortments in the mock

website may reduce the realism of online shopping.

6.3.3. Limited Product Category

This study was necessarily limited in the use of only one product category,

women’s apparel, to examine the effects of various web cues presented in apparel online

stores on consumer emotions and response behaviors while browsing or shopping in the

Page 315: Osu 1135134346

290

apparel websites. Web cues appropriate for one product category may not be appropriate

for other product categories. For example, a color swatch provides key information

helping consumers make a purchase decision particularly for pricier apparel items while

it is a low priority for electronics or sporting goods (“With Site Design, Product Category

Should Guide Display Tool Selection,” 2002). Therefore, the findings of the study

should be interpreted with caution when it is applied to different product categories

available online retail stores.

6.4. Recommendations for Future Studies

Since the sample used in the study was relatively homogeneous in terms of age

and gender (i.e., young female students), examining whether the findings of the current

study are generalizable to other relevant populations could be an important next step.

Thus, future studies need to investigate how diverse consumer groups react to various

web cues available in apparel websites. Consumers with different demographic

characteristics in terms of age, gender, ethnicity, income, and education may exhibit

different responses to online visual merchandising in apparel websites.

Additional research needs to further establish the generalizability of the findings

of the study across products. Effective online visual merchandising techniques may vary

across different product categories. A virtual model can be an important tool in apparel

online stores while video can be an important cue for electronics (“With Site Design,

Product Category Should Guide Display Tool Selection,” 2002). Therefore, future

research should be conducted to find web cues which play an important role in online

Page 316: Osu 1135134346

291

stores for different product categories. Further studies also need to investigate how those

web cues influence consumer emotions and response behaviors.

To increase the reality of the study future researchers need to more realistically

manipulate situational involvement. In the current study, a scenario was provided to

manipulate high situational involvement and participants did not make actual purchases.

Thus, future studies could use a valid gift certificate to allow participants to make real

purchases rather than pretend to do it.

Finally, future studies could extend the current study by reexamining the effects

for visual merchandising cues on consumer emotions and response behaviors across

different types of shopping contexts (e.g., in-store shopping, catalog shopping, television

shopping, and online shopping contexts). Available visual merchandising cues may vary

across diverse shopping contexts. While this study focused on visual merchandising in

apparel websites, future research may apply similar logic to diverse shopping contexts.

Future studies need to investigate how visual merchandising in various shopping contexts

can be used effectively in enhancing consumer pleasure and arousal that in turn influence

consumer response behaviors.

Page 317: Osu 1135134346

292

LIST OF REFERENCES Aladwani, A. M., & Palvia, P. C. (2002). Developing and validating an instrument for

measuring user-perceived web quality. Information & Management, 39, 467-476. Allen, C. (1999). See it in 3D. Retrieved November 18, 2002, from

www.clickz.com/mkt/precis_mkt/article.php/816841 Allen, C. (2000). Effective online merchandising techniques. Retrieved November 18,

2002, from www.clickz.com/mkt/precis_mkt/article.php/832981

A Nation Online: Entering the Broadband Age. (2004). U.S. Department of Commerce. Retrieved from http://www.ntia.doc.gov/reports/anol/index.html

Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1982). Some methods for respecifying measurement

models to obtain unidimensional construct measurement. Journal of Marketing Research, 19, 453-460.

Anderson, J., & Gerbing, D. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review

and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bullutin, 103 (3), 411-423. Anderson, J., Gerbing, D., & Hunter, J. E. (1987). On the assessment of unidimensional

measurement: Internal and external consistency, and overall consistency criteria. Journal of Marketing Research, 26, 432-437.

Apparel gains ground in online sales. (2004, January). DSNretailing Today, 20.

Babin, B. J., & Babin, L. (2001). Seeking something different? A model of schema

typicality, consumer affect, purchase intentions and perceived shopping value. Journal of Business Research, 54, 89-96.

Babin, B. J., Hardesty, D. M., & Suter, T. A. (2003). Color and shopping intentions: The

interviewing effect of price fairness and perceived affect. Journal of Business Research, 56 (7), 541-552.

Page 318: Osu 1135134346

293

Baggozi, R. P. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: A comment. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 375-381.

Bagozzi, R. P., & Phillips, L. W. (1982). Representing and testing organizational

theories: A holistic construal. Administrative Science Quarterly, 27, 459-489. Bagozzi, R. P., & Phillips, L. W. (1991). Assessing construct validity in organizational

research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 421-458. Bagozzi, R. P., & Yi, Y. (1991). Multitrait-multimethod matrices in consumer research.

Journal of Consumer Research, 17, 429-439. Baggozi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. (1991). Assessing construct validity in

organizational research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 421-458. Baggozi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Nassen, K. D. (1999). Representation of measurement error in

marketing variables: Review of approaches and extension to three-facet designs. Journal of Econometrics 89, 393-421.

Baker, J., Levy, M., & Grewal, D. (1992). An experimental approach to making retail

store environmental decisions. Journal of Retailing, 68, 445-460.

Baker, J., Parasuraman, A., Grewal, D., & Voss, G. B. (2002). The influence of multiple store environment cues on perceived merchandise value and patronage intentions. Journal of Marketing, 65, 120-141.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (6), 1173-1182.

Bellizzi, J. A. & Hite, R. E. (1992). Environmental color, consumer feelings, and

purchase likelihood. Psychology & Marketing, 9, 347-363. Beliizzi, J. A., Crowley, A. E., & Hasty, R. W. (1983). The effects of color in store

design. Journal of retailing, 59, 21-45. Bentler, P., & Bonett, D. (1980). Significance test and goodness of fit in the analysis of

covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. Berthon, P., Pitt, L. F., & Watson, R. T. (1996). The world wide web as an advertising

medium: Toward an understanding of conversion efficiency. Journal of Advertising Research, 36 (1), 43-53.

Page 319: Osu 1135134346

294

Bhatnagar, A., Misra, S., & Rao, H. R. (2000). On risk, convenience, and internet shopping behavior. Communications of the ACM, 43 (11), 98-105.

Bitner, M. (1992). Servicescapes: The impact of physical surroundings on customers and

employees. Journal of Marketing, 56, 57-71.

Bitner, M. J., & Obermiller, C. (1985). The elaboration likelihood model: Limitation and extensions in marketing. Advances in Consumer Research, 12 (1), 420-425.

Boker, S. M., & McArdle, J. J. (2005). Path analysis and path diagrams. In B. S. Everitt

& D. C. Howell (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science (Vol. 3, pp.1529-1531). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley. Brown, M., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological

Methods and Research, 21 (2), 230-258. Buckley, P. G. (1991). An S-O-R model of the purchase of an item in a store. Advances

in Consumer Research, 18, 491-500.

Burns, E (2005a). Web usage grows internationally. Retrieved October 1, 2005, from www.clickz.com

Burns, E (2005b). Online retail growth robust. Retrieved September 14, 2005, from

www.clickz.com Cahan, L., & Robinson, J. (1984). A practical guide to visual merchandising. Linda,

Canada: John Wiley & Sons, Inc Campbell, D., & Fiske, D. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the

multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81-105. Chatterjee, A. K. (2001). 10 elements of successful web-site. Retrieved October 7, 2003,

from www.infobanc.com/articles/faida_11.htm Cho, C. (1999). How advertising works on the WWW: Modified elaboration likelihood

model. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 21 (1), 33-50. Cho, C. (2003). Factors influencing clicking of banner ads on the WWW.

CyberPsychology & Behavior, 6 (2), 201-215. Churchill, G. (1979). A paradigm for developing better measures of marketing constructs.

Journal of Marketing Research, 16 (1), 64-73.

Page 320: Osu 1135134346

295

Citrin, A. V., Stem, D. E., Sangenberg, E. R., & Clark, M. J. (2003). Consumer need for tactile input: An Internet retailing challenge. Journal of Business Research, 56, 915-922.

Clark, E. (2003). Spring’s in the air at Ann Taylor. WWD: Women’s Wear Daily, 186

(86), 21. Colborne, R. (1996). Visual merchandising: The business of merchandise presentation.

Albany, NY: Delmar Publishers. Crowley, A. E. (1993). The two dimensional impact of color on shopping. Marketing

Letters, 4, 59-69. Curran, P., West, S., & Finch, J. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality

and specification errors in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 1, 16-29.

Design Web Sites to Merchandise and Communicate with Consumers, Experts Say.

(2005). Retrieved November 11, 2005, from http://www.internetretailer.com/printArticle.asp?id=15195

Diamond, J., & Diamond, E. (2003). Contemporary visual merchandising environmental

design (3rd Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Donovan, R. J., & Rossiter, J. R. (1982). Store atmosphere: An environmental

psychology approach. Journal of Retailing, 58 (1), 34-57.

Donovan, R. J., Rossiter, J. R., Marcoolyn, G., & Nesdale, A. (1994). Store atmosphere and purchasing behavior. Journal of Retailing, 70 (3), 284-294.

Dotson, M. J., & Hyatt, E. M. (2000). Religious symbols as peripheral cues in advertising. Journal of Business Research, 48, 63-68.

Emerson, D. (2000). The mood behind the merchandise. Retrieved August 10, 2005,

from http://www.internetretailer.com/printArticle.asp?id=2805 Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K. A., & Davis, L. M. (2001). Atmospheric qualities of online

retailing: A conceptual model and implications. Journal of Business Research, 54, 177-184.

Eroglu, S. A., Machleit, K. A., & Davis, L. M. (2003). Empirical testing of a model of online store atmospherics and shopper responses. Psychology & Marketing, 20 (2), 139-150.

Page 321: Osu 1135134346

296

Fiore, A. M., Jin, H., & Kim, J. (2005). For fun and profit: Hedonic value from image interactivity and responses toward an online store. Psychology & Marketing, 22 (8), 669-694.

Fiore, A. M., & Kimle, P. A. (1997). Understanding aesthetics for the merchandising

and design professional. New York: Fairchild Publications. Fiore, A. M., & Yu, H. (2001). Effects of imagery copy and product samples on

responses toward the product. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 15 (2), 36-46. Fornell, C. (1983). Issues in the application of covariance structure analysis: A comment.

Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 443-448. Fronell, C., & Larker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with

unobservable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39-50.

Garlin, F. V., & McGiggan, R. L. (2002). Sex, spices and celluloid: Movie content

preference, choice, and involvement. Psychology & Marketing, 19 (5), 427-445. Gerbing, D., & Anderson, J. (1984). On the meaning of within-factor correlated

measurement errors. Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 572-580. Gerbing, D., & Anderson, J. (1988). An updated paradigm for scale development

incorporating unidimensionality and its assessment. Journal of Marketing Research, 25, 186-192.

Goldsmith, R. E., & Goldsmith, E. B. (2002). Buying apparel over the Internet. Journal

of Product & Brand Management, 11 (2), 89-102.

Greenspan, R. (2002, July). E-shopping around the world. Retrieved December 9, 2003, from www.cyberatlas.internet.com Greenspan, R. (2003, November). E-tailers will see green. Retrieved December 22,

2004, from www.clickz.com Greenspan, R. (2004, June). Google gains overall, competition builds niches. Retrieved

August 8, 2005, from www.clickz.com Grefen, D. (2003). Assessing unidimensionality through Lisrel: An explanation and

example. Communications of the Association for Information Systems, 12, 23-47. Ha, Y., & Stoel, L. (2004). Internet apparel shopping behaviors: The influence of general

innovativeness. International Journal of Retailing and Distribution Management, 32 (8), 377-385.

Page 322: Osu 1135134346

297

Harris, K. (1998). Merchandising tips for the Web. SGB: Sporting Goods Business, 31 (17), 12-13.

Hightower, R., Brady, M. K., & Baker, T. L. (2002). Investigating the role of the

physical environment in hedonic service consumption: An exploratory study of sporting events. Journal of Business Research, 55, 697-707.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In R.H.Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure

analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1), 1-55.

Huang, M. (2003). Modeling virtual exploratory and shopping dynamics: an

environmental psychology approach. Information & Management, 41, 39-47. Hui, M., Dube, L., & Chebat, J. (1997). The impact of music on consumers’ reactions to

waiting for services. Journal of Retailing, 73, 87-104. Joines, J. L., Scherer, C. W., & Scheufele, D. A. (2002). Exploring motivatios for

consumer Web use and their implications for e-commerce. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 20 (2), 90-108.

Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). Lisrel 8: Structural equation modeling with the

SIMPLIS command language. Chicago: SPSS Inc. Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (2004). Lisrel 8.7 for Windows [Computer Software].

Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. Karson, E. J., & Korgaonkar, P. K. (2001). An experimental investigation of Internet

advertising and the elaboration likelihood model. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 23 (2), 53-72.

Keppel, G. (1991). Design and analysis: A researcher’s handbook (3rd ed). Upper

Saddle River, NJ:Prentice-Hall, Inc. Kerfoot, S., Davis, B., & Ward, P. (2003). Visual merchandising and the creation of

discernible retail brands. International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 31 (3), 143-152.

Kim, K. H. (2005). The relation among fit indexes, power, and sample size in structural

equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 12 (3), 368-390.

Page 323: Osu 1135134346

298

Kim, H., Damhorst, M. L., & Lee, K. (2002). Apparel involvement and advertisement processing: A model. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 6 (3), 277-302.

Kim, M., & Lennon, S. J. (2000). Television shopping for apparel in the United States:

Effects of perceived amount of information on perceived risks and purchase intention. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 28 (3), 301-330.

Kinley, T. L., Conrad, C. A., & Brown, G. (1999). Internal and external promotional

references: An examination of gender and product involvement effects in the retail apparel setting. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 6, 39-44.

Kokkinak, F., & Lunt, P. (1999). The effect of advertising message involvement on

brand attitude accessibility. Journal of Economic Psychology, 20, 41-51.

Kolbe, R. H., & Burnett, M. S. (1991). Content-analysis research: An examination of applications with directives for improving research reliability and objectivity. Journal of Consumer Research, 18, 243-250

Koufteros, X. A. (1999). Testing a model of pull production: A paradigm for

manufacturing research using structural equation modeling. Journal of Operations Management 17, 467-488.

Kwon, Y. H., Paek, S. L., & Arzeni, M. (1991). Catalog vs. non-catalog shppers of

apparel: Perceived risks, shopping orientations, demographics, and motivations. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 10 (1), 13-19.

Lam, S. Y., & Mukherjee, A. (2005). The effects of merchandise coordination and

juxtaposition on consumers’ product evaluation and purchase intention in store-based retailing. Journal of Retailing, 81 (3), 231-250.

Lea-Greenwood, G. (1998). Visual merchandising: A neglected area in UK fashion marketing? International Journal of Retail & Distribution Management, 26 (8), 324-329.

Lee, M., & Johnson, K. (2002). Exploring differences between Internet apparel

purchasers, browsers and non-purchasers. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 6 (2), 146-157.

Lusch, R., & Brown, J. (1996). Interdependency, contracting, and relational behavior in

marketing channels. Journal of Marketing, 60 (4), 19-38. MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and

determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1 (2), 130-149.

Page 324: Osu 1135134346

299

MacCallum, R. C., & Hong, S. (1997). Power analysis in covariance structure modeling using GFI and AGFI. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32 (2), 193-210.

Machleit, K. A., & Mantel, S. P. (2001). Emotional response and shopping satisfaction:

Moderating effects of shopper attributions. Journal of Business Research, 54, 97-106.

Macias, W. (2003). A beginning look at the effects of interactivity, product involvement

and web experience on comprehension: Brand web sites as interactive advertising. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 25 (2), 31-44.

Mano (1997). Affect and persuasion: The influence of pleasantness and arousal on

attitude formation and message elaboration. Psychology & Marketing, 14 (4), 315-335.

Maoz, e., & Tybout, A. M. (2002). The moderating role of involvement and differentiation in the evaluation of brand extensions. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12 (2), 119-131.

Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to environmental psychology.

Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

Menon, S., & Kahn, B. (2002). Cross-category effects of induced arousal and pleasure on the Internet shopping experience. Journal of Retailing, 78, 31-40.

Mittal, B. (1989). Must consumer involvement always imply more information search?

Advances in Consumer Research, 16, 167-172. Nitse, P. S., Parker, K. R., Krumwiede, D., & Ottaway, T. (2004). The impact of color in

the e-commerce marketing of fashions: an exploratory study. European Journal of Marketing, 38 (7), 898-915.

Park, J. W., & Hastak, M. (1995). Effects of involvement on On-line brand evaluations:

A stronger test of the elaboration likelihood model. Advances in Consumer Research, 22, 435-439.

Park, J. H., & Stoel, L. (2002). Apparel shopping on the Internet: Information availability on US apparel merchant Web sites. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management, 6 (2), 158-176.

Park, J. H., Lennon, S., & Stoel, L. (2005). On-line product presentation: Effects on

mood, perceived risk, and purchase intention. Psychology & Marketing, 22 (9), 695-719.

Page 325: Osu 1135134346

300

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). Source factors and the elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Consumer Research, 11 (1), 668-672.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Attitudes and persuasion: classic and

contemporary approaches (2nd Ed). Colorado: Westview Press. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to

advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 135-146.

Phau, I., & Lum, L. (2000). Effect of “physical attractiveness” in the evaluation of print

advertisements. Asia Pacific Journal of Marketing and Logistics,12 (4), 41-59. Pink Chic. (2005). WWD: Women’s Wear Daily, 189 (2), 6-7. Population Explosion. (2005). Retrieved October 4, 2005, from www.clickz.com Prior, M. (2005). Deborah fine’s blueprint for limited brands position. WWD: Women’s

Wear Daily, 189 (14), 9. Retail Web Sites Need to Improve Usability. (2004). Retrieved November 11, 2005,

from http://ww.internetretailer.com/printArticle.asp?id=11319 Rice, M. (1997). What makes users revisit a Web site? Marketing News, 31 (6), 12.

Rowley, J. (2002). E-business: principles & practice. Chippenham, Wiltshire: Palgrave. Rush, L. (2004). Women, comparison shopping help boost E-commerce holiday revenues.

Retrieved December 22, 2004, from www.clickz.com Segars, A. H. (1997). Assessing the unidimentionality of measurement: A paradigm and

illustration within the context of information systems research. Omega, 25 (1), 107-121.

Segars, A. H., & Grover, V. (1993). Re-examining ease of use and usefulness: A

confirmatory factor analysis. MIS Quarterly, 17 (4), 517-525. Sen, S., Block, L. G., & Chandran, S. (2002). Window displays and consumer shopping

decisions. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 9, 277-290. Shao, C. Y., Baker, J., & Wagner, J. A. (2004). The effects of appropriateness of service

contact personnel dress on customer expectations of service quality and purchase intention: The moderating influences of involvement and gender. Journal of Business Research, 57, 1164-1176.

Page 326: Osu 1135134346

301

Sherman, E., Mathur, A., & Smith, R. B. (1997). Store environment and consumer purchase behavior: Mediating role of consumer emotions. Psychology & Marketing, 14 (4), 361-378.

Singh, S. N., & Dalal, N. P. (1999). Web pages as advertisements. Communications of

the ACM, 42(8), 91-98. Spies, K., Hesse, F., & Loesch, K. (1997). Store atmospheres, mood and purchasing

behavior. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 14, 1-17. Stafford, M. R., & Stern, B. (2002). Consumer bidding behavior on Internet auction sites.

International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 7 (1), 135-150. Statistics: US online shoppers. (2002). Retrieved April 4, 2004 from

www.shop.org/learn/stats_usshop_youngadults.html Sun, H., & Zhang, P. (2004, December). An empirical study of the roles of affective

variables in user adoption of search engines. Proceedings of the Third Annual Workshop on HCI Research in MIS, Washington, D.C.

Szymanski, D. M., & Hise, R. T. (2000). E-satisfaction: An initial examination. Journal

of Retailing, 76 (3), 309-322.

Then, N. K., & Delong, M. R. (1999). Apparel shopping on the web. Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences, 91 (3), 65-68.

Tyreman, D., & Walton, K. (1998). Visual merchandising ups sales. Discount Store News, 37 (13), 19.

Vaughn, R. (1980). How advertising works: A planning model. Journal of Advertising

Research, 20 (5), 27-33.

Vaughn, R. (1986). How advertising works: A planning model revisited. Journal of Advertising Research, 16 (1), 57-66.

Walters, D., & White, D. (1987). Retail marketing management. London: Macmillan Press.

Ward, M. R., & Lee, M. J. (2000). Internet shopping, consumer search and product

branding. Journal of Product and Brand Management, 9 (1), 6-20. Wells, W. D., & Prensk, D. (1996). Consumer behavior. New York: John Wiley & Sons,

Inc.

Page 327: Osu 1135134346

302

With Site Design, Product Category Should Guide Display Tool Selection. (2002). Retrieved November 11, 2005 from http://www.internetretailer.com/printArticle.asp?id=7619

Yoh, E., & Damhorst, M. L. (2003). Consumer adoption of the Internet: The case of

apparel shopping. Psychology & Marketing, 20 (12), 1095-1118. Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1985). Measuring the involvement construct. Journal of Consumer

Research, 12, 341-352. Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1986). Conceptualizing involvement. Journal of Advertising, 15 (2),

4-14.

Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1994). The personal involvement inventory: Reduction, revision, and application to advertising. Journal of Advertising, 23 (4), 59-70.

Zhang, Q., Lim, J., & Cao, M. (2004). Learning and knowledge creation in product

development: A lisrel analysis. International Journal of Product Development, 1 (1), 107-129.

Page 328: Osu 1135134346

303

APPENDIX A

PILOT STUDY 1

Page 329: Osu 1135134346

304

(1) Pilot Study 1: Recruitment Letter Dear Participants,

I’m a Ph. D Candidate of Textiles and Clothing in the department of Consumer and Textile Sciences at the Ohio State University. You are being asked to participate in a study of Internet apparel shopping. This study is concerned with group data and not with your individual responses. Therefore, your responses will remain confidential. Your name will not be associated with the data we collect. Please, understand that your participation in this study is entirely voluntary. You may discontinue participation at any time. I realize your time is at a premium. I greatly appreciated your help and you may expect to take 10 to 15 minutes to participate and respond to the questionnaire. Do not write your name anywhere. After you complete the questionnaire, please return it to me. If you have any question, pleases feel free to ask any questions you may have. Thank you very much. Sincerely, Young Ha Ph. D Candidate Dep. Of Consumer and Textile Sciences Ohio State University (614) 688-4234 [email protected]

Dr. Sharron J. Lennon Professor Dep. Of Consumer and Textiles Sciences Ohio State University (614) 292-4384 [email protected]

Page 330: Osu 1135134346

305

(2) Pilot Study 1: Questionnaire for Low Involvement

PART 1. Please, read the following carefully.

Imagine yourself in the following situation. Imagine that you find a clothing website today. Now, you are going to visit one clothing website. Browse and look around the website for a while. ************************************************************************ Here is the website for you to visit: www.asos.com ************************************************************************ Please, do NOT move on to the next page until you finish browsing the website. If you finish browsing the website, then now CLOSE the website that you browsed and move on to the next page.

Page 331: Osu 1135134346

306

Now, make sure that you closed the website that you browsed and please, answer the

following questions. Now, you cannot go back to the website!!!

PART 3. Please fill in the blank.

1. List and describe what you saw on the website.

i. _________________________________

ii. _________________________________

iii. _________________________________

iv. _________________________________

v. _________________________________

vi. _________________________________

vii. _________________________________

viii. _________________________________

ix. _________________________________

x. _________________________________

PART 4. Please fill in the blank or check the response which best answers the questions

that follow.

2. Age __________

3. Ethnic Background

_______ African American

_______ Caucasian American

_______ Hispanic/Hispanic American

_______ Native American

_______ Asian/Asian American

_______ Other

4. How likely were you involved in browsing the websites today?

Not at all Very much so

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 332: Osu 1135134346

307

(3) Pilot Study 1: Questionnaire for High Involvement

PART 1. Please, read the scenario carefully.

Imagine yourself in the following situation.

Now, you are going to visit one apparel website. Imagine that you have been given a $100 gift certificate to purchase apparel products from the website. Remember! After browsing the website, you should be able to identify and describe two items that you would like to purchase from the website. ************************************************************************ Here is the website for you to visit: www.asos.com ************************************************************************ Please, do NOT move on to the next page until you finish browsing the website. If you finish browsing the website, then now CLOSE the website that you browsed and move on to the next page.

Page 333: Osu 1135134346

308

Now, make sure that you closed the website that you browsed and please, answer the

following questions. Now, you cannot go back to the website!!!

PART 2. List and describe two apparel items.

1. __________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________

2. __________________________________________________________________

__________________________________________________________________ PART 3. Please fill in the blank.

3. List and describe what you saw on the websites.

i. _________________________________

ii. _________________________________

iii. _________________________________

iv. _________________________________

v. _________________________________

vi. _________________________________

vii. _________________________________

viii. _________________________________

ix. _________________________________

x. _________________________________

PART 4. Please fill in the blank or check the response which best answers the questions

that follow.

4. Age __________

5. Ethnic Background

_______ African American

_______ Caucasian American

_______ Hispanic/Hispanic American

Page 334: Osu 1135134346

309

_______ Native American

_______ Asian/Asian American

_______ Other

6. How likely were you involved in browsing the websites today?

Not at all Very much so

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Page 335: Osu 1135134346

310

APPENDIX B

PILOT STUDY 2 – WEBSITE

Page 336: Osu 1135134346

311

Page 337: Osu 1135134346

312

Page 338: Osu 1135134346

313

APPENDIX C

PILOT STUDY 2 – APPAREL STIMULI

Page 339: Osu 1135134346

314

Thirty Four Pairs of Pants Rated in the Pilot Study 2

Page 340: Osu 1135134346

315

Page 341: Osu 1135134346

316

Page 342: Osu 1135134346

317

Two Items Selected for Main Study 1

Page 343: Osu 1135134346

318

Five Items Selected for the Main Study 2

Page 344: Osu 1135134346

319

APPENDIX D

PILOT STUDY 3 – WEBSITE AND QUESTIONNAIRE

Page 345: Osu 1135134346

320

Page 346: Osu 1135134346

321

Page 347: Osu 1135134346

322

Page 348: Osu 1135134346

323

APPENDIX E

MAIN STUDY 1 – WEBSITE: MANIPULATIONS FOR PERIPHERAL CUES

Page 349: Osu 1135134346

324

Page 350: Osu 1135134346

325

Main page: Peripheral cue – Absence

Page 351: Osu 1135134346

326

Main page: Peripheral cue – Presence

Page 352: Osu 1135134346

327

Product page: Peripheral cue – Absence

Page 353: Osu 1135134346

328

Product page: Peripheral cue – Presence

Page 354: Osu 1135134346

329

Peripheral Cues – Presence: Flashing Image and Colorful Icons

1. Brand Logo Flashing Image: Heart dot is blinking (blank heart to filled heart)

2. Colorful Icons with Rollover Image: When a mouse is over the icon, the color of the edge is changing.

Page 355: Osu 1135134346

330

APPENDIX F

MAIN STUDY 1 – THE QUESTIONNAIRE

Page 356: Osu 1135134346

331

*WITHOUT GOING BACK TO THE WEBSITE, PLEASE TELL US ABOUT YOUR THOUGHTS BY RESPONDING TO THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS. Part A. We would like to know how you feel after browsing E-Style.com website. Please indicate the number that best describes your current feelings.

Unhappy 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Happy Annoyed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Pleased

Unsatisfied 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Satisfied Melancholic 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Contented

Despairing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Hopeful Bored 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Relaxed

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Stimulated Calm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Excited

Sluggish 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Frenzied Dull 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Jittery

Sleepy 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Wide-awake Unaroused 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Aroused

Part B. Assume that E-Style.com is an active apparel online store. Please indicate the number that best represents your thoughts based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com today.

Unlikely LikelyHow likely is it that you would buy clothing items if you happened to see them from E-Style.com?

1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-Style.com in the next 12 months?

1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will shop for apparel from E-Style.com when you buy apparel in the upcoming year?

1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will buy apparel from E-Style.com when you find something you like?

1 2 3 4 5

Part C. Please check the response which best represent your thoughts based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com.

Unlikely LikelyHow much would you enjoy exploring this site? 1 2 3 4 5 Not

at all Very

much Do you like this site? 1 2 3 4 5 To what extent is this site a good opportunity to shop? 1 2 3 4 5 Would you enjoy shopping in this site? 1 2 3 4 5

Page 357: Osu 1135134346

332

Part D. Please rate the following items based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com today. Clothing is ___________________.

Unimportant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Important

Irrelevant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Relevant Means a lot to me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Means nothing to me

Valuable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Worthless Boring 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Interesting

Unexciting 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Exciting Appealing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Unappealing Mundane 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Fascinating

Not needed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Needed Involving 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Uninvolving

Part E. Please check the response which best answers the questions that follow.

Strongly

disagree Strongly

agreeThe website you browsed today contained very much information.

1 2 3 4 5

From browsing the website, I learned a great deal about the product.

1 2 3 4 5

The website was very informative. 1 2 3 4 5 After browsing the website, I know enough to make an informed purchase decision.

1 2 3 4 5

I can fully trust information given by the website. 1 2 3 4 5 The website looks attractive. 1 2 3 4 5 The website looks organized. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses fonts properly. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses colors properly. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses multimedia features properly. 1 2 3 4 5

Part G. Please fill the blank or check the response which best answers the question that follow.

1. Age ____________

2. Ethnic background

____ African American ____ Caucasian American ____ Native American ____ Hispanic/Hispanic American ____ Asian/Asian American ____ Other

Page 358: Osu 1135134346

333

APPENDIX G

MAIN STUDY 2 – INVITATION EMAIL

Page 359: Osu 1135134346

334

(1) Invitation Email for High Involvement

TITLE: PARTICIPANTS NEEDED FOR OSU RESEARCH

Greetings!

Hello, my name is Young Ha, a doctoral candidate in the Department of Consumer Sciences at the Ohio State University. I am writing this email to ask for your help in participating in an important OSU research.

Among 15000 OSU female students, you have been selected to participate in a research study of online shopping behaviors. The purpose of this research is to investigate various consumer behaviors in online shopping stores. The result of this research will provide better guidelines for online retailers to better serve online shoppers. Your participation in this study is invaluable to this research and a growing scholarship in online shopping.

The study will be done as a Web survey using a mock apparel website. You can participate in the study by logging onto the following URL (http://hec.osu.edu/forms/young/estudy). IF THIS LINK DOES NOT WORK BY JUST CLICKING IT, THEN PLEASE COPY AND PASTE THIS LINK TO YOUR WEB BROWSER. Please carefully read instructions given in each page and follow the instructions. Your task is to browse for five pairs of pants on the website and then, choose one item that you would like to purchase. After selecting one item, you will be asked to complete the questionnaire measuring your perceptions and behaviors regarding online apparel shopping. Your task will be simple and straight-forward, similar to what we all do in apparel shopping situation.

Because this is a Web-based study, you can participate in the study when and where convenient for you. The survey will take approximately 15 to 20 minutes to complete. Upon completion, randomly selected participants will receive the apparel item they choose during the study or a $20 or $40 cash award. Your participation is strictly voluntary, but I would really appreciate it if you can help me out to complete this research.

Please note that your response will be kept confidential. When I receive your response from the Web server, your response is already aggregated with all other responses without any identifying information. In addition, what we need is the aggregate data, not individual responses. So, please be assured that your response will be kept confidential.

I apologize for sending you the email without your permission, but appreciate your time and consideration. Please feel free to email me ([email protected]) if having any questions/concerns/comments. I very much look forward to receiving your completed survey! I deeply appreciate your help with this research.

Page 360: Osu 1135134346

335

Thank you very much!

Best regards,

Young Ha, Doctoral candidate Dr. Sharron Lennon, Professor Dept. of Consumer Sciences Dept. of Consumer Sciences 265 Campbell Hall 230 Campbell Hall 1787 Neil Avenue 1787 Neil Avenue Ohio State University Ohio State University Columbus, OH 43210-1295 Columbus, OH 43210-1295 Tel: 614-688-4234 Tel: 614-292-4384 Email: [email protected] Email: [email protected]

Page 361: Osu 1135134346

336

(2) Invitation Email for Low Involvement

TITLE: PARTICIPANTS NEEDED FOR OSU RESEARCH

Greetings!

Hello, my name is Young Ha, a doctoral candidate in the Department of Consumer Sciences at the Ohio State University. I am writing this email to ask for your help in participating in an important OSU research study.

Among 15000 OSU female students, you have been selected to participate in a research study of online shopping behaviors. The purpose of this research is to investigate various consumer behaviors in online shopping stores. The result of this research will provide better guidelines for online retailers to better serve online shoppers. Your participation in this study is invaluable to this research and a growing scholarship in online shopping.

The study will be done as a Web survey using a mock apparel website. You can participate in the study by logging onto the following URL (http://hec.osu.edu/forms/young/onlinestudy ). IF THIS LINK DOES NOT WORK BY JUST CLICKING IT, THEN PLEASE COPY AND PASTE THIS LINK TO YOUR WEB BROWSER. Please carefully read instructions given in each page and follow the instructions. Your task is to browse for five pairs of pants on the website. After browsing five apparel items, you will be asked to complete the questionnaire measuring your perceptions and behaviors regarding online apparel shopping. Your task will be simple and straight-forward, similar to what we all do in apparel shopping situation.

Because this is a Web-based study, you can participate in the study when and where convenient for you. The survey will take approximately 15 to 20 minutes to complete. Upon completion, randomly selected participants will receive a $20 or $40 cash award. Your participation is strictly voluntary, but I would really appreciate it if you can help me out to complete this research.

Please note that your response will be kept confidential. When I receive your response from the Web server, your response is already aggregated with all other responses without any identifying information. In addition, what we need is the aggregate data, not individual responses. So, please be assured that your response will be kept confidential.

I apologize for sending you the email without your permission, but appreciate your time and consideration. Please feel free to email me ([email protected]) if having any questions/concerns/comments. I very much look forward to receiving your completed survey! I deeply appreciate your help with this research.

Thank you very much!

Page 362: Osu 1135134346

337

Best regards,

Young Ha, Doctoral candidate Dr. Sharron Lennon, Professor Dept. of Consumer Sciences Dept. of Consumer Sciences 265 Campbell Hall 230 Campbell Hall 1787 Neil Avenue 1787 Neil Avenue Ohio State University Ohio State University Columbus, OH 43210-1295 Columbus, OH 43210-1295 Tel: 614-688-4234 Tel: 614-292-4384 Email: [email protected] Email: [email protected]

Page 363: Osu 1135134346

338

APPENDIX H

MAIN STUDY 2 – INSTRUCTION PAGE

Page 364: Osu 1135134346

339

Instruction Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence

Page 365: Osu 1135134346

340

Instruction Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence

Page 366: Osu 1135134346

341

APPENDIX I

MAIN STUDY 2 – SCENARIO PAGE

(MANIPULATIONS FOR SITUATION INVOLVEMENT)

Page 367: Osu 1135134346

342

Scenario Page: Peripheral Cue – Absence, Involvement – Low

Page 368: Osu 1135134346

343

Scenario Page: Peripheral Cue – Presence, Involvement – Low

Page 369: Osu 1135134346

344

Scenario Page: Peripheral Cue – Absence, Involvement – High

Page 370: Osu 1135134346

345

Scenario Page: Peripheral Cue – Presence, Involvement – High

Page 371: Osu 1135134346

346

APPENDIX J

MAIN STUDY 2 – MAIN PAGE

Page 372: Osu 1135134346

347

Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence

Page 373: Osu 1135134346

348

Main Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence

Page 374: Osu 1135134346

349

APPENDIX K

MAIN STUDY 2 – PRODUCT PAGE

Page 375: Osu 1135134346

350

Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence, Central Cues – Medium Amount

Page 376: Osu 1135134346

351

Product Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence, Central Cues – High Amount

Page 377: Osu 1135134346

352

Product Page: Peripheral Cue – Presence, Central Cue – Medium Amount

Page 378: Osu 1135134346

353

Product Page: Peripheral Cue – Presence, Central Cue – High Amount

Page 379: Osu 1135134346

354

Peripheral Cues – Presence: Flashing Image and Colorful Icons

1. Brand Logo Flashing Image: Heart dot is blinking (blank heart to filled heart)

2. Colorful Icons with Rollover Image: When a mouse is over the icon, the color of the edge is changing.

Page 380: Osu 1135134346

355

Product Page: Size Chart – Peripheral Cues – Absence

Page 381: Osu 1135134346

356

Product Page: Size Chart – Peripheral Cues – Presence

Page 382: Osu 1135134346

357

APPENDIX L

MAIN STUDY 2 – PURCHASE PAGE FOR HIGH INVOLVEMENT

Page 383: Osu 1135134346

358

Purchase Page: Peripheral Cues – Absence

Page 384: Osu 1135134346

359

Purchase Page: Peripheral Cues – Presence

Page 385: Osu 1135134346

360

APPENDIX M

MAIN STUDY 2 – THE QUESTIONNAIRE

Page 386: Osu 1135134346

361

*WITHOUT GOING BACK TO THE WEBSITE, PLEASE TELL US ABOUT YOUR THOUGHTS BY RESPONDING TO THE FOLLOWING QUESTIONS. Part A. We would like to know how you feel after browsing E-Style.com website. Please indicate the number that best describes your current feelings.

Unhappy 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Happy Annoyed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Pleased

Unsatisfied 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Satisfied Melancholic 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Contented

Despairing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Hopeful Bored 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Relaxed

Relaxed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Stimulated Calm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Excited

Sluggish 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Frenzied Dull 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Jittery

Sleepy 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Wide-awake Unaroused 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Aroused

Part B. Please check the response which best answers the questions that follow based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com today. (SD: Strongly Disagree, SA: Strongly Agree)

SD SA

I enjoyed visiting E-Style.com. 1 2 3 4 5 I was satisfied with my shopping experience at E-Style.com. 1 2 3 4 5

Given a choice, I would probably not go back to E-Style.com. 1 2 3 4 5

I would recommend E-Style.com to other people. 1 2 3 4 5 Part C. Assume that E-Style.com is an active apparel online store. Please indicate the number that best represents your thoughts based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com today.

Unlikely LikelyHow likely is it that you would buy clothing items if you happened to see them from E-Style.com? 1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will buy the apparel item from E-Style.com in the next 12 months? 1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will shop for apparel from E-Style.com when you buy apparel in the upcoming year?

1 2 3 4 5

How likely is it that you will buy apparel from E-Style.com when you find something you like? 1 2 3 4 5

Page 387: Osu 1135134346

362

Part D. Please check the response which best represent your thoughts based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com.

Unlikely LikelyHow much would you enjoy exploring this site? 1 2 3 4 5

Not at all

Very much

Do you like this site? 1 2 3 4 5 To what extent is this site a good opportunity to shop? 1 2 3 4 5 Would you enjoy shopping in this site? 1 2 3 4 5

Part E. Please check the response which best answers the questions that follow.

Strongly

disagree Strongly

agreeThe website you browsed today contained very much information. 1 2 3 4 5

From browsing the website, I learned a great deal about the product. 1 2 3 4 5

The website was very informative. 1 2 3 4 5 After browsing the website, I know enough to make an informed purchase decision. 1 2 3 4 5

I can fully trust information given by the website. 1 2 3 4 5 The website looks attractive. 1 2 3 4 5 The website looks organized. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses fonts properly. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses colors properly. 1 2 3 4 5 The website uses multimedia features properly. 1 2 3 4 5

Page 388: Osu 1135134346

363

Part F. Please rate the following items based on your browsing experience with E-Style.com today. Browsing E-Style.com is ___________________.

Unimportant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Important

Irrelevant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Relevant Means a lot to me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Means nothing to me

Valuable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Worthless Boring 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Interesting

Unexciting 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Exciting Appealing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Unappealing Mundane 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Fascinating

Not needed 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Needed Involving 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Uninvolving

Part G. Please fill the blank or check the response which best answers the question that follow.

3. Age ____________

4. Ethnic background

____ African American ____ Caucasian American ____ Native American ____ Hispanic/Hispanic American ____ Asian/Asian American ____ Other

5. What is your academic standing?

____ Freshmen ____ Sophomore ____ Junior ____ Senior ____ Other

6. Please answer the following questions based on your own experience.

Never

Not often

Neutral

Very Often

How often do you use the Internet? 0 1 2 3 4 5 How often do you browse online for information search? 0 1 2 3 4 5

How often do you purchase online? 0 1 2 3 4 5 How often do you browse online for apparel information search? 0 1 2 3 4 5

How often do you purchase apparel online? 0 1 2 3 4 5

Page 389: Osu 1135134346

364

APPENDIX N

STANDARDIZED RESIDUAL EVALUATED IN MAIN STUDY 1

Page 390: Osu 1135134346

365

Page 391: Osu 1135134346

366

APPENDIX O

DATA SCREENING FOR NORMALITY TEST

Page 392: Osu 1135134346

367

Page 393: Osu 1135134346

368

APPENDIX P

STANDARDIZED RESIDUAL EVALUATED IN MAIN STUDY 2

Page 394: Osu 1135134346

369

Page 395: Osu 1135134346

370

APPENDIX Q

DATA SCREENING FOR NORMALITY TEST

Page 396: Osu 1135134346

371

Page 397: Osu 1135134346

372

APPENDIX R

HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL FORM FOR STUDY 1

Page 398: Osu 1135134346

373

Page 399: Osu 1135134346

374

APPENDIX S

HUMAN SUBJECTS APPROVAL FORM FOR STUDY 2

Page 400: Osu 1135134346

375