12
Behavioral Neuroscience 1994, Vol. 108, No. 5, 993-1004 Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0735-7044/94/S3.00 Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color Vision in the Guinea Pig (Cavia porcellus) Gerald H. Jacobs and Jess F, Deegan II Behavioral discrimination tests and electroretinogram (ERG) flicker photometry were used to measure spectral sensitivity and to define the spectral mechanisms of the guinea pig (Cavia porcellus). Results from these 2 approaches converge to indicate that guinea pig retinas contain rods with peak sensitivity of about 494 nm and 2 classes of cone having peak sensitivities of about 429 nm and 529 nm. The presence of 2 classes of cones suggests a retinal basis for a color vision capacity. Behavioral tests of color vision were conducted that verified this prediction: Guinea pigs have dichromatic color vision with a spectral neutral point centered at about 480 nm. The cone pigment complement of the guinea pig is different from that known to characterize other rodents. Cavia porcellus has been so frequently used in scientific and medical investigations that its common name, guinea pig, has entered the English language as an informal descriptor for the subject of any sort of experimentation. In this role as subject, in addition to that as frequent household pet and occasional food source (Bradt, 1988), the guinea pig is surely among the most familiar of the domesticated rodents. Given that fact, it is remarkable how little is known about several basic features of the visual system and vision in the guinea pig. For instance, with regard to the identity of the photoreceptor and photopig- ment types and those visual behaviors most often associated with their variations (e.g., spectral sensitivity and color vision), there is sparse and often contradictory literature. As for most other mammals, the guinea pig has a retina that is rich in rods. The rod photopigment is a typical mammalian rhodopsin with a peak absorption (A. max ) of about 497 nm (Bridges, 1959). There has been considerable inconsistency of opinion about the presence of cones in the guinea pig retina. Granit, who conducted many of the early studies of the electrophysiology of the guinea pig retina, routinely labeled the guinea pig as having a "pure rod-eye" (e.g., Granit, 1944, p. 103). Anatomists have offered variant opinions. O'Day (1947) reported that he was able to confirm the early observations of Kolmer (1936), who had detected cones in the guinea eye and concluded, "To the histologist, the retina of the guinea pig is certainly not "pure rod" (O'Day, 1947, p. 648). In the course of an analysis of the fine structure of the retina by electron microscopy, Sjostrand (reviewed, 1965) conducted a detailed examination of the synaptic terminals of guinea pig photorecep- tors. He differentiated two types of photoreceptors in the guinea pig eye according to features of their synaptic morpholo- gies. Some of these synaptic differences were of the sort that have in other cases been used to differentiate rods and cones Gerald H. Jacobs and Jess F. Deegan II, Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara. This research was supported by Grant EY-02052 from the National Eye Institute. We thank Jason Siegel, Kris Krogh, and John Fenwick. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerald H. Jacobs, Department of Psychology, Universityof California, Santa Barbara, California 93106. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. (e.g., lateral extent of the synaptic surface), but Sjostrand argued these differences alone were not sufficient for determin- ing whether a photoreceptor should be classed as a rod or a cone, and, thus, he too referred to the guinea pig as having a pure-rod retina (Sjostrand, 1965). There are apparently no other contemporary studies of the anatomy of guinea pig photoreceptors. In recent years, however, the presence of different cone types has been demonstrated through the use of various functional markers, including cone-specific, antivisual pigment antibodies. In particular, Szel and Rohlich and their colleagues have used two monoclonal antibodies that are visual-pigment specific to label "blue-sensitive" and "green and red-sensitive cones" in a number of mammalian species. In an article directed to another topic, Szel and Rohlich note that these two classes of antibody also label different populations of receptors in the guinea pig retina, thus providing evidence to suggest that the guinea pig retina not only contains cones but also may contain two different types of such receptors (Szel & Rohlich, 1992). Electrophysiological studies have not clarified the picture. In recordings made from fibers of the optic nerve of the guinea pig, Granit (1947) could find no evidence for a Purkinje shift, a shift that he easily detected in parallel recordings made from cats. This result was in accord with his idea that the guinea pig has a pure-rod eye. However, he did find occasional elements in the optic nerve of the guinea pig that had very narrow spectral sensitivity curves. These elements, termed modulators by Granit, were recorded under conditions of photopic illumi- nation, and their presence led to the speculation that the guinea pig had some photoreceptors that were functionally intermediate between typical rods and cones (Granit, 1947). In a later study, Dodt and Wirth (1953) examined the behavior of a retinal gross potential, the electroretinogram (ERG), under conditions of intermittent light stimulation. Variation in stimu- lation rate and light intensity allowed a determination of the critical fusion frequency (CFF) of the ERG as a function of stimulus intensity. The curve so derived shows clear evidence for two limbs—one limb became asymptotic at stimulus frequen- cies at 10-25 flashes per second and a second limb showed a further increase in fusion frequency up to about 45-50 flashes per second. The appearance of two such limbs in the CFF curve is classically taken as an indication of visual duplicity 993

Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

  • Upload
    leminh

  • View
    217

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

Behavioral Neuroscience1994, Vol. 108, No. 5, 993-1004

Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.0735-7044/94/S3.00

Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color Visionin the Guinea Pig (Cavia porcellus)

Gerald H. Jacobs and Jess F, Deegan II

Behavioral discrimination tests and electroretinogram (ERG) flicker photometry were used tomeasure spectral sensitivity and to define the spectral mechanisms of the guinea pig (Caviaporcellus). Results from these 2 approaches converge to indicate that guinea pig retinas containrods with peak sensitivity of about 494 nm and 2 classes of cone having peak sensitivities of about429 nm and 529 nm. The presence of 2 classes of cones suggests a retinal basis for a color visioncapacity. Behavioral tests of color vision were conducted that verified this prediction: Guinea pigshave dichromatic color vision with a spectral neutral point centered at about 480 nm. The conepigment complement of the guinea pig is different from that known to characterize other rodents.

Cavia porcellus has been so frequently used in scientific andmedical investigations that its common name, guinea pig, hasentered the English language as an informal descriptor for thesubject of any sort of experimentation. In this role as subject, inaddition to that as frequent household pet and occasional foodsource (Bradt, 1988), the guinea pig is surely among the mostfamiliar of the domesticated rodents. Given that fact, it isremarkable how little is known about several basic features ofthe visual system and vision in the guinea pig. For instance,with regard to the identity of the photoreceptor and photopig-ment types and those visual behaviors most often associatedwith their variations (e.g., spectral sensitivity and color vision),there is sparse and often contradictory literature.

As for most other mammals, the guinea pig has a retina thatis rich in rods. The rod photopigment is a typical mammalianrhodopsin with a peak absorption (A.max) of about 497 nm(Bridges, 1959). There has been considerable inconsistency ofopinion about the presence of cones in the guinea pig retina.Granit, who conducted many of the early studies of theelectrophysiology of the guinea pig retina, routinely labeledthe guinea pig as having a "pure rod-eye" (e.g., Granit, 1944, p.103). Anatomists have offered variant opinions. O'Day (1947)reported that he was able to confirm the early observations ofKolmer (1936), who had detected cones in the guinea eye andconcluded, "To the histologist, the retina of the guinea pig iscertainly not "pure rod" (O'Day, 1947, p. 648). In the course ofan analysis of the fine structure of the retina by electronmicroscopy, Sjostrand (reviewed, 1965) conducted a detailedexamination of the synaptic terminals of guinea pig photorecep-tors. He differentiated two types of photoreceptors in theguinea pig eye according to features of their synaptic morpholo-gies. Some of these synaptic differences were of the sort thathave in other cases been used to differentiate rods and cones

Gerald H. Jacobs and Jess F. Deegan II, Department of Psychology,University of California, Santa Barbara.

This research was supported by Grant EY-02052 from the NationalEye Institute. We thank Jason Siegel, Kris Krogh, and John Fenwick.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed toGerald H. Jacobs, Department of Psychology, University of California,Santa Barbara, California 93106. Electronic mail may be sent [email protected].

(e.g., lateral extent of the synaptic surface), but Sjostrandargued these differences alone were not sufficient for determin-ing whether a photoreceptor should be classed as a rod or acone, and, thus, he too referred to the guinea pig as having apure-rod retina (Sjostrand, 1965). There are apparently noother contemporary studies of the anatomy of guinea pigphotoreceptors. In recent years, however, the presence ofdifferent cone types has been demonstrated through the use ofvarious functional markers, including cone-specific, antivisualpigment antibodies. In particular, Szel and Rohlich and theircolleagues have used two monoclonal antibodies that arevisual-pigment specific to label "blue-sensitive" and "greenand red-sensitive cones" in a number of mammalian species. Inan article directed to another topic, Szel and Rohlich note thatthese two classes of antibody also label different populations ofreceptors in the guinea pig retina, thus providing evidence tosuggest that the guinea pig retina not only contains cones butalso may contain two different types of such receptors (Szel &Rohlich, 1992).

Electrophysiological studies have not clarified the picture.In recordings made from fibers of the optic nerve of the guineapig, Granit (1947) could find no evidence for a Purkinje shift, ashift that he easily detected in parallel recordings made fromcats. This result was in accord with his idea that the guinea pighas a pure-rod eye. However, he did find occasional elementsin the optic nerve of the guinea pig that had very narrowspectral sensitivity curves. These elements, termed modulatorsby Granit, were recorded under conditions of photopic illumi-nation, and their presence led to the speculation that theguinea pig had some photoreceptors that were functionallyintermediate between typical rods and cones (Granit, 1947). Ina later study, Dodt and Wirth (1953) examined the behavior ofa retinal gross potential, the electroretinogram (ERG), underconditions of intermittent light stimulation. Variation in stimu-lation rate and light intensity allowed a determination of thecritical fusion frequency (CFF) of the ERG as a function ofstimulus intensity. The curve so derived shows clear evidencefor two limbs—one limb became asymptotic at stimulus frequen-cies at 10-25 flashes per second and a second limb showed afurther increase in fusion frequency up to about 45-50 flashesper second. The appearance of two such limbs in the CFFcurve is classically taken as an indication of visual duplicity

993

Page 2: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II

(Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests thepresence of both rods and cones in the guinea pig. Thiele andMeissl (1987) have recorded light-evoked responses fromsingle units in the pineal stalk and pineal body of the guineapig. The average spectral sensitivity function from such unitsmeasured during light adaptation had a peak appropriate forrod input, at about 500 nm, but it also showed a curioussecondary peak at about 450 nm. The latter could be inter-preted as reflecting the contributions from receptors otherthan rods. That same conclusion may be drawn from an earlierstudy involving the measurement of lights reflected fromguinea pig eyes (Weale, 1955). In that case, the appearance ofseparable bleaching spectra suggested the presence of multipletypes of photopigment in the guinea pig eye.

There are few behavioral studies of vision in guinea pigs. Inseminatural and natural settings, guinea pigs and their caviidrelatives show crepuscular activity rhythms (King, 1956; Rood,1972); whereas in the laboratory, they may display essentiallycontinuous activity under conditions of either constant illumi-nation or constant darkness (Harper, 1976). These observa-tions contain no strong inference about the receptor comple-ment, but they do suggest that the guinea pig eye has a largedynamic intensity range. That range is typically associated withthe joint presence of rods and cones. There appears to be onlya single behavioral study of vision in the guinea pig that bearson the issues of concern here. Miles, Ratoosh, and Meyer(1956) sought to train an operant discrimination where rein-forcement and nonreinforcement for lever-press responseswere signaled by the presence of lights that varied in bright-ness, in color, or in both these dimensions. Although theguinea pigs rather quickly learned a brightness discrimination,they failed to show any discrimination between red and greenlight or between green and blue light. The authors concludedthat guinea pigs lack color vision.

In light of the inconsistencies of results among these variousstudies, we have undertaken an electrophysiological examina-tion of spectral mechanisms in the guinea pig retina and aparallel study of spectral sensitivity and color discrimination inthe guinea pig. Our investigation was impelled by the lack ofconsistent information about basic aspects of vision in animportant mammal and by the belief that there are now bettermethods for studying these issues than were available in thepast, and also because recent studies involving phylogeneticanalyses of amino acid sequence data have raised the intrig-uing possibility that the guinea pig may not be a rodent at all.Rather, it has been argued that the guinea pig may deserve,instead, a separate ordinal status (Graur, Hide, & Li, 1991; Li,Hide, Zharkikh, Ma, & Graur, 1992). If that idea is correct,then comparisons of the spectral mechanisms of the guinea pigwith those of typical rodents could prove useful with respect toideas about the evolution of receptors and photopigments.

Method

Subjects

Adult pignnented guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) were obtained from alocal supplier. Both males and females were examined. The guineapigs were caged separately under standard colony conditions thatincluded a 14:10-hr Jight-dark cycle. The guinea pigs that served as

subjects in visual discrimination tasks were fed daily following testingin an amount sufficient to hold them at a constant body weight. Theremaining guinea pigs were given ad-lib access to food.

Electrophysiological Experiments

Apparatus

The electrophysiological experiments involved an analysis of theflicker ERG in a version we have termed ERG flicker photometry. Thebasic features of the apparatus and the general procedures have beendescribed in earlier publications (Jacobs & Neitz, 1987; Neitz &Jacobs, 1984). Only brief descriptions are given here.

The stimulus lights were produced with a three-channel, Maxwellian-view optical system. Lights from the three channels were opticallycombined to illuminate a 57° circular spot on the retina. The test lightof the flicker photometer constituted one channel; it came from atungsten halide lamp of a high intensity grating monochromator(Bausch & Lomb, Rochester, NY; half-energy passband = 10 nm).The other two channels also originated from tungsten halide lamps.One of these channels provided the test light of the photometer, andthe other channel was used to produce an adaptation light. Theintensity of the test light was varied by adjusting the position of aneutral-density wedge having an attenuation range of 3 log units. Theintensities and spectral characteristics of the reference and adaptationlights were controlled by inserting neutral-density and color filters intothe respective beams. All three lamps were underrun at 11 V from aregulated DC power supply, and each channel contained a high-speedelectromagnetic shutter (Uniblitz, Vincent Associates, Rochester,NY). Light measurements were made with a silicon diode photodetec-tor (PIN 10 DL, United Detector Technology, Culver City, CA).

Electrical signals were recorded with a bipolar, contact-lens elec-trode. A ground electrode was placed against the inside of the guineapig's cheek. In the flicker photometry procedure, ERGs were elicitedby temporally modulated, square-wave lights. Stimuli from the test andreference beams were interleaved such that there was a no-stimulusinterval equal in duration to that of the test and reference lightsbetween each successive light pulse. The frequency of stimulation wasspecified as the total number of light flashes per second. The ERGselicited by test and reference lights were passed through a series offilter stages and electronically compared such that the signals from thetwo sources were cancelled when the two lights were equally effectiveat generating an ERG (Neitz & Jacobs, 1984).

Procedure

Guinea pigs were anesthetized with an intramuscular injection of amixture of ketamine hydrochloride (33.3 mg/kg) and zylazine hydro-chloride (6.7 mg/kg). The eye to be tested was dilated by topicalapplication of atropine sulfate (0.04%) andphenylephredine hydrochlo-ride. The guinea pig was positioned for recording in a modifiedstereotaxic instrument. Body temperature was supported with the useof a heating pad. Except for one experiment explicitly concerned withscotopic sensitivity, recordings were made in a room illuminated byoverhead fluorescent lights yielding an illuminance of 296 Ix.

To determine an ERG photometric equation, we averaged theresponses to the lights for the last 50 cycles of a total presentation of 70cycles. These equations were made by iteratively adjusting the neutral-density wedge in the test light beam until the test and reference lightswere equally effective in producing an electrical response. Thedependent measure was the radiance of the test light required toachieve this equation. The wedge values at the point of equation wereread to the nearest 0.01 log unit. All photometric equations were madeat least twice during the recording sessions, and the separate valueswere subsequently averaged together. Stimulus flicker tate, test

Page 3: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

GUINEA PIG SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY AND COLOR VISION 995

wavelength, characteristics of the reference light, and adaptation stateof the eye were varied. The particular combinations of these featuresare specified later in conjunction with the results from each experi-ment.

Behavioral Experiments

Detailed descriptions of the apparatus and procedure used tomeasure visual discriminations have been published (Jacobs 1983,1984), so only a brief treatment is offered here.

Apparatus

The discrimination was set in the context of a three-alternative,forced choice. The guinea pig viewed three circular panels that weretransilluminated from an optical system located outside the testchamber. There were two light sources; one was a tungsten halidelamp whose output was used to diffusely and equally illuminate each ofthe three panels (background lights). The other source was a gratingmonochromator (Model H-10, Instruments SA, Edison, NJ) having a75-W xenon lamp. The output from this lamp was directed via a mirrorsystem so as to illuminate any one of the three panels; it constitutedthe test light. Depending on the experiment, the test light was eitheradded to the background or replaced it. Both lamps were run fromregulated DC power supplies. Except for measurements of scotopicspectral sensitivity, the interior of the test chamber was alwaysambiently illuminated to 100 Ix.

The guinea pig was trained to discriminate the panel illuminated bythe test light from the other two panels. To indicate its choice, theguinea pig touched a panel and, if the choice was correct, received adrop of liquid (Ocean Spray Cranapple Juice Cocktail). The locationof the panel illuminated by the test light was varied randomly overtrials, and the magnitude of the illumination difference between thepositive and the two negative panels was titrated to determinediscrimination thresholds. All aspects of the stimulus presentation,reinforcement delivery, and response monitoring were done automati-cally under program control of a computer.

Procedure

Using conventional procedures, 2 guinea pigs (1 male and 1 female)were first trained to select the uniquely illuminated panel when therewas a large (> 2 log units) brightness difference between the test andbackground lights. After success at that task, they were tested in aseries of experiments that fell into two categories.

Increment thresholds. Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity wasmeasured under five different test conditions. In each case, monochro-matic test lights (half-energy passband = 16 nm) were presented overa span of intensities in step values of 0.3 log unit. The intensity rangewas selected after initial training so that it yielded performance valuesrunning from about 80%-90% correct to 30%-40% correct. Thistypically required five or six intensity steps. Each of the intensity-wavelength combinations was presented in a three-trial block. Theoccurrence of the trial was signaled by a cuing tone; the trialterminated after the guinea pig responded or after 10 s without aresponse. A noncorrection procedure was used. Testing continued indaily sessions until a total of 100 or more trials had been accumulatedat each wavelength-intensity combination. From these data, psychomet-ric functions were drawn for each test wavelength by plotting meanpercentage correct as a function of test light intensity. These pointswere fit to a logistic function having asymptotes of 100% and 33%correct with the variance and mean as free parameters. The functionthat provided the best least-squares fit to each data set was determinedand from that the test stimulus intensity required to yield performance

corresponding to the upper 99% confidence level was taken as thethreshold value.

The several test conditions under which increment-threshold spec-tral sensitivity was measured were as follows: (a) Background—luminance = 0.92 log cd/m2; color temperature = 5350 K; testlight—420-610 nm in steps of 20 nm. (b) Background—luminance =1.73 log cd/m2; color temperature = 5350 K; test light—420-600 nm insteps of 10 nm. (c) Background—luminance = 1.73 log cd/m2; colortemperature = 5350 K; test light—420-610 nm in steps of 10 nm. (c)Background—long wavelength light produced by the use of a high-passfilter having 50% transmission at 580 nm that yielded a luminance of1.95 log cd/m2, test light—420-580 nm in steps of 10 nm. A second setof spectral sensitivity functions was determined with these sameconditions except that the luminance of the background light wasincreased to 2.91 log cd/m2. (d) Background—short-wavelength lightproduced through the use of a 440 nm interference filter having ahalf-energy passband of 10 nm that yielded a luminance of either 1.3cd/m2 or 0.9 cd/m2; test light—550-630 nm in steps of 10 nm or570-610 nm in steps of 5 nm. (e) Background—none and with thechamber darkened; test light—440-600 nm in steps of 10 nm.

Color discrimination. Following the increment-threshold measure-ments, the 2 guinea pigs were tested to see if they could make colordiscriminations. For this purpose, the test light was presented in thesubstitution mode, that is, on each trial it replaced one of the threebackground lights. The test light was varied in wavelength from 460 nmto 490 nm in steps of 3 nm. The ordering of wavelengths was random.At each test wavelength, the luminance of the positive stimulus wasrandomly varied in steps of 0.1 log unit to span a total intensity range of0.7 to 0.9 log units symmetrically centered around the point where thetest light and the background lights had been calculated to be equallybright for the subject. The procedure used to establish these brightnessequations is described later. The experiment was conducted for twodifferent conditions of background lighting: (a) luminance = 2.43 logcd/m2, color temperature = 5350 K; (b) luminance = 2.79 log cd/m2,color temperature = 5350 K.

Results and Discussion

Electrophysiological Measurementsof Spectral Mechanisms

To establish that ERG flicker photometry yields reliabledata from guinea pig eyes and that the spectra measured withthis technique can provide valid estimates of the photopig-ments, we first measured spectral sensitivity under scotopictest conditions for comparison with the earlier measurementsof the rod photopigment in the guinea pig. Guinea pigs wereadapted to the dark for 20 min and then tested in a fullydarkened room. A slow flicker rate (8 Hz) and a dim achro-matic reference light (radiance of 0.48 \L W/cm2 at the cornea)were used to enhance contributions from scotopic mecha-nisms. ERG flicker-photometric equations were obtained fromeach of 3 guinea pigs for test wavelengths taken at 10 nm stepsfrom 430 nm to 590 nm. The resulting spectral sensitivityfunctions are shown in Figure 1. The solid circles represent logquanta! sensitivity values at the level of the cornea, and theresults for the 3 guinea pigs are arbitrarily spaced on thesensitivity axis. The sensitivity values for these subjects weregreatly similar in that the range of sensitivity values for the 3subjects at any of the test wavelengths did not exceed 0.1 logunit.

Page 4: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

996 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F, DEEGAN II

enatn

a

I

9

3,5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.6

1.0

450 500 550 600

Wavelength (nm)

Figure 1. Scotopic spectral sensitivity functions for guinea pigs. Eachindividual symbol shows sensitivity as determined by electroretino-gram flicker photometry from a dark-adapted eye. Results are shownfor 3 guinea pigs; the data for each guinea pig are arbitrarily positionedon the ordinate. The continuous lines represent visual-pigment absorp-tion curves determined as described in the text. The Xmax values andthe goodness of fit (the least mean difference squared between thetheoretical absorption curve and the data points) are, from top tobottom: 495 nm—1.61 x 10~~3 log unit; 492 nm—4.62 x Ifr4 log unit;495 nm—9.38 x Ifr4 log unit.

To compare these scotopic spectral sensitivity functions withearlier estimates of the rod photopigment, we best fit eachfunction with a photopigment absorption curve (continuousline in Figure 1). The procedure to accomplish the best fit wasto shift a standard retinali photopigment absorption curve(Ebrey & Honig, 1977) along a log wavenumber axis (Baylor,Nunn, & Schnapf; 1987) in steps of 1 nm until the bestleast-squares fit was obtained between the data array and thepigment absorption curve. As can be seen in Figure 1, these fitsare quite good for each of the 3 guinea pigs. The best-fittingcurves for the scotopic functions of the 3 guinea pigs had X.max

values of 495 nm, 492 nm, and 495 nm. The average peak value(494 nm) is in reasonable agreement with the estimate for peakabsorption of guinea pig rhodopsin (497 nm) reported byBridges (1959). This correspondence suggests that ERG flickerphotometry can provide sensitive and valid estimates of phot-opigments of the guinea pig.

Light adaptation and rapid flicker rates are among the toolstraditionally used to diminish rod contributions to the ERG(Armington, 1974; Birch, 1989). We used both of these in anattempt to enhance any photopic components in the guinea pigERG. As the results of Dodt and Wirth (1953) first suggested,such components are easily seen in the guinea pig ERG.Figure 2 shows spectral sensitivity functions obtained from 2guinea pigs under conditions of light adaptation when thestimulus rate was increased to 40 Hz, and the reference lighthad an intensity of 74 u, W/cm2. Relative to the scotopicspectral sensitivity functions, peak sensitivity was shifted tolonger wavelengths (520-550 nm), and there was often a regionof secondary elevation in the short wavelengths. Functions

obtained under these test conditions are not accounted for bycontributions from any single photopigment. Rather, theshape of the functions suggests that multiple spectral mecha-nisms (perhaps both rods and one or more classes of cone)underlie measured spectral sensitivity.

The spectral sensitivity functions of Figure 2 imply thepresence in the guinea pig eye of at least one spectralmechanism that has a peak that is displaced toward the longwavelengths relative to that of the rod peak. We used threestrategies to obtain an estimate of the spectral sensitivity ofthis mechanism. Each involved the use of a stimulus arrange-ment that would be expected to enhance contributions fromthis putative middle-wavelength sensitive (MWS) mechanismand to minimize contributions from other mechanisms havinghigher sensitivity to shorter wavelengths. These strategiesinvolved using higher flicker rates or long-wavelength refer-ence lights, or combining these features. In addition, weminimized the effects of contribution from short-wavelengthmechanisms by restricting measurement to longer test wave-lengths. The results from these three experiments are summa-rized as the spectral sensitivity functions of Figure 3. A total of9 guinea pigs were tested under these three conditions. Theleft panel in Figure 3 shows spectral sensitivity functionsobtained using more rapid flicker (62.5 Hz); the reference lightwas achromatic (intensity of 74 JA W/cm2). The middle panelshows functions obtained from 4 guinea pigs for experimentsinvolving a 50-Hz stimulus and 590-nm reference light (50 u,W/cm2); the 2 guinea pigs with data shown in the right panelwere tested using 40-Hz flicker in conjunction with a long-wavelength reference light (produced by inserting a high-passfilter having a 50% half-energy cutoff at 580 nm; intensitymeasured over the spectral range of 550-650 nm = 3.2 x 10~5

W/cm2).Under the assumption that the spectral sensitivity recorded

under these conditions might principally reflect contributions

> 1.4

"3> , 2i

CO<u 1.0

1«g °-8

O)

5 0.6

0.4

*••

..

t

450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)600

Figure 2. Spectral sensitivity measurements obtained by electroretino-gram flicker photometry from light-adapted guinea pigs. Resultsobtained for 2 guinea pigs are indicated by the separate symbols.Stimulus rate = 40 Hz.

Page 5: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

GUINEA PIG SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY AND COLOR VISION 997

3.5

|? 3.0

35g 2.5«(0

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

525

527

525

450 500 550 600 650 450 500 5SO 600 650

Wavelength (nm)

450 500 550 600 650

Figure 3. Spectral sensitivity measurements of the middle-wavelength sensitive mechanism of the guineapig made with electroretinogram flicker photometry. Each data set shows the results obtained from 1guinea pig. Results for individual guinea pigs have been arbitrarily positioned on the ordinate. Threedifferent combinations of stimulus rates and reference lights were used: left panel (62.5 Hz, achromaticreference light); center panel (50 Hz, long-wavelength reference light); right panel (40 Hz, long-wavelength reference light). Each data set has been best fit with a visual-pigment absorption curve. The

from a single photopigment, each of the spectral sensitivityfunctions was best fit with a photopigment absorption curveusing the procedures described previously. These best-fitcurves are shown in Figure 3 with the appropriate X.max valuesindicated. Over the restricted spectral range that was exam-ined, these photopigment curves reasonably fit the data arrays.The spectral peaks varied modestly for the 9 subjects testedwith three different procedures (mean Xmax = 527.5 nm;SD = 2.3 nm). Any residual contribution from shorter spectralmechanisms would have the net effect of causing the computedpeaks of the best-fit functions to be shifted toward shorterwavelengths. It is thus reasonable to assume that theseexperiments indicate that the guinea pig retina contains a typeof cone whose spectral peak is not shorter than about 528 nm.Given the good fit between the pigment absorption functionsand the long wavelength slope of these spectral sensitivityfunctions, the actual peak is probably also not much longerthan that value.

The spectral sensitivity functions of Figure 2 also suggest thepresence in the guinea pig eye of a spectral mechanism havingpeak sensitivity that is shorter than that of the rods. A series ofexperiments were conducted to see if a more persuasiveindication of such a mechanism might be obtained. Theprincipal tools used to accomplish this were concurrent long-wavelength adaptation and short-wavelength reference lights.Figure 4 shows the average spectral sensitivity function ob-tained from 3 guinea pigs for 25-Hz flicker when the eye wasconcurrently adapted to an orange adaptation light (producedas in the right panel of Figure 3; radiance = 1.6 x 10~3

W/cm2). Under such conditions of adaptation, there is arelative enhancement of sensitivity to the short test wave-lengths (compare Figure 4 and Figure 2). The differentialdistortion of the shape of the spectral sensitivity function with

1.2

1.0

o.s

ja 0.6

201° 0.4

0.2

400 450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)

Figure 4. Electroretinogram spectral sensitivity for guinea pigs ob-tained with 25-Hz flicker and concurrent long-wavelength adaptation.The solid symbols represent mean values obtained from 3 guinea pigsthat were comparably tested. The vertical bar to the lower right showsthe average total range of sensitivity for the 3 subjects.

Page 6: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

998 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN I!

3.0

5 2.5

!COa, 2.0

1"SCC 1.5ato

1.0

400 450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)

Figure 5. Results from electroretinogram experiments intended todefine the spectral sensitivity of the short-wavelength sensitive (SWS)mechanism of the guinea pig. Each data set shows the results obtainedfrom 1 guinea pig with flicker photometry under conditions intendedto advantage contributions from the SWS mechanism. These condi-tions included a short-wavelength reference light and concurrent,intense long-wavelength adaptation. The results for the 4 guinea pigsare arbitrarily positioned along the ordinate. The continuous curvesrepresent the best-fitting summative combination of two pigmentabsorption functions. One of these functions was set to a Xmax value of529 nm, and the spectral position of the other function was sought by abest-fit procedure. The \max and proportion values for the SWSmechanism determined in this fashion were (from top to bottom): 426nm (81%); 425 nm (82%); 432 nm (81%); and 426 nm (83%).

chromatic adaptation indicates that multiple spectral mecha-nisms exist in the guinea pig retina. More importantly, thesemeasurements suggest that there is indeed a short-wavelengthsensitive (SWS) mechanism.

For 4 additional guinea pigs, we recorded spectral sensitivityfunctions at 25 Hz under yet more stringent conditions ofadaptation (reference light of 440 nm at an intensity of 50 u,W/cm2 with concurrent orange light adaptation at a radianceof 3,2 x 10~3 W/cm2). The spectral sensitivity functions aregiven in Figure 5. Under these conditions, sensitivity to testlights of greater than about 500 nm was reduced so as to makethese points unmeasurable. What remained were spectralsensitivity functions that show peak sensitivity in the vicinity of420-430 nm. The functions for the 3 guinea pigs are verysimilar. The derivation and meaning of the fitted curve isdescribed below.

In summary, measurements made with ERG flicker photom-etry suggest the presence of three spectral mechanisms in theguinea pig retina. One is a scotopic mechanism having a peakof about 494 nm. In addition, there are (at least) two spectralmechanisms operative under photopic test conditions. It provedcomparatively difficult to completely isolate these mechanisms,but utilization of a range of different adaptation conditions

suggests that one mechanism has peak sensitivity of about420-430 nm and the other has peak sensitivity in the vicinity of530 nm.

Behavioral Measurements of SpectralSensitivity and Color Vision

Guinea pigs proved to be apt subjects for these tests of visualdiscrimination. Once trained, 2 guinea pigs produced highlyreliable data throughout the 15-month period that was re-quired to complete these tests. Results from our first attemptto measure a photopic spectral sensitivity function are shownin Figure 6. The solid symbols in Figure 6 represent the meanthreshold values obtained from the 2 subjects. There are twonotable features of these spectral sensitivity functions: (a)They have two locations of peak sensitivity, one in the shortwavelengths in the vicinity of 420 nm and one in the middlewavelengths at 520-540 nm; (b) there is a dramatic decrease insensitivity at a location intermediate to these peaks centered atabout 480 nm.

The spectral sensitivity function of Figure 6 has severalfeatures characteristic of increment-threshold measurementsobtained under photopic test conditions from many othermammalian subjects (Jacobs, 1993). In particular, the shape ofthe function suggests the presence of inhibitory interactionbetween two spectral mechanisms. Such inhibitory interactioncan frequently be enhanced by increasing the intensity of theachromatic background light (Kalloniatis & Ffarwerth, 1990;King-Smith & Garden, 1976; Sperling & Harwerth, 1971). Thatwas the purpose of the second experiment in which the

I5°3>c«(A

§>

1.5

1-0

0.5 -

450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)600

Figure 6. Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions for theguinea pig. The points are mean values obtained from 2 guinea pigswith an achromatic background light (0.92 log cd/m2). The averagerange of sensitivity values for all test wavelengths was 0.18 log unit. Thelargest individual variation in threshold occurred at 420 nm; themagnitude of this variation is indicated by the vertical line drawnthrough the data point. The curve represents the best-fit, subtractivecombination of two pigment absorption curves having the following\max values and relative proportions: 529 nm (44%) and 438 nm (56%).

Page 7: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

GUINEA PIG SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY AND COLOR VISION 999

background light was substantially increased (by ca. 0.8 logunit) in intensity. These spectral sensitivity functions areplotted separately for the 2 subjects in Figure 7. As predicted,these functions show even more clearly the contributions tobehavioral discrimination from two spectral mechanisms.Sampled at interval steps of 10 nm, the locations of peaksensitivity for the 2 subjects were similar—430 nm and 440 nmfor the short mechanism and 530 nm for the other mechanism.The intermediate location of lowered sensitivity is even moreapparent for this higher level of light adaptation and is againcentered at about 480 nm. Although the shapes of the curveswere similar for the 2 subjects, the curves differed in therelative heights of the two spectral peaks with one havinghigher relative sensitivity to the short wavelengths.

Full spectral sensitivity functions were measured for 1guinea pig when the achromatic background light was replacedwith an orange light. The intent of this experiment was todetermine whether the two spectral mechanisms contributingto increment-threshold detection responded differentially tochromatic adaptation and to try to get a better estimate of thespectral properties of the SWS mechanism. The two spectralsensitivity functions that were obtained are shown in Figure 8.Note that the long-wavelength adaptation light caused a muchgreater loss of sensitivity to the long wavelengths than it did tothe short wavelengths (compare Figure 7 and Figure 8).Increasing the intensity of this background light (bottomfunction of Figure 8) had a further differential adaptationeffect in that the additional loss of sensitivity in the longwavelengths was much greater than it was in the short testwavelengths. The more intense orange light caused an addi-tional threshold elevation of about 0.8 log unit at 560 nm,

3.5

|3.0

2.5atoa> 2.0

1

CD

1.5

0.5

450 500 550 600

Wavelength (nm)

650

Figure 7. Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions for 2guinea pigs. The data were obtained in the presence of an achromaticbackground light (1.73 log cd/m2). The results for the 2 guinea pigs arearbitrarily positioned on the ordinate. The curves are best-fit, subtrac-tive combinations of two photopigment absorption curves. The Xmax

values and relative proportions of these best-fit curves for the guineapig at the top are 529 nm (21%) and 425 nm (79%); comparable valuesfor the guinea pig at the bottom are 529 nm (45%) and 439 nm (55%).

3.0

f2 .5

•5

I 2'°o>

"I 1-5

1

a 1.0

0.5

450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)

600

Figure 8. Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions obtainedfrom 1 guinea pig under two conditions of chromatic adaptation. Ineach case, the background was an orange light. For the results shownat the bottom, the background light was about 0.3 log unit moreintense than the background used for the data shown at the top. Thefunctions were best fit by additive combinations of two photopigmentabsorption curves; these were 529 nm (18%) and 427 nm (82%) for thetop data set, and 529 nm (11%) and 425 nm (89%) for the bottom dataset.

whereas the same light elevated threshold for a 430 nm light byonly an additional 0.2 log unit. The experiment makes clearthat the two components of the increment-threshold spectralsensitivity function of the guinea pig may be differentiallyadapted. We return below to the question of the spectralspecification of the two underlying mechanisms.

Two additional experiments were conducted to better esti-mate the spectral properties of the MWS mechanism. Inessence, the idea was to carry out behavioral measurementsanalogous to the electrophysiological measurements that weresummarized in Figure 3. Spectral sensitivity measurementswere made when the background light was of short wavelength(440 nm). Our intent was to diminish as much as possible anycontribution from the SWS mechanism or the rods. Data wereobtained at two different intensity levels; the test wavelengthswere chosen to fall on the long wavelength slope of the MWScomponent. Results from the dimmer of these wavelengths areshown in the left panel of Figure 9; the results on the right wereobtained after the background light had been slightly in-creased in intensity. In each case, the sensitivity data have beenbest fit to photopigment absorption curves using the proce-dures described previously. For the dimmer adaptation, theXma>j of the best-fit function was 525 nm; with the brighteradaptation and more detailed sampling (in steps of 5 nm) ofthis long-wavelength slope, the best-fit curve has KmaK of529 nm.

A final spectral sensitivity measurement was made withbackground and test-chamber light extinguished. The singlesubject was adapted to the dark for a 20-min period before theinitiation of each test session. The spectral sensitivity functionobtained is shown in Figure 10. The threshold measurements

Page 8: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

1000 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II

1.5

to» 1.0

CO0)

V3.20)

ocO>o

0.5

0.0 -I I

500 550 600 650I

500 550 600 650

Wavelength (nm)

Figure 9. Attempts to define the spectral positioning of the middle-wavelength sensitive (MWS)photopigment of the guinea pig from increment-threshold spectral sensitivity functions. Both panels showsensitivity measurements obtained with stimulus conditions intended to advantage contributions from theMWS mechanism. The background light in each case was 440 nm; the intensity of this light was higher forthe data presented in the right panel (see text). The absorption curves for visual pigments drawn throughthe data sets was obtained by a best-fit procedure. The Xmax values for the curves are 525 nm (left) and 529nm (right).

were best-fitted in the usual manner. The function that best fitthis data array had a Xmax value of 498 nm (solid line in Fig-ure 10).

The photopic spectral sensitivity functions for guinea pigs

8.0

7.5

I4**

1§>o

7.0

6.5

6.0

>*••

450 500 550

Wavelength (nm)

600

Figure 10. Scotopic spectral sensitivity function obtained in a behav-ioral discrimination test from a dark-adapted guinea pig. The curverepresents the visual pigment absorption function obtained by abest-fit procedure. The Xmax of this curve is 498 nm (fitting er-ror = 8.86 x 10~3 log unit).

(see Figures 6-9) strongly suggest the presence of two conephotopigments, and they accordingly predict the species tohave dichromatic color vision. A color discrimination experi-ment was conducted to evaluate that possibility. In thisexperiment, the test lights were monochromatic and variable inwavelength over the spectral range from 460 nm to 490 nm.The negative lights were achromatic. The stimuli were pre-sented in the substitution mode, that is, on each trial the testlight replaced one of the three background lights. Successfuldiscrimination of a monochromatic light from an achromaticlight under conditions where there is no consistent brightnessdifference between the two is a color discrimination. Themajor obstacle to be overcome in tests of this sort is to ensurethat subjects cannot use consistent brightness cues, that theyare in fact forced to make a color discrimination. To equate themonochromatic and achromatic lights so that they wereequally bright, we used a procedure used earlier (Jacobs,1984). The procedure exploits the fact that in all of theprevious experiments the guinea pigs had been trained to pickthe brightest of the three lights. To establish brightnessmatches, we arranged the situation such that the monochro-matic test light was presented as a substitute for one of thethree background lights. As long as this light was perceived asbrighter than the achromatic lights, the guinea pig selected itconsistently, just as it had in the increment-threshold experi-ments. The test light was then made progressively less intense(in steps of 0.2 log unit). At some point, the two negative lightsbecome relatively brighter than the test light and the subjects'behavior reflected this by shifting from consistent preference

Page 9: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

GUINEA PIG SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY AND COLOR VISION 1001

of the test light to consistent avoidance. The intensity of thetest light leading to this reversal was taken as indicating thelocation of the brightness match. Such matches were made fortest wavelengths drawn from each end of the spectral range.Once these brightness matches were determined, the subjectwas trained in the color discrimination.

The left panel of Figure 11 shows asymptotic discriminationperformance obtained across the spectral range tested for eachsubject. The plotted points are average discrimination re-corded over the last 50 test trials for those test light intensitiesat the point of computed equal brightness. These functions arenot materially changed either by averaging the results obtainedat the equation value and for values ±0.1 log unit from thisvalue or by simply plotting the poorest performance recordedat each test wavelength. These facts suggest that the discrimi-nation results do reflect those from a test where there were noconsistent brightness cues. The 2 guinea pigs performedsimilarly in this test. Both guinea pigs discriminated thespectral lights almost perfectly at either end of the test range;both guinea pigs also showed a loss of consistent discrimina-tion in the vicinity of 480 nm. The horizontal dashed line ofFigure 11 indicates the 95% confidence level. As judged by thisperformance criterion, the spectral range over which theguinea pigs failed to make a color discrimination extendedfrom 470 nm to 484 nm for 1 subject, and from 477 nm to 482nm for the other subject. The midpoints of this range aresimilar for the 2 subjects—477 nm and 479.5 nm, respectively.

The entire color discrimination experiment was run a secondtime after the achromatic lights had been increased in intensityby about 0.3 log unit. These results are shown in the right panelof Figure 11. The conventions were the same, and so were theresults. In this case, discrimination failed for 1 subject for a

spectral range of 471-482 nm (midpoint 476.5 nm) and failedfor the other subject in a narrower range—from 477 nm to 483nm (midpoint of 480 nm). These regions of discriminationfailure define the presence of a spectral neutral point andverify the prediction that guinea pigs have dichromatic colorvision.

General Discussion

Guinea Pig Photopigments and Spectral Mechanisms

The results reported here suggest that the eye of the guineapig contains three classes of photopigment—one is a rodpigment and the other two are cone photopigments. Bothelectrophysiological and behavioral measurements allow esti-mates of the absorption spectra for these pigments. We notedthat the spectral positioning of the MWS photopigment can bemost reliably characterized by fits of absorption spectra to thelong-wavelength portion of the curve. The ERG measure-ments (see Figure 3) and behavioral measurements (seeFigure 9) are in good agreement in this regard. The formeryielded an average Xmax value of about 528 nm, whereas thebehavioral measurement, which was most likely to have in-dexed activity of the MWS pigment alone (see Figure 9, rightcurve), gave a corresponding value of 529 nm. Because anyresidual contributions from other pigments having shorter Xmax

values would tend to shift the peak toward the short wave-lengths, the longer of these two wavelengths is assumed as thebest current estimate of the spectral positioning of the guineapig MWS cone pigment.

By virtue of the considerable spectral overlap of the MWSand SWS photopigments in the short wavelengths, the spectral

oEOu

(Ba»

1001-

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

460 470 480 490 500 510 520 460 470 480 490

Wavelength (nm)

Figure I I . Color discrimination results for 2 guinea pigs. Each data point indicates the asymptotic levelsof performance achieved in a test involving the discrimination of various monochromatic lights (abscissavalues) from equally bright achromatic lights. The two panels show results from two experiments that wereidentical except for the achromatic lights, which were about 0.3 log more intense for the experiment shownto the right. The horizontal dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence level.

Page 10: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

1002 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II

positioning of the SWS photopigment may be best estimatedfrom those experiments in which contributions from MWSreceptors were reduced through the use of chromatic adapta-tion. These are the ERG measurements shown in Figure 5 andthe behavioral measurements in Figure 8. In these experi-ments, even with concurrent long-wavelength adaptation, therelikely remains some residual contributions from the MWSpigment. Accordingly, to estimate the spectral positioning ofthe SWS pigment we assumed that the ERG and behavioralresults of Figures 5 and 8 represent summative contributionsfrom MWS and SWS mechanisms. To determine the spectralpositioning of the SWS mechanism, the array of sensitivityvalues were best fit using summative combinations of aphotopigment having \max value of 529 nm and an SWSphotopigment. The spectral position of the putative SWSphotopigment was varied in steps of 1 nm (over the spectralrange of 420-440 nm) and in relative proportions of 1% untilthe best-fitting combination of the two pigments was obtained.These best-fit curves are the continuous lines drawn throughthe data in Figures 5 and 8. This strategy accounts quite wellfor the shape of the spectral sensitivity functions. The esti-mated peak values for the SWS mechanism show relativelysmall variability. The 4 subjects whose ERG results are shownin Figure 5 have an average SWS Xmax value of 427.3 nm(SD = 3.1); the subject of the behavioral experiments shown inFigure 8 yielded corresponding values of 425 nm and 427 nmfor the two experiments.

The increment-threshold measurements of Figures 6 and 7also show clear evidence of inputs from both cone types. Thecombination of these inputs in this situation, as for otherincrement-threshold measurements on achromatic back-grounds (e.g., Kalloniatis & Harwerth, 1990; Sperling &Harwerth, 1971), involves inhibitory interaction between thetwo mechanisms. These data were best-fitted by using astrategy earlier used to account for such functions obtainedfrom other dichromatic subjects (Jacobs, 1990; Neitz, Geist, &Jacobs, 1989), that is, by searching for the best subtractivecombination of a 529-nm MWS pigment and an SWS photopig-ment. The best-fit functions so obtained are given by the curvesof Figures 6 and 7. This strategy provides a reasonable accountof the discrimination data. Taken together, the summative andsubtractive fits to the electrophysiological and behavioral datayielded a total of nine individual estimates of the spectralpositioning of the SWS cone pigment of the guinea pig. Therange of such estimates was 13 nm; the mean estimate was 429nm.

The behavioral and electrophysiological measurements ofscotopic spectral sensitivity (see Figures 1 and 10) are inreasonable agreement with each other and with the earlierspectrophotometric measurements of rod pigment in theguinea pig (Bridges, 1959). All these measurements suggestthat the rods of the guinea pig contain a pigment whose peaksensitivity is in the range from about 494 nm to 498 nm.

Two other issues of concern in deriving estimates of photopig-ment spectra from spectral sensitivity measurements deservemention. One is that in producing these estimates no correc-tions have been made for possible preretinal filtering of thespectral input. The principal source of such filtering in the eyesof mammals is lens pigmentation (Muntz, 1972). Cooper and

Robson (1969) measured density spectra for the intact lens ofthe guinea pig, and their measurements show little spectrallyselective light losses in the guinea pig lens over the portion ofthe spectrum examined in the present experiments. Accord-ingly, we concluded that no corrections need be made forpreretinal filtering. A second source of general concern incomparing pigment absorption spectra and spectral sensitivitymeasurements are possible effects of pigment self-screening. Ithas long been recognized that the shape of the absorptionspectrum of a photopigment depends on pigment concentra-tion (Dartnall, 1957) and that comparisons of action spectra(such as the spectral sensitivity curves of the present experi-ment) with standard photopigment absorption curves may beinaccurate if there is substantial pigment density. There are nodirect measurements, nor indeed any other information, toallow an estimate of the optical density of guinea pig photore-ceptors, but several aspects of the results suggest that notmuch improvement on the spectral estimates could be ob-tained by consideration of optical density. In the case of cones,this may be true because all of the ERG measurements andmost of the behavioral measurements were obtained fromhighly light-adapted eyes. This would tend to greatly reducepigment density and a priori would seem to obviate the needfor concern about the effects of this variable. On the otherhand, one might expect that the scotopic measurements wouldreflect the operation of rods whose pigment density was nearmaximal. The fact that the photopigment absorption curves(see Figures 1 and 10) provide a good account of the shape ofthe spectral sensitivity curves suggests that any effects ofself-screening must be small. Why this should be so is unclear,but it may be noteworthy that similar uncertainty surroundscomparisons of human rod vision and rhodopsin (Alpern,Fulton, & Baker, 1987; Bowmaker & Dartnall, 1980).

We noted previously that the earlier studies of the spectralmechanisms of the guinea pig were inconsistent in outcome.However, one can perhaps find in them previews of the presentresults. For instance, the narrow-band "modulator" elementsdetected in early studies of the discharges of optic-nerve fibersby Granit (1947) have peaks of about 450 nm and 530 nm.Spectral peaks of about 460 nm and 530 nm also appear inbleaching spectra obtained from retinal densitometric measure-ments (Weale, 1955). The spectra determined from visuallyevoked responses of the guinea pig pineal gland show asecondary peak at about 450 nm (Thiele & Meissl, 1987). Ineach of these cases, the reported spectral peak values are closeenough to the current measurements of the guinea pig conepigments to suggest these earlier results might have reflectedsignals of cone origin in the guinea pig visual system.

Color Vision

Given the historical arguments about whether the retina ofthe guinea pig contained any cones at all and the emphaticdenial of color vision in the guinea pig in the only studydirected to that end (Miles et al., 1956), it was somewhatsurprising to find that cone signals can be easily detected bothelectrophysiologically and behaviorally and that color visioncan be readily demonstrated. Like many other mammals, theguinea pig is a dichromat. That diagnosis rests on the fact that

Page 11: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

GUINEA PIG SPECTRAL SENSITIVITY AND COLOR VISION 1003

both subjects were able to discriminate monochromatic lightsfrom equally bright achromatic lights except when the mono-chromatic light was set to a narrow wavelength range (seeFigure 11). The wavelength of the monochromatic light thatmatched in appearance an achromatic light for the guinea pigwas located in the vicinity of 480 nm. This location, the neutralpoint, represents a color match the details of which depend onthe spectral positioning of the two cone pigments, as well asthe effective spectral characteristics of the achromatic light.For dichromatic observers, the neutral point effectively splitsthe spectrum such that subjects can make color discriminationsbetween lights whose spectral energy distributions fall princi-pally to either side of the neutral point. Dichromatic observersalso can discriminate most stimuli having narrow-band spectralenergy from stimuli having a continuous spectrum (see Figure11). In the previous experiment on guinea pig color vision, thediscriminations tested were between a red and a green lightand between the same green light and a blue light (Miles et al.,1956). These stimuli were produced by passing light throughbroad-band filters. It seems unlikely that the first discrimina-tion would have been possible for the dichromatic guinea pig,because the effective energies of both lights were probablyrestricted to the same side of the neutral point. On other hand,one might expect that the guinea pigs would have been able todiscriminate a blue light from a green light. We can onlyspeculate why they did not; but whatever the reason, theconclusion of the earlier study seems to be in error. Guineapigs do have color vision. Furthermore, the ease of demonstrat-ing color vision in a laboratory test suggests that it may providea useful additional source of information about the environ-ment of the guinea pig.

Phytogeny of Photopigments

There is only limited information about the distribution ofcone photopigments among the mammals. Even so, it may beuseful to briefly consider the cone pigment complement of theguinea pig from a comparative perspective. What has so farbeen learned about the cone pigments of rodents has beenrecently summarized (Jacobs, 1993). In brief, several differentcone pigment arrangements have been documented for ro-dents. Species from the family Sciuridae (the squirrels) typi-cally have two classes of cone pigment. All squirrels (with thepossible exception of the nocturnal flying squirrel) have anSWS cone pigment with a peak somewhere in the range fromabout 435 nm to 445 nm. In addition, there is a class of MWScone whose peak location varies—among the ground-dwellingsquirrels this pigment has a peak of about 520 nm, whereas forthe arboreal squirrels the corresponding peak is about 543 nm.Cone pigments have also been measured for a number ofspecies from the family Muridae (mice, rats, etc.). Many ofthese species also have two classes of cone photopigment.However, one of these pigments has a very short Xmax value—inthe ultraviolet at about 360 nm. The other (MWS) pigmentvaries among species with peak values that extend from about493 nm to about 512 nm. In addition, there is evidence tosuggest that some murid rodents may have only a single conepigment with peak in the vicinity of about 500 nm. Beyondthese two large families, few other rodents have been exam-

ined. The rodents that have been examined seem to fall intoone or another of the patterns just described (Jacobs, 1993). Incomparison to these rodents, the guinea pig cone pigmentsthus appear unique. Like the sciurids (but unlike the murids),the guinea pig has an SWS cone of about 430 nm. However, thespectral location of the MWS pigment of the guinea pig doesnot match the locations so far identified for any other rodent.In fact, a cone pigment with a peak of 530 nm has not beenclaimed for any mammals other than Old World primates. InOld World primates, a 530-nm cone pigment is common andmaybe the rule (Bowmaker, 1991; Jacobs, 1993).

The factors that determine the details of photopigmentcomplement result from events of evolutionary history that canat present only be dimly discerned (Crescitelli, 1990; Gold-smith, 1990; Jacobs, 1993). It was noted earlier that recentmolecular analyses suggest the guinea pig may deserve anordinal status separate from that of Rodentia (Graur et al.,1991; Li et al., 1992). This new order may have arisen from oneof the earliest divergence events in the history of eutherianmammals—at a point before the divergence of carnivores,artiodactyls, lagomorphs, chiropterans, and primates (Li et al.,1992). It is uncertain what other species ought to be includedwith guinea pigs and other caviomorphs in this new order, butLi et al. (1992) suggest that the hystricomorphs (porcupine-like rodents) may represent reasonable candidates. At present,nothing appears to be known about the cone pigments in any ofthese species, but the uniqueness of the guinea pig conecomplement strongly recommends the examination of theretinas of other species in this new grouping.

References

Alpern, M., Fulton, B., & Baker, B. N. (1987). "Self-screening" ofrhodopsin in rod outer segments. Vision Research, 27, 1459-1470.

Armington, J. C. (1974). The electroretinogram. San Diego, CA:Academic Press.

Baylor, D. A., Nunn, B. J., & Schnapf, J. D. (1987). Spectral sensitivityof cones of the monkey Macaco fascicularis. Journal of Physiology,390, 145-160.

Birch, D. G. (1989). Clinical electroretinography. OphthalmologyClinics of North America, 2, 469-497.

Bowmaker, J. K. (1991). Visual pigments, oil droplets and photorecep-tors. In P. Gouras (Ed.), The perception of colour (pp. 108-127). BocaRaton, FL: CRC Press.

Bowmaker, J. K., & Dartnall, H. J. A. (1980). Visual pigments of rodsand cones in a human retina. Journal of Physiology, 298, 501-511.

Bradt, H. (1988). Tasty Andean tidbits. Americas, 40, 64-65.Bridges, C. D. B. (1959). The visual pigments of some common

laboratory mammals. Nature, 184, 1727-1728.Cooper, G. F., & Robson, J. G. (1969). The yellow colour of the lens of

the grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis leucotis). Journal of Physiology,203, 403^110.

Crescitelli, F. (1990). The natural history of visual pigments. Progress inRetinal Research, 11, 1-32.

Dartnall, H. J. A. (1957). The visual pigments. London: Metheun.Dodt, E., & Wirth, A. (1953). Differentiation between rods and cones

by flicker eiectroretinography in pigeon and guinea pig. ActaPhysiologica Scandinavica, 30, 80-89.

Ebrey, T. G., & Honig, B. (1977). New wavelength-dependent visualpigment nomograms. Vision Research, 27, 147-151.

Goldsmith, T. H. (1990). Optimization, constraint, and history in theevolution of eyes. Quarterly Review of Biology, 65, 281-322.

Page 12: Spectral Sensitivity, Photopigments, and Color … · 994 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II (Hecht & Schlaer, 1936), and it accordingly suggests the presence of both rods and

1004 GERALD H. JACOBS AND JESS F. DEEGAN II

Granit, R. (1944). Stimulus intensity in relation to excitation and pre-and post-excitatory inhibition in isolated elements of mammalianretinae. Journal of Physiology, 103, 103-118.

Granit, R. (1947). Sensory mechanisms of the retina. London: OxfordUniversity Press.

Graur, D., Hide, W. A., & Li, W.-H. (1991). Is the guinea pig a rodent?Nature, 351, 649-652.

Harper, L. B. (1976). Behavior. In J. E. Wagner & P. J. Manning(Eds.), The biology of the guinea pig (pp. 31-51). San Diego, CA:Academic Press.

Hecht, S., & Shlaer, S. (1936). Intermittent stimulation by light. V. Therelation between intensity and critical frequency for different partsof the spectrum. Journal of General Physiology, 19, 965-979.

Jacobs, G. H. (1983). Within-species variations in visual capacityamong squirrel monkeys: Sensitivity differences. Vision Research, 23,239-248.

Jacobs, G. H. (1984). Within-species variations in visual capacityamong squirrel monkeys: Color vision. Vision Research, 24, 1267-1277.

Jacobs, G. H. (1990). Duplicity theory and ground squirrels: Linkagesbetween photoreceptors and visual function. Visual Neuroscience, 5,311-318.

Jacobs, G. H. (1993). The distribution and nature of colour visionamong the mammals. Biological Reviews, 68, 413-471.

Jacobs, G. H., & Neitz, J. (1987). Inheritance of color vision in a NewWorld monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Proceedings of the National Acad-emy of Sciences, USA, 84, 2545-2549.

Kalloniatis, M., & Harwerth, R. S. (1990). Spectral sensitivity andadaptation characteristics of cone mechanisms under white-lightadaptation. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 7, 1912-1928.

King, J. A. (1956). Social relations of the guinea pig under semi-natural conditions. Ecology, 37, 221-228.

King-Smith, P. E., & Garden, D. (1976). Luminance and opponent-color contributions to visual detection and to temporal and spatialintegration. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 66, 709-717.

Kolmer, W. (1936). Vergleichende Anatomie der Netzhaut [Compara-tive anatomy of the retina]. In W. von Mallendorf (Ed.), Handbuch

der Mikroskopishcen Anatomie des Menschen (Vol. 3, Pt. 2, pp.321-326 and 420-421). Berlin: Juluis Springer.

Li, W.-H., Hide, W. A., Zharkikh, A., Ma, D.-P., & Graur, D. (1992).The molecular taxonomy and evolution of the guinea pig. Journal ofHeredity, 83, 174-181.

Miles, R. C., Ratoosh, P., & Meyer, D. R. (1956). Absence of colorvision in the guinea pig. Journal of Neurophysiology, 19, 254-258.

Muntz, W. R. A. (1972). Inert absorbing and reflecting pigments. In H.J. A. Dartnall (Ed.), Handbook of sensory physiology: Photochemistryof vision (Vol. 7, Pt. 1, pp. 529-565). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Neitz, J., Geist, T., & Jacobs, G. H. (1989). Color vision in the dog.Visual Neuroscience, 3, 119-125.

Neitz, J., & Jacobs, G. H. (1984). Electroretinogram measurements ofcone spectral sensitivity in dichromatic monkeys. Journal of theOptical Society of America A, 1, 1175-1180.

O'Day, K. (1947). Visual cells of the guinea pig. Nature, 160, 648.Rood, J. P. (1972), Ecological and behavioral comparisons of three

genera of Argentine cmies. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 5, 1-83.Sjostrand, F. S. (1965). The synaptology of the retina. In A. V. S. De

Reuck & J. Knight (Eds.), Colour vision: Physiology and experimentalpsychology (pp. 110-151). Boston: Little, Brown.

Sperling, H. G., & Harwerth, R. S. (1971). Red-green cone interac-tions in increment-threshold spectral sensitivity of primates. Science,172, 180-184.

Szel, A., & Rohlich, P. (1992). Two cone types of rat retina detected byanti-visual pigment antibodies. Experimental Eye Research, 55, 47-52.

Thiele, G., & Meissl, H. (1987). Action spectra of the lateral eyesrecorded from mammalian pineal glands. Brain Research, 424,10-16.

Weale, R. A. (1955). Bleaching experiments on eyes of living guineapigs. Journal of Physiology, 127, 572-586.

Received January 19,1994Revision received April 12,1994

Accepted April 12,1994 •