10
International Journal of Linguistics ISSN 1948-5425 2012, Vol. 4, No. 3 www.macrothink.org/ijl 734 Presupposition Trigger-A Comparative Analysis of Broadcast News Discourse Javad Zare' Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran E-mail: [email protected] Ehsan Abbaspour Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran E-mail: [email protected] Mahdi Rajaee Nia Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran E-mail: [email protected] Received: June 25, 2012 Accepted: July 22, 2012 Published: September 1, 2012 doi:10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2002 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2002 Abstract Presupposition has long been used as a property of language to mold the audience’s ideology. Using presupposition triggers, surprisingly the author or speaker impinges on readers or listeners’ interpretation of facts and events, establishing either a favorable or unfavorable bias throughout the text. The role of presupposition in mass media’s use of language is of paramount importance in that media writers attempt consciously or unconsciously to influence the audience understanding of news events. The present paper is aimed at pinpointing the oral discourse structure of two English news channels i.e. PressTV and CNN as varieties of Persian and American English respectively, in terms of presupposition triggers, employed to share non-asserted meaning. Accordingly, 40 transcripts (20 selected from PressTV and another 20 from CNN) were analyzed in terms of presupposition triggers, namely existential, factive, lexical, non-factive, structural, counter-factual, adverbial, and

Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Presupposition has long been used as a property of language to mold the audience’s ideology. Using presupposition triggers, surprisingly the author or speaker impinges on readers or listeners’ interpretation of facts and events, establishing either a favorable or unfavorable bias throughout the text. The role of presupposition in mass media’s use of language is of paramount importance in that media writers attempt consciously or unconsciously to influence the audience understanding of news events. The present paper is aimed at pinpointing the oral discourse structure of two English news channels i.e. PressTV and CNN as varieties of Persian and American English respectively, in terms of presupposition triggers, employed to share non-asserted meaning. Accordingly, 40 transcripts (20 selected from PressTV and another 20 from CNN) were analyzed in terms of presupposition triggers, namely existential, factive, lexical, non-factive, structural, counter-factual, adverbial, and relative. Analysis of the transcripts revealed that the most frequently used presupposition trigger in both varieties of oral discourse was Existential.

Citation preview

Page 1: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 734

Presupposition Trigger-A Comparative Analysis of

Broadcast News Discourse

Javad Zare'

Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran

E-mail: [email protected]

Ehsan Abbaspour

Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran

E-mail: [email protected]

Mahdi Rajaee Nia

Iran University of Science & Technology, Iran

E-mail: [email protected]

Received: June 25, 2012 Accepted: July 22, 2012 Published: September 1, 2012

doi:10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2002 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/ijl.v4i3.2002

Abstract

Presupposition has long been used as a property of language to mold the audience’s ideology.

Using presupposition triggers, surprisingly the author or speaker impinges on readers or

listeners’ interpretation of facts and events, establishing either a favorable or unfavorable bias

throughout the text. The role of presupposition in mass media’s use of language is of

paramount importance in that media writers attempt consciously or unconsciously to

influence the audience understanding of news events. The present paper is aimed at

pinpointing the oral discourse structure of two English news channels i.e. PressTV and CNN

as varieties of Persian and American English respectively, in terms of presupposition triggers,

employed to share non-asserted meaning. Accordingly, 40 transcripts (20 selected from

PressTV and another 20 from CNN) were analyzed in terms of presupposition triggers,

namely existential, factive, lexical, non-factive, structural, counter-factual, adverbial, and

Page 2: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 735

relative. Analysis of the transcripts revealed that the most frequently used presupposition

trigger in both varieties of oral discourse was Existential.

Keywords: CDA, Presupposition, Presupposition trigger

Page 3: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 736

1. Introduction

Critical Discourse Analysis which Fairclough (1995) defines as discourse analysis aiming “to

systematically explore often opaque relationships of causality and determination between (a)

discursive practices, events and texts, and (b) wider social and cultural structures, relations

and processes”, is considered an attempt to reveal hidden meanings consciously or

unconsciously embedded in an utterance. In other words, CDA attempts to disclose the

ideological values of text writers reflected in the discourse. Widdowson (2000) describes

CDA as “the uncovering of implicit ideologies in texts”. He also asserts that CDA “unveils

the underlying Ideological prejudice” existing in discourse and therefore it studies “the

exercise of power in texts” (Widdowson, 2000). Investigating “how … practices, events and

texts arise out of and are ideologically shaped by relations of power and struggles over

power” is mentioned as the major function of CDA. (Fairclough, 1995: 132)

Presupposition as one of the properties of language which impinges on readers or listeners’

understanding of facts and events through using subtle linguistic devices and constructions is

considered an argumentative concept in CDA. Levinson (2001) defines presupposition as

“the common ground” embedded in an utterance which is taken for granted by all the

participants i.e. speaker & listener, or writer & reader. In another description, Richardson

(2007) delineates it as “implicit claims inherent in the explicit meaning of a text or utterance

which are taken for granted” (p, 63). Put another way, presupposition refers to the

non-asserted information triggered by certain linguistic constructions which is irrefutably

credited as gospel truth by participants in an utterance in a specific context.

Werth (1993) cites Frege who enumerates basic properties of presupposition as 1) being

embedded in referring phrases and temporal clauses, 2) being constant even in their negated

counterparts, and 3) determining the accuracy of the assumption of a sentence. That is, the

assumption of a sentence is true only when the presupposition is true. Moreover, Dryer (1996)

cites Chierchia and McConnell-Ginet (1990) who include “Being back-grounded and taken

for granted” as the main empirical properties of presupposition. Presuppositions are usually

analyzed by using constancy under negation as a rule of thumb. Constancy under negation

which determines the actuality of presuppositions, stresses that the presupposed information

should remain true even after the statement is negated. An example can clarify the point:

a) Everybody knows that John has got married.

b) >> John has got married.

c) Everybody doesn’t know that John has got married.

d) >> John has got married.

(From Yule, 2010: 27)

As the example clarifies, sentence (a) and its negated counterpart (c) both presuppose the

same meaning (b) and (d).

Page 4: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 737

Generally, there are two approaches to studying presupposition which scholars can take, i.e.

semantic and pragmatic, based on which it is analyzed from the aspect of logic and

pragmatics respectively. Schmid (2001) notes that semantic presuppositions hinge on the

meaning of the words used to trigger information. While, pragmatic presuppositions as Caffi

(1993) asserts, do not exist in the meaning of words, or in something that is already known;

instead, they exist in something which is given as information by the speaker, or in something

which is assumed as such (Cited in Schmid, 2001: 153). As a matter of fact, pragmatic

presuppositions share the meaning that more information is to follow. An example can clear

up the distinction:

E.g: “The thing is that he needs a lot of loving.”

>> There is a thing. [Semantic Presupposition]

>> There is a thing (and I am going to tell you what it is). [Pragmatic Presupposition]

(From Schmid, 2001: 153)

As mentioned earlier, presuppositions can be tested by using the constancy under negation

principle. It’s interesting to note that only semantic presuppositions remain true after negation.

As Verschueren (1978) asserts, there are some pragmatic presuppositions that do not remain

constant under negation. In other words, pragmatic presuppositions and their negated

counterparts do not presuppose the same meaning.

2. Presupposition Trigger

There are some linguistic constructions at writers or speakers’ disposal described as

presupposition triggers which enable them to communicate intended information without

stating them. Yule (2010) categorizes presupposition triggers or types into 6 groups,

including existential, factive, lexical, structural, non-factive, and counter-factual. Relative

and adverbial presuppositions are additionally briefly introduced. Examples are taken from

the presuppositions detected in the transcripts.

1) Existential: Presupposition by means of possessive constructions or any definite noun

phrase is called existential. As a matter of fact, by using these linguistic forms the

speaker or writer seems committed to the existence of mentioned entities.

E.g. the deadline for Iranians >> there is a deadline

E.g. Iran’s Guardian Council >> Iran has Guardian Council

2) Factive: A piece of information following verbs like know, realize, regret and phrases

like “It’s odd that …” is considered factive presupposition.

E.g. It’s sad that the Occupations have started out >> the Occupations have started out

3) Lexical: As Yule (2010) states, in lexical presuppositions the use of some forms with

their stated meanings is interpreted as the presentation of some non-asserted

meanings.

Page 5: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 738

E.g. The European Union plans to impose new sanctions against Tehran >> previously there

have been sanctions

4) Structural: In this type, some interrogative forms are used as tools of triggering

presupposed information.

E.g. Why not add one more to the table >> one more should be added to the table.

5) Non-Factive: Some verbs like dream, imagine, pretend, and allege are assumed to

presuppose information which is not true.

E.g. an imagined move by China >> the move is not real

6) Counter-Factual: Conditional forms in subjunctive mood are considered to trigger

“contrary to fact” presuppositions.

E.g. if there was a situation 100% that these people were >> there is not such a situation

7) , 8) Relative and adverbial: Relative and Adverbial clauses are also found to

presuppose information.

E.g. the incident occurred in a region where there is a large Kurdish population >> there is a

large Kurdish population [Relative]

E.g. it started when Tehran’s vice president this week warned >> this week Tehran’s vice

president warned [Adverbial]

The point regarding presupposition types in discourse is that as Yule (2010) notes these

linguistic forms should be considered “potential presuppositions”, which can only become

actual in contexts with speakers who intend to communicate a piece of non-asserted

information. In other words, statements do not possess presuppositions; rather it is speakers

or writers who presuppose intended meaning. (Yule, 2010, p.27)

Among writers and speakers there is an appeal to the notion of presupposition in that certain

pieces of information already assumed to be known by readers and listeners are not required

to be stated. Suppose a subject-predicate structure in which the intended meaning is placed in

the subject part rather than in the predicate. Usually information in the subject part is

considered old information which is accepted as truth while information presented in the

predicate is considered new and listeners or readers rarely credit it as fact. By so doing, the

author or speaker consciously or unconsciously impinges on readers or listeners’

interpretation of the presented information, establishing a favorable or unfavorable bias

throughout the text. That’s why studying presupposition in media’s use of language is of

paramount importance. This notion provides the grounds for this study which is aimed at

broadcast news discourse.

In an earlier study, Bonyadi & Samuel (2011) investigated the linguistic nature of

presupposition in English editorials considered as written discourse. Surprisingly, the results

of his study reveal that editorial writers use some specific linguistic constructions to

communicate certain unstated information. Even though, it is not clear whether they do this

Page 6: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 739

consciously or unconsciously, the tip in point is that presupposition is considered one of the

properties of editorials. With this background, this paper aims to investigate whether

presupposition is employed in news transcripts broadcasted in oral form by two satellite news

channels i.e. PressTV and CNN as two samples of Persian and American English news

channels. As a matter of fact, this study is twofold. First it is to reveal whether presupposition

is used in oral discourse of news transcripts. Additionally, it is aimed to investigate if there is

any difference between Persian and American varieties of English in their use of

presupposition. If so, what are the linguistic constructions or more specifically presupposition

triggers which are frequently used in them?

3. Methodology

3.1 Procedure

To perform this inquiry, PressTV and CNN were chosen as two samples of news channels,

with PressTV representing a Persian variety of oral English and CNN representing an

American variety. Then a clustered sampling of 40 transcripts of news stories taken from

their websites at www.presstv.com and www.cnn.com was done. These transcripts which

include 20 from PressTV and 20 from CNN news channels were singled out without taking

into consideration the principle of random selection. Afterwards, they were subjected to

discourse analysis in terms of utilized presupposition categories. Based on the presupposition

trigger classification put forward by Yule (2010), the frequency and percent of the occurrence

of presupposition triggers were enumerated and tabulated. It’s worth mentioning that

presupposition triggers spotted in the transcripts were tested by using constancy under

negation rule. Ultimately, the most and the least frequently utilized presupposition triggers in

the discourse of the two were identified and compared.

4. Conclusion

4.1 Results

As mentioned earlier, following Yule’s (2010) proposal, this paper classifies the

presupposition triggers detected in the transcripts under the rubrics of existential, factive,

lexical, structural, non-factive, counter-factual, adverbial, and relative, with adverbial and

relative categories added to the main classification. Tables 1, and 2 show the occurrence

frequency of each presupposition trigger in the transcripts.

Page 7: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 740

Table 1. Presupposition triggers identified in PressTV’s transcripts

Transcript Existential Factive Lexical Structural Non-Factive Counter-Factual Adverbial Relative

1. 15 - 5 - - - 1 5

2. 20 1 2 - 1 - 2 5

3. 19 3 6 - - - - 6

4. 20 2 5 - 2 - - 12

5. 15 5 7 - 1 - 1 6

6. 15 3 1 - 1 - - 2

7. 25 2 2 - 1 - - 7

8. 11 - 6 - 1 - - -

9. 10 2 7 - - - 4 6

10. 23 2 3 - - - 3 8

11. 7 3 6 - - - 2 1

12. 9 4 4 - 3 - - 4

13. 10 2 4 3 1 - 1 2

14. 12 1 5 - - - 1 1

15. 17 1 9 - - - 3 6

16. 23 - 5 - 1 - 3 4

17. 21 2 10 - 5 - 2 -

18. 11 1 4 - 1 - 2 3

19. 10 - 3 - - - 1 3

20. 11 3 - - 1 - - 3

Sum 304 37 94 3 19 - 26 84

As indicated by the table, existential (N=304) or presupposition through nominalization and

possessive construction is the most frequently used linguistic construction to spark off

intended meaning in PressTV’s transcripts. Using existential presupposition, as Yule (2010)

maintains, the speaker and hearer are committed to the existence of entities. Lexical (N=94)

and relative (N=84) are the next favored tools of triggering presupposed proposition. Lexical

presupposition might be thought of as one of the best ways to express implicit proposition.

Due to its non-assertive function, lexical construction can best trigger meaning. Factive

(N=37), adverbial (N=26), and non-factive (N=19) linguistic devices have also been

employed to presuppose listeners’ minds toward certain intended meaning. It’s quite

interesting to note that structural construction has been put into service only three times in 20

transcripts. Structural or structurally-based constructions are “subtle ways of expressing

information that the speaker believes to be what the listener should believe”, as Yule (2010)

puts it (p, 29). Accordingly, using them can serve the purpose of making the listener believe

what the speaker is putting in a wh-format statement. To the surprise of the author,

counter-factual has not been employed in PressTV’s transcripts.

Page 8: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 741

Table 2. Presupposition triggers identified in CNN’s transcripts

Transcript Existential Factive Lexical Structural Non-Factive Counter-Factual Adverbial Relative

1. 14 - 2 - - - 3 4

2. 19 - 4 - - 1 1 9

3. 19 5 9 - - - 1 5

4. 8 1 - - - - - 3

5. 15 1 2 - - - - 4

6. 10 12 3 - 2 - 1 7

7. 9 5 3 - - - 2 7

8. 6 1 - - - - 3 5

9. 13 2 3 - - - 5 5

10. 8 1 1 - - - 1 2

11. 4 2 1 - 6 - - 5

12. 5 1 2 - 3 - - 2

13. 15 - 1 - 2 - 6 3

14. 18 3 4 - - - - 8

15. 6 2 5 - - - 3 6

16. 10 3 3 1 - - 2 7

17. 8 1 5 - - - 2 4

18. 11 2 1 - - - 4 6

19. 10 2 6 - - - 1 6

20. 11 - - 1 2 - 1 7

Sum 219 44 55 2 15 1 36 105

As table 2 indicates, existential construction (N=219) is the most frequently occurring

category of presupposition in CNN’s transcripts. Relative (N=105), lexical (N=55), and

factive constructions (N=44) are also preferred in sparking off unstated meaning.

Furthermore, adverbial clauses (N=36) are among frequently employed presupposition

triggers. The least frequently used presupposition triggers include non-factive, structural, and

counter-factual categories with frequencies of 15, 2, and 1 respectively.

Table 3. Comparison of presupposition triggers identified in PressTV and CNN’s transcripts

- Existential Factive Lexical Structural Non-Factive Counter-Factual Adverbial Relative

PressTV 304 37 94 3 19 - 26 84

CNN 219 44 55 2 15 1 36 105

As table 3 shows, the analysis of PressTV and CNN’s transcripts does not show drastic

difference in Persian and American English use of presupposition triggers. Surprisingly they

share the same properties with slight variations in their frequency. Existential trigger, as the

table reveals, is the most frequently used presupposition in the transcripts of both varieties.

Moreover, it should be noted that PressTV writers are more predisposed to use lexical

presuppositions while CNN writers are inclined to employ relative clauses. The least

frequently used presuppositions are reported to be the same in both varieties of oral English.

Concerning existential trigger, it can be concluded that existential presupposition, due to its

simple structure, is the easiest tool at writers’ disposal to give information readily credited for

by the listeners. Moreover, it seems that there is an appeal among the writers to prefer factive

to non-factive presuppositions. As a matter of fact, news discourse writers tend to give a

sense of certainty to the propositions instead of presupposing information which is not true.

Page 9: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 742

In addition, their use of certain words and phrases for triggering non-asserted information

described as lexical presupposition is of high frequency which can be attributed to their

intention not to mention every piece of information. The frequency of adverbial and relative

presuppositions also indicates their importance in oral discourse. In fact, adverbial and

relative clauses can be considered sound textual devices in that they enable the writer to make

listener believe what s/he asserts.

4.2 Discussion

Analysis of the chosen transcripts from the two English news channels reveals that both

varieties of oral English, namely American and Persian, do put into service presupposition

triggers. Using these linguistic constructions, their authors impinge on listeners’

interpretation of facts and events. Schmid (2001) notes that discourse writers share their

views by presenting them disguised as truths in presuppositions.

Further inquiry shows that existential presupposition being the most frequently used category

is a constant property of news discourse. With its simple structure composed of possessive

constructions or definite noun phrases, existential presupposition is considered the most

readily credited for presupposition. Schmid (2001) also asserts that “people are more

likely to object to the propositional content of that-clause that is

represented as necessarily true than to the attitudinal meaning of the noun”

(p, 154). As a matter of fact, existential presupposition is stronger or more difficult to detect

in comparison to other categories. This can be ascribed to its ability in diverting attention to

other parts of the sentence. Schmid and Caffi are among the scholars who strongly stress that

existential presupposition is one of the least refutable presuppositions ever used. Interestingly,

in an earlier study of written news discourse, Bonyadi & Samuel (2011) concluded that

existential or presupposition through nominalization is among the most frequently used

presupposition triggers.

Another important result the study yields is the writers’ predisposition to use more factive

triggers than non-factive ones. As table 3 indicates, in both groups of transcripts factive

presuppositions are more frequently used than non-factive ones. By so doing, writers add a

sense of certainty to the propositions. On the contrary, Bonyadi & Samuel (2011) concluded

that written news discourse enjoys the use of non-factive presuppositions more than factive

ones. Accordingly, it can be concluded that factive and non-factive presuppositions are

respectively preferred by oral and written news discourse writers.

Compared to the results of the study by Bonyadi & Samuel (2011), this study also concludes

that lexical presupposition is more frequent in oral news discourse than in written form of

news. As a matter of fact presupposing unasserted proposition does not seem to intrigue

scriptwriters of written news genre.

In sum, as Levinson (1983) notes, the detected presupposition triggers confirm the idea that

propositions are triggered by parallel linguistic structures in different languages or varieties

of languages. However, some difference might be witnessed in their frequency of use which

can be attributed to writers’ different attitudes toward certain linguistic constructions.

Page 10: Presupposition trigger a comparative analysis of broadcast news discourse

International Journal of Linguistics

ISSN 1948-5425

2012, Vol. 4, No. 3

www.macrothink.org/ijl 743

References

Bonyadi, A., & Samuel, M. (2011). Linguistic nature of presupposition in American and

Persian newspaper editorials. International Journal of Linguistics, 3, 1-16.

Dryer, M. S. (1996). Focus, pragmatic presupposition, and activated propositions. Journal of

Pragmatics, 26, 475-523. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(95)00059-3.

Fairclough, N. (1995). Critical discourse analysis. London: Longman.

Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Richardson, J. E. (2007). Analyzing newspapers: An approach from critical discourse

analysis. NY: Palgrave, Macmillan.

Schmid, H. J. (2001). Presupposition can be a bluff. Journal of Pragmatics, 33, 152-173.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(01)00027-3.

Verschueren, J. (1978). Reflections on presupposition failure: A contribution to an integrated

theory of pragmatics. Journal of Pragmatics, 2, 107-152.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(78)90009-7.

Werth, P. (1993). Accommodation and the myth of presupposition: The view from discourse.

Lingua, 89, 39-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(93)90039-Y.

Widdowson, H. G. (2000). On the limitations of linguistics applied. Applied Linguistics, 21,

3-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/applin/21.1.3.

Yule, G. (2010). Pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511757754.