10
Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan William J. Chopik Michigan State University Aging happens to everyone everywhere. At present, however, little is known about whether life-span adult development—and particularly development in late adulthood—is pancultural or culture-bound. Here, we propose that in Western cultural contexts, individuals are encouraged to maintain the active, positive, and independent self. This cultural expectation continues even in late adulthood, thus leading to a mismatch between aspirations to live up to the cultural expectation and the reality of aging. This mismatch is potentially alienating. In contrast, in Asian cultural contexts, a critical task throughout life is to achieve attunement with age-graded social roles. This ideal may be more attainable even in late adulthood. Our review of existent evidence lends support to this analysis. Specifically, in late adulthood, Americans showed a robust psychological bias toward high-arousal positive (vs. negative) emotions. This positivity, however, concealed a somber aspect of aging that manifested itself in more demanding realms of life. Thus, Americans in late adulthood also showed marked declines in certain desirable personality traits (e.g., extraversion and conscientiousness) and some aspects of the meaning in life (e.g., personal growth and purpose in life). None of these effects were apparent among East Asians. The current work underscores a need to extend research on life-span development beyond Western populations. Public Significance Statement Well-being in the last decade of life is increasingly important as the aging population grows. Our work illuminates the critical role of culture in affording opportunities for healthy aging. Keywords: adult development, culture, aging, meaning in life Around 1970, Bob Dylan composed a song called “For- ever Young.” He did so as a lullaby for one of his sons. In this beautiful tune, Dylan prays for his son’s happiness, success, and most important, for him to stay forever young. In writing this song, Dylan also underscores a central tenet of American culture. America is a country for those who are independent, active, and positive. These features would require youthful energy and enthusiasm—thus, a cultural imperative of staying “forever young.” Ever since Dylan sang this song, the demographics of the United States have changed. Every newborn is now ex- pected to live up to 80 years of age. With the rapidly declining birth rate, the median age for Americans, which was 30.2 in 1955, has steadily gone up, reaching 37.5 in 2015. The average American is no longer “young,” and the number of older adults is expected to double in the next 30 years (United Nations, 2017). How will Americans handle the cultural imperative and expectation of staying positively enthusiastic and energetic, or metaphorically, forever young, when they are not in fact young anymore? Might this task be unattainable for many of them, which could lead to some degree of alienation and disengagement? Other countries are graying even more quickly. For ex- ample, the median age of Japan was 23.6 and 46.7 in 1955 and 2015, respectively (United Nations, 2017). By looking into the life-span trajectory of well-being and health in such countries, aging Americans might be able to learn some lessons as they themselves age. Aside from such practical benefits, the effort of examining health and well-being in other countries will inform theories of psychology more Editor’s note. This article is part of a special issue, “Rethinking Adult Development: New Ideas for New Times,” published in the May–June 2020 issue of American Psychologist. Jeffrey Jensen Arnett, Margie E. Lachman, and Oliver Robinson served as guest editors of the special issue with Nancy Eisenberg as advisory editor. Authors’ note. X Shinobu Kitayama and X Martha K. Berg, Depart- ment of Psychology, University of Michigan; William J. Chopik, Depart- ment of Psychology, Michigan State University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shinobu Kitayama, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. E-mail: [email protected] This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. American Psychologist © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 75, No. 4, 567–576 ISSN: 0003-066X http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000614 567

Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    7

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence

Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. BergUniversity of Michigan

William J. ChopikMichigan State University

Aging happens to everyone everywhere. At present, however, little is known about whetherlife-span adult development—and particularly development in late adulthood—is panculturalor culture-bound. Here, we propose that in Western cultural contexts, individuals areencouraged to maintain the active, positive, and independent self. This cultural expectationcontinues even in late adulthood, thus leading to a mismatch between aspirations to live upto the cultural expectation and the reality of aging. This mismatch is potentially alienating. Incontrast, in Asian cultural contexts, a critical task throughout life is to achieve attunementwith age-graded social roles. This ideal may be more attainable even in late adulthood. Ourreview of existent evidence lends support to this analysis. Specifically, in late adulthood,Americans showed a robust psychological bias toward high-arousal positive (vs. negative)emotions. This positivity, however, concealed a somber aspect of aging that manifested itselfin more demanding realms of life. Thus, Americans in late adulthood also showed markeddeclines in certain desirable personality traits (e.g., extraversion and conscientiousness) andsome aspects of the meaning in life (e.g., personal growth and purpose in life). None of theseeffects were apparent among East Asians. The current work underscores a need to extendresearch on life-span development beyond Western populations.

Public Significance StatementWell-being in the last decade of life is increasingly important as the aging population grows. Ourwork illuminates the critical role of culture in affording opportunities for healthy aging.

Keywords: adult development, culture, aging, meaning in life

Around 1970, Bob Dylan composed a song called “For-ever Young.” He did so as a lullaby for one of his sons. Inthis beautiful tune, Dylan prays for his son’s happiness,success, and most important, for him to stay forever young.In writing this song, Dylan also underscores a central tenetof American culture. America is a country for those who areindependent, active, and positive. These features wouldrequire youthful energy and enthusiasm—thus, a culturalimperative of staying “forever young.”

Ever since Dylan sang this song, the demographics of theUnited States have changed. Every newborn is now ex-pected to live up to 80 years of age. With the rapidlydeclining birth rate, the median age for Americans, whichwas 30.2 in 1955, has steadily gone up, reaching 37.5 in2015. The average American is no longer “young,” and thenumber of older adults is expected to double in the next 30years (United Nations, 2017). How will Americans handlethe cultural imperative and expectation of staying positivelyenthusiastic and energetic, or metaphorically, foreveryoung, when they are not in fact young anymore? Might thistask be unattainable for many of them, which could lead tosome degree of alienation and disengagement?

Other countries are graying even more quickly. For ex-ample, the median age of Japan was 23.6 and 46.7 in 1955and 2015, respectively (United Nations, 2017). By lookinginto the life-span trajectory of well-being and health in suchcountries, aging Americans might be able to learn somelessons as they themselves age. Aside from such practicalbenefits, the effort of examining health and well-being inother countries will inform theories of psychology more

Editor’s note. This article is part of a special issue, “Rethinking AdultDevelopment: New Ideas for New Times,” published in the May–June2020 issue of American Psychologist. Jeffrey Jensen Arnett, Margie E.Lachman, and Oliver Robinson served as guest editors of the special issuewith Nancy Eisenberg as advisory editor.

Authors’ note. X Shinobu Kitayama and X Martha K. Berg, Depart-ment of Psychology, University of Michigan; William J. Chopik, Depart-ment of Psychology, Michigan State University.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to ShinobuKitayama, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 ChurchStreet, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

American Psychologist© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 75, No. 4, 567–576ISSN: 0003-066X http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000614

567

Page 2: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

broadly. The field must start examining aging and life-spandevelopment with a broader lens.

The current article builds on an early analysis of cultureand cognitive aging (Kitayama, 2000) and proposes that theAmerican ideal of preserving youth can extend to lateadulthood, which gives rise to an important mismatch be-tween personal aspirations to stay enthusiastic, energetic, or“youthful,” and the reality of aging. We submit that thismismatch might engender certain difficulties to stay en-gaged in society and culture. In contrast, in East Asiancultures, there is a strong emphasis on adjustment to age-graded roles and tasks, which might protect Asian olderadults against the difficulties faced by their European Amer-ican counterparts.

Culture and Psychological Processes

Independent Model of the Self

Our analysis is based on a view that culture’s meaningsand practices are organized by the norms and values thatconstitute cross-culturally divergent models of the self. InWestern societies today in general, but in North Americanmiddle-class culture in particular, there is a strong valueplaced on the independence of the self (Markus & Ki-tayama, 1991). The self is seen as separated from other suchselves. In this model, social relations are typically seen aspersonally chosen and, hence, as derived from the indepen-dence of the self and the personal autonomy it entails. Thus,dominant moral frameworks place a strong emphasis oneach individual’s rights, which are often seen as “God-

given.” Correspondingly, social duties and obligations areseen as a matter of individual choice.

This moral landscape that surrounds the independent self isreinforced by a number of life tasks made available in Westernsocieties today (Kitayama, Park, Sevincer, Karasawa, &Uskul, 2009). Independent cultures provide various goalstates, such as uniqueness, control, influence, and self-reliance. In each case, there exists a clearly defined set ofbehaviors that are needed to accomplish such goals. Forexample, to influence others requires a strong sense ofagency, paired with enthusiasm about the value of one’sopinions and judgments (Tsai, Miao, Seppala, Fung, &Yeung, 2007). These tasks require the very qualities—agency, enthusiasm, and positivity—that metaphorically de-fine youthfulness.

Interdependent Model of the Self

In non-Western societies today in general, but in EastAsian culture in particular, there is a contrasting view of theself as interdependent with others in significant social units(Markus & Kitayama, 1991). The self is conceptualized asinherently connected with the others. In this model, socialrelations are the primary context in which the self is defined.For example, interests, goals, and attitudes that are basedonly on personal considerations are seen as secondary atbest. The focus on these efforts may be perceived as bothimmature and childish. Instead, the self is defined by socialroles and the duties and obligations defined therein.

The model of the self as interdependent is reinforced bya number of life tasks available in Eastern societies today(Kitayama et al., 2009). Culture provides various goal statessuch as similarity with others, fitting in, adjustment, mutualreliance, and sympathy. These tasks require an agent (or theself) that actively adjusts to significant others while exer-cising due moderation on emotions and motivations. Thisorientation results in, for example, a broad cognitive scopeand the value placed on low-arousal emotions. Moreover,within this view, life stages are defined by social roles,which are typically age-graded (Arnett, 2016; Kitayama,2000). Throughout, the self is expected to adjust peacefully,and perhaps calmly, to fulfill the roles prototypical of eachlife stage.

Cultural Variation in Psychological Processes

Evidence is growing that the divergent cultural systemsdiscussed above are reflected in the habitual mode of psy-chological operation (Kitayama & Uskul, 2011; Markus &Kitayama, 1991). For example, independent people (e.g.,European Americans) are more focused on personal goalsand thus “know” what to look at, resulting in focusedattention. In contrast, interdependent people (e.g., EastAsians) are more attuned to social expectations and norms,

ShinobuKitayama

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

568 KITAYAMA, BERG, AND CHOPIK

Page 3: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

leading to holistic attention (Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Noren-zayan, 2001). Further, sources of happiness vary accord-ingly. Happiness is unequivocally positive and personal forAmericans. Japanese, however, recognize more clearly thesignificance of social relations for happiness (Kitayama etal., 2009). Most important, the motivations of influencingand adjusting (Morling, Kitayama, & Miyamoto, 2002),called primary and secondary control, respectively (Heck-hausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, 2010), are present in all cultures.However, cultures vary in the relative weight given to oneor the other (Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984). EastAsians are more accommodating to social expectations and

norms, whereas European Americans may be more insistenton their personal goals and values.

Life-Span Development in Japan and theUnited States

At the outset, it should be made clear that aging is oftenpainful and typically seen negatively in all known societies(North & Fiske, 2015). Indeed, a subtle reminder of agingcan cause stereotype threat among European American andAsian older adults alike (Barber, Seliger, Yeh, & Tan, 2019;Tan & Barber, 2018). Nevertheless, negative stereotypes ofolder adults could motivate people to offer help and otherpositive responses to them (Luong, Charles, & Fingerman,2011). Moreover, especially in Asian societies, older adultsmay be protected from the negative stereotypes of aging inanother way. Society may have relatively more benignviews of aging (Ackerman & Chopik, 2020; Löckenhoff etal., 2009), in part because of traditional views of aging in,for example, Confucianism and Buddhism, which promoteactive adjustment of expectations and aspirations in age-graded fashion (Bedford & Yeh, 2019). Thus, the vigor andenthusiasm emphasized during early adulthood may no lon-ger be positively sanctioned during late adulthood. Suchage-graded views of developmental stages are still dominantin East Asian cultures (Tan & Barber, 2018).

With this backdrop, we now articulate our hypothesis. InEuropean American contexts (see Figure 1A), there is astrong emphasis on the independent self. The notion ofindependence in these contexts has been historically elabo-rated to promote personal initiatives, an effort to influenceothers as well as the surrounding situations, and positive,enthusiastic, future-oriented pursuit of one’s own goals andpersonal agenda. All of these tasks would require positivity,energy, and other high-arousal psychological states (Sims,

Martha K. Berg

Independent self Posi�ve enthusiasm and “youthfulness” stronglyvalued

Interdependent self Adjustment to age-graded roles strongly valued

Dissonance with age-related decline

Resonance with age-related decline

Rela�vely nega�ve personal prospect

Declines of posi�ve personality traits andthe meaning in life

Rela�vely posi�ve personal prospect

No decline of posi�ve personality traits or the meaning in life

A. European American aging dynamics

B. East Asian aging dynamics

Figure 1. Schematic models of aging in two cultural contexts: Panel A: In European American contexts, thereis a strong push toward positivity and enthusiasm, which results in conflicts and frustration over age-relateddecline, which in turn leads eventually to alienation and disengagement. Panel B: In East Asian contexts, thereis a strong push toward adjustment to age-graded roles and tasks, which results in relatively few conflicts withage-related decline, which in turn leads eventually to new meanings and engagement. See the online article forthe color version of this figure.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

569CULTURE AND WELL-BEING IN LATE ADULTHOOD

Page 4: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

Tsai, Jiang, Wang, Fung, & Zhang, 2015). Moreover, theAmerican mainstream culture has never articulated age-graded social roles and tasks for older adults in ways othercultural traditions such as Buddhism and Confucianismhave (Arnett, 2016; Kitayama, 2000). Hence, the emphasison positive energy and vigor may extend over to late adult-hood.1 We argue that there will arise substantial discordanceor dissonance between the aspiration to be independent inculturally prescribed (i.e., vigorous and energetic) fashionand the inevitable age-related decline. Accordingly, the U.S.cultural imperative of staying positively enthusiastic or vig-orous may present a great challenge for many older adults.Moreover, the cultural practices are tailored to require suchvigor and energy, and as a consequence, it may also becomechallenging to sustain new prospects in life in culturallyprescribed terms during late adulthood. Due to this chal-lenge, it may also be hard to maintain some desirable stylesof personally engaging in society, such as conscientiousnessand extraversion. All this may eventually lead to passivity,alienation, and disengagement from social life.

Although sharing negative stereotypes of older adults,East Asian contexts seem to present a distinctly differentoutlook for older adults (see Figure 1B). The interdependentview of the self, positively sanctioned in these contexts, hashistorically been elaborated in family-based terms (Bedford& Yeh, 2019). In particular, there is an emphasis on filialpiety that involves deference toward older adults in theirfamilies or communities. On their part, older adults may bemotivated to actively adjust to age-graded social roles thatare offered by culture, for example, by the Confucian modelthat specifies different ideal states for different stages of life(Arnett, 2016; Bedford & Yeh, 2019; Kitayama, 2000).

There will result a greater resonance between age-gradedsocial roles and the inevitable mental and physical decline.Hence, East Asian older adults may be better able to adjustwhat they want to think and feel to the societal expectationsand norms of aging. Their thought, desire, and action maybe more closely attuned to the socially prescribed agingroles. It may therefore be more realistic to craft some newgoals, pleasures, and most of all, meanings in life duringolder adulthood in East Asian societies than in EuropeanAmerican societies. To examine this broad possibility, weaddress five topics, (a) emotional norms, (b) positivity inemotional experience, (c) personal prospects of aging, (d)personality traits, and (e) meaning in life.

Norms and Desire for Highly ArousingPositive Emotions

One central premise of our theoretical model (see Figure1) is that the social norms and personal desire for highlyarousing positive emotions such as enthusiasm and excite-ment may remain quite strong even in late adulthood amongEuropean Americans. This age-related trajectory is unlikelyin cultures where adjustment to age-related tasks and rolesis strongly valued.

In recent work, Tsai and colleagues (2018) examined thedegree to which people want to feel high-arousal (vs. low-arousal) positive emotions, such as excitement, enthusiasm,and joy. They thus tested the subjective norms and desiresfor these emotions as reported by both European Americanand Chinese adults of a wide age range. As illustrated inFigure 2, for European Americans, the norms and desire forhigh-arousal positive emotions are quite strong throughoutthe life course, including late adulthood. In contrast, bothChinese Americans and Chinese in Hong Kong showed anage-graded adjustment. That is, the norms for high-arousalpositive emotions were quite strong at relatively youngadulthood (up to approximately 40 years old), but towardlate adulthood, these emotions declined precipitously.

Positivity Bias in Emotional Experience

The Tsai et al. (2018) evidence suggests that the normsand desire for intense emotional positivity remain strong inlate adulthood among European Americans. Among EastAsians, however, the norms and desire for intense positivitydecrease in late adulthood. Might this cultural difference bereflected in the actual emotional experience?

1 Given this, the positive energy may be linked to “youth,” and it wouldbe of interest to ask the question whether the link between the positiveenergy and youth is culturally acknowledged or elaborated. For the currentpurposes, however, this question is beside the point. We argue that in U.S.culture, culture’s resources (e.g., beliefs and practices) are set up in such away that they require positive energy and enthusiasm. This feature of thecultural resources poses challenges to those who are not fully capable ofmaintaining this psychological state, as is likely to be true for many (if notall) older adults.

William J.Chopik

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

570 KITAYAMA, BERG, AND CHOPIK

Page 5: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

In a recent study, both Americans and Japanese whovaried in age reported on how strongly they experiencedvarious emotions in 10 different emotionally ambiguoussituations (Grossmann, Karasawa, Kan, & Kitayama, 2014).Figure 3 plots the reported intensity of positive and negative

emotions as a function of age. Across cultures, positiveemotions are reportedly experienced more strongly thannegative emotions are. This valence effect could be due tothe situations used in the study being relatively more pleas-ant or positive. Of importance, however, the intensity ofpositive (in comparison to negative) emotions increasedsteadily as a function of age among Americans, but no suchtrend was evident among Japanese. Similar cross-culturalvariation in the positivity in late adulthood has been ob-served with measures of subjective well-being (Nakagawaet al., 2018; Pethtel & Chen, 2010). Moreover, using atten-tion to emotional stimuli as the dependent variable, similarcross-cultural variations in the psychological bias towardpositive (vs. negative) emotions have been obtained (Fung,Isaacowitz, Lu, & Li, 2010; Fung et al., 2008).

Why might Americans become relatively more positive asa function of age? The personal quest for emotional posi-tivity may exist throughout the life course in Americanculture. However, as proposed by socioemotional sensitivitytheory (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999), this questfor positivity is much constrained by many competing lifedemands when one works, raises children, or advances in acareer. By late adulthood, however, these life demands oftendissipate, leaving individuals unfettered to maximize theirpersonal well-being—in other words, to chase the Americannorm of positivity and enthusiasm. This active effort to liveup to the cultural imperative may contain a contradiction initself, insofar as both physical vigor and the level of mentalenergy may decrease for many (if not all) older adults. In

Figure 2. Norms and desire for highly arousing positive emotions (e.g.,enthusiasm, excitement, joy) over the life course for European Americans,Chinese Americans, and Hong Kong Chinese. Gray areas represent stan-dard errors. HAP � high-arousal positive emotions. �� p � .01. ��� p �.001. From “Valuing Excitement Makes People Look Forward to Old AgeLess and Dread It More,” by J. L. Tsai, T. Sims, Y. Qu, E. Thomas, D.Jiang, and H. H. Fung, 2018, Psychology and Aging, 33, pp. 975–992.Copyright 2018 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted withpermission.

Figure 3. Life-span trajectory in the psychological bias favoring positive (vs. negative) emotional experience.Gray areas represent standard errors. From “A Cultural Perspective on Emotional Experiences Across the LifeSpan,” by I. Grossmann, M. Karasawa, C. Kan, and S. Kitayama, 2014, Emotion, 14, pp. 679–692. Copyright2014 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

571CULTURE AND WELL-BEING IN LATE ADULTHOOD

Page 6: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

contrast, in East Asian cultures, there may be a strongcultural imperative of adjusting to age-related roles. Theymay therefore find ways to fit into such new roles and newplaces in life. Their emotional positivity may therefore notincrease.

Challenges of Being “Enthusiastic”in Late Adulthood

At first glance, the outlook of the American older agewould seem quite positive (Hong, Charles, Lee, & Lach-man, 2019; Nakagawa et al., 2018; Pethtel & Chen, 2010).This observation is consistent with the socioemotional se-lectivity theory, which proposes that older adults prioritizethe goal of emotional positivity. Thus, older Americans maysometimes actively select social networks to connect to soas to maximize the chances of achieving this goal (Luong etal., 2011). At the same time, however, emotional positivityis also relatively easy to attain by reappraising situations inrose-colored terms or merely ignoring inconvenient sides oftheir experience. Indeed, the Grossmann et al. (2014) studydid not involve any active selection of social networks. Itmerely tapped psychological reactions to emotional scenar-ios. Hence, the emotional positivity among American olderadults may reflect the effort of feeling positive by somepsychological means. It therefore may be easily attainableeven when mental and physical vigor becomes more chal-lenging to sustain. Accordingly, one might wonder whetherthe hedonism apparent in late adulthood among EuropeanAmericans might conceal aging-linked difficulties, whichcould show up in more demanding tasks of living life incertain styles to remain, say, “extraverted” or “purposeful.”

Imagine someone was extraverted. The person may havefrequented parties and enjoyed conversations. The ambientnoise did not bother him. In late adulthood, however, hemay well find it increasingly difficult to do the same. Doingso requires the energy, vigor, and health of youth, to saynothing about the acuity in hearing. And the problem willnot end at the party. It will go on at work as well as at home.Gradually then, many European American older adults mayshow less positive energy (extraversion). For similar rea-sons, they may lose the habit of hard work (conscientious-ness) or curiosity (openness). Furthermore, this processmight also make it hard to sustain some aspects of meaningin life. Imagine another older adult who always tried every-thing to “personally grow” when she was young. She reg-ularly attended yoga lessons. She jogged. She may also havemade it a priority to read numerous books across a varietyof genres. She had a “purpose in life.” As she ages, how-ever, it will become increasingly challenging to maintainthe old regimens. Inevitably, the meaning in life (personalgrowth or purpose in this case) may suffer.

The difficulty the older adults face in the examples abovemay result because European American culture neither elab-

orates nor positively sanctions tasks or norms that are spe-cially tailored for older adults (Arnett, 2016; Kitayama,2000; Tsai et al., 2018). Hence, many older adults may endup trying to do what they used to be doing at work, atleisure, or in their relationship with friends and familymembers. They may therefore find it increasingly hard tomaintain the behavioral routines they have taken forgranted. In this regard, Asian older adults may be different.Asian societies provide people with age-graded tasks androles (Arnett, 2016; Kitayama, 2000). For example, theremay be culturally sanctioned ways of being, say, extravertedor conscientious, in an age-dependent fashion. Correspond-ingly, achievement at work or vigorous exercise may nolonger be encouraged or appreciated. These activities, seenas appropriate and praiseworthy while one is relativelyyoung, may no longer be regarded as such in late adulthood.They must be replaced, for example, with the care-taking ofgrandchildren, much lighter physical therapy sessions, andthe like. Asian cultures may delineate such age-adjustedregiments for positive engagement. Likewise, there may begreater culture-level understanding that the ways to grow orto be in charge of work or family matters could differ,depending on life stages. The culture may then providepeople with age-appropriate ways for, say, personal growthor being purposeful. Recall many Asians willingly adjust tothe age-graded roles. To the extent that they do, they mayfind new ways of engagement in late adulthood, which maygive rise to new reasons to live.

Personal views of aging. The Tsai et al. (2018) studythat tested the norms and desire for high-arousal positiveemotions (see Figure 2) also probed personal views of oldage. The participants were asked to list things they “arelooking forward to about being 75 or older” and those they“are dreading about being 75 or older.” As shown in Figure4, European Americans show no change across the life span.On average, they were as likely to list positive views asnegative views. However, both Chinese Americans andHong Kong Chinese were different. They were increasinglymore likely to list positive (vs. negative) views of aging asthey got older. Thus, older Chinese and Chinese Americansseem to cultivate more positive prospects for themselves,but this effect is missing in older Americans.

Tsai et al. (2018) interpreted this finding by referring toadjustment to age-graded norms for emotion among EastAsians. East Asians adjust their expectations as they getolder. They thus no longer want to experience excitement,enthusiasm, and the like as much as they used to while theywere young. Because of this age-graded adjustment, theytend to find new things to do, new plans to try, and newmeanings to create in late adulthood. Conversely, EuropeanAmerican older adults tend to endorse the norms of stayingenthusiastic and excited. This commitment to the norms andvalues of vigorous independence will make it difficult toadjust their aspirations and motivations to the reality of

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

572 KITAYAMA, BERG, AND CHOPIK

Page 7: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

physical and mental decline. In support of this analysis, theage-linked increase of positive personal views of aging,more evident among Chinese and Chinese Americans (seeFigure 4), was mediated by the age-graded adjustment of thenorms and desire for highly arousing positive emotions (seeFigure 2).

Personality traits. Some traits, such as conscientious-ness and openness, carry positive connotations across many(if not all) contexts. Will people find it hard to maintain thelevel of such positively valenced personality traits in lateadulthood, and if so, will this difficulty be more apparent inEuropean Americans than in East Asians? Chopik and Ki-

tayama (2018) relied on the longitudinal portion of theMIDUS/MIDJA data set to investigate changes in person-ality over the life span. The Big Five global traits (i.e.,agreeableness, conscientiousness, extraversion, neuroti-cism, and openness to experience) were assessed twice, withintervals of approximately five years in Japan and 10 yearsin the United States. The researchers used information aboutlongitudinal changes of each trait for each participant andfitted linear, curvilinear, and/or quadratic functions to thechange data to estimate the best fitting trajectory of life-span change for the trait. In Figure 5, it is evident thatAmerican means are consistently higher than Japanesemeans regardless of trait or age. It is possible that U.S.culture promotes stronger personalities than Japanese cul-ture does, consistent with a view that the ethos of indepen-dence fosters more clear-cut, disambiguated self-concepts(Campbell et al., 1996). Another possibility is a responsebias toward positivity known to be far stronger in the UnitedStates than in East Asia (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Ki-tayama, 1999). With this massive cultural difference setaside, it is also clear that the life-course trajectories betweenthe two cultural groups were remarkably different for fourof the five traits.

First, conscientiousness showed statistically significantcurvilinear trajectories in both cultures. But the exact shapeof the function was markedly different, resulting in a sig-nificant interaction between the curvilinear term and cul-ture. Japanese showed a drop of conscientiousness inmidlife. The trait gradually increased, however, toward lateadulthood. In contrast, the curvilinear function of Ameri-cans was equally highly significant, but the shape of thefunction was very different. The level of conscientiousnesspeaked in middle adulthood, after which it steadily de-creased toward late adulthood. Thus, a decline of conscien-tiousness in late adulthood is evident. Second, neuroticism

Figure 4. Personal views of aging over life course for European Amer-icans, Chinese Americans, and Hong Kong Chinese. Gray areas representstandard errors. �p � .05. ��� p � .001. From “Valuing Excitement MakesPeople Look Forward to Old Age Less and Dread It More,” by J. L. Tsai,T. Sims, Y. Qu, E. Thomas, D. Jiang, and H. H. Fung, 2018, Psychologyand Aging, 33, pp. 975–992. Copyright 2018 by the American Psycholog-ical Association. Adapted with permission.

Figure 5. Life-span trajectory in four global personality traits: Conscientiousness, neuroticism, extraversion,and openness to experience. From “Personality Change Across the Life Span: Insights From a Cross-Cultural,Longitudinal Study,” by W. J. Chopik and S. Kitayama, 2018, Journal of Personality, 86, pp. 508–521.Copyright 2018 by John Wiley and Sons. Adapted with permission.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

573CULTURE AND WELL-BEING IN LATE ADULTHOOD

Page 8: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

showed a significant linear effect in both cultures, showing asteady decrease over the life course. However, this decreasewas significantly less pronounced among Americans thanamong Japanese. Third, extraversion (a positive trait) showeda strong quadratic function among Americans, with a sharpdrop apparent in late adulthood. This pattern was absentamong Japanese. Fourth, a similar pattern was evident inopenness to experience. Americans showed a statisticallysignificant quadratic effect, with a sharp drop in late adult-hood. However, Japanese did not show this drop, although,in this case, caution is warranted because the QuadraticTerm � Culture interaction did not reach statistical signif-icance.

The findings in Figure 5 are consistent with our theoret-ical model in Figure 1. The aspiration to stay positive,energetic, and young is very strong in Americans because ofcultural expectations of independence and primary control.In fact, American older adults seem to live up to the culturalstandard as long as doing so is less demanding and thus easyeven in the face of declining vigor and health, as may be thecase in the positivity in emotional experience (see Figure 3).Moreover, when it is hard to do so, they may well accom-modate to the reality of aging to some degree (Heckhausenet al., 2010). However, the cultural pull of primary controlis still strong (Weisz et al., 1984). Moreover, Americanculture does not seem to place an emphasis on age-gradedlife roles as much as East Asian traditions, such as Confu-cianism (e.g., “filial piety”) and Buddhism (e.g., “life cy-cle”), patently do (Arnett, 2016; Bedford & Yeh, 2019;Kitayama, 2000). As a consequence, many Americans maystill have strong aspirations to remain enthusiastic, excited,and positive even in late adulthood. Gradually then, due toinsufficient adjustment to the reality of aging, they may losea habit of hard work (conscientiousness) while showing lesspositive energy (extraversion) and curiosity (openness).This change may happen even while the older adults showpositivity in domains that are less taxing or demanding (e.g.,emotional experience; see Figure 3). Similar age trajectoriesof personality have been observed in Germany (Wagner,Ram, Smith, & Gerstorf, 2016). Japanese, in contrast, sup-posedly adjust their behaviors and habits of thought tonewly prescribed age-graded norms and roles, which couldenable them to find new age-proper ways of being consci-entious, extraverted, or perhaps curious and open.

Meaning in life. If the imperative of staying vigorousand positively enthusiastic alienates Americans in theirold age, it may also result in a loss of the meaning in life.Ryff and Keyes (1995) developed a well-validated scale thatassesses several facets of the meaning in life.2 These facetsinclude autonomy, environmental mastery, personal growth,positive relations with others, purpose in life, and self-acceptance. In combination, they are referred to as psycho-logical well-being. In a recent analysis of the MIDUS/MIDJA longitudinal data (Chopik, Berg, & Kitayama,

2019), we again used the longitudinal change data fromeach participant and modeled these data to identify the bestfitting age functions for each facet of the meaning in life.3

The results are summarized in Figure 6. As was the casein the analysis of personality, American means were con-sistently higher than Japanese means were. It is possible thatU.S. culture is affording more meanings than Japaneseculture does across the board, although a clear alternativecomes from a positivity bias that Americans (but not EastAsians) are known to exhibit (Heine et al., 1999). Of note,above and beyond the cultural variation in positivity, therewas a marked decline in personal growth and purpose in lifein late adulthood among Americans. This age-linked declineof the two meaning dimensions was not apparent amongJapanese. Further, in self-acceptance, one can note a clearsign of decline toward very late adulthood among Ameri-cans. However, Japanese showed increases around the sameage range. Last, but not least, there was one trend in lateadulthood that appeared more positive among Americansthan among Japanese. Positive relations with others in-creased throughout life among Americans—an effect thatwas not apparent in Japanese. We suspect that this Ameri-can effect is caused by an effort to select pleasant people intheir social circles (Carstensen et al., 1999). Given the highrelational mobility of American society (Thomson et al.,2018), this goal may be easily attainable even in late adult-hood. Thus, the effect evident in the positive relations couldbe more analogous to a hedonic effect seen earlier in ourdiscussion of the age-related increase of emotional positiv-ity among Americans (see Figure 3).

Regardless of the validity of our interpretation for the agetrend of the positive relation facet of the Ryff and Keyes(1995) scale, the overall weight of evidence suggests that inlate adulthood, Americans hold less positive personal viewsof aging (see Figure 4) and show declines of both positivelyvalenced personality traits (see Figure 5) and some dimen-sions of meaning in life (see Figure 6). In contrast, EastAsian older adults appear to develop more positive pros-pects for their life, maintain the level of desirable person-ality traits, and keep engaging meanings in life.

Limitations and Future Directions

Some limitations of the current work must be noted. First,we proposed that the cultural ethos of independence is linked

2 The meaning in life is sometimes equated with purpose in life (Frankl,2006). Here, however, we follow Charles Taylor, the Canadian philosopher(Taylor, 1989), and take a broader view that the meaning in life can resultfrom the symbolic means of orienting the self in the world. Purpose is onesuch means. But there may be others, including the dimensions of eudai-monic well-being proposed by Ryff and Keyes (1995).

3 There is an earlier analysis that focused exclusively on cross-sectionaldata from the first survey (Karasawa et al., 2011). The current analysisexplicitly takes longitudinal change information into account and thus islikely to reveal the age trajectory more accurately.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

574 KITAYAMA, BERG, AND CHOPIK

Page 9: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

to an emphasis on positive, future-oriented vigor and enthusi-asm. We also suggested that the ethos of interdependence istied to contrastingly “calm” adjustment to age-graded roles.These links, however, could be particular instantiations ofindependence or interdependence (Kitayama, San Martin, &Savani, 2019; San Martin et al., 2018). They may be contin-gent on local social ecologies, demographics, and the like(Kitayama & Uskul, 2011). This complex contingency has yetto be fully unpacked. Moreover, other factors, unrelated toindependence or interdependence, could also be operative.

Second, one major shortcoming of the current literaturecomes from the fact that it is based exclusively on self-reports.This begs a number of intriguing questions. For example, canthe emotional positivity posited for American older adults beobserved in the activity of a reward-processing network of thebrain? Alternatively, might the putative decline of meaning inlife Americans be revealed in, say, defensive responses tothreats? Future work must utilize diverse methodologies, in-cluding both behavioral and neural measures, to seek furthersupport for our thesis (Kitayama, Varnum, & Salvador, 2018).In the process, the current framework will have to be modifiedand refined.

Third, we portrayed a positive side of aging for Asians,which might have inadvertently highlighted somewhat nega-tive sides of aging for Americans. However, the Americannorm that emphasizes “youthful” energy may sometimes leadto positive outcomes, especially for older adults who are phys-ically and mentally fit. Conversely, the Asian age-graded so-cial norms can prematurely encourage healthy older adults toquit jobs or stop active involvement in social circles andactivities. Clearly, successful aging is a multifaceted process(Rowe & Kahn, 1997), and we hope that our review can serveas a beginning of the effort to fully explicate the bio-culturaldynamic that plays out over the life course.

In closing, we wish to note that a critical mission of scientificresearch is to offer well-informed prescriptions for the public

good, including concerning healthy aging. This mission maybe well served by a self-conscious effort to learn from othercultures and the wisdom they have accumulated over genera-tions. For example, one may learn from Asian older adults thatactive adjustment to age-graded roles and tasks is not an act ofdespair or passivity. On the contrary, this may be a way to beboth respected and accepted by their community. It may enableone to cultivate new age-adjusted ways of contributing to thecommunity and eventually to society at large. This view wastraditionally upheld in many Asian societies (as revealed, e.g.,in Confucianism and Buddhism). It is age-old. And this cul-turally cultivated idea of aging may have to be reevaluated andperhaps reappreciated in both Eastern and Western culturesalike. It can even be a basis for intervention programs to encour-age older adults to actively engage in society and thereby tocultivate a meaningful life in the last decades of their lives.

References

Ackerman, L., & Chopik, W. J. (2020). Cross-cultural comparisons inimplicit and explicit bias. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan StateUniversity, Lansing, MI.

Arnett, J. J. (2016). Life stage concepts across history and cultures:Proposal for a new field on indigenous life stages. Human Development,59, 290–316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000453627

Barber, S. J., Seliger, J., Yeh, N., & Tan, S. C. (2019). Stereotype threatreduces the positivity of older adults’ recall. Journals of Gerontology:Series B, 74, 585–594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby026

Bedford, O., & Yeh, K.-H. (2019). The history and the future of thepsychology of filial piety: Chinese norms to contextualized personalityconstruct. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 100. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00100

Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., &Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality corre-lates, and cultural boundaries: Correction. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 70, 1114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1114

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking timeseriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psycholo-gist, 54, 165–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

Figure 6. Life-span trajectory in the meaning of life in three domains—personal growth, purpose in life, andself-acceptance—and positive relations with others.

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

575CULTURE AND WELL-BEING IN LATE ADULTHOOD

Page 10: Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence · Culture and Well-Being in Late Adulthood: Theory and Evidence Shinobu Kitayama and Martha K. Berg University of Michigan

Chopik, W. J., Berg, M., & Kitayama, S. (2019). The meaning in life acrosslifespan: A U.S. and Japan comparison. Unpublished manuscript, Mich-igan State University, Lansing, MI.

Chopik, W. J., & Kitayama, S. (2018). Personality change across the lifespan: Insights from a cross-cultural, longitudinal study. Journal ofPersonality, 86, 508–521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12332

Frankl, V. E. (2006). Man’s search for meaning (1st ed.). Boston, MA:Beacon Press.

Fung, H. H., Isaacowitz, D. M., Lu, A. Y., & Li, T. (2010). Interdependentself-construal moderates the age-related negativity reduction effect inmemory and visual attention. Psychology and Aging, 25, 321–329.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019079

Fung, H. H., Isaacowitz, D. M., Lu, A. Y., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., &Wilson, H. R. (2008). Age-related positivity enhancement is not univer-sal: Older Chinese look away from positive stimuli. Psychology andAging, 23, 440–446. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.440

Grossmann, I., Karasawa, M., Kan, C., & Kitayama, S. (2014). A culturalperspective on emotional experiences across the life span. Emotion, 14,679–692. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036041

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory oflife-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017668

Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is therea universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106,766–794. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.766

Hong, J. H., Charles, S. T., Lee, S., & Lachman, M. E. (2019). Perceivedchanges in life satisfaction from the past, present and to the future: Acomparison of U.S. and Japan. Psychology and Aging, 34, 317–329.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000345

Karasawa, M., Curhan, K. B., Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S. S., Love, G. D.,Radler, B. T., & Ryff, C. D. (2011). Cultural perspectives on aging andwell-being: A comparison of Japan and the United States. InternationalJournal of Aging & Human Development, 73, 73–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/AG.73.1.d

Kitayama, S. (2000). Cultural variations in cognition: Implications foraging research. In P. C. Stern & L. L. Carstensen (Eds.), The aging mind:Opportunities in cognitive research (pp. 218–237). Washington, DC:National Academies Press.

Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009).A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North Amer-ica, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 97, 236–255. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015999

Kitayama, S., San Martin, A., & Savani, K. (2019). Varieties of interde-pendence and the emergence of the modern West: Toward the global-izing of psychology. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan,Ann Arbor, MI.

Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Currentevidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357

Kitayama, S., Varnum, M. W. E., & Salvador, C. E. (2018). Culturalneuroscience. In D. Cohen & S. Kitayama (Eds.), Handbook of culturalpsychology (2nd ed., pp. 79–118). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., De Bolle,M., Costa, P. T., Jr., . . . Yik, M. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging, 24,941–954. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016901

Luong, G., Charles, S. T., & Fingerman, K. L. (2011). Better with age:Social relationships across adulthood. Journal of Social and PersonalRelationships, 28, 9–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407510391362

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implicationsfor cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practicesemphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 311–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286003

Nakagawa, T., Cho, J., Gondo, Y., Martin, P., Johnson, M. A., Poon, L. W.,& Hirose, N. (2018). Subjective well-being in centenarians: A compar-ison of Japan and the United States. Aging & Mental Health, 22,1313–1320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1348477

Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture andsystems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. PsychologicalReview, 108, 291–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291

North, M. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2015). Modern attitudes toward older adultsin the aging world: A cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychological Bul-letin, 141, 993–1021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039469

Pethtel, O., & Chen, Y. (2010). Cross-cultural aging in cognitive andaffective components of subjective well-being. Psychology and Aging,25, 725–729. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018511

Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Gerontologist,37, 433–440.

Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychologicalwell-being revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69,719–727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

San Martin, A., Sinaceur, M., Madi, A., Tompson, S., Maddux, W. W., &Kitayama, S. (2018). Self-assertive interdependence in Arab culture.Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 830 – 837. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0435-z

Sims, T., Tsai, J. L., Jiang, D., Wang, Y., Fung, H. H., & Zhang, X. (2015).Wanting to maximize the positive and minimize the negative: Implica-tions for mixed affective experience in American and Chinese contexts.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 292–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0039276

Tan, S. C., & Barber, S. J. (2018). Confucian values as a buffer againstage-based stereotype threat for Chinese older adults. Journals of Ger-ontology: Series B: gby049 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby049

Taylor, C. (1989). Sources of the self: The making of the modern identity.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Thomson, R., Yuki, M., Talhelm, T., Schug, J., Kito, M., Ayanian, A. H.,. . . Visserman, M. L. (2018). Relational mobility predicts social behav-iors in 39 countries and is tied to historical farming and threat. Proceed-ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States ofAmerica, 115, 7521–7526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713191115

Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., Seppala, E., Fung, H. H., & Yeung, D. Y. (2007).Influence and adjustment goals: Sources of cultural differences in idealaffect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1102–1117.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1102

Tsai, J. L., Sims, T., Qu, Y., Thomas, E., Jiang, D., & Fung, H. H. (2018).Valuing excitement makes people look forward to old age less and dreadit more. Psychology and Aging, 33, 975–992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pag0000295

United Nations. (2017). World population prospects: The 2017 revision,Volume II: Demographic profiles (ST/ESA/SERA/400). Retrieved fromhttps://population.un.org/wpp/Publications/Files/WPP2017_Volume-II-Demographic-Profiles.pdf

Wagner, J., Ram, N., Smith, J., & Gerstorf, D. (2016). Personality traitdevelopment at the end of life: Antecedents and correlates of mean-leveltrajectories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 411–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000071

Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing outand standing in. American Psychologist, 39, 955–969.

Received June 1, 2019Revision received January 21, 2020

Accepted January 22, 2020 �

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

576 KITAYAMA, BERG, AND CHOPIK